Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Psychiatry, 17 January 2024
Sec. ADHD
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights In ADHD: 2023 View all 7 articles

Arousal dysregulation and executive dysfunction in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)

  • 1Center for Social and Cognitive Neuroscience, School of Psychology, Universidad Adolfo Ibáñez, Santiago, Chile
  • 2School of Psychology, Pontificia Universidad Católica de Chile, Santiago, Chile

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a heterogeneous neurodevelopmental condition, that continues to have an elusive etiological background. A number of extant models and theories have historically intended to explain the many factors contributing to ADHD behaviors. One of the most accepted hypotheses has been the executive dysfunction theory associating reduction in executive control to abnormalities in structure and operational dysfunction of dopaminergic signaling networks. Nevertheless, executive functions are not always impaired in ADHD, and the literature describes other symptoms commonly reported suggesting individuals with ADHD would appear to suffer from a more general deficit. Another existing line of research, that has gained much attention recently, establishes that ADHD would have dysregulated states of brain arousal that would account for its commonly observed cognitive deficits and behavioral symptoms, described as the state regulation theory, which has now included measures of autonomic function. This article describes some important aspects that compose and challenge these two most influential theoretical constructs, executive dysfunction and state-regulation, based on their empirical evidence, implying the need to reevaluate the norms used to classify individuals and establish ADHD diagnosis. Large number of controversial results continue to exist within the study of ADHD biological and/or performance markers, possibly due to such heterogeneity and variability within the same diagnosis. The need to resolve these issues and establish newly revised diagnostic criteria for ADHD is critical, as therapeutic success depends on having accurately identified underlying neurophysiological factors in order to appropriately address them in treatment.

1 Introduction

Attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by the presence of inattention and/or hyperactivity and impulsiveness (1) associated to disturbances in the maturation process of executive function (EF) (2, 3) and with high risk levels of developing co-morbidity with other mental disorders (such as anxiety, depression, and personality disorders). The prevalence of ADHD worldwide is approximately 3 to 9.5% (4) predominantly affecting children although many adults also suffer from personal and social impairment due to ADHD. Since it was first described as a Hyperkinetic Reaction of Childhood in the DSM-II in 1968, and later introduced as the current term of ADHD in the DSM-III-R (5), many theoretical constructs have emerged with the intention of explaining the underlying causes of this disorder. However, this diagnosis continues to be based exclusively on behavioral symptoms since many aspects of its etiology are still poorly understood (6).

One of the most currently accepted hypotheses of its underlying physiopathology is the deficit in dopamine-signaling mechanisms, associated to genetic factors encoding for dopamine receptor DRD4 and dopamine transporter DAT1, affecting prefrontal cortex, basal ganglia, thalamus, and amygdala circuits, which participate in EF (7). These neural constructs are directly relevant to the executive dysfunctional theory, introduced by Barkley (2), which suggest a reduction of executive control associated to abnormalities in frontoparietal and frontostriatal network function (8). Anatomical and functional studies have found evidence of structural differences and altered activation of the prefrontal cortex, frontoparietal and frontostriatal circuits in children with ADHD (9) supporting this hypothesis, in addition to dopaminergic and noradrenergic neurotransmitter dysfunction (10) which is critical to the operational efficiency of these circuits. This would explain impulsivity and distractibility as deficits in EF (such as in response inhibition) which would directly impact the ability to sustain attention to goal directed tasks and self-regulate social–emotional behaviors. However, the hyperactivity aspect of ADHD is largely ignored by this approach (11) in conjunction of other manifestations commonly documented in ADHD such as motor impairments (12, 13) particularly balance and gait disorders (14, 15). Despite the fact that for many decades the hypothesis that a primary EF deficit underlies ADHD behavioral symptoms has predominated in research studies, empirical findings have challenged these assumptions questioning EF deficits as a single etiology in ADHD (1618) suggesting that poor neuropsychological performance might also relate to motivational or activation deficits (11).

A different approach explaining ADHD etiopathogeneses was developed through the state-regulation theory (i.e., cognitive-energetic model) introduced by Sanders (19) addressing neurophysiological autonomic dysregulation directly impacting brain arousal as a key underlying factor to ADHD behavioral and cognitive symptoms (20).

Arousal (or brain arousal) refers to a physiological dimension of functional brain activation comprising different levels of wakefulness in adaptation for situational requirements (21). The physiological state of arousal influences cognitive activity through locus coeruleus (LC) connectivity (22) and norepinephrine pathways, coordinated by the central autonomic network (CAN) (23). The CAN consists of interconnected areas between cortical, including insular and medial prefrontal cortices, and subcortical structures such as amygdala, hypothalamus, periaqueductal gray, parabrachial regions of pons, nucleus of the solitary tract, and ventrolateral medulla (24). The main function of the CAN is to maintain homeostasis in the current and predicted behavioral context (25) and is also integrated with affective, motivational, and cognitive processes reflected in brain function (24) which include arousal regulation (26). Optimal levels of brain arousal that induce a physiologically activated state are required to support cognitive processes (22, 24). This notion extends the original Yerkes-Dodson principle of arousal and psychophysiological engagement (27), which states the need to increase level of brain arousal in order to meet cognitive challenges. Therefore, relevant for executive functioning is the regulation of arousal through autonomic function, and the adaption of a physiological activation state (i.e., alertness, wakefulness, vigilance) to meet environmental and situational needs, so that the optimal balance between energy mobilization and conservation for responsiveness can be obtained (28).

At present state-regulation hypotheses have gained much attention and interest in research, as increasing evidence is showing altered patterns of autonomic balance in ADHD subjects, particularly in the component of arousal regulation (29) where difficulties in acquiring and sustaining an optimal physiological activation state would explain cognitive, as well as motor, performance deficits in ADHD subjects (30, 31). Arousal regulation theories of ADHD not only explain inattention and impulsivity through non-optimal regulatory processes that affect cortical EF, but also interpret hyperactivity and sensation seeking as an autoregulatory reaction to an unstable regulation of brain arousal (32) thereby addressing all 3 core aspects of ADHD diagnostic criteria.

Nonetheless, studies continue to show mixed results and large variability of how and when symptoms manifest. Many attempts have been made to identify subgroups within ADHD based on neurobiological and/or neuropsychological dimensions. Is ADHD’s disorganized behavior a consequence of deficient higher order EF control or is cognitive impairment a consequence of underlying dysregulated neurophysiological mechanisms? Another thought is the probability that ADHD diagnosis might be harboring a series of different etiologies, all sharing similar behavioral symptoms but different underlying neuropathology. If this were the case, would the study of autonomic neurophysiological indicators subserve as a means to identify a separate subgroup within ADHD? This would become relevant for more successful therapeutic treatment.

Presently, an etiological basis for ADHD continues to remain elusive, as have been efforts to subtype ADHD using biological indicators rather than solely relying on clinical assessments (18) as ADHD is considered a heterogenous condition with regard to its underlying biology and physiology, leading to variable characterizations of its symptoms. Here we will discuss part of the evidence incorporated in the theoretical constructs of executive dysfunction and state regulation hypotheses, to further suggest an integrated perspective for the observed variability in behavioral and cognitive symptoms in ADHD.

2 Executive dysfunction theory

EFs are defined as cognitive processes that underlie goal-directed behavior (33) involving high-order prefrontal and parietal cortical areas (34) representing a top-down approach that affects the voluntary guidance of attention, allowing for voluntary processing of relevant over irrelevant input according to task goals (35). Research on executive functions has emerged mainly on studying patients with frontal lobe damage where neuropsychological studies demonstrate severe problems in the regulation of goal-oriented behavior (36), particularly in inhibitory control, suggesting similar etiology between ADHD symptoms and presumed cognitive deficits and those of patients with frontal lobe disorders (16). The Executive Dysfunction theory suggests that the symptoms of ADHD arise wholly as a result of a reduction in executive control, caused by abnormalities in the structure, function and biochemical operation of the frontoparietal and frontostriatal neural networks (11).

In the neuropsychological assessment of ADHD, cognitive tasks are frequently used to determine whether differences in a particular neurocognitive domain would distinguish individuals with ADHD from typically developing peers. In a meta-analysis done by Frazier et al. (37), which examined the magnitude of differences in performance on several neuropsychological measures between ADHD and controls, reported that overall cognitive abilities are significantly lower among ADHD subjects, including significant impairment on all EF tasks when compared to typically developing controls. A subsequent meta-analysis by Willcutt et al. (38) looked at studies which utilized measure specifically for EF in response inhibition, working memory, set shifting, and interference control; and although results showed differences between ADHD and controls on all EF tasks suggesting that ADHD is associated with weaknesses in several key EF domains, not all aspects of EF were equally impaired, indicating that these results would not support the hypothesis that EF deficits are the single necessary and sufficient cause of all ADHD. Spatial working memory and inhibitory control, considered to be two core EF domains impaired in ADHD, failed to find group differences in a study by Brocki et al. (39) leading to conclude that these deficits are not dependent on variation in cognitive modality but rather on the effect of difficulty level or cognitive load put on the executive control system.

Deficits on non-executive functioning cognitive measures and in other developmental areas have also been reported in ADHD (4042). For example, past studies have revealed that a subset of children with ADHD have abnormal gait and balance (43, 44) evidencing an association of these impairments with vestibular (45) and/or cerebellar dysfunction (46, 47). These findings reflect the possibility of altered underlying processes affecting cognition, but not impairment on cognition itself, calling into question the specificity of neuropsychological deficits, whether ADHD is characterized by mild global cognitive inefficiencies or by specific deficits affecting overall cognitive performance.

Brain imaging techniques have been largely applied to the study of ADHD, where magnetic resonance imaging studies have revealed smaller global brain volumes in ADHD subjects, particularly children (48, 49). Moreover, neuroimaging literature on ADHD is inconclusive with small effect sizes unable to inform clinical practice (50) as these differences are not specific to ADHD, where neuroimaging studies on other developmental disorders, such as autism, have also shown similar differences in brain volume (51, 52) when compared to controls.

From a pharmaceutical perspective, methylphenidate hydrochloride (MPH) is recommended as first line medication in clinical treatment guidelines for ADHD. The exact mechanism of action of MPH in ADHD is not completely understood, but they are presumed to act through the dopaminergic and adrenergic pathways of the frontostriatal areas in the brain by blocking the reuptake of dopamine into the presynaptic cleft (53) supported by executive dysfunction theoretical constructs on dopamine signaling deficits. The efficacy of MPH as documented in terms of reduction of at least one core symptom as determined by a parent or teacher using rating scales has been estimated at 70% (54), however, with adverse effects largely associated (most common are anorexia, insomnia, irritability) (55) and an estimated 30% of ADHD children resulting with severe negative outcomes (56) suggesting a different neural mechanism for their symptoms.

Another important aspect is that the current DSM-5 considers ADHD a neurodevelopmental condition (1) as symptoms exhibit normative change over time (57). Studies by Shaw et al. show ADHD follow a similar sequential pattern of typical cortical development yet delayed by as much as 2–3 years, depending upon the specific cortical region, concluding that the congruent delay in both cortical thickness and surface area in ADHD represents a global perturbation in the mechanisms that guide cortical maturation (58, 59). Therefore, higher order networks might not be directly impaired in ADHD, but influenced by other circuits which might be altering the optimal development and performance of higher order cognitive functions such as EF.

3 State-regulation theory

State-regulation theory suggests that a decreased ability to regulate arousal may contribute to the higher-level cognitive deficits in ADHD. This theory constructed in the cognitive-energetic model emphasizes a deficit of energetic (alerting) factors among ADHD patients, leading to both executive dysfunction and hyperactivity symptoms (20, 60). It states that overall efficiency of cognitive information processing is determined by state factors, also considered “energetic pools” (effort, arousal, and activation), as much as it is by computational factors (cognitive processing, executive control). The effort pool is characterized by assimilating the necessary energy to meet demands of a task, said to be activated when the current energetic state of the organism does not meet the state required to perform a task. It includes the arousal factor, defined as a phasic response that is time-locked to stimulus processing and is typically influenced by signal intensity and novelty, and behaviorally indexed by sleep–wake patterns; and the activation factor defined as the tonic physiological readiness to respond. The cognitive-energetic approach suggests that aspects of higher order executive control are dependent upon the energetic state of the individual, therefore inhibition deficits associated with ADHD may, at least in part, be due to energetic dysfunction since reduction in energy predicts failures of inhibition (61).

This follows closely the line of research earlier developed by Posner & Petersen (62) who identified three specialized attentional neural networks subtending three different attentional functions: (1) alerting or sustained attentional system– defined as achieving (phasic alerting) and maintaining (tonic alerting or vigilance) a general state of activation (or arousal) of the cognitive system for prolonged periods of time, (2) orienting or directed attentional system– defined as allocating the attentional focus to potentially relevant sensory events, and (3) executive control or selective attentional system – defined as the ability to control and inhibit impulsive responses focusing to what is relevant to the task at hand as to achieve intended goals. This attentional network perspective proposes that attention is an organic system that comprises a variety of neural processes including cortical top-down control as much as bottom-up influences (63).

The alerting system, described as the ability to prepare and sustain a vigilant state to cognitively process high priority signals as proposed by Posner and Petersen (62), has been associated to the noradrenergic system, including the right frontal and parietal lobe and the LC modulating attention and arousal (64) which allows for prolonged sustained attention to task. In other words, maintaining vigilance or sustained attention to task requires an increased and regulated arousal, with the specific role of the noradrenergic nucleus LC in the induction and regulation of cortical activation (65), attentional shifting (66) and modulating forebrain networks mediating cognitive activity, especially those related to the prefrontal cortex (22). This relationship is such that arousal level acts as a parameter which will alter the overall efficiency of cognitive performance (67). Stronger arousal reactions have been associated with better focused attention (68) emphasizing the role of heightened sympathetic activity in more efficient attention related cognitive processing (69). A number of studies have demonstrated that cognitive challenges tend to elicit increases in sympathetic activation (70) as arousal must increase to meet the energy expenditure required to invest in cognitive capacity (68). The implication of the LC in behavioral and cognitive processes probably involves a complex and dynamic interaction of LC with both subcortical structures controlling autonomic arousal and cortical structures directly involved in attentional and executive functions (22). This has led to suggest that alterations in the ability to regulate arousal may contribute to the cognitive and attentional deficits commonly found in ADHD.

Neurophysiological studies in ADHD support the hypothesis of arousal dysregulation as an underlying feature of ADHD, measuring activity from the autonomic nervous system (ANS) (29) as links between arousal and cognition are governed by interactions between central nervous system and autonomic networks (71). As previously mentioned, one of the main brainstem regions mediating interactions between ANS and cortical areas is the LC (66). The LC, one of the main sources of norepinephrine supplying the cortex, influences a range of cognitive functions including perception, working memory, and sustained attention (22) where the prefrontal cortex, essential for top-down regulation of attention and behavior, is especially sensitive to neurochemical environment, and small changes in levels of norepinephrine and dopamine produces significant changes in its functions (72).

Autonomic function measures have been used to record arousal state and change in ADHD research during cognitive and attentional effort, as LC-norepinephrine activates sympathetic networks resulting in excitatory effects increasing heart rate (HR) (73) electrodermal activity (EDA) (74) and dilation pupil size (75, 76). These ANS peripheral indices have been recorded during resting state as well as during task performance and are known to show differences between ADHD and controls (29) more often in the direction of reduced sympathetic activation in ADHD suggesting a generalized hypo-aroused state (7779) especially during effortful cognitive tasks. For instance, lower skin conductance level (tonic component of EDA) in ADHD children during resting state (8083) and during cognitive tasks, such in continuous performance (84) and reaction time tasks (85) have been reported. Pupil size has been found to be reduced among off-medication children and adolescents with ADHD performing a visuo-spatial working memory task, suggesting signs of hypo-arousal in ADHD resulting in specific difficulties in allocating constant and appropriate levels of attentional resources during tasks involving executive function abilities (86). Dysregulation of cortisol morning levels in ADHD have also been reported (87) suggesting difficulties in activation of sympathetic driven arousal.

Furthermore, advances in pharmaceutical development have also targeted arousal networks for ADHD treatments, as an alternative to the classically administered methylphenidate. Atomoxetine, a drug effectively used for ADHD treatment, acts as an inhibitor highly selective of noradrenergic reuptake (72) and evidences a decrease in distractibility, motor hyperactivity, and an improvement of cognitive functions contributing to the therapeutic effects of stimulants in patients with ADHD (88, 89) through stimulation of the LC-norepinephrine system. However, this is not always effective with a reported success rate of approximately 40% (90).

4 Integrating the evidence

At present no single model has been developed explaining all heterogeneity in ADHD symptoms. Executive dysfunction theory intends to explain ADHD disruptive and disorganized behavior from a top-down deficiency in inhibitory control but is highly dependent on task and contextual characteristics leaving several aspects of ADHD unexplained. State-regulation theory better explains variability in ADHD performance considering the activation-state and energy distribution as having direct influence on cognitive outcomes, however, during cognitive effortful tasks an optimal activation state not only must be reached but also kept at optimal level according to task demands, which would require top-down regulatory influence. This self-regulatory control not only is defined as the ability to inhibit but also to activate responses, through top-down neurocognitive processes in the service of goal attainment (91) and deliberate use of EF (92). This again calls to consider the executive dysfunction theoretical construct, however top-down activation and sustained brain arousal required for optimal cognitive performance is dependent on bottom-up energy mobilization circuits responding accordingly. Therefore, bottom-up and top-down regulatory effects function as a continuum, successive reciprocal neural feedback loops, modulating and optimizing one another during cognitive tasks (93) which results in a regulated behavior and stable performance. One of the repeated manifestations observed in ADHD is the high prevalence of intra-individual variability and inconsistency in performance (16) which may possibly be related to activation state irregularity. Findings leading to support the idea that impaired arousal regulation contributes to higher-level cognitive deficits in ADHD, is that during cognitive performance, when tasks require less effort, children with ADHD exhibit fewer cognitive impairments, reaching similar performance level as controls (30, 85, 94). Some studies show that the event rate (the speed with which stimuli are presented) affects the energetic state and level of performance in ADHD. A slow event rate (which would induce a rather underarousal/underactivation state) exhibit poor performance in ADHD with a significant slowing in reaction times (30, 95, 96), whereas a fast event rate would tend to increase arousal therefore improve attention and performance. ADHD performance improvement has been evidenced when stimulus rate is optimal (96) and when rewards are given (97) consistent with the assumption that the underlying impairment in ADHD is associated with the ability to self-regulate arousal rather than having a constant under or over aroused physiological state.

To date the origin for ADHD impairment to self-regulate arousal remains unclear. Some insights on this matter might be achieved looking at how the condition tends to evolve over time in children diagnosed with ADHD, where two important aspects should be considered. The first one, is ADHD’s high comorbidity with many other developmental disorders and conditions, reporting that as many as 60% of children with ADHD are to experience a comorbid condition across their lifespan (98). Among the most common are oppositional defiant and conduct disorders (99) as well as anxiety (100) and mood disorders (101). The second aspect of ADHD is that it is marked as being the most heritable psychiatric disorder, estimating its heritability at 80% (102) where offspring of parents with ADHD are at significant risk for ADHD and its associated psychiatric, cognitive, and educational impairments (103). Both of these features might suggest ADHD symptoms as early indicators of emerging difficulties in neurodevelopment. In other words, what we are labeling as ADHD as a single diagnosis could be the first symptomatic evidence in a spectrum of emerging future disorders. Children that begin early in development showing signs of ADHD symptoms which if not addressed in a timely manner will progressively develop into other psychiatric diagnoses later in life (for example, elevated body temperature is an early indicator of an emerging disease among many possible outcomes, but not a diagnosis in itself). Within the ADHD diagnosed group of children, a variety of underlying etiopathogeneses might be emerging. Identifying biological markers, such as autonomic manifestations, closely correlated with behavioral outcomes under specific conditions, can be a means of identifying a subgroup (possibly still at a subthreshold of becoming a psychiatric condition in the future) and tailor treatments according to more specific deficits before they develop into other diagnoses.

5 Conclusion

During the last two decades authors have been continuously suggesting the need to develop an updated integrative model to explain ADHD, considering the high probability that only a subset of behaviorally defined children will have deficits in a given neurocognitive mechanism believed to contribute to the disorder (17, 18, 104). To date the large amount of heterogeneity in ADHD diagnosed individuals remains and studies continue to report inconsistent results when trying to find a single underlying etiology or biological marker. Although numerous studies seem to support the hypothesis that the core problem in ADHD is an unstable arousal regulation (32, 94, 105) these continue to show mixed results (29).

The large variability and heterogeneity in performance found in children and older individuals diagnosed with ADHD supports the need for a better understanding and definition of these observed behaviors to better correlate each symptom with corresponding underlying neurobiological processes. Research is now needed to distinguish those strongest indicators and conditions for measurement that will better detect each symptom and differentiate between subjects within the ADHD diagnosis in a consistent manner, to further define subgroups or discuss an altogether differentiated diagnostic group based on neurophysiological markers. A more granular classification of a better described disorder based on known neurobiological circuits and accurate measures of these functions, would result in greater diagnostic accuracy and, most importantly, in better targeted intervention programs.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

VI: Conceptualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. VL: Writing – review & editing. ME: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This work was supported by grants from Agencia Nacional de Investigación y Desarrollo (ANID) in Chile (folio 21231682) and funds from the Research Department of Adolfo Ibañez University.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. (5th ed., text rev.; DSM-5-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association Publishing.

Google Scholar

2. Barkley, RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. (1997) 121:65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Savcı, U, Tufan, AE, Öztürk, Y, and Cansız, MA. Executive function problems and treatment in children and adolescents with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Psikiyatride Guncel Yaklasimlar - Current Approaches in Psychiatry. (2019) 11: 223–38. doi: 10.18863/pgy.424793

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Polanczyk, G, Silva de Lima, M, Lessa Horta, B, Biederman, J, and Augusto Rohde, L. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. (2007) 164:942–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Pehlivanidis, A, and Papanikolaou, K. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD): from hyperkinetic to neurodevelopmental disorders. Archives of Hellenic Med. (2022) 39:151–62.

Google Scholar

6. Drechsler, R, Brem, S, Brandeis, D, Grünblatt, E, Berger, G, and Walitza, S. ADHD: current concepts and treatments in children and adolescents. Neuropediatrics. (2020) 51:315–35. doi: 10.1055/s-0040-1701658

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Aboitiz, F, Ossandón, T, Zamorano, F, Palma, B, and Carrasco, X. Irrelevant stimulus processing in ADHD: catecholamine dynamics and attentional networks. Front Psychol. (2014) 5:183. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00183

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Antshel, KM, Hier, BO, and Barkley, RA. Executive functioning theory and ADHD In: S Goldstein and JA Naglieri (editors). Handbook of executive functioning. New York: Springer (2014). 107–20.

Google Scholar

9. Hoogman, M, Muetzel, R, Guimaraes, JP, Shumskaya, E, Mennes, M, Zwiers, MP, et al. Brain imaging of the cortex in ADHD: a coordinated analysis of large-scale clinical and population-based samples. Am J Psychiatry. (2019) 176:531–42. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.18091033

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Arnsten, AFT. Catecholamine influences on dorsolateral prefrontal cortical networks. Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 69:e89–e99. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Johnson, KA, Wiersema, JR, and Kuntsi, J. What would Karl popper say? Are current psychological theories of ADHD falsifiable? Behav Brain Funct. (2009) 5:15. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-5-15

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Farran, EK, Bowler, A, D’Souza, H, Mayall, L, Karmiloff-Smith, A, Sumner, E, et al. Is the motor impairment in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) a co-occurring deficit or a phenotypic characteristic? Advan Neurodevelop Disor. (2020) 4:253–70. doi: 10.1007/s41252-020-00159-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Goulardins, JB, Marques, JCB, and De Oliveira, JA. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and motor impairment: a critical review. Percept Mot Skills. (2017) 124:425–40. doi: 10.1177/0031512517690607

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Buderath, P, Gärtner, K, Frings, M, Christiansen, H, Schoch, B, Konczak, J, et al. Postural and gait performance in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Gait Posture. (2009) 29:249–54. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.08.016

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Caldani, S, Acquaviva, E, Moscoso, A, Landman, B, Michel, A, Delorme, R, et al. Motor capabilities in children with ADHD are improved after brief visuopostural training. Neurol Int. (2023) 15:792–803. doi: 10.3390/neurolint15030050

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Castellanos, FX, Sonuga-Barke, EJS, Milham, MP, and Tannock, R. Characterizing cognition in ADHD: beyond executive dysfunction. Trends Cogn Sci. (2006) 10:117–23. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Castellanos, FX, and Tannock, R. Neuroscience of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the search for endophenotypes. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2002) 3:896. doi: 10.1038/nrn896

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Posner, J, Polanczyk, GV, and Sonuga-Barke, E. Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Lancet. (2020) 395:450–62. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(19)33004-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sanders, AF. Towards a model of stress and human performance. Acta Psychol (Amst). (1983) 53:61–97. doi: 10.1016/0001-6918(83)90016-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Sergeant, J. The cognitive-energetic model: an empirical approach to attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2000) 24:7–24. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00060-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Huang, J, Ulke, C, and Strauss, M. Brain arousal regulation and depressive symptomatology in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). BMC Neurosci. (2019) 20:43. doi: 10.1186/s12868-019-0526-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Sara, SJ, and Bouret, S. Orienting and reorienting: the locus coeruleus mediates cognition through arousal. Neuron. (2012) 76:130–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.011

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Benarroch, EE. The central autonomic network: functional organization, dysfunction, and perspective. Mayo Clin Proc. (1993) 68:988–1001. doi: 10.1016/S0025-6196(12)62272-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Quadt, L, Critchley, H, and Nagai, Y. Cognition, emotion, and the central autonomic network. Autonomic Neurosci: Basic and Clinical. (2022) 238:102948. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2022.102948

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Shouman, K, and Benarroch, EE. Central autonomic network In: S Chokroverty and P Cortelli, editors. Autonomic nervous system and sleep. Cham: Springer (2021)

Google Scholar

26. McEwen, BS. Central role of the brain in stress and adaptation: allostasis, biological embedding, and cumulative change In: G Fink (editor). Stress: Concepts, cognition, emotion, and behavior: Handbook of stress. Netherlands: Elsevier (2016). 39–55.

Google Scholar

27. Yerkes, R, and Dodson, J. The relation of strength of stimulus to rapidity of habit-formation. J Comparative Neurorol Psychol. (1908) 18:459–82. doi: 10.1002/cne.920180503

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sander, C, Hensch, T, Wittekind, DA, Böttger, D, and Hegerl, U. Assessment of wakefulness and brain arousal regulation in psychiatric research. Neuropsychobiology. (2016) 72:195–205. doi: 10.1159/000439384

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bellato, A, Arora, I, Hollis, C, and Groom, MJ. Is autonomic nervous system function atypical in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)? A systematic review of the evidence. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 108:182–206. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.11.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Börger, N, and Van Der Meere, J. Motor control and state regulation in children with ADHD: a cardiac response study. Biol Psychol. (2000) 51:247–67. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(99)00040-X

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Börger, N, Van Der Meere, J, Ronner, A, Alberts, E, Geuze, R, and Bogte, H. Heart rate variability and sustained attention in ADHD children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (1999) 27:25–33. doi: 10.1023/A:1022610306984

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Strauß, M, Ulke, C, Paucke, M, Huang, J, Mauche, N, Sander, C, et al. Brain arousal regulation in adults with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Psychiatry Res. (2018) 261:102–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.12.043

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Diamond, A. Executive functions. Annu Rev Psychol. (2013) 64:135–68. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143750

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Corbetta, M, and Shulman, GL. Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2002) 3:201–15. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Thiele, A, and Bellgrove, MA. Neuromodulation of attention. Neuron. (2018) 97:769–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.01.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Stuss, DT, and Alexander, MP. Executive functions and the frontal lobes: a conceptual view. Psychol Res. (2000) 63:289–98. doi: 10.1007/s004269900007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Frazier, TW, Demaree, HA, and Youngstrom, EA. Meta-analysis of intellectual and neuropsychological test performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology. (2004) 18:543–55. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.18.3.543

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Willcutt, EG, Doyle, AE, Nigg, JT, Faraone, SV, and Pennington, BF. Validity of the executive function theory of attention-deficit/ hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:1336–46. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Brocki, KC, Randall, KD, Bohlin, G, and Kerns, KA. Working memory in school-aged children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder combined type: are deficits modality specific and are they independent of impaired inhibitory control? J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2008) 30:749–59. doi: 10.1080/13803390701754720

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Garcia-Sanchez, C, Estevez-Gonzalez, A, Suarez-Romero, E, and Junque, C. Right hemisphere dysfunction in subjects with attention-deficit disorder with and without hyperactivity. J Child Neurol. (1997) 12:107–15. doi: 10.1177/088307389701200207

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Pievsky, MA, and McGrath, RE. The neurocognitive profile of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a review of meta-analyses. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2018) 33:143–57. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acx055

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Rucklidge, JJ, and Tannock, R. Psychiatric, psychosocial, and cognitive functioning of female adolescents with ADHD. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2001) 40:530–40. doi: 10.1097/00004583-200105000-00012

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Bucci, MP, Stordeur, C, Acquaviva, E, Peyre, H, and Delorme, R. Postural instability in children with ADHD is improved by methylphenidate. Front Neurosci. (2016) 10:163. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00163

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Shum, SBM, and Pang, MYC. Children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder have impaired balance function: involvement of somatosensory, visual, and vestibular systems. J Pediatr. (2009) 155:245–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpeds.2009.02.032

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Isaac, V, Olmedo, D, Aboitiz, F, and Delano, PH. Altered cervical vestibular-evoked myogenic potential in children with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder. Front Neurol. (2017) 8:90. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2017.00090

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Cundari, M, Vestberg, S, Gustafsson, P, Gorcenco, S, and Rasmussen, A. Neurocognitive and cerebellar function in ADHD, autism and spinocerebellar ataxia. Front Syst Neurosci. (2023) 17:1168666. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2023.1168666

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Goetz, M, Schwabova, JP, Hlavka, Z, Ptacek, R, and Surman, CBH. Dynamic balance in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder and its relationship with cognitive functions and cerebellum. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2017) 13:873–80. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S125169

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Castellanos, XF, Lee, PP, Sharp, W, Neal Jeffries, MO, Greenstein, DK, Clasen, LS, et al. Developmental trajectories of brain volume abnormalities in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. JAMA. (2002) 288:1740–8. doi: 10.1001/jama.288.14.1740

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Wang, Z, Zhou, X, Gui, Y, Liu, M, and Lu, H. Multiple measurement analysis of resting-state fMRI for ADHD classification in adolescent brain from the ABCD study. Translational. Psychiatry. (2023) 13:45. doi: 10.1038/s41398-023-02309-5

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Pereira-Sanchez, V, and Castellanos, FX. Neuroimaging in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2021) 34:105–11. doi: 10.1097/YCO.0000000000000669

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Arai, T, Kamagata, K, Uchida, W, Andica, C, Takabayashi, K, Saito, Y, et al. Reduced neurite density index in the prefrontal cortex of adults with autism assessed using neurite orientation dispersion and density imaging. Front Neurol. (2023) 14:1110883. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2023.1110883

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Van Rooij, D, Anagnostou, E, Arango, C, Auzias, G, Behrmann, M, Busatto, GF, et al. Cortical and subcortical brain morphometry differences between patients with autism spectrum disorder and healthy individuals across the lifespan: results from the ENIGMA ASD working group. Am J Psychiatry. (2018) 175:359–69. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2017.17010100

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Nair, R, and Moss Baylor, SB. Neuropsychiatric disease and treatment management of attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder in adults: focus on methylphenidate hydrochloride. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2009) 5:5–421. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S4101

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Bodey, C. Effectiveness and tolerability of methylphenidate in children and adolescents with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Clinical Med Insights: Therapeutics. (2011) 3:353–63. doi: 10.4137/CMT.S6615

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Khalili, H, Mahmoudi-Gharaei, J, Mohammadi, M, Faghihi, T, Karimzadeh, I, and Khajehpiri, Z. Adverse reactions of methylphenidate in children with attention deficit-hyperactivity disorder: report from a referral center. Journal of research. Pharm Pract. (2014) 3:130. doi: 10.4103/2279-042X.145389

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Trenque, T, Herlem, E, Abou Taam, M, and Drame, M. Methylphenidate off-label use and safety. Springerplus. (2014) 3:286. doi: 10.1186/2193-1801-3-286

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Martel, MM, Levinson, CA, Langer, JK, and Nigg, JT. A network analysis of developmental change in ADHD symptom structure from preschool to adulthood. Clin Psychol Sci. (2016) 4:988–1001. doi: 10.1177/2167702615618664

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Shaw, P, Eckstrand, K, Sharp, W, Blumenthal, J, Lerch, JP, Greenstein, D, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder is characterized by a delay in cortical maturation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2007) 104:19649–54. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0707741104

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Shaw, P, Malek, M, Watson, B, Sharp, W, Evans, A, and Greenstein, D. Development of cortical surface area and gyrification in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2012) 72:191–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.01.031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Sergeant, J. Modeling attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a critical appraisal of the cognitive/energetic model. Biol Psychiatry. (2004) 57:1248–55. doi: 10.1016/j.bps.2004.09.010

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Segerstrom, SC, and Nes, LS. Heart rate variability reflects self-regulatory strength, effort, and fatigue. Psychol Sci. (2007) 18:275–81. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01888.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Posner, MI, and Petersen, SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annu Rev Neurosci. (1990) 13:25–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.13.030190.000325

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Posner, MI, Rothbart, MK, and Ghassemzadeh, H. Restoring attention networks. Yale J Biol Med. (2019) 92:139–43.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

64. Berger, A, and Posner, MI. Pathologies of brain attentional networks. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2000) 24:3–5. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(99)00046-9

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Berridge, CW. Noradrenergic modulation of arousal. Brain Res Rev. (2008) 58:1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresrev.2007.10.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Aston-Jones, G, and Cohen, JD. Adaptive gain and the role of the locus coeruleus-norepinephrine system in optimal performance. J Comp Neurol. (2005) 493:99–110. doi: 10.1002/cne.20723

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Diamond, DM. Cognitive, endocrine and mechanistic perspectives on non-linear relationships between arousal and brain function. Nonlinearity in Biol, Toxicol, Med. (2005) 3:1–7. doi: 10.2201/nonlin.003.01.001

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Dahl, MJ, Mather, M, Sander, MC, and Werkle-Bergner, M. Noradrenergic responsiveness supports selective attention across the adult lifespan. J Neurosci. (2020) 40:4372–90. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0398-19.2020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Giuliano, RJ, Karns, CM, Bell, TA, Petersen, S, Skowron, EA, Neville, HJ, et al. Parasympathetic and sympathetic activity are associated with individual differences in neural indices of selective attention in adults. Psychophysiology. (2018) 55:e13079. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13079

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Backs, RW, and Seljos, KA. Metabolic and cardiorespiratory measures of mental effort: the effects of level of difficulty in a working memory task. Int J Psychophysiol. (1994) 16:57–68. doi: 10.1016/0167-8760(94)90042-6

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Aston-Jones, G, and Cohen, JD. An integrative theory of locus coeruleus-norepinephrine function: adaptive gain and optimal performance. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2005) 28:403–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.28.061604.135709

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Arnsten, AFT, and Pliszka, SR. Catecholamine influences on prefrontal cortical function: relevance to treatment of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder and related disorders. Pharmacol Biochem Behav. (2011) 99:211–6. doi: 10.1016/j.pbb.2011.01.020

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Wang, X, Piñol, RA, Byrne, P, and Mendelowitz, D. Optogenetic stimulation of locus ceruleus neurons augments inhibitory transmission to parasympathetic cardiac vagal neurons via activation of brainstem α1 and β1 receptors. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:6182–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5093-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Wass, SV, de Barbaro, K, and Clackson, K. Tonic and phasic co-variation of peripheral arousal indices in infants. Biol Psychol. (2015) 111:26–39. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.08.006

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Rajkowski, J, Kubiak, P, and Aston-Jones, G. Correlations between locus coeruleus (LC) neural activity, pupil diameter and behavior in monkey support a role of LC in attention. Soc Neuroscie Abstracts. (1993) 19:974.

Google Scholar

76. Reimer, J, McGinley, MJ, Liu, Y, Rodenkirch, C, Wang, Q, McCormick, DA, et al. Pupil fluctuations track rapid changes in adrenergic and cholinergic activity in cortex. Nat Commun. (2016) 7:13289. doi: 10.1038/ncomms13289

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

77. de Carvalho, TD, Wajnsztejn, R, de Abreu, LC, Marques Vanderlei, LC, Godoy, MF, Adami, F, et al. Analysis of cardiac autonomic modulation of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2014) 10:613–8. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S49071

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Griffiths, KR, Quintana, DS, Hermens, DF, Spooner, C, Tsang, TW, Clarke, S, et al. Sustained attention and heart rate variability in children and adolescents with ADHD. Biol Psychol. (2017) 124:11–20. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.01.004

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Wang, TS, Huang, WL, Kuo, TBJ, Lee, GS, and Yang, CCH. Inattentive and hyperactive preschool-age boys have lower sympathetic and higher parasympathetic activity. J Physiol Sci. (2013) 63:87–94. doi: 10.1007/s12576-012-0238-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Barry, RJ, Clarke, AR, Johnstone, SJ, McCarthy, R, and Selikowitz, M. Electroencephalogram θ/β ratio and arousal in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: evidence of independent processes. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 66:398–401. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.027

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Clarke, AR, Barry, RJ, Dupuy, FE, McCarthy, R, Selikowitz, M, and Johnstone, SJ. Excess beta activity in the EEG of children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a disorder of arousal? Int J Psychophysiol. (2013) 89:314–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.04.009

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Dupuy, FE, Clarke, AR, Barry, RJ, Selikowitz, M, and McCarthy, R. EEG and electrodermal activity in girls with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Clin Neurophysiol. (2014) 125:491–9. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2013.09.007

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Lazzaro, I, Gordon, E, Li, W, Lim, CL, Plahn, M, Whitmont, S, et al. Simultaneous EEG and EDA measures in adolescent attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Int J Psychophysiol. (1999) 34:123134. doi: 10.1016/S0167-8760(99)00068-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Lawrence, CA, Barry, RJ, Clarke, AR, Johnstone, SJ, McCarthy, R, Selikowitz, M, et al. Methylphenidate effects in attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: electrodermal and ERP measures during a continuous performance task. Psychopharmacology (Berl). (2005) 183:81–91. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0144-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

85. James, SN, Cheung, CHM, Rijsdijk, F, Asherson, P, and Kuntsi, J. Modifiable arousal in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and its etiological association with fluctuating reaction times. Biological Psychiatry: Cogn Neurosci Neuroimag. (2016) 1:539–47. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2016.06.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Wainstein, G, Rojas-Líbano, D, Crossley, NA, Carrasco, X, Aboitiz, F, and Ossandón, T. Pupil size tracks attentional performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:8228. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-08246-w

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Garzón Rodríguez, N, Briceño-Balcázar, I, Díaz-Barrera, LE, Nicolini, H, Genis-Mendoza, AD, Flores-Lázaro, JC, et al. Moderating effects of impulsivity and morning cortisol on the genotype-phenotype relationship of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in young adults. Stress Health. (2023) 1. doi: 10.1002/smi.3308

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Arnsten, AFT. Fundamentals of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: circuits and pathways. J Clin Psychiatry. (2006) 67:7–12.

Google Scholar

89. Fu, D, Wu, DD, Guo, HL, Hu, YH, Xia, Y, Ji, X, et al. The mechanism, clinical efficacy, safety, and dosage regimen of atomoxetine for ADHD therapy in children: a narrative review. Front Psych. (2022) 12:780921. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.780921

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Hammerness, P, Mccarthy, K, Mancuso, E, Gendron, C, and Geller, D. Atomoxetine for the treatment of attention-defi cit/ hyperactivity disorder in children and adolescents: a review. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2009) 5:215–26. doi: 10.2147/ndt.s3896

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Liew, J. Effortful control, executive functions, and education: bringing self-regulatory and social-emotional competencies to the table. Child Dev Perspect. (2012) 6:105–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00196.x

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Nigg, JT. Annual research review: on the relations among self-regulation, self-control, executive functioning, effortful control, cognitive control, impulsivity, risk-taking, and inhibition for developmental psychopathology. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2017) 58:361–83. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12675

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Evans, JSBT, and Stanovich, KE. Dual-process theories of higher cognition: advancing the debate. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2013) 8:223–41. doi: 10.1177/1745691612460685

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Du Rietz, E, James, SN, Banaschewski, T, Brandeis, D, Asherson, P, and Kuntsi, J. Autonomic arousal profiles in adolescents and young adults with ADHD as a function of recording context. Psychiatry Res. (2019) 275:212–20. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.03.039

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Metin, B, Roeyers, H, Wiersema, JR, Van Der Meere, J, and Sonuga-Barke, E. A meta-analytic study of event rate effects on go/no-go performance in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2012) 72:990–6. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2012.08.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Wiersema, R, van der Meere, J, Roeyers, H, Van Coster, R, and Baeyens, D. Event rate and event-related potentials in ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2006) 47:560–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01592.x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Groom, MJ, Scerif, G, Liddle, PF, Batty, MJ, Liddle, EB, Roberts, KL, et al. Effects of motivation and medication on electrophysiological markers of response inhibition in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 67:624–31. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.09.029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Kessler, RC, Adler, L, Berkley, R, Biederman, J, Conners, CK, Demler, O, et al. The prevalence and correlates of adult ADHD in the United States: results from the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Am J Psychiatry. (2006) 163:716–23. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.4.716

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Frick, PJ, and Nigg, JT. Current issues in the diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, oppositional defiant disorder, and conduct disorder. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2012) 8:77–107. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032511-143150

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

100. D’Agati, E, Curatolo, P, and Mazzone, L. Comorbidity between ADHD and anxiety disorders across the lifespan. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. (2019) 23:238–44. doi: 10.1080/13651501.2019.1628277

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Fornaro, M, Caiazza, C, and Comparelli, A. Differentiation and comorbidity of bipolar disorder and attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder in children, adolescents, and adults: a clinical and nosological perspective. Front Psych. (2022) 13:949375. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.949375

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Grimm, O, Kranz, TM, and Reif, A. Genetics of ADHD: what should the clinician know? Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2020) 22:18. doi: 10.1007/s11920-020-1141-x

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Uchida, M, DiSalvo, M, Walsh, D, and Biederman, J. The heritability of ADHD in children of ADHD parents: a post-hoc analysis of longitudinal data. J Atten Disord. (2023) 27:250–7. doi: 10.1177/10870547221136251

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Nigg, JT, Willcutt, EG, Doyle, AE, and Sonuga-Barke, EJS. Causal heterogeneity in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: do we need neuropsychologically impaired subtypes? Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:1224–30. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.08.025

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Hegerl, U, and Hensch, T. The vigilance regulation model of affective disorders and ADHD. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 44:45–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.10.008

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, arousal dysregulation, executive dysfunction, autonomic function, state regulation

Citation: Isaac V, Lopez V and Escobar MJ (2024) Arousal dysregulation and executive dysfunction in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Front. Psychiatry. 14:1336040. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1336040

Received: 09 November 2023; Accepted: 29 December 2023;
Published: 17 January 2024.

Edited by:

Susan DosReis, University of Maryland, United States

Reviewed by:

Thomas Gift, University of Rochester, United States

Copyright © 2024 Isaac, Lopez and Escobar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Valeria Isaac, mvisaac@ug.uchile.cl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.