Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 11 December 2023
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroimaging of Sleep Disturbances and Sleep Targeted Interventions View all 5 articles

Single sessions of transcranial direct current stimulation and transcranial random noise stimulation exert no effect on sleepiness in patients with narcolepsy and idiopathic hypersomnia

  • 1Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, University of Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany
  • 2Department of Hematology and Oncology, Krankenhaus der Barmherzigen Brüder Regensburg, Regensburg, Germany

Background: Hypersomnia poses major challenges to treatment providers given the limitations of available treatment options. In this context, the application of non-invasive brain stimulation techniques such as transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) may open up new avenues to effective treatment. Preliminary evidence suggests both acute and longer-lasting positive effects of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on vigilance and sleepiness in hypersomniac patients. Based on these findings, the present study sought to investigate short-term effects of single sessions of tDCS and transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) on sleepiness in persons suffering from hypersomnia.

Methods: A sample of 29 patients suffering from narcolepsy or idiopathic hypersomnia (IH) was recruited from the Regensburg Sleep Disorder Center and underwent single sessions of tES (anodal tDCS, tRNS, sham) over the left and right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex on three consecutive days in a double-blind, sham-controlled, pseudorandomized crossover trial. The primary study endpoint was the mean reaction time measured by the Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) before and directly after the daily tES sessions. Secondary endpoints were additional PVT outcome metrics as well as subjective outcome parameters (e.g., Karolinska Sleepiness Scale; KSS).

Results: There were no significant treatment effects neither on objective (i.e., PVT) nor on subjective indicators of sleepiness.

Conclusion: We could not demonstrate any clinically relevant effects of single sessions of tDCS or tRNS on objective or subjective measures of sleepiness in patients with hypersomnia. However, we cannot exclude that repeated sessions of tES may affect vigilance or sleepiness in hypersomniac patients.

1 Introduction

Narcolepsy and idiopathic hypersomnia (IH) are yet incurable neurological disorders characterized by excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) and sleep attacks despite quantitatively sufficient sleep at night for a period of at least 3 months (1). Sudden involuntary sleep attacks may occur repeatedly even under inadequate or potentially dangerous circumstances. The prevalence of narcolepsy (both type 1 and 2) is estimated at 0.03–0.06% (2, 3), whereas IH is even more rare (4, 5). In addition to EDS, the classic symptom of narcolepsy type 1 (NT 1) is cataplexy. Cataplexy is defined as a sudden loss of muscle tone of differing severity occurring in striated muscles except for the respiratory muscles. Cataplexy is provoked by strong emotions and has a short duration of usually less than 2 min (1, 6). Additional symptoms of narcolepsy (both types) include the potential occurrence of sleep paralysis, defined as the transient inability to move during the onset of sleep, and hypnagogic hallucinations, characterized by commonly frightening dream-like visual, acoustical or sensory misperceptions (7). Additionally, disrupted nocturnal sleep patterns and a decline in sleep quality, which tend to worsen with the progression of the disease, can further burden individuals afflicted by these conditions. A heterogeneous pattern of clinical symptoms and the absence of cataplexy are typical for NT 2 and IH (4, 8). NT 1 is assumed to result from a reduction or absence of the hypothalamic neuromodulatory peptide hypocretin-1 that controls and modifies the sleep–wake cycle (912), whereas the etiology of NT 2 and IH is still largely unknown (12, 13). An irregularity in rapid eye movement (REM) sleep, characterized by a shift in the timing of REM sleep characteristics within the sleep cycle or during waking periods, is a typical symptom of narcolepsy, but not of IH (8, 1416). All three diagnostic groups (NT 1, NT 2, and IH) share the characteristic finding of a short mean sleep latency of less than 8 min in the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT). The occurrence of 2 or more sleep onset REM sleep periods (SOREM) with a latency of less than 15 min from sleep onset in the MSLT discriminates narcolepsy (both NT 1 and NT 2) from IH (1, 1719).

At the brain level, mounting neuroimaging evidence has suggested several brain regions to play a role in the pathophysiology of EDS disorders (20, 21). Hence, structural and functional findings imply that the frontal cortex plays a crucial role in narcolepsy. Studies have demonstrated frontal cortex hypoactivity to be associated with the pathophysiology of narcolepsy. This hypoactivity also affects several diencephalic and cortical areas connected to the so-called orexin network. These findings underscore the potential importance of the frontal cortex in regulating sleep–wake states and emotional processes that may play a role in the manifestation of narcolepsy symptoms. Some of the neuroimaging studies on IH suggest a dysfunction or alterations in the frontal cortex. Reduced regional cerebral blood flow (rCBF) in the medial prefrontal cortex and posterior cingulate cortex was observed in a SPECT study (22). Taken together, there is preliminary evidence to suggest that hypofrontality plays a role in both narcolepsy and IH.

Pharmacological treatment options of EDS include amphetamine- or other-type stimulants (23, 24). However, patients with narcolepsy and IH frequently experience loss of effectiveness of stimulant drugs, drug intolerance and dose limiting side-effects (2427). Due to these limitations, there is an urgent need for further treatment options for patients with hypersomnia, and transcranial electrical stimulation (tES) has been suggested as a potential new non-pharmacological therapy (2831). tES represents an emerging non-invasive neuromodulatory technique enabling electrical stimulation of the brain (32). Amongst others, transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) and transcranial random noise stimulation (tRNS) fall into this category. In both procedures, weak electric currents with intensities of 1–2 mA are applied to the human brain by pairs of conductive electrodes (32, 33). Anodal tDCS typically increases neuronal excitability in the area under the electrode, while cathodal tDCS typically reduces excitability (32, 3438). While tDCS operates with continuous current, tRNS is an oscillatory stimulation technique that affects electrical brain activity by using randomly generated noise current (39). Importantly, tRNS is polarity-independent and has been shown to increase neuronal excitability under both electrodes when applied in the high frequency spectrum between 100 and 640 Hz (39, 40), hence mimicking the effect of anodal tDCS. Both tDCS and tRNS have been shown to produce short- or long-lasting after-effects on neuronal excitability depending on a variety of factors such as the placement and polarity of stimulation electrodes (34, 41), the employed current density (41, 42) or frequency spectrum (43). Likewise, the duration of the stimulation sessions (44, 45), the repetition of the treatment (38, 46) and intraindividual characteristics (47, 48) were shown to be of importance in determining stimulation effects. In conclusion, tDCS and tRNS can induce and contribute to neuromodulatory processes similar to long-term potentiation or long-term depression (40, 43, 49, 50). Due to its neuromodulatory effects, tDCS represents a promising non-pharmaceutical treatment approach in the context of several neuropsychiatric disorders and has a growing evidence base (51). tRNS, when compared to tDCS, has been shown to lead to larger neuromodulatory effects in terms of motor cortex excitability (52, 53). Moreover, there is comparative evidence to suggest tRNS may also lead to larger neuromodulatory effects when studied in the context of clinical applications in neuropsychiatric settings. As an example, a comparative study of tRNS and tDCS in Tinnitus patients showed significant suppressive effects of tRNS on Tinnitus loudness, while tDCS was without effect (54). When safety standards of tES are observed, tDCS and tRNS are free of serious adverse effects, pain and long-term impairment. As a minor side effect subjects could feel sensations such as itching and tingling limited to the duration of the stimulation (39, 43, 55, 56). Sham stimulation can be reliably realized by using weak electrical currents only at the beginning and at the end of tES (32, 43, 57). Sham stimulation causes mild sensory feelings under the electrical stimulus electrodes, but does not induce any detectable neuromodulatory effects (58, 59).

The use of tES in the modulation of sleep and wakefulness relies on the targeted induction of local changes in cortical excitability, potentially leading to improvements in sleep patterns. The application of tES has been linked to the modulation of specific brain oscillations, such as slow wave activity during non-REM sleep (60). This modulation is thought to occur through a “top-down” pathway involving the frontal cortex and thalamus, emphasizing the role of fronto-thalamic feedback in sleep regulation (60, 61). While further research is required to fully understand its therapeutic potential, tES has demonstrated relatively minor side effects and holds promise for the treatment of sleep-related neuropsychiatric conditions (60).

Modulatory effects of tES on sleep and wakefulness in healthy adults, e.g., in terms of sleep duration, sleep efficiency and vigilance could be demonstrated in recent studies (29, 60, 6264). Cheng et al. (63) were able to show that a single session of tDCS, applied over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), led to improvements in several cognitive functions and subjective levels of wakefulness in sleep-deprived subjects for at least 2 h. Likewise, a recent review by Annarumma et al. (65) showed that tES has the potential to acutely enhance vigilance levels and reduce sleepiness: for example, acute effects of direct current include increased levels of vigilance, as reflected in improved stimulus detection during prolonged attentional tasks (66). Moreover, anodal tDCS has been shown to induce an EEG pattern of cortical arousal, potentially contributing to increased vigilance levels in healthy subjects. In a study of healthy subjects during a 30-h period of wakefulness by McIntire et al. (31), it was demonstrated that a single 30-min session of anodal tDCS over the PFC exhibited superior subjective and objective effectiveness in preventing sleepiness-related neurocognitive performance decrements compared to placebo stimulation and was comparable to caffeine (dose: 200 mg).

While there is convincing evidence regarding wakefulness-enhancing effects of tES in healthy subjects, there only is a paucity of corresponding research of tES effects in EDS disorders. Frase et al. (28) employed a six-day period of treatment, during which one patient with organic hypersomnia underwent three sessions of anodal tDCS, alternating with three sessions of sham stimulation on six consecutive days in phase I of the study. The stimulation parameters were identical as compared to the present study with the stimulation electrodes placed bi-frontally on the scalp (FP1/FP2 according to the international EEG 10-20-System) and the reference electrodes placed in a parietal position (P3/P4). Across the three tDCS treatment days, there were acute positive effects of tDCS on PVT reaction times as the variance of response speed (SD iRT) significantly improved after tDCS and deteriorated after sham stimulation. In phase II of the study of Frase et al. (28), two courses of three sessions of tDCS followed by a month of self-observation improved subjective vigilance (visual analog scale) and reduced the self-reported duration of daytime sleep. In contrast, the stimulation protocol used by Galbiati et al. (30) included repeated anodal tDCS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in eight patients with untreated IH (anodal stimulation electrode: left F3; 2 mA over the stimulation electrode; reference electrode: over the right orbit; 20 min per day; 3 stimulations per week for 4 weeks). Operationalized by the reaction time in the modified Attention Network Test, there was a significant improvement of attention functions and decrease in ESS scores in a pre−/post-comparison after the treatment series, suggesting beneficial therapeutic effects on sleepiness and performance in patients with untreated IH.

The present sham-controlled study examined short-term effects of tDCS and tRNS on sleepiness and sustained attention in patients with narcolepsy and IH. We chose to include tRNS as an additional stimulation mode, since evidence suggests that tRNS may be more effective in enhancing neuronal excitability (52, 53) and up to now no study made use of tRNS in hypersomniac patients. Given the limited evidence base in this field of study and uncertainties regarding the number of tES sessions required to induce sleepiness-modulating effects in hypersomniac patients, we investigated the exploratory hypothesis that single sessions of tES (with tDCS or tRNS) would lead to acute changes in both sustained attention and subjective levels of momentary sleepiness in hypersomniac patients.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Sample

For this double-blind, sham-controlled and pseudo-randomized crossover trial, we recruited 29 patients with hypersomnia and excessive daytime sleepiness (EDS) from the Regensburg Sleep Disorder Center. The sample includes 27 patients with narcolepsy (14 NT 1 and 13 NT 2) and two patients with IH. The following inclusion criteria were applied:

• age between 18 and 75 years

• male or female gender

• established diagnosis of narcolepsy or IH based on International Classification of Sleep Disorders (Third Edition) criteria (1)

• stable drug or non-drug therapy of EDS and other narcolepsy symptoms for at least 4 weeks

• subjective burden of EDS as defined by an Epworth Sleepiness Scale [ESS; (67, 68); RRID:SCR_024211] score of at least 10. As a notable exception, one patient with a definite diagnosis of NT 2 and an ESS score below 10 was also included, as she suffered from comorbid conditions preventing her from falling asleep during daytime (i.e., Periodic Limb Movement Disorder, Chronic Pain Syndrome).

Patients were not included who fulfilled the following exclusion criteria:

• any exclusion criteria for tES (32)

• current substance abuse

• untreated and severe internal, neurological or psychiatric comorbidities.

The present study received approval from the local ethics committee in Regensburg (reference: 15-101-0295). After adequate explication, patients gave their written informed consent before participation in the study procedures.

2.2 Randomization and procedures

The study was conducted at the Regensburg Center of Neuromodulation, where patient recruitment took place between March and October 2016. Enrolled study patients underwent sessions of tES on three consecutive days in a double-blind, sham-controlled and pseudo-randomized crossover trial. Before participation in the trial, patients were informed about the presence of one sham stimulation in the course of the study. The randomization list, which specified the order of the different treatment conditions for each subject, was only known to the operators applying the actual tES treatments and who were not involved in rating and psychometric testing procedures. Unblinding took place after the completion of data entry in the employed statistical software. This procedure ensured the blinding of patients and examiners during the execution of study procedures.

Following the procedures outlined in Frase et al. (28), daily tES sessions were comprised of two stimulation periods of 13 min duration, with an intersession interval of 20 min (total tES session duration per day with preparation of electrodes: about 50 min). The first stimulation period started at noon between 11.30 am and 12.30 pm. During the tES stimulations, patients were positioned comfortably in a treatment chair in a semi-reclined position. As it could not be ensured to reliably prevent the patients from falling asleep during tES sessions, the study participants were allowed to spend the treatment time while being awake or asleep. Based on a self-report, it was documented whether the patients spent the treatment time awake or asleep. During treatment breaks, patients were instructed not to leave the building, to remain awake and to avoid caffeine, nicotine or other-type stimulants.

Stimulation modes were delivered by a battery-driven, micro-processor-controlled and CE-certified constant current stimulator (DC-Stimulator PLUS, neuroConn GmbH, Illmenau, Germany; RRID:SCR_015520). Electrode setup comprised two frontal sponge electrodes (5 × 7 cm, 10–20 electrode positions FP1 and FP2) and two parietal sponge electrodes (10 × 10 cm, P3 and P4) moistened with NaCl solution. While the frontal electrodes used the standard size for effective stimulation, parietal electrodes were increased in size in order to reduce current density to a level shown to be functionally inert to the cerebral cortex (69). Bi-frontal stimulation was selected to target the proposed ‘top-down’ pathway of sleep–wake regulation (60) and to keep the present procedures in line with those presented by Frase et al. (28). For tDCS, a current of 1 mA was applied over each anodal electrode (2 mA stimulator output, Y-cable split for stimulation and reference electrodes). For tRNS, the same setup with 100–640 Hz was used. For sham stimulation, we used the tDCS stimulation with 30 s fade-in and 30 s fade-out phase without active stimulation in between.

Directly before and after the daily tES sessions, psychometric ratings (Karolinska Sleepiness Scale, see below) and the Psychomotor Vigilance Task (PVT) developed by Dinges & Powell (70) were carried out. The test required subjects to monitor a display and to respond as fast as possible after the occurrence of a visual target. A single test session took 10 min. The test is known for its sound psychometric properties (70, 71) providing measures of both speed (inverse of the mean reaction time, mean iRT; standard deviation of the inverse reaction time, SD of iRT) and accuracy (number of lapses; number of errors). A lapse was defined as a reaction time longer than 500 milliseconds, and an error was defined as each reaction without a preceding visual stimulus or a reaction time shorter than 100 ms that exceeds the human performance limitations (72). In the present study, the portable version of the PVT (PVTL192, Ambulatory Monitoring Inc., Ardsley, NY; RRID:SCR_024213) with a test duration of 10 min and an interstimulus interval of 2–10 s was used. Additionally, subjects rated their momentary level of sleepiness with the Karolinska Sleepiness Scale before and after stimulation [KSS; (73, 74); RRID:SCR_024212]. Following the nights after each of the tES sessions, subjects were asked for the quality of their nighttime sleep using a 7-point Likert scale [ranging from 1 – “Unaffected (very restful)” to 7 – “Severly impaired (not restful at all)”].

While subjective ratings were obtained from all 29 patients, only 24 patients provided complete data of the PVT. This was due to logistical problems in the procedures, as PVT testing was conducted at the Regensburg Sleep Disorder Center (2 min walking distance from the neuromodulation unit) and some of the patients missed single assessments (hence dropping out of the data analysis). Statistical analysis of subjective ratings with the whole sample versus the subsample applying full PVT data showed no differences in results (data not shown), hence the results of the whole sample data will be presented.

Finally, during the last visit, patients were asked to guess on which day the sham treatment was applied. The sham stimulation was correctly identified by 44.8% of the patients, while 17.2% mistook the tDCS session and 37.9 the tRNS as the sham treatment.

2.3 Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were performed using IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows [Version 28; (75); RRID:SCR_016479]. Due to the exploratory nature of the hypotheses, all inferential tests were conducted with a two-sided approach, and the significance threshold (α) was set at 0.05 to control the Type I error rate.

The primary endpoint was the PVT mean reaction time (mean iRT) after each tES session, as compared to the PVT mean reaction time assessed before the session. Secondary endpoints were the remaining PVT outcome metrics (standard deviation of the iRT, number of lapses, number of errors) after each tES session, as compared to the assessment before the session.

Additionally, ratings of momentary sleepiness assessed by the KSS after each tES were analyzed, as compared to KSS ratings before the session. Lastly, ratings of sleep quality for the night after the tES session were compared.

PVT and KSS outcome metrics were analyzed in a pre/post comparison on each day of study treatment (tDCS, tRNS, sham). For this purpose, a 3 × 2 two-factorial repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) was implemented to detect the effects of active treatment (tDCS, tRNS) and sham stimulation in a pre/post treatment design. For the ratings of sleep quality following each of the three tES sessions, a one-factorial RM-ANOVA was conducted. In case of violations of sphericity, as assessed with Mauchly’s Test of Sphericity, a Greenhouse–Geisser correction was performed (76). Likert-type items (KSS, item on sleep quality) were analyzed using parametric statistical procedures, as ANOVA procedures have been shown to be robust against violations of their assumptions (e.g., normality) even with single Likert-type items (77, 78). Additionally, effect size measures were calculated for pre/post treatment comparisons of PVT and KSS measures for all treatment conditions. The employed effect size measure was a measure of Cohen’s d paying attention to the repeated measures design, the correlation between measurements as well as the (pooled) standard deviations of the measurements [dRM, pooled; (see 79), formulas 8 and 9]. Post hoc power analysis was conducted using the software MorePower 6.0 introduced by Campbell and Thompson [(80); RRID:SCR_024210].

3 Results

3.1 Patient characteristics

A total of 29 patients with a mean age of 42.86 ± 14.39 years and a gender ratio of 18:11 (female:male) were enrolled in the study (see also Table 1). A total of 27 patients (93.1%) showed clinical EDS values (ESS scores >10) indicating clinical levels of excessive daytime sleepiness, even though the majority of study patients (75.9%) received pharmacological treatment such as amphetamine- or other-type stimulants (e.g., modafinil, pitolisant, methylphenidate and sodium oxybate; see also Table 1). At the subgroup level, 84.6% of the patients diagnosed with NT 1, 64.3% of the patients with NT 2 and both patients suffering from IH were treated with medication.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Patient characteristics.

3.2 PVT outcome measures

Concerning mean RT, descriptive analyses revealed minimal increases or decreases (<10 ms) in RT after the different stimulation conditions (see Table 2). The two-factorial RM-ANOVA showed no significant interaction between stimulation mode (tDCS, tRNS, sham) and time of measurement (pre/post stimulation) on mean RT values (Table 3). Likewise, neither the main effect for the stimulation mode nor that for time of measurement was significant (see Table 3). For none of the secondary PVT outcome metrics were there any significant main or interaction effects, as revealed through the conducted two-factorial RM-ANOVAs (see Table 3). In descriptive terms, there was a trend toward a higher number of errors after the tES stimulation sessions (see Table 2), as underlined by a near-significant main effect of Time of measurement in the corresponding RM-ANOVA model (p = 0.07, ηp2 = 139; see Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. PVT reaction parameters before and after tDCS, tRNS and sham stimulation (M ± SD; [Cohen’s dRM, pooled]; n = 24).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Results of two-factorial RM-ANOVAs with PVT outcome metrics.

In terms of effect sizes (dRM,pooled), tDCS only had minimal effects on PVT measures of response speed (mean RT, SD RT) and small-to-medium but somewhat inconsistent effects on measures of response accuracy (lapses, errors; see Table 2). tRNS, on the other hand, had minimal-to-small effects on PVT measures of response speed in terms of faster and more consistent responding, but also had small effects on measures of response accuracy in terms of more erroneous responding. Sham stimulation had minimal-to-small effects on PVT measures of response speed (slower and more consistent responding) and only minor effects on measures of response accuracy (see Table 2).

3.3 Subjective sleepiness (KSS) and sleep quality

In terms of subjective level of sleepiness as assessed with the KSS, descriptive analyses pointed to lower scores (corresponding to heightened levels of wakefulness) after the tES sessions across all the stimulation modes (see Table 4). The conducted two-factorial RM-ANOVA corroborated this apparent effect, showing a significant main effect of Time of measurement (p = 0.01, partial-η2: 209; see also Table 5). Importantly, there was no significant interaction effect between Stimulation mode and Time of measurement and there was no main effect of Stimulation mode (Table 5). In terms of effect sizes (dRM,pooled), tRNS only had minimal, sham stimulation had small and tDCS had moderate favorable effects on subjective levels of sleepiness.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Descriptive statistics of subjective sleepiness and sleep quality indicators before and after tES treatments (M ± SD; [Cohen’s dRM, pooled]; n = 29).

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Results of RM-ANOVAs with subjective sleepiness (KSS) and sleep quality (Likert scale) indicators.

The assessed sleep quality of the night sleep following each of the tES treatments showed almost identical values in descriptive terms (with a value of “3” corresponding to a rating of “mildly impaired”; see Table 4). The conducted RM-ANOVA showed no significant effect of stimulation mode on subjectively rated sleep quality (see Table 5).

3.4 Tolerability

There were no serious or major adverse reactions or events during or after tES treatment. Adverse reactions in the present study included itching and tingling sensations at the stimulation site in one single case (caused by both tDCS and tRNS) and, in one further case, a transient worsening of symptoms relating to restless legs syndrome (after tRNS). Apart from this, tDCS and tRNS did not impair subjective quality of nighttime sleep.

4 Discussion

In the present study, we examined the impact of single sessions of tDCS and tRNS on vigilance in patients suffering from narcolepsy and idiopathic hypersomnia. In contrast to the results presented by Frase et al. (28) and Galbiati et al. (30), there was no evidence for any significant short-term effect of anodal bifrontal tDCS on objective or subjective measures of sleepiness within the present study. We could also not demonstrate any acute effects of tRNS. Moreover, sleep quality during the night following the stimulation session was not affected neither by tDCS nor by tRNS.

One possible reason for the absence of therapeutic effects of tES on EDS in our study could be an insufficient statistical power. In our preliminary sample size planning, we assumed the occurrence of large effects (Cohen’s d ≥ 0.8) of active treatment conditions on both PVT responding and subjective levels of sleepiness. This assumption was grounded in the reports of very large effects (i.e., Cohen’s d > 1.5) of tDCS on subjective daytime sleepiness (30) and on objective vigilance (28). In their study, Frase et al. (28) reported a very large effect of tDCS on PVT reaction time. We estimated Cohen’s d > 2.8 from their data using an assumed correlation of r = 0.5 between the reported measurements (and the formula suggested by (81), p. 171). In terms of required sample size, we calculated that a sample size of N = 30 would be sufficient to detect large effects following the different stimulation modes (i.e., ηp2 ≥ 0.14 for the interaction ‘Stimulation mode*Time of measurement’) in our two-factorial repeated measures design. As can be seen from the results of the conducted two-factorial RM-ANOVAS, the effect sizes for the comparison of stimulation modes on subsequent responding (i.e., the interactions ‘Stimulation mode*Time of measurement’) were all in the small-to-medium range (values of ηp2 between 0.030 and 0.066, see Tables 3, 5). The calculated power indices (1-β) pointed to insufficient statistical power to detect statistically significant results given the small effect sizes of the present trial, especially for the interaction terms ‘Stimulation mode*Time of measurement’ (1-β ≤ 0.33; see Tables 3, 5). In addition, the effect sizes calculated for the single pre/post comparisons within stimulations modes (see Tables 2, 4) pointed toward mostly small effects of both tDCS and tRNS on subsequent sleepiness and vigilance. Moreover, there was no indication that any of the two tES stimulation modes yielded superior effects on subsequent responding. While tRNS had somewhat more consistent effects on PVT responding, leading to faster and more consistent reaction times (at the cost of more erroneous responses), tDCS had only minimal effects on PVT reaction times and inconsistent effects on response accuracy. Hence, the unexpectedly low effects of our employed single session treatments left us with insufficient statistical power to detect significant results of such (small) magnitude. Contrary to this lack of effects on objective indicators of sustained attention, tDCS had moderate positive effects on subjective levels of wakefulness, while sham stimulation had small and tRNS only had minimal effects (see Table 4). Nevertheless, there was a statistically significant main effect of Time of measurement, pointing toward significant increases in subjective levels of wakefulness independently from treatment condition. A possible explanation for these subjective increases relate to unspecific effects of stimulation (e.g., induced by expectation) that might be more pronounced at the subjective level. Another possibility for this increase in subjective levels of wakefulness relates to the occurrence of short-time sleep periods during the tES treatment sessions. Since we did not take measures to ensure wakefulness throughout the daily tES sessions, such sleep periods might have occurred and hence have led to subjective increases of wakefulness after the tES sessions. In fact, 13 patients (44.8%) were awake during the whole treatment time of both tDCS sessions. During the tRNS and sham stimulation, eight (27.6%) and six (20.7%) patients spent both sessions awake. While these differences between the three treatments were statistically significant (Q = 6.75, df = 2, p = 0.034), a post hoc correlational analysis of the association between sleep occurrence and the degree of differences in the outcome variables of interest (difference pre- to post treatment for all PVT measures and the KSS) showed mainly insignificant results (data not shown). The only significant association was between sleep during tRNS treatment and the difference in lapses in the PVT, in that there was a significantly reduced number of lapses after the stimulation session in those patients reporting sleep during the treatment period (r = 0.46, p = 0.025).

Another possible reason for the present null results of tDCS and tRNS relates to the dosing of tES stimulation. When compared to both Frase et al. (28) and Galbiati et al. (30), the present study employed the shortest tES treatment protocol with only single session treatments. It is possible that in the case study provided by Frase et al. (28), which was employed throughout a six-day period encompassing a total of three active treatment sessions with otherwise identical stimulation parameters (as compared to the present study), there were additive effects of tDCS across the treatment period. This interpretation is in line with the observed additional therapeutic effects of tDCS on subjective levels of sleepiness and duration of nighttime sleep throughout an extended treatment period in the case study presented by Frase et al. (28). However, since the vigilance of the patient was tested on a daily basis before and after each daily session (sham/active tDCS), the reported effects of tDCS must have occurred acutely after a single session. Likewise, the stimulation protocol used by Galbiati et al. (30) included repeated anodal tDCS over the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex three times a week for a total duration of 4 weeks. As they were interested in additive effects of several sessions of tDCS treatment, the positive results presented by this group also add to the notion of possible additive effects of tDCS on sleepiness in hypersomniac patients. We chose for the investigation of “bottom-line” effects of single tES sessions on objective indicators of sustained attention against the background of robust evidence for such acute effects in healthy subjects (31, 62, 63, 65). Contrary to healthy subjects, the majority of patients enrolled in the present study (75.9%) received pharmacological treatment for their EDS symptoms and had a rather long history of formal diagnosis (~ 8.5 years before study entry). Therefore, the reduced reactivity of our sample to the employed tES protocol might also be due to the medicated state and a potentially resultant lessened reactivity of the patients’ brains to tES. Moreover, the postulated hypofrontality in EDS disorders (21) could be related to reduced reactivity of the frontal cortex to tES effects in the population of interest. This understudied topic clearly deserves further research in order to arrive at a better understanding for the therapeutic potential of tES in EDS disorders.

Lastly, another reason for the present null results refers to the disease entities studied and their underlying (partly unclear) neurobiological underpinnings. tES is a neuromodulatory technique with a limited focality, not only affecting specific target regions of the human brain, but rather widespread neural circuits (32). The stimulation protocol used by Frase et al. (28) was used for treating a patient with isolated organic hypersomnia following reanimation. In this context, it is important to note that the brain regions involved in organic forms of hypersomnia resulting from traumatic brain injury (82, 83) differ significantly from other forms of hypersomnia (9, 13, 84). Organic hypersomnia is often caused by thalamic or brainstem strokes (82, 83). With differences in the pathophysiology of the different types of hypersomnia, the results from the case report of Frase et al. (28) might not be transferrable to our patient cohort, which comprised patients with narcolepsy (NT 1, NT 2) and IH. Since we intermixed different subgroups of patients in the presented statistical analysis, we cannot rule out the possibility of more pronounced tES effects at the level of specific diagnostic subgroups. We abstained from a subgroup-specific analysis due to limited statistical power. Based on a recent review of neuroimaging studies pointing at hypofrontality across diagnostic subgroups of EDS disorders (21), together with the rationale of tES-induced effects on a “top-down” network for sleep regulation (60), we chose this syndrome- and not etiology based approach as a first step. Future studies might focus on tES effects on specific diagnostic subgroups of patients with EDS in order to provide more insight to this important research question.

Nonetheless, the validity of the present null results should also be considered. While differing from previous studies, the present null findings might also be due to the application of sound study procedures. Previous publications could be confounded with methodological limitations, Galbiati et al. (30) used an uncontrolled study design that is susceptible for potential placebo effects. The case report of Frase et al. (28) included only a single patient and therefore only has very limited generalizability. Moreover, the statistical design of their study seems somewhat flawed (i.e., they conducted a RM-ANOVA and post hoc testing based on data of a single case). Our study procedures tried to control for potential placebo effects by employing a double-blind, pseudo-randomized crossover design with a sham control condition. While 44.8% of our sample was able to correctly identify the sham condition, the tRNS condition was mistakenly regarded as sham treatment by 37.9% of patients. Only 17.8% mistakenly regarded the tDCS session as the sham treatment, pointing to partial success of our blinding procedure. Therefore, future studies of tDCS effects in hypersomniac patients should take care to control for these important effects in order to provide sound empirical evidence of therapeutic effects. In case of future studies, the present results clearly point toward the need of repeated sessions of the different treatment modalities, hence enabling the occurrence of additive effects of tES. In line with previous studies (28, 30), we observed a favorable side-effects profile of tDCS and tRNS within our patient sample. Future studies might attempt to implement control procedures in order to avoid episodes of short-time sleep during the tES treatment sessions.

Further research on non-drug approaches in patients with EDS such as tES is required to investigate whether – and to what extent – a modification of the stimulation parameters used in the present study could improve sleepiness in patients with hypersomnia. As cumulated therapy effects of tDCS and tRNS are suggested in numerous previous studies on healthy adults (38, 46) and patients with EDS (28, 30), further tES study protocols for reducing sleepiness should be conducted, employing repeated stimulation sessions and larger patient collectives. Furthermore, the evaluation of the stimulation protocol used in the trial of Galbiati et al. (30) by means of a double-blind sham-controlled randomized study design could be helpful to generate new knowledge on tES in the treatment of EDS.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Ethics Committee of the University of Regensburg (Universität Regensburg, 93040 Regensburg, ethikkommission@ur.de, reference: 15-101-0295). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

MH: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Formal analysis. BL: Conceptualization, Resources, Writing – review & editing. TW: Conceptualization, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. PG: Conceptualization, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. MS: Conceptualization, Writing – review & editing, Project administration, Resources, Supervision. AR: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our sincere appreciation to the study nurses for their invaluable support and professional demeanor throughout the course of the study. Their dedication to ensuring the smooth execution of the research and the meticulous implementation of the transcranial electric stimulation protocol greatly contributed to the success of this endeavor.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

The author(s) declared that they were an editorial board member of Frontiers, at the time of submission. This had no impact on the peer review process and the final decision.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International classification of sleep disorders. 3rd ed. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine (2014).

Google Scholar

2. Silber, MH, Krahn, LE, Olson, EJ, and Pankratz, VS. The epidemiology of narcolepsy in Olmsted County, Minnesota: a population-based study. Sleep. (2002) 25:197–202. doi: 10.1093/sleep/25.2.197

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Hublin, C, Kaprio, J, Partinen, M, Koskenvuo, M, Heikkila, K, Koskimies, S, et al. The prevalence of narcolepsy: an epidemiological study of the Finnish twin cohort. Ann Neurol. (1994) 35:709–16. doi: 10.1002/ana.410350612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Bassetti, C, and Aldrich, MS. Idiopathic hypersomnia. A series of 42 patients. Brain. (1997) 120:1423–35. doi: 10.1093/brain/120.8.1423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Billiard, M, Merle, C, Carlander, B, Ondze, B, Alvarez, D, and Besset, A. Idiopathic hypersomnia. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (1998) 52:125–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.1998.tb00987.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Overeem, S, van Nues, SJ, van der Zande, WL, Donjacour, CE, van Mierlo, P, and Lammers, GJ. The clinical features of cataplexy: a questionnaire study in narcolepsy patients with and without hypocretin-1 deficiency. Sleep Med. (2011) 12:12–8. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2010.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Yoss, RE, and Daly, DD. Criteria for the diagnosis of the narcoleptic syndrome. Proc Staff Meet Mayo Clin. (1957) 32:320–8.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

8. Anderson, KN, Pilsworth, S, Sharples, LD, Smith, IE, and Shneerson, JM. Idiopathic hypersomnia: a study of 77 cases. Sleep. (2007) 30:1274–81. doi: 10.1093/sleep/30.10.1274

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Thannickal, TC, Moore, RY, Nienhuis, R, Ramanathan, L, Gulyani, S, Aldrich, M, et al. Reduced number of hypocretin neurons in human narcolepsy. Neuron. (2000) 27:469–74. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00058-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Nishino, S, Ripley, B, Overeem, S, Lammers, GJ, and Mignot, E. Hypocretin (orexin) deficiency in human narcolepsy. Lancet. (2000) 355:39–40. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(99)05582-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Pizza, F, Barateau, L, Dauvilliers, Y, and Plazzi, G. The orexin story, sleep and sleep disturbances. J Sleep Res. (2022) 31:e13665. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13665

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Trotti, LM. Central disorders of hypersomnolence. Continuum. (2020) 26:890–907. doi: 10.1212/CON.0000000000000883

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Kumar, S, and Sagili, H. Etiopathogenesis and neurobiology of narcolepsy: a review. J Clin Diagn Res. (2014) 8:190–5. doi: 10.7860/JCDR/2014/7295.4057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Scammell, TE. Narcolepsy. N Engl J Med. (2015) 373:2654–62. doi: 10.1056/NEJMra1500587

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ali, M, Auger, RR, Slocumb, NL, and Morgenthaler, TI. Idiopathic hypersomnia: clinical features and response to treatment. J. Clin. Sleep Med. (2009) 5:562–8. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.27658

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Roth, B. Narcolepsy and hypersomnia: review and classification of 642 personally observed cases. Schweiz Arch Neurol Neurochir Psychiatr. (1976) 119:31–41.

Google Scholar

17. Carskadon, MA, and Dement, WC. The multiple sleep latency test: what does it measure? Sleep. (1982) 5:S67–72. doi: 10.1093/sleep/5.S2.S67

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Carskadon, MA, Dement, WC, Mitler, MM, Roth, T, Westbrook, PR, and Keenan, S. Guidelines for the multiple sleep latency test (MSLT): a standard measure of sleepiness. Sleep. (1986) 9:519–24. doi: 10.1093/sleep/9.4.519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Cairns, A, and Bogan, R. Prevalence and clinical correlates of a short onset REM period (SOREMP) during routine PSG. Sleep. (2015) 38:1575–81. doi: 10.5665/sleep.5050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Wada, M, Mimura, M, Noda, Y, Takasu, S, Plitman, E, Honda, M, et al. Neuroimaging correlates of narcolepsy with cataplexy: a systematic review. Neurosci Res. (2019) 142:16–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2018.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Cavaliere, C, Longarzo, M, Fogel, S, Engström, M, and Soddu, A. Neuroimaging of narcolepsy and primary Hypersomnias. Neuroscientist. (2020) 26:310–27. doi: 10.1177/1073858420905829

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Boucetta, S, Montplaisir, J, Zadra, A, Lachapelle, F, Soucy, J-P, Gravel, P, et al. Altered regional cerebral blood flow in idiopathic hypersomnia. Sleep. (2017) 40:zsx140. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsx140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Mignot, EJM. A practical guide to the therapy of narcolepsy and hypersomnia syndromes. Neurotherapeutics. (2012) 9:739–52. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0150-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Wozniak, DR, and Quinnell, TG. Unmet needs of patients with narcolepsy: perspectives on emerging treatment options. Nat Sci Sleep. (2015) 7:51–61. doi: 10.2147/NSS.S56077

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Berman, SM, Kuczenski, R, McCracken, JT, and London, ED. Potential adverse effects of amphetamine treatment on brain and behavior: a review. Mol Psychiatry. (2009) 14:123–42. doi: 10.1038/mp.2008.90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Leonard, BE, McCartan, D, White, J, and King, DJ. Methylphenidate: a review of its neuropharmacological, neuropsychological and adverse clinical effects. Hum Psychopharmacol. (2004) 19:151–80. doi: 10.1002/hup.579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Thakrar, C, Patel, K, D’ancona, G, Kent, BD, Nesbitt, A, Selsick, H, et al. Effectiveness and side-effect profile of stimulant therapy as monotherapy and in combination in the central hypersomnias in clinical practice. J Sleep Res. (2018) 27:e12627. doi: 10.1111/jsr.12627

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Frase, L, Maier, JG, Zittel, S, Freyer, T, Riemann, D, Normann, C, et al. Bifrontal anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) improves daytime vigilance and sleepiness in a patient with organic hypersomnia following reanimation. Brain Stimul. (2015) 8:844–6. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2015.05.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Frase, L, Piosczyk, H, Zittel, S, Jahn, F, Selhausen, P, Krone, L, et al. Modulation of total sleep time by transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuropsychopharmacology. (2016) 41:2577–86. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Galbiati, A, Abutalebi, J, Iannaccone, S, Borsa, VM, Musteata, S, Zucconi, M, et al. The effects of transcranial drect current stimulation (tDCS) on idiopathic hypersomnia: a pilot study. Arch Ital Biol. (2016) 154:1–5. doi: 10.12871/00039829201611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. McIntire, LK, McKinley, RA, Goodyear, C, and Nelson, J. A comparison of the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation and caffeine on vigilance and cognitive performance during extended wakefulness. Brain Stimul. (2014) 7:499–507. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2014.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Nitsche, MA, Cohen, LG, Wassermann, EM, Priori, A, Lang, N, Antal, A, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation: state of the art 2008. Brain Stimul. (2008) 1:206–23. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Zaghi, S, Acar, M, Hultgren, B, Boggio, PS, and Fregni, F. Noninvasive brain stimulation with low-intensity electrical currents: putative mechanisms of action for direct and alternating current stimulation. Neuroscientist. (2010) 16:285–307. doi: 10.1177/1073858409336227

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Bindman, LJ, Lippold, OC, and Redfearn, JW. The action of brief polarizing currents on the cerebral cortex of the rat (1) during current flow and (2) in the production of long-lasting after-effects. J Physiol. (1964) 172:369–82. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1964.sp007425

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Creutzfeldt, OD, Fromm, GH, and Kapp, H. Influence of transcortical d-c currents on cortical neuronal activity. Exp Neurol. (1962) 5:436–52. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(62)90056-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Purpura, DP, and McMurtry, JG. Intracellular activities and evoked potential changes during polarization of motor cortex. J Neurophysiol. (1965) 28:166–85. doi: 10.1152/jn.1965.28.1.166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Nitsche, MA, Seeber, A, Frommann, K, Klein, CC, Rochford, C, Nitsche, MS, et al. Modulating parameters of excitability during and after transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J Physiol. (2005) 568:291–303. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2005.092429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Fricke, K, Seeber, AA, Thirugnanasambandam, N, Paulus, W, Nitsche, MA, and Rothwell, JC. Time course of the induction of homeostatic plasticity generated by repeated transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J Neurophysiol. (2011) 105:1141–9. doi: 10.1152/jn.00608.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Terney, D, Chaieb, L, Moliadze, V, Antal, A, and Paulus, W. Increasing human brain excitability by transcranial high-frequency random noise stimulation. J Neurosci. (2008) 28:14147–55. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4248-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. van der Groen, O, Potok, W, Wenderoth, N, Edwards, G, Mattingley, JB, and Edwards, D. Using noise for the better: the effects of transcranial random noise stimulation on the brain and behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2022) 138:104702. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2022.104702

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Nitsche, MA, and Paulus, W. Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. J Physiol. (2000) 527:633–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Moliadze, V, Atalay, D, Antal, A, and Paulus, W. Close to threshold transcranial electrical stimulation preferentially activates inhibitory networks before switching to excitation with higher intensities. Brain Stimul. (2012) 5:505–11. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Paulus, W, Antal, A, and Nitsche, MA. Physiological basis and methodological aspects of transcranial electric stimulation In: C Miniussi, W Paulus, and PM Rossini, editors. Transcranial brain stimulation. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press (2013). 91–111.

Google Scholar

44. Nitsche, MA, and Paulus, W. Sustained excitability elevations induced by transcranial DC motor cortex stimulation in humans. Neurology. (2001) 57:1899–901. doi: 10.1212/WNL.57.10.1899

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Chaieb, L, Paulus, W, and Antal, A. Evaluating aftereffects of short-duration transcranial random noise stimulation on cortical excitability. Neural Plast. (2011) 2011:105927:1–5. doi: 10.1155/2011/105927

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Monte-Silva, K, Kuo, M-F, Hessenthaler, S, Fresnoza, S, Liebetanz, D, Paulus, W, et al. Induction of late LTP-like plasticity in the human motor cortex by repeated non-invasive brain stimulation. Brain Stimul. (2013) 6:424–32. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2012.04.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Rademacher, J, Caviness, VS, Steinmetz, H, and Galaburda, AM. Topographical variation of the human primary cortices: implications for neuroimaging, brain mapping, and neurobiology. Cereb Cortex. (1993) 3:313–29. doi: 10.1093/cercor/3.4.313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Li, LM, Uehara, K, and Hanakawa, T. The contribution of interindividual factors to variability of response in transcranial direct current stimulation studies. Front Cell Neurosci. (2015) 9:181. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2015.00181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Liebetanz, D, Nitsche, MA, Tergau, F, and Paulus, W. Pharmacological approach to the mechanisms of transcranial DC-stimulation-induced after-effects of human motor cortex excitability. Brain. (2002) 125:2238–47. doi: 10.1093/brain/awf238

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Ranieri, F, Podda, MV, Riccardi, E, Frisullo, G, Dileone, M, Profice, P, et al. Modulation of LTP at rat hippocampal CA3-CA1 synapses by direct current stimulation. J Neurophysiol. (2012) 107:1868–80. doi: 10.1152/jn.00319.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Lefaucheur, J-P, Antal, A, Ayache, SS, Benninger, DH, Brunelin, J, Cogiamanian, F, et al. Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Clin Neurophysiol. (2017) 128:56–92. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.10.087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Inukai, Y, Saito, K, Sasaki, R, Tsuiki, S, Miyaguchi, S, Kojima, S, et al. Comparison of three non-invasive transcranial electrical stimulation methods for increasing cortical excitability. Front Hum Neurosci. (2016) 10:668. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00668

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Moliadze, V, Fritzsche, G, and Antal, A. Comparing the efficacy of excitatory transcranial stimulation methods measuring motor evoked potentials. Neural Plast. (2014) 2014:837141:1–6. doi: 10.1155/2014/837141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Vanneste, S, Fregni, F, and De Ridder, D. Head-to-head comparison of transcranial random noise stimulation, transcranial AC stimulation, and transcranial DC stimulation for tinnitus. Front Psych. (2013) 4:158. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00158

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Furubayashi, T, Terao, Y, Arai, N, Okabe, S, Mochizuki, H, Hanajima, R, et al. Short and long duration transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) over the human hand motor area. Exp Brain Res. (2008) 185:279–86. doi: 10.1007/s00221-007-1149-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Fertonani, A, Ferrari, C, and Miniussi, C. What do you feel if I apply transcranial electric stimulation? Safety, sensations and secondary induced effects. Clin Neurophysiol. (2015) 126:2181–8. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2015.03.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Dissanayaka, TD, Zoghi, M, Farrell, M, Egan, GF, and Jaberzadeh, S. Sham transcranial electrical stimulation and its effects on corticospinal excitability: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Rev Neurosci. (2018) 29:223–32. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2017-0026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Ambrus, GG, Al-Moyed, H, Chaieb, L, Sarp, L, Antal, A, and Paulus, W. The fade-in – short stimulation – fade out approach to sham tDCS – reliable at 1 mA for naïve and experienced subjects, but not investigators. Brain Stimul. (2012) 5:499–504. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2011.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Tang, MF, Hammond, GR, and Badcock, DR. Are participants aware of the type and intensity of transcranial direct current stimulation? PLoS One. (2016) 11:e0148825. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0148825

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Krone, L, Frase, L, Piosczyk, H, Selhausen, P, Zittel, S, Jahn, F, et al. Top-down control of arousal and sleep: fundamentals and clinical implications. Sleep Med Rev. (2017) 31:17–24. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2015.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Scammell, TE, Arrigoni, E, and Lipton, JO. Neural circuitry of wakefulness and sleep. Neuron. (2017) 93:747–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Alfonsi, V, D‘Atri, A, Scarpelli, S, Gorgoni, M, Giacinti, F, Annarumma, L, et al. The effects of bifrontal anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) on sleepiness and vigilance in partially sleep-deprived subjects: a multidimensional study. J Sleep Res. (2023) 32:e13869. doi: 10.1111/jsr.13869

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Cheng, J-X, Zhao, X, Qiu, J, Jiang, Y, Ren, J, Sun, S, et al. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on performance and recovery sleep during acute sleep deprivation: a pilot study. Sleep Med. (2021) 79:124–33. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2021.01.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Dai, J, Wang, H, Yang, L, Wang, C, Cheng, S, Zhang, T, et al. The neuroelectrophysiological and behavioral effects of transcranial direct current stimulation on executive vigilance under a continuous monotonous condition. Front Neurosci. (2022) 16:910457. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.910457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Annarumma, L, D‘Atri, A, Alfonsi, V, and De, GL. The efficacy of transcranial current stimulation techniques to modulate resting-state EEG, to affect vigilance and to promote sleepiness. Brain Sci. (2018) 8:137. doi: 10.3390/brainsci8070137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Nelson, JT, McKinley, RA, Golob, EJ, Warm, JS, and Parasuraman, R. Enhancing vigilance in operators with prefrontal cortex transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS). Neuroimage. (2014) 85:909–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Johns, MW. A new method for measuring daytime sleepiness: the Epworth sleepiness scale. Sleep. (1991) 14:540–5. doi: 10.1093/sleep/14.6.540

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Kendzerska, TB, Smith, PM, Brignardello-Petersen, R, Leung, RS, and Tomlinson, GA. Evaluation of the measurement properties of the Epworth sleepiness scale: a systematic review. Sleep Med Rev. (2014) 18:321–31. doi: 10.1016/j.smrv.2013.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Nitsche, MA, Doemkes, S, Karaköse, T, Antal, A, Liebetanz, D, Lang, N, et al. Shaping the effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of the human motor cortex. J Neurophysiol. (2007) 97:3109–17. doi: 10.1152/jn.01312.2006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Dinges, DF, and Powell, JW. Microcomputer analyses of performance on a portable, simple visual RT task during sustained operations. Behav Res Methods Instrum Comput. (1985) 17:652–5. doi: 10.3758/BF03200977

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Dorrian, J, Rogers, NL, and Dinges, DF. Psychomotor vigilance performance: neurocognitive assay sensitive to sleep loss In: CA Cushida, editor. Sleep deprivation. New York: CRC Press (2004). 39–70.

Google Scholar

72. Basner, M, and Dinges, DF. Maximizing sensitivity of the psychomotor vigilance test (PVT) to sleep loss. Sleep. (2011) 34:581–91. doi: 10.1093/sleep/34.5.581

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Akerstedt, T, and Gillberg, M. Subjective and objective sleepiness in the active individual. Int J Neurosci. (1990) 52:29–37. doi: 10.3109/00207459008994241

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Geisler, P. Hypersomnie, Narkolepsie und Tagesmüdigkeit. Bremen: UNI-MED Verl (2009).

Google Scholar

75. IBM. IBM SPSS statistics for windows. Version 28.0. Armonk, NY: IBM Corp (2021).

Google Scholar

76. Field, A. Discovering statistics using SPSS. 3rd ed. Los Angeles: Sage (2009).

Google Scholar

77. Carifio, J, and Perla, R. Resolving the 50-year debate around using and misusing Likert scales. Med Educ. (2008) 42:1150–2. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2008.03172.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Norman, G. Likert scales, levels of measurement and the laws of statistics. Adv Health Sci Educ. (2010) 15:625–32. doi: 10.1007/s10459-010-9222-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Lakens, D. Calculating and reporting effect sizes to facilitate cumulative science: a practical primer for t-tests and ANOVAs. Front Psychol. (2013) 4:863. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00863

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Campbell, JID, and Thompson, VA. MorePower 6.0 for ANOVA with relational confidence intervals and Bayesian analysis. Behav Res Methods. (2012) 44:1255–65. doi: 10.3758/s13428-012-0186-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Dunlap, WP, Cortina, JM, Vaslow, JB, and Burke, MJ. Meta-analysis of experiments with matched groups or repeated measures designs. Psychol Methods. (1996) 1:170–7. doi: 10.1037/1082-989X.1.2.170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Bassetti, C, Mathis, J, Gugger, M, Lovblad, KO, and Hess, CW. Hypersomnia following paramedian thalamic stroke: a report of 12 patients. Ann Neurol. (1996) 39:471–80. doi: 10.1002/ana.410390409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Blanco, M, Espinosa, M, Arpa, J, Barreiro, P, and Rodríguez-Albariño, A. Hypersomnia and thalamic and brain stem stroke: a study of seven patients. Neurologia. (1999) 14:307–14.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

84. Khan, Z, and Trotti, LM. Central disorders of hypersomnolence: focus on the narcolepsies and idiopathic hypersomnia. Chest. (2015) 148:262–73. doi: 10.1378/chest.14-1304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: anodal tDCS, tRNS, hypersomnia, narcolepsy, idiopathic hypersomnia, vigilance, sleepiness

Citation: Hohenester M, Langguth B, Wetter TC, Geisler P, Schecklmann M and Reissmann A (2023) Single sessions of transcranial direct current stimulation and transcranial random noise stimulation exert no effect on sleepiness in patients with narcolepsy and idiopathic hypersomnia. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1288976. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1288976

Received: 05 September 2023; Accepted: 28 November 2023;
Published: 11 December 2023.

Edited by:

Andrea Galbiati, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Italy

Reviewed by:

Ludovica Annarumma, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy
Samantha Mombelli, San Raffaele Hospital (IRCCS), Italy

Copyright © 2023 Hohenester, Langguth, Wetter, Geisler, Schecklmann and Reissmann. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Andreas Reissmann, andreas.reissmann@medbo.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.