Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 07 December 2023
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Positive Psychological Characters and Internet Addiction among Adolescents: A Positive Psychology Perspective View all articles

Longitudinal relationship between internet self-control and problematic internet use among Chinese adolescents: mediating role of meaning in life

Weijun Wang,,Weijun Wang1,2,3Jianmei Ye,
Jianmei Ye1,2*Yimeng Zhu,Yimeng Zhu2,4Dawei Huang,Dawei Huang1,2Xin Zhao
Xin Zhao5*
  • 1Key Laboratory of Adolescent Cyberpsychology and Behavior, Ministry of Education, Wuhan, Hubei Province, China
  • 2School of Psychology, Central China Normal University, Wuhan, Hubei Province, China
  • 3Institute of Digital Commerce, Wuhan Technology and Business University, Wuhan, China
  • 4Key Laboratory of Human Development and Mental Health of Hubei Province, Wuhan, China
  • 5Information School, The University of Sheffield, Sheffield, United Kingdom

Introduction: While studies indicate that high self-control may serve as a safeguard against problematic internet use, there’s evidence suggesting that problematic internet use can, in turn, diminish self-control. This study aimed to elucidate the longitudinal interplay between internet self-control and problematic internet use in adolescents, employing cross-lagged panel modeling. Furthermore, drawing from a positive psychology perspective, we examined the potential role of ‘meaning in life’ as a protective mediator within this longitudinal relationship. We then constructed a mediation model to explore protective factors against problematic internet use.

Methods: Through a questionnaire, we tracked 659 adolescents (331 males and 328 females; mean age=13.61) in a longitudinal design across two time points, spaced at five-month intervals, to assess their internet self-control, problematic internet use, and meaning in life.

Results: Results of the cross-lagged panel models showed that: Internet self-control had a significant negative impact on problematic internet use after five months (β = −0.094, p < 0.01). Conversely, problematic internet use had a significant negative impact on internet self-control after five months (β = −0.099, p < 0.05). Results from the longitudinal mediation model showed that: Meaning in life mediated the effect of internet self-control on problematic internet use after five months (βinternet self-control(T1)-meaning in life(T2) = 0.142, p < 0.01; βmeaning in life(T1)-problematic internet use (T2) = −0.075, p < 0.05).

Conclusion: Our study uncovers a reciprocal predictive relationship between internet self-control and problematic internet use, while highlighting the mediating role of meaning in life within this relationship. These findings suggest that fostering internet self-control and cultivating a sense of meaning in life among adolescents can serve as effective prevention and intervention strategies for addressing the issue of problematic internet use.

1 Introduction

According to the latest report from the “China Statistical Yearbook 2021,” the number of Chinese adolescents, aged 7–18, engaging as internet users has surged to 191 million, resulting in a staggering internet penetration rate of 96.8% among minors (1). Minors use the Internet for an average of more than 5 h on holidays (2). According to the standard criteria for identifying behavioral addictions, excessive involvement in any type of activity (e.g., compulsive buying, binge eating, excessive work involvement, and excessive internet use) may be classified as a psychiatric disorder (3). Excessive, compulsive, and uncontrollable use of the Internet is referred to as problematic Internet use (PIU, also known as internet addiction). PIU may lead to functional impairment in daily activities (4, 5), adversely affecting the overall well-being and societal functioning of adolescents (6, 7). PIU has emerged as a significant mental health concern (8), particularly among adolescents (9). Unregulated internet usage among adolescents has been found to have detrimental effects on their self-adaptation and social skills (10). Furthermore, it is associated with an elevated risk of social anxiety, depression, and loneliness (11). Additionally, excessive internet use has been linked to higher levels of academic procrastination (12), poorer academic performance, and reduced academic achievement among adolescents (13). Research has indicated a considerable prevalence of PIU among Chinese adolescents, estimated at 7.7% (14). These findings underscore the need for further research exploring the mental health correlates and potential interventions for PIU in adolescents in China, who appear to be particularly vulnerable.

Owing to the detrimental effects of PIU on the psychological development of adolescents, prior research has endeavored to explore associated risk and protective factors, such as, impulsivity, codependency (15), meaning in life, and self-esteem (16), self-efficacy and self-control (17). Notably, self-control has emerged as a prominent factor, with empirical evidence demonstrating its associations with the severity of PIU among adolescents (1820). However, bidirectional effects have been noted, with PIU also inversely predicting adolescent self-control (21). At present, the temporal precedence and mediating pathways underlying the relationship between adolescent self-control and PIU remain elusive. Understanding causal pathways and underlying mechanisms linking self-control and PIU will inform targeted preventative interventions. Therefore, the objective of our study is to elucidate the associations between self-control and PIU in the online context among adolescents, while also exploring potential mediators within this relationship.

1.1 Internet self-control and problematic internet use

According to the basic psychological needs theory (22), the failure to satisfy an individual’s three fundamental psychological needs of autonomy, competence, and belonging can have a negative impact on them. Wu et al. (23) found that adolescents often develop an addiction to online games as a result of their unmet needs for autonomy and a sense of competence in real life. Throughout the developmental process of adolescents, various needs arise, and when these needs are inadequately fulfilled, a process of “pathological compensation” occurs (24). The Compensatory internet use (CITU) posits that individuals might turn to online activities as a way to cope with negative life situations and alleviate distressing feelings (25). In this process, adolescents may engage in activities such as online gaming, which can provide a readily accessible sense of accomplishment as a form of compensation. Consequently, the capacity for self-control and regulation becomes a significant determinant in the occurrence of PIU (26).

Self-control refers to the ability to suppress immediate impulses and regulate one’s behavior (17), while internet self-control specifically pertains to an individual’s capacity to regulate their behavior on the internet (27). Current research suggests that high levels of self-control serve as a protective factor against PIU in adolescents (20, 28). Individuals with greater self-control are shown to effectively regulate their internet use, resisting the allure of stimulating elements in the virtual online world. They tend to exhibit rational control over their online activities and are less prone to excessive immersion in the pleasurable aspects offered by the online environment (29). Conversely, individuals with lower levels of self-control are more susceptible to becoming absorbed in the online world. They often lack effective management and regulation of negative emotions, turning to PIU as an escape from real-life issues (26). Furthermore, individuals with lower levels of self-control are more inclined to depend on the internet to meet their emotional needs. This indulgence in utilizing the internet for personal gratification can potentially exacerbate the progression of PIU (20).

According to basic psychological needs theory, individuals with high levels of self-control often exhibit greater autonomy and possess the ability to independently manage their online behavior. They can effectively regulate their internet usage time and activities, thereby minimizing the risk of excessive immersion and addiction (30). In contrast, individuals with low levels of self-control, upon engaging with the internet, tend to find that the diversity and convenience of the online world expeditiously fulfill their basic needs. As the internet’s capacity to meet individuals’ psychological needs increases, they become more susceptible to excessive immersion in the online world (31).

However, other studies have found that PIU can negatively predict self-control (21, 32). Ko et al. (33) found that college students with higher levels of PIU exhibited higher novelty seeking and risk-taking tendencies, as well as poorer self-control. Similarly, Reed et al. (34) through experiments discovered that individuals reporting higher levels of PIU exhibited greater impulsivity and lower levels of self-control when exposed to the internet. These research findings consistently indicate that self-control can also be an outcome variable of PIU.

The aforementioned arguments underscore the necessity for longitudinal investigations to explore the relationship between self-control and PIU. As cross-sectional data precludes testing directionality, we propose a longitudinal study utilizing a cross-lagged panel model to delineate predictive associations between internet self-control and PIU use over time.

1.2 Mediating role of meaning in life

Basic psychological needs theory posits that effective self-control enables adolescents to make self-directed choices aligned with internalized values, thereby fulfilling their core autonomy requirements (35). Eakman (36) states that meaningful activity fulfills basic psychological needs (i.e., autonomy, competence, and relatedness) and contributes to meaning in life. Conversely, those whose core needs remain unfulfilled are more likely to perceive their lives as lacking meaning. Meaning in Life refers to the sense made of, and significance felt regarding, the nature of one’s being and existence. This concept emphasizes the individual’s sense of purpose and values, highlighting their subjective sense of goals and the significance they attach to their own existence (37).

Research indicates that a high level of control is effective in promoting positive self-evaluation, goal-seeking, and positive actions, thereby facilitates a meaningful life for an individual (38). Conversely, low levels of control may lead individuals to develop negative emotions and withdrawal behaviors, and even to develop learned helplessness, which undermines the experience of meaning in life (39). That is, self-control is associated with successful progress toward goals and involves the ability to organize and structure one’s life in a way that may imbue it with coherence, comprehensibility and order. The benefits of self-control may extend beyond general life success and happiness, making a salient contribution to the meaningfulness of one’s life (40). Individuals who experience a greater sense of meaning in life tend to view their existence as meaningful and exhibit stronger self-control in managing undesirable emotions and behaviors (41). On the other hand, a prolonged absence of meaning in life might give rise to impulsive ideas taking momentary control, potentially leading to detrimental outcomes, such as addiction and criminal behavior (42).

Liu et al. (43) suggest that PIU arises as a consequence of unmet psychological needs in individuals, leading them to seek compensation through excessive internet use. Individuals who experience long-term deficits in their meaning in life often find their psychological needs unfulfilled in reality. As a result, they are more likely to engage in excessive internet use as a “pathological compensation,” ultimately leading to PIU (24). Students with a lower sense of meaning in life are more vulnerable to the impact of stress (44). Therefore, they may resort to addictive behaviors as a coping mechanism to escape from stress (25), such as PIU (16), alcohol consumption, smoking, and substance abuse (4547). Research on mindfulness interventions based on existential therapy has also found that students who experience meaning in life through mindfulness meditation can reduce the severity of their PIU (48). Furthermore, excessive internet use has been found to have a detrimental effect on an individual’s sense of meaning in life (49). A bidirectional relationship may exist between meaning in life and PIU. Individuals with a low sense of meaning in life may seek refuge in the online world as a means of escaping the pressures of reality, which can lead to the development of PIU. Conversely, individuals who are extensively addicted to the internet may encounter difficulties in achieving personal accomplishments and fulfillment in real-life activities. Consequently, when detached from the virtual realm, they may experience a sense of inner emptiness and a lack of meaning in life. This, in turn, intensifies their PIU, giving rise to increasingly severe psychological problems (50).

Drawing on the basic psychological needs theory, prior research indicates that internet self-control fulfills core autonomy and competence demands, thereby enhancing one’s perceptions of meaning in life (51, 52). In turn, the sense of meaningfulness, arising from relatedness needs being met, serves to mitigate risks of PIU (53, 54). In addition, increased PIU can diminish meaning in life, further weakening internet self-control. Therefore, we hypothesized that meaning in life may act as a mediator in the relationship between internet self-control and PIU.

1.3 The present study

Based on the aforementioned literature, our study explores the relationship between self-control and PIU in the online environment. Taking a positive psychology perspective, our study aims to identify effective strategies for the prevention and intervention of PIU among Chinese adolescents. Therefore, our initial step involves utilizing a cross-lagged panel model with a 5-month time lag to examine the relationship between internet self-control and PIU. Subsequently, we constructed separate mediation models, with internet self-control and PIU as the independent variables, to explore the longitudinal relationships between the two constructs and the role of meaning in life as a mediator. Our first objective is to examine the relationship between internet self-control and PIU using a cross-lagged panel model with a 5-month time lag. Subsequently, we developed a mediation model to examine the longitudinal relationship between internet self-control and PIU, with a specific emphasis on the mediating role of meaning in life. Our second objective is to identify effective intervention strategies that can promote positive outcomes. To guide our study, we put forth two hypotheses:

H1: Internet self-control and PIU have a negative bidirectional predictive relationship.

H1a: Internet self-control at T1 negatively predicts PIU at T2.

H1b: PIU at T1 negatively predicts internet self-control at T2.

H2: Meaning in life mediates the relationship between internet self-control and PIU.

H2a: Meaning in life mediates the negative predictive effect of internet self-control at T1 on PIU at T2.

H2b: Meaning in life mediates the negative predictive effect of PIU at T1 on internet self-control at T2.

2 Materials and methods

2.1 Procedure and participants

This study has received approval from the Research Ethics Committee of Central China Normal University. Participants were recruited from two schools in the central region of China. All participants, along with their parents and teachers, were informed about the purpose of the study and signed informed consent forms. Participants were also informed that they had the right to withdraw from the study at any time without facing any negative consequences. After each measurement, participants received a monetary compensation of ¥5.

Employing a cluster sampling methodology, the current study employed classrooms as the sampling units. Prior studies employing cross-lagged investigation methodologies have recommended a temporal span of 4–6 months between assessments. In alignment with these findings and in consideration of the practical aspects of the assessment environment within the project context, this study has chosen to implement a 5-month interval between two consecutive rounds of assessments (55, 56). At the baseline (T1), a total of 850 questionnaires were administered, out of which 790 were completed, resulting in a response rate of 92.94%. For the follow-up assessment (T2), conducted 5 months later, 800 questionnaires were distributed, and 713 surveys were returned, yielding a response rate of 89.13%. No interventions took place between the two tests. After removing invalid responses, the final longitudinal sample consisted of 659 adolescents, including 331 males (50.23%) and 328 females (49.77%). Among them, 205 (31.11%) were in seventh grade, 187 (28.38%) were in eighth grade, 135 (20.49%) were in ninth grade, and 132 (20.03%) were in 10th grade. The average age of the participants was 13.61 years (SD = 1.409).

2.2 Measurement

2.2.1 Internet self-control

Internet self-control was assessed in this study using a subscale from Adolescent Internet Adaptability Scale of Wang et al. (57). This subscale comprises five items, with an example item being “I will arrange my time on the Internet in a planned way.” Participants rated their agreement with each item on a six-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 6 (strongly agree). The Internet self-control subscale demonstrated strong internal consistency (T1α = 0.784, T2α = 0.783). The average score was then calculated for data analysis.

2.2.2 Meaning in life

Meaning in life was evaluated using the 10-item Meaning in Life Questionnaire (58), adapted from the original scale developed by Steger (37). The Meaning in life questionnaire utilized a seven-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 (strongly disagree) to 7 (strongly agree). It consisted of two dimensions: presence of meaning and searching for meaning. An example item from the scale is “I am searching for the meaning of my life.” Higher scores on the scale indicated higher levels meaning in life. The Meaning in life questionnaire demonstrated strong internal consistency (T1α = 0.841, T2α = 0.872). The average score of Meaning in life was used for further analysis.

2.2.3 Problematic internet use

Problematic internet use was assessed using the Chinese version of the self-rating Diagnostic Questionnaire of PIU questionnaire (C-IAD) (59), adapted from the PIU Disorder questionnaire developed by Young (5). The C-IAD utilizes a five-point Likert scale (1 = not at all, 5 = always) to measure the severity of PIU. An example item includes “Do you stay online longer than originally intended?” Higher scores on the scale indicate higher levels of PIU. This scale has also demonstrated strong internal consistency (T1α = 0.882, T2α = 0.918). The average score was used for data analysis.

2.3 Data analysis

The data entry process was conducted using Epi-data, and subsequent statistical analyses were performed after thorough error checking. Firstly, Welch’s t-tests in SPSS 25.0 were employed to compare the follow-up and attrition samples, with the aim of determining whether any significant differences existed between the two groups. Pearson’s correlation coefficient was then utilized to explore the associations and detect correlations between the main variables. Prior to conducting the mediation analyses, the data were checked to ensure they met the assumptions for mediation analysis. These include: linear relationships between variables, uncorrelated residuals, no multicollinearity, and univariate and multivariate normal distributions of the variables. Subsequently, a cross-lagged panel model was developed using Mplus 8.0 to investigate the predictive relationship between Internet self-control and PIU. Lastly, a longitudinal mediation model was constructed using Mplus 8.0 to examine the potential mediating role of meaning in life in the relationship between internet self-control and PIU.

3 Results

3.1 Attrition rate analysis

Welch’s t-test was employed to compare differences in the main variables between the follow-up sample and the attrition sample at T1 (60). The results, as presented in Table 1, indicated no significant differences between the two groups of participants on all key variables (p > 0.05). This indicated that the data were missing at random.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparison of differences between the tracking and attrition samples (M ± SD).

3.2 Correlation analysis

Table 2 presents the descriptive statistics and analysis of adolescent internet self-control, meaning in life, and PIU. Pearson correlations were conducted to examine the associations between these variables at baseline (T1) and the 5-month follow-up (T2). The results revealed significant negative correlations between internet self-control at both time points and PIU at T1 and T2. Additionally, significant positive correlations were found between internet self-control and meaning in life at T1 and T2. Furthermore, meaning in life at T1 and T2 exhibited significant negative correlations with PIU at T1 and T2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and correlations among variables.

3.3 Cross-lagged panel analysis of internet self-control and PIU

Before the structural equation model analysis, we conducted a regression analysis. In this analysis, T1 PIU was set as the dependent variable, with T1 Internet self-control and T1 meaning in life as the independent variables. The results indicated a Variance Inflation Factor (VIF) of less than 10, suggesting that the variables exhibited linear relationships and met the assumptions of normality, homoscedasticity, and the absence of multicollinearity (see Supplementary Material sections).

To examine the predictive relationships between internet self-control and PIU (H1a and H1b), a cross-lagged panel modeling was conducted using Mplus 8.0 (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Cross-lagged panel model of internet self-control and PIU. **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

After controlling for baseline (T1) PIU, T1 internet self-control exhibited a significant negative prediction on PIU at the 5-month follow-up (T2) (β = −0.094, p < 0.01). Similarly, after controlling for T1 internet self-control, T1 PIU significantly negatively predicted T2 internet self-control (β = −0.099, p < 0.05). These results suggest a reciprocal predictive relationship between PIU and self-control. Thus, H1a and H1b are supported.

3.4 Longitudinal mediation model analysis of meaning in life

Cross-lagged panel modeling verified the bidirectional predictive relationship between internet self-control and PIU. To examine the mediating role of meaning in life, we conducted a Mediation Analysis using Mplus 8.0 and constructed two models. We tested the structural model to examine the proposed associations among variables. Indirect effects were calculated using bias-corrected bootstrapping (5,000 bootstrap samples) with 95% confidence intervals (CIs). In Mediation Analysis Model 1, we tested H2a with internet self-control as the independent variable and PIU as the dependent variable. In Mediation Analysis Model 2, we reversed the variables and tested H2b, with PIU as the independent variable and internet self-control as the dependent variable.

3.4.1 Mediation analysis model 1: longitudinal effects of internet self-control on problematic internet use: the mediating role of meaning in life

To test H2a, a mediation analysis was conducted to examine the longitudinal effects of internet self-control on PIU and the potential mediating role of meaning in life (see Figure 2: Mediation Analysis Model 1). The analysis provided satisfactory support for this hypothesis.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Longitudinal effects of internet self-control on PIU: the mediating role of meaning in life. **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

Results revealed that baseline (T1) internet self-control and T1 meaning in life significantly predicted meaning in life at the five-month follow-up (T2) (β = 0.142, p < 0.01; β = 0.477, p < 0.01). Furthermore, T1 meaning in life and T1 PIU significantly predicted T2 PIU (β = −0.075, p < 0.01; β = 0.547, p < 0.01). These findings suggest that higher levels of internet self-control are associated with higher levels of meaning in life, while higher levels of meaning in life are associated with lower levels of PIU.

When controlling for T1 meaning in life (as a covariate), T1 internet self-control continued to be a significant predictor of T2 meaning in life (β = 0.142, p < 0.01). Similarly, when controlling for T1 PIU, T1 meaning in life remained a significant predictor of T2 PIU (β = −0.075, p < 0.05). Model fit was assessed using established criteria of CFI and TLI values ≥0.9, RMSEA values ≤0.8, and SRMR values <0.05 for an acceptable fit (61). The model demonstrated strong fit (CFI = 0.985, TLI = 0.948, RMSEA = 0.075, SRMR = 0.023). In summary, the results suggest that meaning in life serves as a longitudinal mediator in the longitudinal effects of internet self-control on PIU. Thus, H2a was supported.

3.4.2 Mediation analysis model 2: longitudinal effects of problematic internet use on internet self-control: the mediating role of meaning in life

To test H2b, mediation analysis was also conducted to examine the longitudinal effects of PIU on self-control, and the potential mediating role of meaning in life (see Figure 3: Mediation Analysis Model 2). However, this model demonstrated poor fit with the data and failed to provide satisfactory support for this hypothesis.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mediating analysis model 2: Longitudinal effects of PIU on internet self-control: the mediating role of meaning in life. **p < 0.01, *p < 0.05.

The results revealed that baseline (T1) PIU and T1 meaning in life significantly predicted meaning in life at the 5-month follow-up (T2) (β = −0.080, p < 0.05; β = 0.498, p < 0.01). Additionally, T1 meaning in life and T1 internet self-control significantly predicted T2 internet self-control (β = 0.224, p < 0.01; β = 0.265, p < 0.01). These findings suggest that higher levels of initial PIU are associated with lower levels of subsequent meaning in life, while higher levels of baseline meaning in life are linked to improved follow-up internet self-control.

When controlling for T1 meaning in life, T1 PIU still significantly predicted T2 meaning in life (β = −0.080, p < 0.05). Similarly, when controlling for T1 internet self-control, T1 meaning in life remained a significant predictor of T2 internet self-control (β = 0.224, p < 0.01). However, Model 2 demonstrated poor fit based on recommended criteria (61) (CFI = 0.963, TLI = 0.871, RMSEA = 0.110, SRMR = 0.035), with TLI < 0.9 and RMSEA > 0.8. In summary, meaning in life did not mediate the longitudinal effects of PIU on internet self-control. Thus, H2b was not supported.

The results of the two mediation analyses (Mediation Analysis Model 1 and 2) indicated that meaning in life mediated the longitudinal effect of internet self-control on PIU but did not mediate the longitudinal effect of PIU on internet self-control. Therefore, H2a is supported, while H2b is not supported.

4 Discussion

Adopting a positive psychology perspective, this two-wave longitudinal study aimed to elucidate the relationship and underlying mechanisms between internet self-control and PIU. Using cross-lagged panel models, the reciprocal predictive relationships between internet self-control and PIU were examined. Through longitudinal mediation models, the research elucidated the longitudinal mediating role of meaning in life within this association. This approach addressed limitations of cross-sectional studies and offered insights into the bidirectional relationships between self-control and PIU over time and broadening the research perspective on PIU beyond static assessments.

4.1 Bidirectional effects between self-control and PIU

The cross-lagged panel analysis demonstrated a reciprocal prediction relationship between internet self-control and PIU across timepoints.

Specifically, the results of the cross-lagged panel analysis indicated that baseline (T1) internet self-control significantly negatively predicted follow-up (T2) PIU, providing support for research H1a. This finding aligns with the basic psychological needs theory, which suggests that individuals with high levels of internet self-control are more capable of autonomously managing their online behaviors (27). They can effectively control their internet usage time and activities to avoid excessive immersion. Conversely, those with low internet self-control, particularly inexperienced adolescents, are more susceptible to becoming addicted to the short-term gratification offered by the open, anonymous, and virtual nature of the online world. They tend to struggle to exert self-control and effectively regulate their intended usage, ultimately leading to PIU (29). In the same vain, previous research has shown that modifying specific cognitions and expectations related to internet use can enhance internet self-control and reduce levels of PIU (62).

Moreover, T1 PIU was found to significantly negatively predict T2 internet self-control, indicating that higher initial levels of PIU undermine subsequent self-control capabilities, supporting research H1b. This finding is consistent with previous studies, as one of the diagnostic criteria for PIU is “unsuccessful attempts to control” or “loss of control” (28, 63). This criterion is also commonly included in questionnaires assessing PIU (5). Consequently, individuals with higher scores of PIU often exhibit characteristics of impaired online control, such as an inability to regulate the time and frequency of their internet use and becoming deeply engrossed in the online world, finding it difficult to detach themselves.

Overall, the results reveal bidirectional effects, whereby internet self-control mitigates later PIU severity, while PIU in turn erodes self-control resources. This reciprocal causation highlights the important interplay between self-control capacities and problematic internet use. Building upon this insight, this study introduces meaning in life as a mediating variable to further explore the underlying mechanisms between these two constructs.

4.2 Meaning in life mediates internet self-control and PIU

Meaning in life is an important protective factor against PIU (64). However, the current body of research has not adequately addressed the role of meaning in life in the reciprocal relationship between internet self-control and PIU. In contributing to the literature, our study demonstrates that “meaning in life” serves as a crucial mediating factor between internet self-control and PIU over time. In other words, internet self-control influences adolescents’ PIU both directly and indirectly through meaning in life, supporting H2a.

This finding is consistent with the basic psychological needs theory proposed by Ryan and Deci (65). According to this theory, individuals with higher levels of internet self-control are more likely to experience a sense of autonomy and competence, enabling them to engage in meaningful activities and pursue personal goals. As a result, they develop a stronger sense of purpose and meaning in life. This increased meaning in life provides individuals with a sense of fulfillment in their everyday lives, reducing their inclination toward PIU.

Therefore, our results suggest that reducing an individual’s level of PIU can be achieved not only by directly improving internet self-control but also indirectly by enhancing their sense of meaning in life. These research findings highlight the significance of interventions in PIU that go beyond cultivating individuals’ internet self-control abilities. It is also important to focus on enhancing their meaning in life. By encouraging individuals to pursue fulfillment and achieve goals in real-life activities, it is possible to reduce their reliance on the internet and mitigate the risk of addiction.

Furthermore, our research revealed that meaning in life does not mediate the impact of PIU on internet self-control. Our second hypothesis (H2b) did not find empirical support. The impact of PIU on reducing self-control may occur through direct mechanisms (e.g., reduced inhibitory control or impaired cognitive functioning) (32). Therefore, PIU may directly undermine self-control abilities without being mediated by meaning in life. Moreover, unmeasured factors like depression, anxiety, stress, or social support might mediate the relationship between PIU and self-control. Therefore, the null finding in our study highlights the complexity of the relationships between PIU, self-control, and meaning in life, suggesting the possibility of other moderating factors or bidirectional relationships.

4.3 Limitations and future research directions

While this was grounded in the perspective of positive psychology and employed a longitudinal research design, offering insights into the underlying mechanisms of the internal role of internet self-control on PIU, it is not without limitations.

Firstly, the reciprocal predictive relationship between internet self-control and PIU was examined using longitudinal data, which has inherent constraints. For instance, while this study examined mediation effects using a 5-month interval, which is informed by the established literature, future research is recommended to conduct measurements over varied lag periods (e.g., 3, 6, and 9 months, etc.) to validate our results and gain nuanced insights into how these relationships evolve over time. Secondly, we opted for path analysis over structural equation modeling (SEM) in our study due to our specific research design and sample size. Future studies could consider employing SEM for a more nuanced understanding of the relationships between variables we examined in this study. Lastly, the study primarily focused on a sample of Chinese middle and high school students, with a relatively small sample size. Future research should aim to expand the sample size to encompass diverse populations in different regions.

4.4 Significance

Our study addresses the ongoing debates surrounding self-control and PIU in the online context and holds significant implications. Theoretically, investigating the longitudinal relationship between internet self-control and PIU helps reveal the nature of their relationship and underlying mechanisms. It enriches the existing research on PIU from a positive psychology perspective. Moreover, while meaning in life did not mediate the effect of PIU on self-control in this study, further research is warranted to understand the mechanisms linking PIU and self-control. The null finding provides opportunities to develop more nuanced models of these relationships in future studies.

From a practical standpoint, our research provides empirical evidence and data support for interventions targeting PIU. Specifically, it highlights the importance of internet self-control and meaning in life in fostering healthy internet use habits among adolescents. Firstly, internet self-control directly influences adolescents’ internet usage behavior. Adolescents with higher levels of internet self-control are better able to regulate their online time and behaviors, resist online temptations, and maintain moderate usage. This helps prevent excessive immersion on the internet and reduces the risk of PIU. Secondly, meaning in life plays a mediating role between adolescents’ internet self-control and PIU. For adolescents with lower levels of internet self-control, enhancing their meaning in life can guide them in contemplating and exploring their life goals, values, and interests. It helps them recognize the role of the internet in achieving these goals, enabling them to use the internet purposefully and avoid the risks of PIU.

Therefore, by delving into the mechanisms through which internet self-control and meaning in life impact adolescent PIU, we can develop targeted intervention strategies and methods. This offers support to adolescents already ensnared by PIU.

5 Conclusion

Utilizing a two-wave longitudinal design and a sample of Chinese adolescents, this study revealed the reciprocal predictive relationships between internet self-control and PIU through cross-lagged modeling. Specifically, internet self-control at baseline predicted subsequent PIU, and baseline PIU also predicted subsequent internet self-control. The longitudinal mediation model demonstrated that meaning in life mediated the longitudinal effect of internet self-control on PIU, but not the effect of PIU on self-control. These findings suggest the feasibility of reducing adolescent PIU levels directly or indirectly by improving internet self-control and meaning in life. The study provides empirical evidence and potential pathways for PIU interventions from a positive psychology perspective.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Research Ethics Committee of Central China Normal University. The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin.

Author contributions

WW: Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. JY: Data curation, Formal analysis, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. YZ: Formal analysis, Visualization, Writing – original draft. DH: Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing. XZ: Funding acquisition, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article. This research was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China under the “Research on adolescent internetadaptation-oriented optimization method for personalized information service (NSFC Grant No. 71974072).”

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1258673/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Compiled by National Bureau of Statistics of China (2021). China statistical yearbook 2021.

Google Scholar

2. CYLC, CNNIC (2021). Study on internet use by minors in China in 2021. Available at: https://qnzz.youth.cn/qckc/202211/t20221130_14166885.htm

Google Scholar

3. Billieux, J , Schimmenti, A , Khazaal, Y , Maurage, P , and Heeren, A . Are we overpathologizing everyday life? A tenable blueprint for behavioral addiction research. J Behav Addict. (2015) 4:119–23. doi: 10.1556/2006.4.2015.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kalaitzaki, AE , and Birtchnell, J . The impact of early parenting bonding on young adults’ internet addiction, through the mediation effects of negative relating to others and sadness. Addict Behav. (2014) 39:733–6. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Young, KS . Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychol Behav. (1998) 1:237–44. doi: 10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Ciarrochi, J , Parker, P , Sahdra, B , Marshall, S , Jackson, C , Gloster, AT, et al. The development of compulsive internet use and mental health: a four-year study of adolescence. Dev Psychol. (2016) 52:272–83. doi: 10.1037/dev0000070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Winkler, A , Dörsing, B , Rief, W , Shen, Y , and Glombiewski, JA . Treatment of internet addiction: a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2013) 33:317–29. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.12.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Spada, MM . An overview of problematic internet use. Addict Behav. (2014) 39:3–6. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2013.09.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Lam, LT . Risk factors of internet addiction and the health effect of internet addiction on adolescents: a systematic review of longitudinal and prospective studies. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2014) 16:508. doi: 10.1007/s11920-014-0508-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Fumero, A , Marrero, RJ , Voltes, D , and Peñate, W . Personal and social factors involved in internet addiction among adolescents: a meta-analysis. Comput Hum Behav. (2018) 86:387–400. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Ye, XL , Zhang, W , and Zhao, FF . Depression and internet addiction among adolescents: a meta-analysis. Psychiatry Res. (2023) 326:115311. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2023.115311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Tian, L , Zhang, W , and Chen, G . Effects of parental support, friendship quality on loneliness and depression: to test an indirect effect model. Acta Psychol Sin. (2014) 46:238. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2014.00238

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Zhang, Y , Qin, X , and Ren, P . Adolescents’ academic engagement mediates the association between internet addiction and academic achievement: the moderating effect of classroom achievement norm. Comput Hum Behav. (2018) 89:299–307. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2018.08.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Gao, M , Teng, Z , Wei, Z , Jin, K , Xiao, J , Tang, H, et al. Internet addiction among teenagers in a Chinese population: prevalence, risk factors, and its relationship with obsessive-compulsive symptoms. J Psychiatr Res. (2022) 153:134–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2022.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Diotaiuti, P , Mancone, S , Corrado, S , De Risio, A , Cavicchiolo, E , Girelli, L, et al. Internet addiction in young adults: the role of impulsivity and codependency. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:893861. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.893861

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Zhang, Y , Mei, S , Li, L , Chai, J , Li, J , and Du, H . The relationship between impulsivity and internet addiction in Chinese college students: a moderated mediation analysis of meaning in life and self-esteem. Weinstein AM, editor. PLoS One. (2015) 10:e0131597. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0131597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Du, Z , and Zhang, X . Analysis of the mediating effects of self-efficacy and self-control between physical activity and internet addiction among Chinese college students. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:1002830. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1002830

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Iftikhar, M , and Tariq, S . Self-control, narcissistic tendencies and internet addiction among adolescents. J Arts Soc Sci. (2014) 1:37–52.

Google Scholar

19. Li, J , Chen, Y , Lu, J , Li, W , and Yu, C . Self-control, consideration of future consequences, and internet addiction among Chinese adolescents: the moderating effect of deviant peer affiliation. IJERPH. (2021) 18:9026. doi: 10.3390/ijerph18179026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Li, S , Ren, P , Chiu, MM , Wang, C , and Lei, H . The relationship between self-control and internet addiction among students: a meta-analysis. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:735755. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.735755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Agbaria, Q . Internet addiction and aggression: the mediating roles of self-control and positive affect. Int J Ment Heal Addict. (2021) 19:1227–42. doi: 10.1007/s11469-019-00220-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ryan, RM , and Deci, EL . Self-Determination Theory: Basic Psychological Needs in Motivation, Development, and Wellness, vol. xii. New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press (2017). 756 p.

Google Scholar

23. Wu, AMS , Chen, JH , Tong, KK , Yu, S , and Lau, JTF . Prevalence and associated factors of internet gaming disorder among community dwelling adults in Macao. Chin J Behav Addict. (2018) 7:62–9. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Ghaderi Rammazi, M , Askarizadeh, G , Ahmadi, G , and Divsalar, K . The role of psychiatric symptoms, social support and meaning in life in predicting internet addiction among university students: a causal model. Pract Clin Psychol. (2018) 6:101–10. doi: 10.29252/nirp.jpcp.6.2.101

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kardefelt-Winther, D . A conceptual and methodological critique of internet addiction research: towards a model of compensatory internet use. Comput Hum Behav. (2014) 31:351–4. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Mei, S , Chai, J , and Guo, J . Subjective well-being and internet addiction of adolescents: mediating roles of self-esteem and self-control. Psychol Dev Educ. (2015) 31:603–9. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2015.05.12

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wang, W , Dong, R , Niu, G , and Zhou, Z . Internet adaptation: concept and model. J Nanchang Univ. (2021) 52:45–54. doi: 10.13764/j.cnki.ncds.2021.02.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Song, WJ , and Park, JW . The influence of stress on internet addiction: mediating effects of self-control and mindfulness. Int J Ment Heal Addict. (2019) 17:1063–75. doi: 10.1007/s11469-019-0051-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Zhou, E , and Zhou, H . An empirical study of college students’ subjective well-being self-control and internet addiction. J Univ Chin Acad Soc Sci. (2017) 5:17–24.

Google Scholar

30. Chen, X , and Jiang, Y . Intervention in adolescent lnternet addiction from the perspective of embodied cognition: return to the integration of mind and body. Henan Soc Sci. (2023) 31:107–15.

Google Scholar

31. Rigby, CS , and Ryan, RM . “Time well-spent? Motivation for entertainment media and its eudaimonic aspects through the lens of self-determination theory,” in The Routledge handbook of media use and well-being: International perspectives on theory and research on positive media effects. (Eds.) Reinecke L, Oliver MB New York, NY, US: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group (2017). 34–48.

Google Scholar

32. Teng, Z , Li, Y , and Liu, Y . Online gaming, internet addiction, and aggression in Chinese male students: the mediating role of low self-control. IJPS. (2014) 6:p89. doi: 10.5539/ijps.v6n2p89

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Ko, CH , Hsiao, S , Liu, GC , Yen, JY , Yang, MJ , and Yen, CF . The characteristics of decision making, potential to take risks, and personality of college students with internet addiction. Psychiatry Res. (2010) 175:121–5. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Reed, P , Osborne, LA , Romano, M , and Truzoli, R . Higher impulsivity after exposure to the internet for individuals with high but not low levels of self-reported problematic internet behaviours. Comput Hum Behav. (2015) 49:512–6. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.03.064

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Deci, EL , and Ryan, RM . Self-determination theory: a macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Can Psychol. (2008) 49:182–5. doi: 10.1037/a0012801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Eakman, AM . Relationships between meaningful activity, basic psychological needs, and meaning in life: test of the meaningful activity and life meaning model. OTJR Occup Particip Health. (2013) 33:100–9. doi: 10.3928/15394492-20130222-02

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Steger, MF , Frazier, P , Oishi, S , and Kaler, M . The meaning in life questionnaire: assessing the presence of and search for meaning in life. J Couns Psychol. (2006) 53:80–93. doi: 10.1037/0022-0167.53.1.80

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hagger, MS . The multiple pathways by which self-control predicts behavior. Front Psychol. (2013) 4:849. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00849

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Cui, W , and Wan, M . A review of theories of control. J Psychol Sci. (2007) 1:235–7. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2007.01.061

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Stavrova, O , Pronk, T , and Kokkoris, MD . Finding meaning in self-control: the effect of self-control on the perception of meaning in life. Self Identity. (2020) 19:201–18. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2018.1558107

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Liu, Y , Di, S , Shi, Y , and Ma, C . Meaning in life and adolescent self-control: effect of perceived social support and its gender differences. Front Psychol. (2022) 13:1087668. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.1087668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Vötter, B , and Schnell, T . Bringing giftedness to bear: generativity, meaningfulness, and self-control as resources for a happy life among gifted adults. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:1972. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01972

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Liu, QX , Fang, XY , Wan, JJ , and Zhou, ZK . Need satisfaction and adolescent pathological internet use: comparison of satisfaction perceived online and offline. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 55:695–700. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.09.048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Czekierda, K , Banik, A , Park, CL , and Luszczynska, A . Meaning in life and physical health: systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. (2017) 11:387–418. doi: 10.1080/17437199.2017.1327325

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Csabonyi, M , and Phillips, LJ . Meaning in life and substance use. J Humanist Psychol. (2020) 60:3–19. doi: 10.1177/0022167816687674

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Konkolÿ Thege, B , Bachner, YG , Kushnir, T , and Kopp, MS . Relationship between meaning in life and smoking status: results of a national representative survey. Addict Behav. (2009) 34:117–20. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Sliedrecht, W , Seesink, HJ , Vrijmoeth, C , de Waart, R , Wiers, RW , Ostafin, B, et al. Alcohol use disorder relapse factors: an exploratory investigation of craving, alcohol dependence severity, and meaning in life. Addict Res Theory. (2022) 30:351–9. doi: 10.1080/16066359.2022.2040488

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Liu, X , Jiang, J , and Zhang, Y . Effects of Logotherapy-based mindfulness intervention on internet addiction among adolescents during the COVID-19 pandemic. Iran J Public Health. (2021) 50:789–97. doi: 10.18502/ijph.v50i4.6005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Ge, Y , Deng, L , and Ji, L . The relationship between left-at-home internet-addicted urban Children’s personality traits, internet self-efficacy and sense of meaning in life. Chin J Spec Educ. (2018) 2:89–96.

Google Scholar

50. Cao, R , Mei, S , Liang, L , Li, C , and Zhang, Y . Relationship between gratitude and internet addiction among college students: the mediating role of Core self-evaluation and meaning in life. Psychol Dev Educ. (2023) 39:286–94. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2023.02.15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Billieux, J , Maurage, P , Lopez-Fernandez, O , Kuss, DJ , and Griffiths, MD . Can disordered mobile phone use be considered a behavioral addiction? An update on current evidence and a comprehensive model for future research. Curr Addict Rep. (2015) 2:156–62. doi: 10.1007/s40429-015-0054-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Steger, MF , Kashdan, TB , Sullivan, BA , and Lorentz, D . Understanding the search for meaning in life: personality, cognitive style, and the dynamic between seeking and experiencing meaning. J Pers. (2008) 76:199–228. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2007.00484.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lambert, NM , Stillman, TF , Hicks, JA , Kamble, S , Baumeister, RF , and Fincham, FD . To belong is to matter: sense of belonging enhances meaning in life. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. (2013) 39:1418–27. doi: 10.1177/0146167213499186

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Shek, DT , and Yu, L . Adolescent internet addiction in Hong Kong: prevalence, change, and correlates. J Pediatr Adolesc Gynecol. (2016) 29:S22–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jpag.2015.10.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Chen, Z , Feng, Y , Song, W , and Liu, X . The relationship of parent-child separation, peer victimization and depression in adolescents: a longitudinal study. Psychol Dev Educ. (2021) 37:429–38. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2021.03.14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Li, X , Wu, L , and Lv, H . Relationships between self-esteem, future time perspective, and academic performance: a longitudinal study. Chinese. J Appl Psychol. (2021) 27:244–61.

Google Scholar

57. Wang, W , Ye, J , Huang, D , and Dong, R . Development and Reliability Testing of the Adolescent Internet Adaptation Questionnaire. Wuhan: The Seventh Annual Conference on Quality Monitoring and Evaluation of Basic Education in China and the Forum for Master and Doctoral Students. (2022).

Google Scholar

58. Chen, W , Ge, Y , Hu, Y , and Zhang, J . Applicability and generalizability of the revised meaning in life questionnaire: based on classical test theory and multidimensional Rasch model. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2015) 23:604–14. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2015.04.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Li, Y , Zhong, B , Liu, X , Zhang, Y , Zhu, J , and Hao, W . Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of self-rating young’s diagnostic questionnaire of internet addiction: a preliminary study. Chin J Drug Depend. (2012) 21:390–4. doi: 10.13936/j.cnki.cjdd1992.2012.05.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Delacre, M , Lakens, D , and Leys, C . Why psychologists should by default use Welch’s t-test instead of Student’s t-test. Int Rev Soc Psychol. (2017) 30:92–101. doi: 10.5334/irsp.82

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Zheng, W , and Wu, W . Evaluation of structural equation modeling fitting: the overall fitting, the internal fitting and cross-validation testing. Psychol Explor. (2014) 34:57–61.

Google Scholar

62. Brand, M , Young, KS , and Laier, C . Prefrontal control and internet addiction: a theoretical model and review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:375. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00375

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Tao, R , Huang, X , Wang, J , Liu, C , Zhang, H , Xiao, L, et al. A proposed criterion for clinical diagnosis of internet addiction. Med J Chin People’s Liberat Army. (2008) 10:1188–91.

Google Scholar

64. Halusic, M , and King, LA . What Makes Life Meaningful: Positive Mood Works in a Pinch American Psychological Association (2013) 445–64. doi: 10.1037/14040-022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Ryan, RM , and Deci, EL . “Overview of self-determination theory: an organismic dialectical perspective,” in Handbook of self-determination research. University of Rochester Press, (Eds.) Deci EL, Ryan RM vol. 2 (2002). 3–33.

Google Scholar

Keywords: problematic internet use, internet self-control, meaning in life, cross-lagged panel model, longitudinal mediation model, adolescents

Citation: Wang W, Ye J, Zhu Y, Huang D and Zhao X (2023) Longitudinal relationship between internet self-control and problematic internet use among Chinese adolescents: mediating role of meaning in life. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1258673. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1258673

Received: 14 July 2023; Accepted: 17 November 2023;
Published: 07 December 2023.

Edited by:

Masum Öztürk, Manisa Celal Bayar University, Türkiye

Reviewed by:

Mohammad Seydavi, Kharazmi University, Iran
Xinyi Huang, Durham University, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2023 Wang, Ye, Zhu, Huang and Zhao. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jianmei Ye, moyerye@outlook.com; Xin Zhao, Xin.Zhao@sheffield.ac.uk

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.