Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 10 November 2023
Sec. Autism

Prevalence, comorbidities, and profiles of neurodevelopmental disorders according to the DSM-5-TR in children aged 6 years old in a European region

Lorena Francs
Lorena Francés1*Antoni RuizAntoni Ruiz2C. Virgínia SolerC. Virgínia Soler3Joan FrancsJoan Francés4Jessica CaulesJessica Caules5Amaia Hervs,,,Amaia Hervás6,7,8,9Carolina CarreteroCarolina Carretero10Brbara CardonaBárbara Cardona11Elizabeth QuezadaElizabeth Quezada12Alberto FernndezAlberto Fernández13Javier Quintero,Javier Quintero13,14
  • 1Child and Adolescent Psychiatrist, IBSMIA, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, Madrid, Spain
  • 2Research Group on Socio-Educational Interventions in Childhood and Youth, University of Barcelona, Barcelona, Spain
  • 3Dalt Sant Joan Center, Mahón, Spain
  • 4Physical Activity and Sport Sciences, Miguel Hernández University, Elche, Spain
  • 5Teaching, Arrels Institute, Ciutadella de Menorca, Spain
  • 6Child–Adolescent Mental Health Unit, Mutua Terrasa University Hospital, Terrassa, Catalonia, Spain
  • 7Psychiatry, Autonomous University of Barcelona, Bellaterra, Catalonia, Spain
  • 8Saint George Hospital, London, United Kingdom
  • 9Child–Adolescent Psychiatry, Maudsley Hospital, London, United Kingdom
  • 10UCSMIA, UBS Es Mercadal, Menorca, Spain
  • 11UBS Es Castell, Menorca, Spain
  • 12CS Canal Salat, Ciutadella de Menorca, Spain
  • 13Psychiatry Department of Complutense University of Madrid, Madrid, Spain
  • 14Psychiatry Service of Infanta Leonor Hospital, Madrid, Spain

Background: There are no studies that measure the prevalence and real comorbidities of neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) according to the DSM-5-TR in 6-year-old children in population and clinical samples or studies that measure them as a whole. The data on the prevalence of these disorders are usually disparate because of the estimation methods (direct/indirect), the type of sample (population/clinical/school), and the ages studied.

Methods: The initial sample (289 subjects) was representative of 6-year-old children in the entire population of Menorca, obtained from pediatric primary care services (100% of the sample). The patients were divided into two groups based on the criterion of verification of clinical warning signs. One of the groups represented the clinical or experimental sample (EG) (81 subjects) at risk of NDDs; the other group was considered the control sample (CG) (210 subjects), and they were subjects without risk of suffering NDDs. A direct clinical assessment of the clinical sample was carried out, and they were administered the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC-V), the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals (CELF-5), the Battery for the evaluation of the processes of revised reading (Batería para la evaluación de los procesos de lectura revisada – PROLEC-R), the Test for the Diagnosis of Basic Mathematical Competences, (TEDI-MATH), and the Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCDQ).

Results: A total of 21.5% of the initial sample suffered from an NDD. A total of 2.4% presented autism spectrum disorder (ASD); 14% presented attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD); 0.34% presented mild intellectual disability; 9.54% presented communication disorder (CD) (5.8% language disorder, 3.4% phonological disorder, and 0.34% stuttering); 10% presented learning disorder with reading difficulties; 5.8% presented learning disorder with difficulties in writing; 3.11% presented learning disorder with difficulties in mathematics; 1% presented transitory tic disorder; 0.34% presented chronic tic disorder; 1% presented Tourette syndrome; 2% presented motor coordination disorder (MCD); and 0.34% presented stereotypic movement disorders. Male children were more affected than female children in general, with male/female ORs of 0.14/0.92 for the presence of comorbidities, 0.11/0.88 for combined ADHD, 0.06/0.87 for language disorder, 1.02/1.27 for MCD, and 1.39/1.02 for inattentive ADHD.

Conclusion: In disadvantaged contexts, there was a higher prevalence of NDDs and comorbidities, unless the disorder was extreme, in which case only the NDD manifestations were presented. A significant proportion of the sample had not been previously diagnosed (88.6%); therefore, early detection programs are recommended to identify warning signs and develop policies that help and support the most disadvantaged sectors of the population.

Background

According to the latest revised version of the DSM-5 (1), neurodevelopmental disorders (NDDs) are a group of conditions that appear during the developmental period and usually manifest at an early stage, often before the child enters school. These deficits, although they improve with age, are generally related to functional interference in adult life. They are characterized by developmental deficits or differences in brain processes that produce alterations in personal, social, academic, or occupational functioning. The range of developmental deficits or differences varies from very specific limitations in learning or the control of executive functions to global deficits in social skills or intellectual capacity. Importantly, there has been a paradigm shift in the diagnosis of NDDs; in previous versions, NDDs were considered categorically defined, a fact that entailed differences in diagnosis and, as a consequence, in the prevalence obtained. In the new versions of both the ICD-11 (2) and DSM-5, dimensional approaches are considered to account for ranges of severity, often without a very clear boundary with neurodevelopment. Thus, the diagnosis of a disorder requires the presence of symptoms and functional alterations.

The NDD category includes disorders that manifest in a general way in almost all domains of development, such as intellectual disability (ID), as well as those that affect more specific domains, such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and its three presentations (inattentive, hyperactive–impulsive, and combined); autism spectrum disorder (ASD); communication disorders (CDs), which include phonological disorders, language disorders and stuttering; specific learning disorders (including reading, writing, and mathematics); and motor disorders (tics, Tourette syndrome, motor coordination disorder, and stereotypic movement disorders).

NDDs often coexist with each other, and it is rare for them to occur alone. Homotypic comorbidity data are still scarce, and there are studies that investigate comorbidities within each disorder in particular, usually coinciding with those that are more present in the literature, such as autism (3, 4) or ADHD (5). It is unusual for comorbidities to be investigated in such studies, and there are also few studies that even consider them. However, our team studied and estimated comorbidity risk figures, with the most frequent combination being the presence of learning and language disorders, affecting 6.9% of the sample. The second most frequent combination was the presence of learning, language, and ADHD difficulties, affecting 4.5% of the sample (6). In Japan, the comorbidities among ADHD, ASD, and dyslexia were investigated (7). In Scotland (8), the most frequent comorbidities between ASD and ID were identified.

Prevalence and meta-analysis studies appear more frequently in scientific annals. The methods for estimating prevalence are sometimes unclear, thus potentially introducing bias (9). Depending on where the analyzed samples were recruited (i.e., a clinical, school, or population sample), very different figures are obtained. NDDs are considered underdiagnosed (10). In a previous systematic review by our research team (9), we found that the global prevalence rate of NDDs fluctuates globally between 4.70% in Scotland (8) to 55.5% in Norway (11) and 88.50% in Japan (7). In the United States, according to data published by the National Center for Health Statistics (NCHS) in 2015, an estimated 15% of children between the ages of 3 and 17 years are affected by NDDs (12). In a study carried out by our team and on a population sample, we found a global risk of presenting an NDD of 55.4% (6). An important fact to consider is that, intuitively, higher prevalence figures should coincide with more selected populations, that is, clinical samples, as is the case for the Norwegian study (11). However, in a population sample of 5-year-olds in Japan (13), estimated rates of ASD were similar to those for a clinical sample in Catalonia, covering a wider age range (2–17 years) (14). In addition, another fact that confirms the disparity in reported results is the low prevalence figures in a Spanish study carried out by pediatricians, in which the sample was selected based on follow-up in a child–adolescent psychiatry unit (15) and in which the age range was broad (0–14 years). Perhaps prevalence estimates would be more precise if studies employed more homogeneous criteria, such as narrower age ranges or similar or close ages not exceeding a 2-year margin, as is the case in this study. If samples were homogenized by narrower age ranges, the variability of results would decrease and results could be better compared. To be able to compare results, many studies would be needed in different populations (populations, schools, and clinics), with more homogeneous ages and similar evaluation methods (direct or indirect). In short, although these types of studies are on the rise, the number is still insufficient, and they yield mixed results.

In the literature reviewed in 2022 by our research team (9), the prevalence rates reported were as follows: ID, 0.63%; ADHD, 5–11%; ASD, 0.70–3%; specific learning disorders (SLDs), 3–10%; communication disorders (CDs), 1–3.42%; and motor disorders (MDs), 0.76–17% (3, 611). The estimated prevalence rates of the most common NDDs were as follows: ADHD, 7.9–9.5% (16, 17); ASD, 0.7–2.2% (16, 18, 19); SLDs (including developmental dyslexia [DD]), 1.2–24% (20, 21); and MDs, 1.4–19% (22, 23).

In our previous screening study, carried out through direct evaluations on children and parents (6), we established the following risks: a 23.4% risk of presenting ADHD in any of its modalities (inattentive, hyperactive–impulsive, and combined), a 2.8% risk of ASD, a 30.6% risk of presenting a learning disorder with reading difficulties, a 5.5% risk of tics, and a 22.5% risk of language problems (incomprehensible language or minor language problems).

The data on the prevalence of these disorders are usually disparate depending on the sample analyzed (clinical, school, or population) and the method used for estimating the prevalence (direct or indirect). In addition, the wide range of ages that are taken into account in each study in the scientific literature adds heterogeneity.

From a gender perspective, boys tend to be more affected by any NDD except inattentive ADHD, for which girls are more affected. In Scotland (8), the most frequent comorbidities were ASD and ID, occurring in 0.3% of children, 81.0% of whom were boys. Multimorbidity was prevalent with ASD and ID. ADHD, by itself or coexisting with other conditions, was the factor with the greatest weight in the increase in school exclusion. Multimorbidity was more common among boys, and the prevalence increased with school deprivation. In contrast, there was a greater negative impact on girls than boys. Regarding the NDDs studied and their comorbidities, 66.3% of the children included in a study by Hansen et al. (11) were boys, and Saito et al. (13) reported a male:female ratio of 2.2:1. Regarding ADHD, male:female ratios of 4:1 and 2:1 have been reported (24), coinciding in general with 3.2:1 reported by Sayal et al. (25) and Faraone et al. (26). Finally, in a study by Pérez-Crespo et al. (14), the male:female ratio was 4.5:1 for children with ASD. It is important to consider biological (genetic) and contextual risk factors (economic resources and educational level) to understand environmental factors as substantial epigenetic modulators. The factors that contribute to the increase in the number of male individuals with NDDs are complex and involve interactions among genetics, hormones, and environmental factors (27). Likewise, there are numerous studies that demonstrate the interference of prenatal and perinatal risk factors that modulate genetic expression in neurodevelopment (3, 2832) and that investigate the influence of the environment (33, 34). Therefore, being a man and having low socioeconomic resources constitute a risk factor for suffering from one or more NDDs (6). It is important to recognize the general underdiagnosis of NDDs in female individuals and determine the reasons for this striking difference between male and female individuals. There is a long way to go to understand the reasons why the prevalence of NDDs is lower among female individuals than male individuals. One of the most relevant reasons is that research has focussed more on masculinity (35); other possible explanations could be the socially learned behaviors and the expected behavioral stereotypes for each gender. More research from different gender perspectives and studies that take into account gender differences in the expression of NDDs are needed.

The main objectives of this study were to determine the prevalence rates, comorbidities, and NDD profiles according to the latest version of the DSM-5-TR in a population of 6-year-old boys and girls who were followed longitudinally up to 8 years of age. In the data analysis, gender was taken into account, considering the different manifestations of symptoms and consequences for boys and girls.

The secondary objectives, derived from the prevalence data obtained, were to recommend resources to improve the early detection of these disorders and improve clinical care in the studied region.

It is important to promote research with direct methodologies through clinical interviews with children, parents, and teachers. As this was a study with direct evaluations, neuropsychological examinations were important in our study, and school information served as a very valuable tool for clinical diagnosis.

Methods

The objective of our study was to determine the prevalence of NDDs in children on the island of Menorca who attended a 6-year check-up at a pediatric primary care center affiliated with public health services, as per the child–adolescent program of the Balearic Islands (36), as well as to determine different profiles through multivariate analysis. A sample was recruited directly from the population registered and affiliated with the health centers of each municipality through consecutive opportunistic selection. The sample was recruited from the Menorcan population, and all the health centers of Menorca participated: the health center (Centro de Salud – SC) of Mahón (Dalt Sant Joan), CS Es Castell, CS Ferreries, CS Es Banyer, CS Mercadal, CS Sant Lluís, and Ciutadella (Canal Salat). Figure 1 shows the proportion of participation by municipality and the number of participating subjects. Collaboration rates were higher in Canal Salat (Ciutadella) (75%), CS Es Castell (70%), Dalt Sant Joan (Mahón) (50%), and Es Banyer (45%).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Map of the participating municipalities.

The sample size for an estimated maximum prevalence of NDDs of 25% on the island of Menorca to achieve a precision of ± 5% with a confidence interval of 95% and p = 0.25 was 289 subjects. The sample size was calculated using the 2021 registry (which refers to 1 January 21); the 5-year-old population (born in 2015) included 850 subjects, and the 6-year-old population (born in 2014) included 821 subjects. Therefore, to obtain a sample with adequate representativeness for this type of study (sampling error of 3 to 5%), 289 subjects were necessary. After receiving the approval of the Ethics Committee of the Balearic Islands (comité ético de las Islas Baleares—CEIB) in December 2020, the sample was recruited consecutively by pediatricians and nurses during the months of January, February, and March 2021, the time necessary to obtain a representative sample size of 289 children. Parents of children who attended the 6-year check-up were invited to participate in the study, and subjects who agreed to participate were included. The researcher and collaborators evaluated the parents who agreed to participate in the study after they had signed the informed consent form. At all times, security measures were taken to guarantee the confidentiality of the data. A total of 345 subjects were initially recruited through pediatricians. In this recruitment phase, 38 subjects were lost due to personal reasons and travel difficulties; therefore, 307 children were evaluated in the first phase of the study. Of these 307 participants, the sample was reduced to 289 (83.7% of the initial sample), with 18 losses due to incomplete evaluations, lack of information, and dropout.

This sample of 289 subjects (initial sample) was divided into a clinical or experimental group (EG) composed of those individuals who presented a risk of presenting NDDs measured through tests and/or clinical interviews. The EG was followed longitudinally to confirm all diagnoses, including learning disorders, and was carried out in the Community Mental Health Unit for Children and Adolescents (UCSMIA, for its acronym in Spanish) within the Balearic Institute for Childhood and Adolescent Mental Health (IBSMIA, for its acronym in Spanish).

The control group (CG) was composed of those individuals who did not present any risk of developing NDDs based on the results of the assessment and screening phase.

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

Children who attended the 6-year-old consultations per the Infant-Youth Health Program at primary care centers in Menorca, which could be carried out from 2 months before reaching the age of 6 years up to 1 month before turning 7 years, were included.

Children diagnosed with NDDs at previous ages were not excluded and were included in the EG, and reports from accredited entities with specialized professionals were accepted.

All children younger than 5 years and 11 months and older than 7 years at the time of evaluation were excluded.

Sociodemographic characteristics of the study population

The sample population was children affiliated with social security; therefore, 100% of the sample had data in the public health system database (Ib-salut). Importantly, 5% of the children had concomitant private and public monitoring, that is, they were affiliated with the social security system but also received care from private services, a common practice on the island. Data on ethnic and racial diversity were not collected. Of the included population obtained through consecutive random selection, 46.7% were girls (n = 136), and 53.3% were boys (n = 155). These children attended a total of 54 different schools on the island. The level of perceived economic resources was predominantly medium (89.7%), determined by a subjective evaluation completed by the parents about their perceived socioeconomic situation.

The study was carried out 1 year after the declaration of COVID-19 as a pandemic; all required safety measures were adopted, and masks were used during evaluations, facts that must be taken into account when interpreting the results.

Study description

The sample was recruited from children attending routine child wellness consultations at primary care centers per the Child and Youth Health Program of the Balearic Islands (36). Subsequently, the families who decided to participate in the study were contacted by professionals who specialized in neurodevelopment and participated in an exhaustive general evaluation (of the child and family, separately) with different instruments that assessed different areas and warning signs. The clinical data were collected through a case report form that included risk factors associated with NDDs previously described in the literature, for example, prematurity, low birth weight, prenatal and perinatal infections, medical history, parental age and exposure to toxic substances, exposure to screens, type of diet, and participation in sports. Instruments that evaluate warning signs (Table 1) were used as screening tools to measure the risk of developing NDDs. The children were classified as having a risk or no risk of presenting NDD, yielding the experimental group (EG) and control group (CG), respectively. During the evaluation of the 289 subjects and their families, approximate times of 20–30 min and 30–40 min were required for the direct observation of the child and his/her parents, respectively.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Instruments used for the direct assessment of subjects.

During the assessments of children in the EG, a total of approximately 10 h was required for the neuropsychological examination, and a total of 6 h of clinical interviews was conducted with a child psychiatrist in the Child and Adolescent Mental Health Unit. Individuals with suspected cases of ASD and ADHD were discussed and supervised by different professionals within the existing protocols and working groups. Individuals with suspected ASD were evaluated with Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (ADI-R) and Autism Diagnosed Observation Schedule-Second Edition (ADOS-2). In addition, pertinent school reports were requested considering established territorial protocols. Individuals from whom there was clinical suspicion of an NDD were supervised by trained clinicians. Notably, the evaluations were conducted at different times to adhere to the following principles: do not spend more than 1 h per session to avoid distractions and use games to promote motivation and rest to avoid test fatigue. The schools that provided the required reports were informed. It is important to note that the clinical diagnoses followed the DSM-5-TR criteria (1).

Measurements

Table 1 shows the measures used for the neuropsychological examination and the complete assessment.

The tests that were used were all adapted to the Spanish language, which was the language used to carry out the examinations.

Data analysis

For the data analysis, a mixed methodology was used, and information exploration methods were combined through the use of standardized diagnostic tests and semistructured clinical interviews. The EG completed the WISC-5, CELF-5, PROLEC, PRO-ESCRI, TEDI-MATH, DCDQ, and a clinical assessment through interviews. Both groups (EG and CG) completed the KIDMED, the SENA, the PROLEXIA, the AQC, and the case report form. For the analysis of the data obtained, descriptive and inferential statistics were calculated, and “cluster” procedures were performed to obtain the profiles from a criterion variable.

The analysis was descriptive and quantitative in nature and included a univariate and/or bivariate analysis and a multivariate analysis. Specifically, for the descriptive analyses (univariate and bivariate), the percentages for each of the variables involved were calculated, and for the multivariate analysis, classification analysis with a criterion variable was used. SPSS (version 27) (66) was used for the univariate analysis, and SPAD (version 5.6) was used for the multivariate analysis (67). The latter allows profiles to be obtained from a variable that is to be characterized by the variables used in the tests administered in this study.

Results

The variables used in the study and the prevalence results obtained for the initial population (n = 289) are presented below.

Subsequently, the two groups, i.e., experimental group (EG) and control group (CG), are compared.

Additionally, the profiles of the EG are described.

Univariate analysis

The variables analyzed in the study are shown in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Study variables.

Initial sample

Of the initial sample, 21.5% suffered from NDDs. Of the 289 subjects included in the initial sample, that is, all individuals in the CG and EG, 2.4% presented ASD; 14% presented ADHD; 0.34% presented mild intellectual disability; 5.8% presented language disorders; 3.4% presented phonological disorder; 0.34% presented stuttering; 10% presented learning disorder with reading difficulties; 5.8% presented learning disorder with difficulties in writing; 3.11% presented learning disorder with difficulties in mathematics; 1% presented transitory tic disorder; 0.34% presented chronic tic disorder, 1% presented Tourette syndrome; 2% presented MCD; and 0.34% presented stereotypic movement disorders.

Table 3 provides the prevalence rates obtained for the initial sample and the experimental group (EG).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Prevalence of NDDs in the experimental group (EG) and initial sample.

Comorbidity in the clinical sample (EG)

Table 4 provides the comorbidity sequences for the disorders identified in the clinical sample (EG) and the corresponding frequencies.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Sequence of diagnoses case by case (n = 81).

A total of 23.4% of the examined subjects did not receive any neurodevelopmental diagnosis (19 cases without diagnosis). Of this group, some received other heterotypic diagnoses, such as emotional distress reactive to different situations (20%), and other neurodevelopmental conditions, such as intellectual precocity (1%).

The remaining 76.5% received between 1 and 6 homotypic diagnoses. A total of 27.2% received one diagnosis; 18.5% received two diagnoses; 8.6% received three diagnoses; 16% received four diagnoses; 4.9% received five diagnoses; and 1.2% received six diagnoses.

A frequent association between ADHD and learning disorders with reading and writing difficulties was observed. The association between communication disorder and learning disorder was also frequent. Although the sample was small, for ASD, there was an association with language disorder and cognitive precocity (the latter is not a homotypic diagnosis). Finally, the existence of phonological disorder is striking.

Multivariate analysis

Profile analysis

First, the results of the descriptive statistics are presented, and later, the characterization analysis of the selected variables (“cluster” with criterion variable) is shown, which, as indicated by Martí and Ruiz-Bueno (68), is about finding “the most explanatory set of individuals of the modalities of a qualitative variable” and taking into account that each group must be as homogeneous as possible among its members and as heterogeneous in relation to the others (67).

A value of p of <0.01 was used to identify the significant categories in the profiles.

Profiles according to the second phase variable

In this section, the profiles of the populations into which the sample was divided, i.e., the EG and the CG, are identified.

To identify these profiles, the presence of warning signs was taken into account.

Table 5 shows the categories of the variables that are characteristic of each profile.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Characterization of the experimental group (EG) and control group (CG).

In the EG, individuals tested positive in the Mini-Kid test for ADHD and tics presented homotypic comorbidities, had language problems in early childhood, presented risks in the PROLEXIA test and in the AQC test, had low economic resources, were from Mahón, had parents with a primary education level, and did not usually participate in sports.

In the CG, the individuals tested negative in the Mini-Kid test for ADHD and tics did not have associated comorbidities, did not have language problems in early childhood, did not present risks in the PROLEXIA test and the AQC, had parents with abundant economic resources, usually participated in sports, lived in Es Castell, and had parents with a secondary education level.

Profiles according to the second phase variables: EG and CG

The sample was analyzed considering gender differences, with male individuals being more affected than female individuals in general, except for inattentive ADHD. In the comparison between the groups (EG and CG), an important influence of context (socioeconomic level, parental studies, and sports) was evidenced in the presence of comorbidities and in disorders such as ADHD and language disorders.

Analysis of the experimental (or clinical) group

Profiles based on sex

In this section, EG profiles were identified based on sex (male or female) (Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Characterization of the EG by sex.

For female individuals, there was a predominance of inattentive ADHD, and in general, there were no alterations in language. There were no comorbidities or other disorders present (tics, motor coordination disorders (D), language disorder, and combined ADHD), and the age of the mother was usually 28 years or younger. For male individuals, there was a predominance of combined ADHD, language problems, presence of comorbidities, tics, language disorders, emotional problems (as detected using the SENA), and motor coordination disorder, and the mother’s age was usually in the range of 29–35 years.

Profiles based on sex

Table 7 details the proportion of diagnoses in the EG by sex; the statistical significance (chi-square test) is shown in bold.

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7. Diagnostic odds ratio by sex for the EG.

Profiles based on the WISC level

In this section, profiles are identified based on the WISC level. To identify these profiles, scalar variables were converted into categorical variables, following the criterion of the quartile score. The values have been grouped as follows: LOW WISC, total WISC scores lower than 89; MIDDLE WISC, total WISC scores within 90–109; HIGH WISC, total WISC scores greater than 110; and DK – NA WISC, no WISC score.

As seen in Table 8, individuals with a low WISC level had higher PROLEXIA test scores, altered CELF-5 test results, comorbidities, alterations in the development of language before 3 years, and borderline intelligence levels.

TABLE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Table 8. Characterization of EG by WISC-5 level.

Individuals with a high WISC level had very low risk (PROLEXIA test), crossed laterality, positive Mini-Kid ADHD HYPERACTIVE–IMPULSIVE and COMBINED, prolonged breastfeeding (between 21 and 25 months), medium-high economic resources, and parents with university studies.

Profiles based on high PROLEXIA results

Table 9 shows the categories of the variables that are characteristic of the profiles analyzed.

TABLE 9
www.frontiersin.org

Table 9. Characterization of the EG by high PROLEXIA results.

The group with high PROLEXIA results had comorbidities, presented risks (PROLEXIA), had a learning disorder with difficulties in reading, had language problems, presented three or more comorbid homotypic diagnoses, had a language disorder, had low CELF test results, had low WISC test results, used drugs during pregnancy, and did not usually play sports.

The group that did not have high PROLEXIA results did not present comorbidities, did not present risks (PROLEXIA), did not have language problems or other associated disorders, did not usually have associated homotypic diagnoses, had low WISC levels, did not use drugs during pregnancy, and usually participated in sports.

As seen, the education level of the parents and financial resources did not have much of an influence.

Profiles based on comorbidities

In this section, profiles are identified by the presence of comorbidities (Table 10).

TABLE 10
www.frontiersin.org

Table 10. Characterization of EG profiles by comorbidities.

Individuals with comorbidities had 2, 3, and 4 comorbid diagnoses, had alterations in language in early childhood, had learning disorders with difficulties in reading, writing, and mathematics, had combined and inattentive ADHD, had motor coordination disorder, had language disorder, had phonological disorder, had emotional alterations, as determined using the SENA, had erroneous Sally–Anne test results, had a low WISC level, and were male individuals.

Individuals with no comorbid disorders, without comorbidities, had no risk (PROLEXIA), did not have language alterations, nor phonological disorder, language disorder, or motor coordination disorder, did not have abnormal CELF-5 and DCDQ results, had correct Sally–Anne test answers, and had high or mid-range WISC levels.

Profiles as a function of participation in sports

In this section, EG profiles are identified based on those who did and did not participate in sports (Table 11).

TABLE 11
www.frontiersin.org

Table 11. Characterization of EG profiles by participation in sports.

Individuals who did participate in sports came from families with high or medium-high economic resources, did not present risk (PROLEXIA), did not have associated disorders or language alterations, did not have low WISC levels, did not participate in repetitive play, and had a eutocic delivery.

Individuals who did not play sports had a low income, had very high risk (PROLEXIA), had language disorder and language development problems, had comorbidities, had ADHD with inattentive presentation, presented a borderline total intellectual coefficient, had motor coordination disorder, did not participate in repetitive play, and were born by cesarean section.

Profiles based on the use of screens

In this section, profiles are identified based on the use of screens.

Individuals who used screens presented with combined ADHD and had an instrumental delivery.

Individuals who did not use screens had psychotic disorders in the paternal branch, had parents with a university education, were delivered by cesarean section, were usually from CEIP Tramuntana (school in contact with nature), and had correct Sally–Anne test responses.

Profiles based on the AQC test

The profile based on the AQC test suggests that individuals with results greater than or equal to 75 had a diagnosis of ASD.

Discussion

The difference found in this study between the global prevalence of having one or more NDD, estimated at 21.5%, and the figure calculated in the initial screening, 55.4%, is striking (6). This difference could indicate non-diagnosis in screening tests, potentially due to an over detection by some screening tests, which can be highly polarized and only detect very high risks, to the speed with which some tests are performed in relation to other tests, and to the detailed a posteriori examinations performed on subjects in clinical samples. Regarding the possible over detection by screening tests, it is preferable to detect more risks than to miss some of them because it is important to prevent as many cases as possible and avoid false-negative cases. In short, for screening programs, tests should be simple to apply, accepted by patients or the general population, have minimal adverse effects, and be financially supportable (69). These aspects of sensitivity and specificity could not be estimated due to the limitations of the study design. However, in screening programs, it is important to have highly sensitive tests that avoid false negatives. In our study, we consider that the PROLEXIA test, Mini-Kid ADHD, and AQC could be quite sensitive and easily applicable screening tools due to their rapidity. We have observed that the PROLEXIA is a powerful test for detecting comorbidity.

As shown in Table 3, when comparing the prevalence rates for the EG with those for the initial sample, the rates are higher in the EG. Likewise, the percentage of subjects who did not receive a diagnosis was higher in the population sample (78.5%) than in the EG (23.4%).

One of the strengths of our study is the direct assessment of each individual both in the screening phase and in the diagnostic phase, using the neuropsychological examination as a tool to provide information to the clinician to make a diagnosis with the greatest precision and information possible, avoiding errors associated with estimates or rapid and indirect tests. In addition to direct evaluations, supervision of each patient was carried out by a team of professionals trained in neurodevelopment, and the evaluations by teachers and the children’s parents were taken into account.

The prevalence rates for each NDD in this study are similar to estimates reported in the literature. These figures are consistent regardless of age, which in our case were 6-year-old children, with associated cons (age limitation) and pros (large sample of subjects of the same age). The fact that studies present wide age ranges can lead to biases in the sense of identifying cases at an early age, which are of interest to us because early identification favors an early diagnosis of subtle warning signs. The age chosen for this study should be discussed. From the age of 6 years, clinicians diagnose almost all NDDs, except learning disorders. In this study, a longitudinal follow-up was performed that began at 6 years and ended at 8 years with the assessment of the learning sphere to confirm diagnoses. In this way, we were able to confirm diagnoses of those individuals at risk of suffering from learning disorders identified in the screening phase, i.e., when the children were 6 years old. With regard to ASD, 6 years is not an age of early diagnosis; however, our objective was to detect more subtle cases that could go unnoticed at an early age, e.g., cases of autism associated with a superior IQ (of which two cases were detected) or female autism. Both types tend to go unnoticed and are diagnosed in adulthood. It is likely that there is an underreporting of women with ASD, especially among those with high cognitive performance, an effect possibly related to the assessment techniques used. In general, women with autism have better early language development and better social skills, and their play may even develop as expected (70). We did not detect cases of severe ASD or moderate or severe ID; if there were any, they would have already been diagnosed at an earlier age.

The sample size (n = 289) could be considered a limitation of this study if compared with that in other population prevalence studies from other territories; however, it should be taken into account that this n is representative of the Menorcan population and that, assessing subjects in two periods with direct evaluations, it would be unfeasible and extremely expensive in terms of time spent, money, and personnel needed. In addition, there are no studies that cover specific ages but rather broader age ranges.

We observed that inattentive ADHD was predominant in women and that combined and hyperactive–impulsive ADHD was predominant in men, who were also more affected by the rest of the disorders, such as language disorder, motor disorder, ASD, and learning disorder. In addition, male sex is associated with higher rates of comorbidity.

Some findings of the multivariate analysis are notable. Being a girl is associated with younger mothers. The profile of children with a higher total intelligence quotient (IQ) was more likely to have hyperactive–impulsive ADHD (potentially because they are more curious), and a high TIQ was associated with longer breastfeeding (between 15 and 25 months). The authors of previous studies concluded that “breastfeeding could significantly improve the intelligence of children, with a duration >6 months result in an intelligence score slightly but significantly higher than that for a duration ≤6 months” (71) and that breastfeeding is related to higher performance on intelligence tests (72, 73). Furthermore, the authors of a previous review (74) concluded that “breastfeeding has a small positive effect on IQ in late childhood. The evidence to suggest that breastfeeding is a protective factor in the development of conduct disorders and the achievement of greater executive function is limited.” All these could be influenced by context, that is, higher educational and socioeconomic levels. In the profiling, we only found that an “optimal diet” determined by KIDMED is characteristic of individuals with an average IQ determined by WISC. This is the reason why we have not gone on to make profiles based on diet, since it seems that the variables sports and screen use appear more frequently in the characterization of profiles. In this sense, sports and less exposure to screens could be linked to less comorbidity.

Disadvantaged contexts in the development of disorders such as language disorders and ADHD are more independent of genetic factors. As seen in learning disorders, the educational level of the parents and financial resources does not influence learning disorders as much as other disorders (35, 75, 76).

In this study, the environment is an island with a population that has tended to remain stable over time, indicating that the results would be more consistent with reality, i.e., with little change. However, the environment of the island could be a limitation; it is a semirural area, where the largest urban centers are two small cities that do not exceed 30,000 inhabitants. This should be taken into account when comparing the results herein with those of studies where the population is more changeable and where population centers tend to be more diverse.

The analyses used were selected on the basis of the intentions of the study, that is, a descriptive and exploratory focus on the EG. Bivariate analyses were used to contrast population-type samples and sociodemographic aspects, more specifically gender, due to its importance as indicated by the latest evidence for each disorder (27, 77, 78). As the EG had a small sample, adequate statistical inferences are difficult because it was not possible to use parametric tests; for this reason, cluster analysis by classification (classification analysis with criterion variable) was used.

In this study, a population sample that could have clinical manifestations was analyzed. The sample size could have been increased by recruiting from schools; it is unknown how this approach would have influenced the representativeness of the results obtained. The method chosen is consistent with the goal of promoting early detection at primary care centers and facilitating communication between primary and specialized services, which is scarce and necessary in our environment.

Conclusion

The importance of this study is the direct assessment of each individual in the sample through screening tests and clinical interviews and the use of a neuropsychological examination as a complementary diagnostic tool that, in many mental health centers, continues to be difficult to apply due to time limitations for consultations. The multidisciplinary work that has been carried out throughout the study’s trajectory is noteworthy, starting with primary care pediatric services to specialized mental health units, schools, and a neuropsychology team with extensive experience. One limitation of the public health system is the lack of time and professionals for the exhaustive assessment of each disorder. There can be doubts regarding the diagnosis of certain disorders when there is a lack of neuropsychological and human tools to perform assessments, which is the case for language and learning disorders in our community, a fact that delays diagnoses and results in higher rates of school failure (76, 79).

The results for the sample appear to be consistent with those reported in the scientific literature and with the predictions of clinicians.

The comorbidity of ADHD and learning disorder was observed, as also evidenced in scientific annals (76), as was general multimorbidity in male individuals (35, 80) and a predominance of inattentive ADHD in female individuals. The prevalence of communication disorder, both language disorder and phonological disorder, and its association with learning disorder and ADHD is apparent. Future research should study the nature of these associations.

In disadvantaged contexts (low economic resources and lower levels of education of parents), there is a higher prevalence of NDDs, except if the disorder is very extreme or genetically determined (such as dyslexia or ASD), which will manifest regardless of the environment. An important influence of context has been evidenced in the presentation of comorbidities in language disorders and ADHD.

Sociodemographic variables could be as powerful predictors as screening tests, or having a sociodemographic variable from a disadvantaged context should alert clinicians to the possibility of an NDD. Profiles of the disadvantaged context include low economic resources, lower levels of education of parents, and lifestyle habits that can be improved.

Additionally, policies should be implemented that provide public services with tools and personnel to be able to detect, diagnose, and treat NDDs, with an emphasis on learning and language difficulties, which are not easily detected because of a lack of resources.

In addition, we urge the development of health promotion programs in schools and CSs (balanced diet and physical activity) so that they can be extended to families and, therefore, societies. As Farholm and Sørensen (81) note, the modification of lifestyle factors, such as increased physical activity and improved diet, is associated with a reduction in health problems as well as improvements in mental health. Zaman et al. (82) affirm that positively modifying lifestyle factors, with an emphasis on food, diet, and exercise, can help to improve and/or prevent medical and psychiatric disorders. Exercise can be a marker of those populations with better socioeconomic conditions and greater cognitive abilities of parents who choose better lifestyle habits; however, it could not be ruled out that exercise per se is a factor associated with better neurodevelopment because it is a factor in very early brain development and implicated in many neurodevelopmental disorders.

Notably, a significant proportion of the sample had never been diagnosed (88.6%); for this reason, early detection programs are recommended that include psychoeducation for parents and the detection of warning signs by primary care services and schools. In short, policies are needed that help and support the most disadvantaged sectors of the population: The more socioeconomic resources in a population, the less risk it will have. In conclusion, context takes on a role almost as important as genetics or gender, with the difference that it can be modified. In this way, focusing on secondary prevention, risks can be reduced by improving contexts.

Context and the epigenetic modification that it exerts when accelerating the manifestation and presentation of NDDs can be evidenced in exposure to screens (and its association with higher rates of ADHD diagnosis as well as greater emotional dysregulation), a reduction in the risk of NDDs through participating in sports (6), and the vulnerability of individuals in the most disadvantaged contexts, i.e., low income and lower education level of parents (in language disorder and ADHD, it exerts accelerating by exposure to screens). For this reason, it is of paramount importance in view of health planning to support and invest in policies that support these most affected sectors to prevent and reduce risk factors for NDDs.

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Comité de ética de les Illes Balears (CEIB). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

LF: Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AR: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Software, Writing – original draft. CS: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Resources, Writing – original draft. JF: Writing – review & editing. JC: Funding acquisition, Investigation, Resources, Writing – original draft. AH: Writing – review & editing. CC: Investigation, Writing – original draft. BC: Investigation, Writing – original draft. EQ: Investigation, Writing – review & editing. AF: Supervision, Writing – review & editing. JQ: Supervision, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This study was funded by the Menorquin Institute of Studies (Instituto Menorquín de Estudios—IME). Approval can be found in the Official Gazette of the Balearic Islands (Boletín Oficial de las Islas Baleares—BOIB) number 128 (16 September 2021) at the following URL: http://www.ime.cat/WebEditor/Pagines/file/BOIB-Aprovaci%C3%B3%20ajuts%20IME%202021.pdf.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank the research team (Arrels Institute) for making this study possible and for their work and altruistic dedication. The authors also thank the Balearic Islands Health System and the Menorca Health Area for the support provided during the process. Likewise, the authors thank the Menorquin Institute of Studies (IME), Balearic Islands, for financially supporting our study titled Prevalencia de trastornos del neurodesarrollo en las consultas de atención primaria del Programa Infanto-Juvenil de la isla de Menorca en niños y niñas de 6 años (“Prevalence of neurodevelopmental disorders in primary care consultations of the Infant-Juvenile Program of the island of Menorca in boys and girls aged 6 years”). Finally, the authors thank all the professionals, including pediatricians, statisticians, and nurses, who altruistically dedicated their time to this study.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. APA. (2022). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders fifth edition text revision DSM-5-TR. Available at: https://www.appi.org/dsm-5-tr

Google Scholar

2. ICD. (2023). ICD-11. Available at: https://icd.who.int/en

Google Scholar

3. Simonoff, E, Pickles, A, Charman, T, Chandler, S, Loucas, T, and Baird, G. Psychiatric disorders in children with autism spectrum disorders: prevalence, comorbidity, and associated factors in a population-derived sample. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2008) 47:921–9. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e318179964f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Leyfer, OT, Folstein, SE, Bacalman, S, Davis, NO, Dinh, E, Morgan, J, et al. Comorbid psychiatric disorders in children with autism: interview development and rates of disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2006) 36:849–61. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0123-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Catalá-López, F, Ridao, M, Núñez-Beltrán, A, Gènova-Maleras, R, Alonso-Arroyo, A, Aleixandre-Benavent, R, et al. Prevalence and comorbidity of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Spain: study protocol for extending a systematic review with updated meta-analysis of observational studies. Syst Rev. (2019) 8:49. doi: 10.1186/s13643-019-0967-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Francés, L, Caules, J, Ruiz, A, Soler, CV, Hervás, A, Fernández, A, et al. An approach for prevention planning based on the prevalence and comorbidity of neurodevelopmental disorders in 6-year-old children receiving primary care consultations on the island of Menorca. BMC Pediatr. (2023) 23:32. doi: 10.1186/s12887-023-03844-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Kita, Y, Ashizawa, F, and Inagaki, M. Prevalence estimates of neurodevelopmental disorders in Japan: a community sample questionnaire study. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2019) 74:118–23. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12950

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Fleming, M, Salim, EE, Mackay, DF, Henderson, A, Kinnear, D, Clark, D, et al. Neurodevelopmental multimorbidity and educational outcomes of Scottish schoolchildren: a population-based record linkage cohort study. PLoS Med. (2020) 17:e1003290. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1003290

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Francés, L, Quintero, J, Fernández, A, Ruiz, A, Caules, J, Fillon, G, et al. Current state of knowledge on the prevalence of neurodevelopmental disorders in childhood according to the DSM-5: a systematic review in accordance with the PRISMA criteria. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. (2022) 16:27. doi: 10.1186/s13034-022-00462-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Zwaigenbaum, L, and Penner, M. Autism spectrum disorder: advances in diagnosis and evaluation. BMJ. (2018) 361:k1674. doi: 10.1136/bmj.k1674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Hansen, BH, Oerbeck, B, Skirbekk, B, Petrovski, BÉ, and Kristensen, H. Neurodevelopmental disorders: prevalence and comorbidity in children referred to mental health services. Nord J Psychiatry. (2018) 72:285–91. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2018.1444087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. US EPA. America’s children and the environment. EPA; United States Environmental Protection Agency (2015).

Google Scholar

13. Saito, M, Hirota, T, Sakamoto, Y, Adachi, M, Takahashi, M, Osato-Kaneda, A, et al. Prevalence and cumulative incidence of autism spectrum disorders and the patterns of co-occurring neurodevelopmental disorders in a total population sample of 5-year-old children. Mol Autism. (2020) 11:35. doi: 10.1186/s13229-020-00342-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Pérez-Crespo, L, Prats-Uribe, A, Tobias, A, Duran-Tauleria, E, Coronado, R, Hervás, A, et al. Temporal and geographical variability of prevalence and incidence of autism spectrum disorder diagnoses in children in Catalonia, Spain. Autism Res. (2019) 12:1693–705. doi: 10.1002/aur.2172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Mariño, MC, Ageitos, AG, Alvarez, JA, Garma, MDR, Cendón, CG, Castaño, AG, et al. Prevalencia de trastornos del neurodesarrollo, comportamiento y aprendizaje en Atención Primaria. An Pediatr. (2018) 89:153–61. doi: 10.1016/j.anpedi.2017.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Elsabbagh, M, Divan, G, Koh, YJ, Kim, YS, Kauchali, S, Marcín, C, et al. Global prevalence of autism and other pervasive developmental disorders. Autism Res. (2012) 5:160–79. doi: 10.1002/aur.239

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Polanczyk, GV, Willcutt, EG, Salum, GA, Kieling, C, and Rohde, LA. ADHD prevalence estimates across three decades: an updated systematic review and meta-regression analysis. Int J Epidemiol. (2014) 43:434–42. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyt261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Baio, J, Wiggins, L, Christensen, DL, Maenner, MJ, Daniels, J, Warren, Z, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years – autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. MMWR Surveill Summ. (2018) 67:1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Fombonne, E. Epidemiology of pervasive developmental disorders. Pediatr Res. (2009) 65:591–8. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Al-Yagon, M, Cavendish, W, Cornoldi, C, Fawcett, AJ, Grünke, M, Hung, L-Y, et al. The proposed changes for DSM-5 for SLD and ADHD: international perspectives--Australia, Germany, Greece, India, Israel, Italy, Spain, Taiwan, United Kingdom, and United States. J Learn Disabil. (2013) 46:58–72. doi: 10.1177/0022219412464353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Taanila, A, Yliherva, A, Kaakinen, M, Moilanen, I, and Ebeling, H. An epidemiological study on Finnish school-aged children with learning difficulties and behavioural problems. Int J Circumpolar Health. (2011) 70:59–71. doi: 10.3402/ijch.v70i1.17799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Lingam, R, Hunt, L, Golding, J, Jongmans, M, and Emond, A. Prevalence of developmental coordination disorder using the DSM-IV at 7 years of age: a UK population–based study. Pediatrics. (2009) 123:e693–700. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-1770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Tsiotra, GD, Flouris, AD, Koutedakis, Y, Faught, BE, Nevill, AM, Lane, AM, et al. A comparison of developmental coordination disorder prevalence rates in Canadian and Greek children. J Adolesc Health. (2006) 39:125–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2005.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Catalá-López, F, Peiró, S, Ridao, M, Sanfélix-Gimeno, G, Gènova-Maleras, R, and Catalá, MA. Prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder among children and adolescents in Spain: a systematic review and meta-analysis of epidemiological studies. BMC Psychiatry. (2012) 12:168. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-12-168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Sayal, K, Prasad, V, Daley, D, Ford, T, and Coghill, D. ADHD in children and young people: prevalence, care pathways, and service provision. Lancet Psychiatry. (2018) 5:175–86. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(17)30167-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Faraone, SV, Banaschewski, T, Coghill, D, Zheng, Y, Biederman, J, Bellgrove, MA, et al. The World Federation of ADHD International Consensus Statement: 208 evidence-based conclusions about the disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2021) 128:789–818. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.01.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. May, T, Adesina, I, McGillivray, J, and Rinehart, NJ. Sex differences in neurodevelopmental disorders. Curr Opin Neurol. (2019) 32:622–6. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0000000000000714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Dzib-Goodin, A, and Sanders, L. Relación entre la infección viral, citoquinas y trastornos del neurodesarrollo. Rev Mex Neurosci. (2014) 15:218–23.

Google Scholar

29. Linsell, L, Malouf, R, Morris, J, Kurinczuk, JJ, and Marlow, N. Prognostic factors for poor cognitive development in children born very preterm or with very low birth weight: a systematic review. JAMA Pediatr. (2015) 169:1162–72. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2015.2175

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Voltas, N, Arija, V, Hernández-Martínez, C, Jiménez-Feijoo, R, Ferré, N, and Canals, J. Are there early inflammatory biomarkers that affect neurodevelopment in infancy? J Neuroimmunol. (2017) 305:42–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jneuroim.2017.01.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Owens, EB, and Hinshaw, SP. Perinatal problems and psychiatric comorbidity among children with ADHD. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2013) 42:762–8. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2013.785359

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Huang, L, Wang, Y, Zhang, L, Zheng, Z, Zhu, T, Qu, Y, et al. Maternal smoking and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in offspring: a meta-analysis. Pediatrics. (2018) 141:e20172465. doi: 10.1542/peds.2017-2465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Beckman, L, Janson, S, and von Kobyletzki, L. Associations between neurodevelopmental disorders and factors related to school, health, and social interaction in schoolchildren: results from a Swedish population-based survey. Disabil Health J. (2016) 9:663–72. doi: 10.1016/j.dhjo.2016.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Palamar, JJ, Calzada, EJ, Theise, R, Huang, KY, Petkova, E, and Brotman, LM. Family- and neighborhood-level factors as predictors of conduct problems in school among young, urban, minority children. Behav Med. (2015) 41:177–85. doi: 10.1080/08964289.2014.907770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Krafnick, AJ, and Evans, TM. Neurobiological sex differences in developmental dyslexia. Front Psychol. (2019) 9:2669. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Programa de Salud Infanto. (2018). Adolescente del servei de salut de les illes balears. Available at: https://www.ibsalut.es/apmallorca/attachments/article/1599/psia-es-anexos.pdf

Google Scholar

37. WISC-V. (2014). Escala de inteligencia de Wechsler para niños-V. Available at: https://www.pearsonclinical.es/wisc-v-escala-de-inteligencia-de-wechsler-para-ninos-v

Google Scholar

38. Auyeung, B, Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, and Allison, C. The autism spectrum quotient: children’s version (AQ-Child). J Autism Dev Disord. (2008) 38:1230–40.

Google Scholar

39. Cuetos, F, Arribas-Aguila, D, Suárez-Coalla, P, and Martínez-García, C. PROLEXIA. Diagnóstico y detección temprana de la dislexia. (2020).

Google Scholar

40. Cuetos, F, Rodríguez, B, Ruano, E, and Arribas, D. (2007). Batería de evaluación de los procesos lectores – Revisada. Available at: https://web.teaediciones.com/PROLEC-R-Bateria-de-Evaluacion-de-los-Procesos-Lectores--Revisad.aspx

Google Scholar

41. Oliveira, AMD, Santos, JLF, Vega, FC, and Capellini, SA. Tradução e adaptação cultural da bateria de avaliação dos processos de leitura – PROLEC-SE-R. CoDAS. (2020) 32:e20180204. doi: 10.1590/2317-1782/20192018204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Cuetos, F, Ramos, JL, and Ruano, E. (2002). PROESC. Batería de evaluación de los procesos de escritura. Available at: https://web.teaediciones.com/proesc-bateria-de-evaluacion-de-los-procesos-de-escritura.aspx

Google Scholar

43. Grégoire, J, Noël, MP, and Nieuwenhoven, CV. (2015). TEDI-MATH. Test para el diagnóstico de las competencias básicas en matemáticas. Available at: https://web.teaediciones.com/tedi-math.aspx.

Google Scholar

44. Kiechl-Kohlendorfer, U, Ralser, E, Peglow, UP, Pehboeck-Walser, N, and Fussenegger, B. Early risk predictors for impaired numerical skills in 5-year-old children born before 32 weeks of gestation. Acta Paediatr. (2012) 102:66–71. doi: 10.1111/apa.12036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Fischer, U, Moeller, K, Bientzle, M, Cress, U, and Nuerk, HC. Sensori-motor spatial training of number magnitude representation. Psychon Bull Rev. (2010) 18:177–83. doi: 10.3758/s13423-010-0031-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Wiig, E, Semel, E, and Secord, W. (2018). Evaluación clínica de los fundamentos del lenguaje-5. Available at: https://www.pearsonclinical.es/celf-5-evaluacion-clinica-de-los-fundamentos-del-lenguaje-5

Google Scholar

47. Malhotra, PS, Densky, J, Melachuri, M, Melachuri, S, Onwuka, A, Winner, K, et al. The impact of cochlear implantation on speech and language outcomes in children with asymmetric sensorineural hearing loss. Int J Pediatr Otorhinolaryngol. (2022) 152:110979. doi: 10.1016/j.ijporl.2021.110979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Poth, LD, Love, T, and Mattson, SN. Profiles of language and communication abilities in adolescents with fetal alcohol spectrum disorders. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2022) 29:724–33. doi: 10.1017/S1355617722000789

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Wilson, B, and Crawford, S. The developmental coordination disorder questionnaire 2007©© (DCDQ’07). BN Wilson (2007).

Google Scholar

50. Hudson, KN, and Willoughby, MT. Evaluating the factor structure and criterion validity of the Canadian little DCDQ: associations between motor competence, executive functions, early numeracy skills, and ADHD in early childhood. Assessment. (2021) 29:1134–43. doi: 10.1177/10731911211003967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Ray-Kaeser, S, Thommen, E, Martini, R, Jover, M, Gurtner, B, and Bertrand, AM. Psychometric assessment of the French European developmental coordination disorder questionnaire (DCDQ-FE). PLoS One. (2019) 14:e0217280. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Caravale, B, Baldi, S, Gasparini, C, and Wilson, BN. Cross-cultural adaptation, reliability and predictive validity of the Italian version of Developmental Coordination Disorder Questionnaire (DCDQ). Eur J Paediatr Neurol. (2014) 18:267–72. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2013.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Pannekoek, L, Rigoli, D, Piek, JP, Barrett, NC, and Schoemaker, M. The revised DCDQ: is it a suitable screening measure for motor difficulties in adolescents? Adapt Phys Act Q. (2012) 29:81–97. doi: 10.1123/apaq.29.1.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Serra-Majem, L, Ribas, L, Ngo, J, Ortega, RM, García, A, Pérez-Rodrigo, C, et al. Food, youth and the Mediterranean diet in Spain. Development of KIDMED, Mediterranean Diet Quality Index in children and adolescents. Public Health Nutr. (2004) 7:931–5. doi: 10.1079/PHN2004556

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ojeda-Rodríguez, A, Zazpe, I, Morell-Azanza, L, Chueca, MJ, Azcona-Sanjulian, MC, and Marti, A. Improved diet quality and nutrient adequacy in children and adolescents with abdominal obesity after a lifestyle intervention. Nutrients. (2018) 10:1500. doi: 10.3390/nu10101500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Auyeung, B, Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, and Allison, C. (2008). doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0504-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Takenoshita, S, Hayashi, S, Shinya, T, Miki, T, Yokota, O, Maki, Y, et al. Sally–Anne test and regional cerebral blood flow in Alzheimer's disease dementia. Psychogeriatrics. (2020) 20:549–56. doi: 10.1111/psyg.12533

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Sánchez-Sánchez, F, Fernández-Pinto, I, Santamaría, P, Carrasco, M, and del Barrio, V. SENA, sistema de evaluación de niños y adolescentes: proceso de desarrollo y evidencias de fiabilidad y validez. Rev Psicol Clín Niños Adolesc. (2016) 3:23–34.

Google Scholar

59. Gioia, GA, Isquith, PK, Guy, SC, and Kenworthy, L. (2017). Evaluación conductual de la función ejecutiva – 2. Available at: https://web.teaediciones.com/BRIEF-2-Evaluacion-Conductual-de-la-Funcion-Ejecutiva.aspx.

Google Scholar

60. Belmonte, MJM, del Castillo, CF, Arias, RM, Marqués, JG, JME-S, Roca, and Santamaría, P. (2017). XXX.

Google Scholar

61. Pino-Muñoz, M, and Filippetti, VA. Confirmatory factor analysis of the BRIEF-2 parent and teacher form: relationship to performance-based measures of executive functions and academic achievement. Appl Neuropsychol Child. (2019) 10:219–33. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2019.1660984

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Thurstone, LL, and Yela, M. (2021). Test de percepción de diferencias-revisado. Available at: https://web.teaediciones.com/CARAS-R-Test-de-Percepcion-de-Diferencias---Revisado.aspx

Google Scholar

63. Servera, M, and Llabrés, J. (2018). CSAT-R: tarea de atención sostenida en la infancia revisada. Available at: https://hablemosdeneurociencia.com/csat-r-tarea-de-atencion-sostenida/.

Google Scholar

64. Pérez, JP, Arias, MDRM, and Astorqui, LZ. Evaluación neuropsicológica de las funciones ejecutivas en niños (ENFEN). Madrid: TEA (2011).

Google Scholar

65. Navarro-Soria, I, de Mier, RJR, García-Fernández, JM, González-Gómez, C, Real-Fernández, M, de León, MS-M, et al. Detection of executive performance profiles using the ENFEN battery in children diagnosed with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:552322. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.552322

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Pallant, J. SPSS survival manual: a step by step guide to data analysis using IBM SPSS. London: Routledge (2020).

Google Scholar

67. Bécue-Bertaut, M. Minería de textos. Aplicación a preguntas abiertas en encuestas. Madrid, Spain: Editorial La Muralla (2010).

Google Scholar

68. Martí, A, and Ruiz-Bueno, A. Análisis de clasificación con variable criterio en SPAD. Rev Innov Recer Educ. (2018) 11:41–53.

Google Scholar

69. Guía. (2003). Pruebas diagnósticas: sensibilidad y especificidad. Available at: https://www.fisterra.com/formacion/metodologia-investigacion/pruebas-diagnosticas-sensibilidad-especificidad/.

Google Scholar

70. Ruggieri, VL, and Arberas, CL. Autism in females: clinical, neurobiological and genetic aspects. Rev Neurol. (2016) 62:S21–6. doi: 10.33588/rn.62S01.2016009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Hou, L, Li, X, Yan, P, Li, Y, Wu, Y, Yang, Q, et al. Impact of the duration of breastfeeding on the intelligence of children: a systematic review with network meta-analysis. Breastfeed Med. (2021) 16:687–96. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2020.0364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Horta, BL, de Mola, CL, and Victora, CG. Breastfeeding and intelligence: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Paediatr. (2015) 104:14–9. doi: 10.1111/apa.13139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kanazawa, S. Breastfeeding is positively associated with child intelligence even net of parental IQ. Dev Psychol. (2015) 51:1683–9. doi: 10.1037/dev0000060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. McGowan, C, and Bland, R. The benefits of breastfeeding on child intelligence, behavior, and executive function: a review of recent evidence. Breastfeed Med. (2023) 18:172–87. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2022.0192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Pennington, BF, and Lefly, DL. Early reading development in children at family risk for dyslexia. Child Dev. (2001) 72:816–33. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Lindgren, M, Jensen, J, Dalteg, A, Meurling, AW, Ingvar, DH, and Levander, S. Dyslexia and AD/HD among Swedish prison inmates. J Scand Stud Criminol Crime Prev. (2002) 3:84–95. doi: 10.1080/140438502762467227

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Valls, C. Mujeres invisibles para la medicina. Madrid: Capitán Swing (2021).

Google Scholar

78. Moseley, RL, Hitchiner, R, and Kirkby, JA. Self-reported sex differences in high-functioning adults with autism: a meta-analysis. Mol Autism. (2018) 9:33. doi: 10.1186/s13229-018-0216-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Rivera, FB. La elevada prevalencia del TDAH: posibles causas y repercusiones socioeducativas. Psicol Educ. (2016) 22:81–5. doi: 10.1016/j.pse.2015.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Nowak, S, and Jacquemont, S. The effects of sex on prevalence and mechanisms underlying neurodevelopmental disorders. Handb Clin Neurol. (2020) 173:327–39. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-444-64150-2.00025-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Farholm, A, and Sørensen, M. Motivation for physical activity and exercise in severe mental illness: a systematic review of intervention studies. Int J Ment Health Nurs. (2016) 25:194–205. doi: 10.1111/inm.12214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Zaman, R, Hankir, A, and Jemni, M. Lifestyle factors and mental health. Psychiatr Danub. (2019) 31:217–20.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Glossary

Keywords: neurodevelopmental disorders, prevalence, childhood, DSM-5-TR, ADHD, ASD, dyslexia, language disorders

Citation: Francés L, Ruiz A, Soler CV, Francés J, Caules J, Hervás A, Carretero C, Cardona B, Quezada E, Fernández A and Quintero J (2023) Prevalence, comorbidities, and profiles of neurodevelopmental disorders according to the DSM-5-TR in children aged 6 years old in a European region. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1260747. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1260747

Received: 27 September 2023; Accepted: 16 October 2023;
Published: 10 November 2023.

Edited by:

Guiomar Gonçalves Oliveira, University of Coimbra, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Teresa Castelo, Coimbra Hospital and University, Portugal
Micaela Guardiano Domingues, University Hospital Center of Porto, Portugal

Copyright © 2023 Francés, Ruiz, Soler, Francés, Caules, Hervás, Carretero, Cardona, Quezada, Fernández and Quintero. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lorena Francés, bG9yZWZyYW5AdWNtLmVz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.