Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 18 September 2023
Sec. Autism

Emotional dysregulation as a part of the autism spectrum continuum: a literature review from late childhood to adulthood

Liliana Dell&#x;OssoLiliana Dell’OssoLeonardo MassoniLeonardo MassoniSimone BattagliniSimone BattagliniChiara De FeliceChiara De FeliceBenedetta NardiBenedetta NardiGiulia AmatoriGiulia AmatoriIvan Mirko CremoneIvan Mirko CremoneBarbara Carpita
Barbara Carpita*
  • Department of Clinical and Experimental Medicine, University of Pisa, Pisa, Italy

The concept of emotional dysregulation (ED) has recently gained interest in the scientific literature and is commonly defined as the inability to use the modulatory mechanisms involved in emotion regulation, resulting in a functioning meaningfully below the baseline. Even though the data available are still limited, an increasing number of studies have hypothesized a promoting role for some of the core features of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) in the development of ED, in particular being repetitive behaviors, social difficulties and alexythimia. In this framework, the purpose of this study was to review the literature that is currently available about presence and correlates of ED in young adults with autism spectrum conditions as well as to offer some insights about possible implications for illness trajectories. The data reported seems to point to a shared etiology between ED and repetitive/restricted ASD symptoms, with perseveration features serving as the foundation for the inability to control one’s emotions. In this context, a neurodevelopmental basis for ED could be consistent with the transnosographic conceptualization of ASD, which hypothesizes a potential neurodevelopmental basis for several psychiatric disorders, whose autistic traits would be the phenotypical presentation.

1. Introduction

Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) is a heterogeneous neurodevelopmental disorder that can manifest with varying degrees of symptom severity, with or without intellectual impairment, and is characterized by a pervasive impairment in social and reciprocal communication and interactions, restricted and repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, or activities, a lack of socio-emotional reciprocity, and impaired sensory integration processing (1). Difficulties in socio-cognition are considered among the core symptoms of ASD, being at the basis of the deficits in social communication and interaction. In this framework, some authors also suggested that ASD deficits in affective empathy could be specific for the negative emotional valence while, on the other hand, subjects may still be able to empathize with the emotional experience of others when they express emotions with a positive valence (2, 3). Researches focusing on ASD have recently emphasized the need to examine not only the severe clinical forms but also the milder and sub-clinical manifestations, referred to as autistic traits (AT) which appear to be distributed along a continuum from the general to the clinical population (4, 5), and particularly represented in specific population such as students of scientific courses or psychiatric patients with other kinds of disorders (614). These traits can be classified in different dimensions, although research suggests that different dimensions may be associated to different outcomes (15, 16). In particular, subthreshold autistic features are of interest because they appear to have a negative impact on quality of life and to represent a major risk factor for the emergence of various psychiatric disorders, as well as suicidal thoughts and behaviors (1719). The need for an early identification and intervention is therefore evident, since high intensity interventions have been reported to be able to reduce the majority of the main ASD symptoms (20). While in the psychiatric literature a growing interest has been paid to the concept of Emotional Dysregulation (ED), to date is still quite difficult to reach an agreement on ED definition. Despite the fact that the importance of “emotional problems” in clinical circumstances has led to an active search for causes, in which ED occupies a major place (21, 22), a common description of ED is still lacking. In 1991, Thompson defined emotion regulation as “a set of processes by which any individual assesses, inhibits, maintains, or modifies the intensity, frequency, or duration of emotional reactions in order to have the appropriate social behavior or to achieve goals” (23), while the concept of ED was proposed only few years later. To this date, one of the most welcomed definitions comes from Bunford, that defined ED as “an individual’s inability to exercise any or all aspects of the modulatory processes involved in emotion regulation, to such a degree that the inability results in the individual functioning meaningfully below his or her baseline” (24). It is now common opinion that ED is characterized by three main alterations: the first is an abnormal and excessive emotional response in comparison to social norms; the second is uncontrollable and rapid fluctuations in emotions; and the third is an aberrant allocation of attention to emotional stimuli (25). Moreover, ED comprehends the inability to detect and accept emotions as well as a lack of ability to adaptively select efficient coping mechanisms (2628). According to an increasing number of studies, the core features of autism spectrum may be at the basis of the development of ED, the latter being particularly associated with repetitive behaviors, social difficulties and alexythimia as promoting factors (2931). In this framework, ED may be considered one of the steps of the illness trajectory that, from the presence of autistic traits, often lead to the development of mood disorders (3033). This issue is of particular importance considering the increased prevalence of autistic traits in different kinds of psychiatric conditions, to the point that some authors supported a possible neurodevelopmental approach to mental disorders, with autistic traits conceptualized as a trans-nosographic dimension (3437). According to this model the specific timing, localization and severity of neurodevelopmental alterations, interacting with environmental factors and life events, may shape specific illness trajectories in different subjects (3436).

Moreover, in recent years, there has been substantial attention in the relationship between ED and non-suicide self-injury, particularly considering that non suicidal self harm rates have been growing and they are considered a critical risk factor for suicidal behavior (38, 39). Two main models have been proposed as possible explanations for this link. The experiential avoidance model of deliberate self-harm, makes the case that non-suicide self-injury fulfills the function of avoidance or escape from undesirable emotional arousal (40). While, similarly, the emotional cascade model proposes a clear link between ED and dysregulated behaviors (such as self harming) suggesting that rumination, which is, on the other hand, often underlain by autistic traits, increases emotional experiences, causing people to engage in dysregulated activities as a temporary distraction from negative emotion (41).

Several different scales have been proposed to measure ED and are applicable in ASD individuals. Those include the Emotion Dysegulation Inventory (EDI), that consists of 66 items, the Aberrant Behavior Checklist (ABC), that takes into account irritability, lethargy/social withdrawal, stereotypic behavior, hyperactivity and inappropriate speech, and the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), that includes four domains: Anxious/Depressed (13 items), Withdrawn/Depressed (8 items), and Aggressive Behavior (18 items) (42). In this framework, many studies searched for the bio-neurological correlates of ED in ASD subjects, hypothesizing a role for some dopaminergic gene polymorphisms that may promote ED in children with ASD (43), and reporting a correlation between specific brain regions, including middle frontal gyrus (MFG), posterior insula (PI), amygdala and prefrontal cortex (PFC) and problem in emotional regulation in ASD subjects (30, 44).

In this framework, the aim of this work was to provide a narrative review of the available literature about presence and correlates of ED in adolescents and young adults with autism spectrum conditions, providing also some insights about possible implications for illness trajectories. For this purpose, articles were searched on two electronic databases (PubMed and Scopus) from January 26th to February 14th 2023. The following search terms, without filters, restriction or limits, were used to identify all potentially eligible records: (“emotional dysregulation” OR “affective dysregulation” OR “emotion dysregulation”) AND (“neurodevelopmental disorders” OR “autism” OR “ASD” OR “autism spectrum disorder”). Papers were screened separately by reviewers for inclusion and disagreements were resolved by discussion. Because we aimed to investigate the prevalence of ED in human subjects, studies investigating ED in animal models were excluded. Moreover, reviews, case reports and editorials were also excluded due to the nature of the articles.

2. ED in ASD: from adolescence to adulthood

2.1. Emotional regulation strategies

ED is reported to be a frequent feature of ASD that is often present from the earliest ages. A study conducted by Samson et al. (45) investigated a wide range of emotion regulation techniques in a sample of 32 ASD children and adolescents, diagnosed according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual (DSM) IV-TR criteria (F = 3, M = 29, age range = 8–20 years) and 31 typically developing (TD) participants (F = 8, M = 23, age range 8–20 years). Parents of both groups were interviewed about their child’s emotional experience and the use and efficacy of 10 ER strategies and answered the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), the CBCL and the Stanford Binet for evaluating cognitive function. Results highlighted that, when asked about their children’s behaviors, ASD parents reported a reduced use of adaptive strategies (problem solving, cognitive reappraisal) and increased of maladaptive ones (repetitive behaviors) in comparison to TD parents (45). The same authors also investigated emotional regulation in 21 ASD children and adolescents diagnosed through the DSM-IV criteria (M = 18, F = 3, age range = 8–20 years) and 22 matched TD controls (M = 16, F = 6, age range 8–20 years). The two groups were evaluated for cognitive functions through the Full Scale Intelligence Quotient (FSIQ), cognitive function was assessed using Stanford Binet and emotional reactivity and regulation in children and adolescents with and without ASD was assessed through the Reactivity and Regulation Situation Task (RRST). Additionally, TD subjects were screened via face-to-face evaluations, telephone interviews and observation during psychometric tests. According to the findings, individuals with ASD showed the same level of reactivity to negative stimuli as TD participants. Furthermore, ASD adolescents presented a different emotion regulation profile than TD individuals, characterized by a less frequent use of cognitive reappraisal and more frequent use of suppression. In addition, when instructed to use cognitive reappraisal, ASD participants showed difficulties to use it, but benefitted from this strategy when they were able to generate a reappraisal (46). Results from both studies prompted the authors to suggest that ED could be considered a core feature of ASD and that the development of treatment programs that focus on improving the use of adaptive forms of emotion regulation may reduce emotional difficulties and improve long-term results in children and adolescents with ASD.

Another study (29) compared a group of 56 ASD individuals (M = 47, F = 9, age range = 6–16 years) with 38 TD controls (M = 26, F = 12, age range = 6–16 years). Participants were assessed for ASD core features through: the Stanford Binet 5th edition (SB5), to determine cognitive functioning; the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd Edition (VABS-2) to measure adaptive behavioral functioning in the domains of Socialization, Communication, Daily Living Skills, Motor Skills, and an overall Adaptive Behavior Composite score; the SRS, to investigate Social Cognition, Social Motivation, Social Awareness, Social Communication, and Autism Mannerisms; the Repetitive Behavior Scale-Revised (RBS-R), to capture the domains of Stereotyped Behavior, Self-Injurious Behavior, Compulsive Behavior, Ritualistic Behavior, Sameness Behavior and Restricted Behavior; the Short Sensory Profile (SSP) to provide subscales that reflect atypical sensory processing in different domains (Tactile Sensitivity, Taste/Smell Sensitivity, Movement Sensitivity, Underresponsive/Seeks Sensation, Auditory Filtering, Low Energy/Weak, and Visual/Auditory Sensitivity) and a total score. The emotional deregulation index was provided through the CBCL, a parent-report measure assessing problem behaviors (e.g., aggressive behavior). Results reported that ED was related to all core features of the ASD, but interestingly the strongest association was with repetitive behaviors which appeared to be the best predictors of ED. This finding could imply that people with ASD who have significant repetitive and restricted symptoms are less able to regulate their emotions due to difficulties inhibiting ongoing behaviors. This finding is congruent with the findings of Mazefsky et al. (47), who proposed that perseveration (an example of a repetitive action) can lead to the development and maintenance of emotion control issues. However, another possibility is that ED in ASD activates compensatory control systems, which manifest as restricted and repetitive behaviors.

A further research studied the impact of different emotion regulation strategies among subjects with ASD (48). The author focused on reappraisal, an emotion regulation strategy based on re-interpreting a situation in order to change the way one feels about it. Another evaluated strategy was suppression, which features the inhibition of emotional expression. The authors enrolled 56 ASD adolescents and young adults (M = 39, F = 17, age range = 14–24 years), who were divided into 4 groups according to Emotion Regulation Questionnaire subscale scores. It was found that individuals in the high suppression and low reappraisal group showed higher depressive symptoms and reduced well-being compared with subjects in the low suppression and high reappraisal group. Meanwhile, those who reported to use both high suppression and reappraisal expressed relatively high positive well-being and low depression symptoms. It was speculated that the habitual use of reappraisal may buffer the effect of regular suppression usage (48).

2.2. Relationship between ED and ASD core features

A work by Neuhaus et al. (49) explored the role of social skills as a mediator of ED in ASD. A group of 2079 individuals (M = 1803, F = 276, age range = 72–216 months) were evaluated for ED, social functioning, and cognitive skills. The authors reported direct effects of social motivation, internalizing symptoms, aggression, attention problems, irritability, and self-injurious behavior on children’s social skills. In addition, ED moderated the association between social motivation and social skills. Globally, these results not only identified risks for poorer social and emotional outcomes in children and adolescents with ASD, but also suggest areas for intervention. Whereas the main effects of dysregulation on social skills indicate a clear need to intervene in emotion-related processes to facilitate social functioning, the moderating role of ED on social functioning suggests that such support may be beneficial even when dysregulation difficulties are not readily apparent (49).

A more recent study from Patel et al. (50), aimed to investigate the link between ED and other symptoms frequently related to ASD, such as anger rumination and depression. For this purpose, a sample of 25 ASD adolescents and 24 controls (age range 12–19 years) was recruited and they were all evaluated with the Anger Rumination Scale (ARS) to assess feelings of anger, the SRS and the Mood and Feelings Questionnaire (SMFQ) for depressive symptomatology. Results suggested that anger rumination was related to the presence of depression and general emotional and behavioral dysregulation in individuals with ASD (50). Taken together, these findings are in line with previous researches (47) thesis that the proclivity to persevere in ASD includes recurrent and persistent thoughts regarding negative emotional events, such as anger/frustration and depression symptoms. Moreover, the relationship between anger rumination and worse emotional and behavioral control in non-ASD samples is consistent with the emotional cascade model of behavioral dysregulation, which proposes that rumination enhances negative affect, which can then lead to dysregulated behaviors (51). Thus, treatment strategies to minimize ruminating in ASD may be beneficial in alleviating a variety of symptoms. In a pilot study of people with ASD, for example, a mindfulness intervention reduced both rumination and mood symptoms (52). If indicated, intervention addressing ruminating may be especially significant because those who ruminate tend to do so indefinitely (53).

When discussing the relationship between core symptoms of ASD and ED, one major goal has been the development of a useful as a psychometric tool, such as the Difficulties with Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS), a scale that contains 36 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (almost never) to 5 (almost always). A study on the psychometric properties of DERS, conducted by McVey et al. (54) analyzed datas from 4 different studies collecting a total of 156 ASD adolescents and adults. Inclusion criteria were 1) having item-level data on the DERS and 2) being classified as autistic in their respective studies. Results highlighted that DERS was positively associated with anxiety, depression and alexithymia, thus suggesting the usefulness to test DERS in autistic population (54).

2.3. ED and intolerance of uncertainty

Furthermore, a recent interest has been posed to the role of ED and intolerance of uncertainty (IU), described as the “inability to tolerate the unwanted cognitive, emotional and behavioral effects triggered by a perceived lack of information for a given situation” (55) in the ASD population. A study by Saez-Suanes et al. (56) shed new light on the role of ED and UI as mediators of anxiety in ASD. A sample of 121 individuals (M = 81, F = 40, age range 18–62 years), with a diagnosis of ASD and intellectual disability (ID), were evaluated to determine the predictive and mediating role of executive functioning, emotional regulation and IU, by using the Diagnostic Behavioral Assessment for Autism Spectrum Disorder-Revised (DiBAS-R), the Anxiety Scale for Adults with ASD (ASA-ASD-I), the Emotional Regulation Checklist (ERC) and the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale – Parent Version (IUS-12-P). Results showed that IU was a predictor of anxiety, and supported the mediating role of UI and emotional regulation between ASD and anxiety, thus suggesting that interventions designed to reduce anxiety symptoms in people with ASD and ID should include among their goals emotional regulation and especially improvement in intolerance of uncertainty (56). In addition, a study from Conner et al. (57) evaluated 78 adolescents and young adults (M = 61, F = 15, transgender = 2, age range = 12–20 years) through the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale (IUS-12), the PROMIS anxiety and depression scales and the EDI. In particular, the latter is one of the scales most commonly used to measure ED in ASD and structured in a caregiver-report questionnaire designed to capture emotional distress and problems with emotion regulation without requiring verbal information (37). The authors concluded that ED contributes to how IU affects psychopathology, specifically, ED, IU, anxiety, and depression scores were all correlated with each other. Additionally, both Reactivity and Dysphoria were found to mediate IU-depression and IU-anxiety (58). Overall, these findings are consistent with the rising body of literature indicating the extensive role of ED and IU in ASD psychopathology (59). Furthermore, they add to the growing body of literature highlighting the significance of IU as a transdiagnostic construct related with different kinds of psychopathology in ASD (5961), as well as its potential as a transdiagnostic, trans-therapy changes process (62).

2.4. ED and alexithymia

Some authors examined the role of ED together with interoceptive sensibility and alexithymia, defined as difficulties in identifying and verbalizing emotions, in somatoform disorder in ASD. Zdankiewicz-Ścigała et al. (63) compared 79 ASD individuals (M = 22, F = 57%, age range 20–45 years) and 126 controls (M = 26, F = 100, age range = 18–63 years). Participants were evaluated through The Toronto Alexithymia Scale–20 (TAS-20), the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) and the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS). Results showed a positive correlation between interoceptive sensibility and alexithymia, and ED in the lack of emotional awareness and lack of emotional clarity (63). The role of alexithymia together with ED as a mediator between anxiety /depression and autistic symptoms was also explored by Morie et al. (30), who analyzed 64 ASD individuals (M = 17, F = 47, age range = 18–65 years) through the SRS, Depression, Anxiety and Stress Scale (DASS), TAS-20 and DERS. It was found that the autistic symptoms mediated both y anxiety and depression through a pathway featuring alexithymia and ED as intermediate steps. As a result, targeting alexithymia may benefit therapies designed to alleviate mood disorders in ASD (30). Gormley et al. (31) reported similar findings in a work carried out in 43 ASD subjects (M = 32, F = 11, age range = 10–18 years) assessed through self and parent reported questionnaires such as the Alexithymia Questionnaire for Children (AQC), Emotion Regulation Index for Children and Adolescents (ERICA), Emotion Regulation Checklist (ERC) and Autism Spectrum Rating Scale (6–18 years Parent Ratings) (ASRS). High levels of alexithymia and ED were reported by both patients and their parents. Moreover, alexithymia significantly correlated with emotion regulation (31). These findings are consistent with previous studies that established a link between alexithymia and interoception in patients with ASD. For instance, Gaigg et al. (64) established that alexithymia is caused by a disturbance in how physiological arousal regulates subjective sensations in people with and without ASD. The findings support the validity of models that consider physiological arousal perception (interoception) to be crucial in the subjective experience of feelings (6567). Subjects with alexithymia may either fail to recognize normally normal physiological arousal or exhibit atypical arousal, which has an impact on their subjective emotional experience (68).

Another interesting study compared 60 ASD individuals (M = 30, F = 30, mean age = 14 years), 60 adolescents with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) (M = 30, F = 30, mean age = 14 years) and 60 controls (M = 30, F = 30, mean age = 13.55 years) for ED aspects (69). Reading the mind in the eyes test, the Faux Pas Test, and the hinting task were given to patients to evaluate the Theory of Mind (ToM) skills. Results pointed out that all these individuals had defects in emotional regulation, with ASD subjects presenting more difficulties than the ADHD ones (69).

3. Bio-anatomical correlates of ED in ASD

From a biochemical point of view, the available literature reported the involvement of some dopaminergic gene polymorphisms in the modulation of ED in children with ASD (43) as well as its correlation with specific brain regions. In particular, a work by Ibraihim et al. (70) compared three different groups of children (age range = 8–16 years): ASD-plus-Disruptive-Behavior (ASD + DB) (n = 18), ASD without disruptive behavior (ASD) (n = 20) and TD (n = 19). ASD was diagnosed through the DSM-5, and participants were evaluated with the K-SADS-PL. Parents were invited to complete the CBCL and the Inventory of Callous-Unemotional Traits (ICU). Participants were also examined through a functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), and completed an emotion perception task of fearful vs. calm faces. The authors analyzed task-specific changes in amygdala reactivity and connectivity by using whole-brain, psychophysiological interaction, and multiple-regression analyses and concluded that reduced amygdala-vIPFC connectivity during fear processing may differentiate children with ASD + DB from children with only ASD. In addition, they revealed that the presence of callous-unemotional traits could be useful for identifying patterns of amygdala activity associated with increased risk of aggression in ASD patients. These findings suggested a neural mechanism of ED associated with disruptive behavior in children with ASD (70). In order to better investigate the neurostructural correlates of ED in ASD, Ni et al. (71) acquired diffusion spectrum imaging in two groups: 59 ASD children (mean age = 12.5 years) and 62 TD (mean age = 12 years). The relationship between participants’ dysregulation levels and microstructural property of 76 white matter (WM) tracts was evaluated in a multivariate analysis across diagnostic groups concluding that both ASD and TD individuals had a broad distinct WM microstructural property underpinning self-regulation (71). Another study analyzing the mechanisms of sustained processing of negative information in brain activity in ASD by using fMRI, enrolled 25 ASD adolescents (M = 24, F = 1, mean age = 14.95 years) and 23 TD (M = 22, F = 1, mean age 15.5 years). Results highlighted that the salience network and the prefrontal dorsolateral cortex, which previous studies also reported to be implicated in ED, were the brain regions identified as having greater and more continued processing following negative stimuli in the ASD group compared to the TD group. Interestingly, there was also an association between brain activity in the identified regions and parent-reported ED in ASD (72).

Moreover, it should be noted that ED has also been associated with autonomic nervous system deregulation, another typical feature of ASD (73). Poor emotional regulation in ASD is knowingly associated with physiological arousal, negative and positive effects, alteration in the amygdala and prefrontal cortex (74). Furthermore, irritability in ASD boys has been associated with physiological reactivity, as reported in a study from Mikita et al. (75). The sample consisted in 47 high functioning male ASD (HF-ASD) (mean age = 12.8 years) and 23 TD subjects (mean age = 13.9 years), who completed a psychosocial stress test. The variables recorded were: changes in cortisol, heart rate and heart rate variability throughout the test as well as self- and parent-reported measures of irritability. Irritability symptoms reported in the HF-ASD group were compared to two groups of subjects without ASD: 40 highly irritable boys or severe mood dysregulation (SMD) group and 30 healthy-control (HC). Results reported that the HF-ASD group had higher score on irritability than the HC, together with a pattern of irritability symptoms closely resembling that of the SMD group. Even though boys with HF-ASD presented significant stress-induced changes in cortisol and heart rate, a lower cortisol level throughout the test was found in those who rated themselves as highly irritable compared to those low on irritability. Blunted cortisol and heart rate responses to stress emerged in participants rated as highly irritable by their parents. The effects of irritability on heart rate, but not cortisol, were accounted for by trait anxiety (75). Another nervous system variable examined in ASD emotional deregulation is heart rate variability (HRV). A work was carried out in 23 ASD individuals (M = 16, F = 7) and 32 TD controls (M = 8, F = 24). Participants, aged between 13 and 18 years, were assessed for ED through the EDI inventory, while a short-term electrocardiogram was used for HRV. It was found that lower resting HRV was strongly associated with greater ED in ASD than in TD controls, thus suggesting that alterations of autonomic functioning may contribute to emotion dysregulation in ASD (76).

4. Therapeutic perspectives in ASD

Non-pharmacological approaches to the treatment of ED in ASD include various meaningful therapeutic strategies. Of primary importance is an evidence-based practice, namely the use of functional communication training, which systematically teaches a functional equivalent to the maladaptive behavior (77). Another therapeutic strategy was presented by Torrado et al. (78) and consists in two devices: a smartwatch that detects the user’s inner state and displays the self-regulation strategies and a smartphone, used by caregivers or family members in an adaptive way to create, edit and customize such interventions. The smartwatch monitors the user’s heart rate and infers outburst patterns from physiological signals and movements in order to be able to manage a lot of minor outbursts. The behavior’s long-term issues are based on repeated episodes of exposure to unpleasant, self-harming sensations not properly addressed due to. According to the authors, this system may allow a major improvement on the quality of life of people with ASD and ED problems, considering that small-scale intervention applied to the daily life of these individuals seems to prevent long-term behavioral issues (78). More specific therapeutic approaches, such as cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is often used with patients with ASD in comorbidity with mood disorder, anxiety, and/or behavioral difficulties that have at least average verbal capabilities (77). In this framework, a study conducted by Bemmouna et al. (79) valuated the feasibility, acceptability and preliminary efficacy of Dialectical Behavior Therapy (DBT) in seven ASD adults without ID exhibiting self-harm and/or suicidal behaviors linked to severe ED (age range = 19–56, mean age = 27.71 ± 13.34) assessed with the DERS. DERS total scores ranged from 107 to 144 (mean score = 123.57 ± 13.56). The results showed that mean scores decreased significantly post-treatment and at 4-month follow-up, suggesting that DBT might be efficacious in reducing ED in this population. The majority of participant also showed a decrease in the frequency and severity of self-harm and reported a decline in the frequency of suicidal thoughts (79). Another promising research by Huntjens et al. (80) aimed to evaluate the efficacy of DBT in ASD subjects and its influence on suicidal and self-destructive behaviors. For this purpose, 128 subjects with autism and suicidal and/or self-harming behavior were recruited and divided in two groups, one where the participants have weekly individual cognitive behavioral therapy sessions and a 2.5 h skills training group session twice per week during 6 months and another where subjects are treated with weekly individual therapy sessions of 30–45 min with a psychotherapist or social worker. To this date, the study is ongoing and in the data collection stage, however results from this study could be promising in giving an evaluation of the efficacy of DBT treatment in persons with ASD on suicidal and self-harming behavior (80).

5. Discussion

Overall, a limited number of studies focused on ED in ASD from adolescence to adulthood, while more data are available on child or mixed samples. ASD individuals have been reported to show lower adaptive behaviors, less use of cognitive reappraisal and more use of emotional suppression than TD controls (46) and even ADHD patients (69). Additionally, the scientific literature highlighted difficulties in identifying and verbalizing emotions with lack of emotional awareness and clarity in ASD (63). Besides, ASD people tend to present more anger, rumination and depression than controls (50). According to some authors, ED and IU seem to be related to increased depression and anxiety levels in ASD, possibly influencing each other (57, 66). Noticeably, ED in ASD has been related with specific genetic polymorphisms (43) as well as with anatomical alteration such as reduced amygdala connectivity (70). Another brain region implicated in ED is the prefrontal dorsolateral cortex (72) while, together with genetic and anatomical alterations, other studies reported autonomic nervous system deregulation, such as an alteration in cortisol levels and heart rate, in ASD individuals (73, 75). Intriguingly, the same autonomic nervous system deregulation seems to promote ED according to another study (76), possibly suggesting that the development of ED could be linked to the underlying ASD condition also from a neurophysiological point of view (76). In this framework, it should be noted that, besides stressing a frequent presence of ED among subject with ASD, some authors are also hypothesizing a link between the core features of autism spectrum and ED: ED was reported indeed to be linked with social difficulties and, in particular, with the presence of repetitive behaviors and restricted interest (29). This data may suggest a common pathophysiology between ED and repetitive/restricted symptoms of ASD, with perseveration traits at the basis of the difficulties in regulating emotions (29). A neurodevelopmental basis for ED could be in line with the transnosographic conceptualization of autism spectrum (39), which, on the basis of the frequent presence of autistic traits in different psychopathological conditions, hypothesizes a possible neurodevelopmental underpinning (whose autistic traits would be the phenotypical presentation) for several psychiatric disorders (40). The presence of a neurodevelopmental alteration would also feature a vulnerability towards life events, facilitating the development of trauma and stress related symptoms as well as a reduced ability to regulate emotions (40). This hypothesis seems to be further supported by the findings of Morie et al. (30), which reported a mediating pathway that, starting from the presence of autistic traits, leads, through the development of alexithymia and ED, to full-blown anxiety and mood disorders. This data is somewhat in line with another study by Dell’Osso et al. (32) which reported a significant effect of autistic traits in enhancing the development of mood disorders, directly and indirectly, through the effect of post-traumatic symptoms (often associated in the literature with ED) and rumination (32).

Globally, despite the limited literature available, some interesting evidences on the prevalence of ED in ASD, as well as on its bio-neurological correlates, were highlighted. However, many aspects of this dimension remain controversial and should be further investigated. In particular, further studies are needed for evaluating ED, in light of a neurodevelopmental approach to psychopathology, as a possible step of the pathway that from autistic traits may promote the development of other psychiatric conditions.

Author contributions

LD’O, IMC, and BC: conceptualization. LD’O, IMC, and BC: methodology. LM, SB, and CDF: investigation. LM, SB, CDF, GA, and BN: writing—original draft preparation. BC, GA, and BN: writing—review and editing. LD’O, BC, and IMC: supervision. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-5-TR. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2022).

Google Scholar

2. Mazza, M, Pino, MC, Mariano, M, Tempesta, D, Ferrara, M, De Berardis, D, et al. Affective and cognitive empathy in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:791. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00791

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Pino, MC, Masedu, F, Vagnetti, R, Attanasio, M, Di Giovanni, C, Valenti, M, et al. Validity of social cognition measures in the clinical Services for Autism Spectrum Disorder. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:4. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Dell’Osso, L, Carmassi, C, Cremone, IM, Muti, D, Salerni, A, Barberi, FM, et al. 324 Politi P, Aguglia E, Maj M, Carpita B. defining the optimal threshold scores for adult autism subthreshold Spectrum (AdAS Spectrum) in clinical and general population. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. (2020) 16:204–11. doi: 10.2174/1745017902016010204

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Rosen, NE, Lord, C, and Volkmar, FR. The diagnosis of autism: from kanner to DSM-III to DSM-5 and beyond. J Autism Dev Disord. (2021) 51:4253–70. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-04904-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Baron-Cohen, S, Wheelwright, S, Skinner, R, Martin, J, and Clubley, E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. (2001) 31:5–17. doi: 10.1023/a:1005653411471

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Billeci, L, Calderoni, S, Conti, E, Gesi, C, Carmassi, C, Dell’Osso, L, et al. The broad autism (Endo)phenotype: Neurostructural and Neurofunctional correlates in parents of individuals with autism Spectrum disorders. Front Neurosci. (2016) 10:346. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00346

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Carpita, B, Carmassi, C, Calderoni, S, Muti, D, Muscarella, A, Massimetti, G, et al. The broad autism phenotype in real-life: clinical and functional correlates of autism spectrum symptoms and rumination among parents of patients with autism spectrum disorder. CNS Spectr. (2020) 25:765–73. doi: 10.1017/S1092852919001615

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Dell’Osso, L, Cremone, IM, Carpita, B, Fagiolini, A, Massimetti, G, Bossini, L, et al. Correlates of autistic traits among patients with borderline personality disorder. Compr Psychiatry. (2018) 83:7–11. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2018.01.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Dell’Osso, L, Carpita, B, Bertelloni, CA, Diadema, E, Barberi, FM, Gesi, C, et al. Subthreshold autism spectrum in bipolar disorder: prevalence and clinical correlates. Psychiatry Res. (2019) 281:112605. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2019.112605

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Carpita, B, Muti, D, Cremone, IM, Fagiolini, A, and Dell’Osso, L. Eating disorders and autism spectrum: links and risks. CNS Spectr. (2022) 27:272–80. doi: 10.1017/S1092852920002011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Carpita, B, Cremone, IM, Amatori, G, Cappelli, A, Salerni, A, Massimetti, G, et al. Investigating the relationship between orthorexia nervosa and autistic traits in a university population. CNS Spectr. (2022) 27:613–20. doi: 10.1017/S1092852921000420

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Dell’Osso, L, Cremone, IM, Chiarantini, I, Arone, A, Massimetti, G, Carmassi, C, et al. Autistic traits and camouflaging behaviors: a cross-sectional investigation in a university student population. CNS Spectr. (2022) 27:740–6. doi: 10.1017/S1092852921000808

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Dell’Osso, L, Nardi, B, Benedetti, F, Cremone, IM, Casagrande, D, Massimetti, G, et al. Orthorexia and autism spectrum in university workers: relationship with gender, body mass index and dietary habits. Eat Weight Disord. (2022) 27:3713–23. doi: 10.1007/s40519-022-01514-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Davis, J, McKone, E, Zirnsak, M, Moore, T, O’Kearney, R, Apthorp, D, et al. Social and attention-to-detail subclusters of autistic traits differentially predict looking at eyes and face identity recognition ability. Brit J Psychol. (2017) 108:191–219. doi: 10.1111/bjop.12188

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. English, MCW, Gignac, GE, Visser, TAW, Whitehouse, AJO, and Maybery, MT. A comprehensive psychometric analysis of autism-Spectrum quotient factor models using two large samples: model recommendations and the influence of divergent traits on total-scale scores. Autism Res. (2020) 13:45–60. doi: 10.1002/aur.2198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Dell’Osso, L, Bertelloni, CA, Di Paolo, M, Avella, MT, Carpita, B, Gori, F, et al. Problematic internet use in university students attending three superior graduate schools in Italy: is autism Spectrum related to suicide risk? Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2019) 16:1098. doi: 10.3390/ijerph16071098

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Dell’Osso, L, Cremone, IM, Amatori, G, Cappelli, A, Cuomo, A, Barlati, S, et al. Investigating the relationship between autistic traits, ruminative thinking, and suicidality in a clinical sample of subjects with bipolar disorder and borderline personality disorder. Brain Sci. (2021) 11:621. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11050621

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Carpita, B, Muti, D, Muscarella, A, Dell’Oste, V, Diadema, E, Massimetti, G, et al. Sex differences in the relationship between PTSD Spectrum symptoms and autistic traits in a sample of university students. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. (2019) 15:110–9. doi: 10.2174/1745017901915010110

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Mazza, M, Pino, MC, Vagnetti, R, Filocamo, A, Attanasio, M, Calvarese, A, et al. Intensive intervention for adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: comparison of three rehabilitation treatments. Int J Psychiatry Clin Pract. (2021) 25:28–36. doi: 10.1080/13651501.2020.1800042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Kring, AM, and Sloan, DM. Emotion regulation and psychopathology: A Transdiagnostic approach to etiology and treatment. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2009).

Google Scholar

22. Gross, JJ, and Jazaieri, H. Emotion, emotion regulation, and psychopathology: an affective science perspective. Clin Psychol Sci. (2014) 2:387–401. doi: 10.1177/2167702614536164

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Thompson, RA. Emotional regulation and emotional development. Educ Psychol Rev. (1991) 3:269–307. doi: 10.1007/BF01319934

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Bunford, N, Evans, SW, and Wymbs, F. ADHD and emotion dysregulation among children and adolescents. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. (2015) 18:185–217. doi: 10.1007/s10567-015-0187-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Shaw, P, Stringaris, A, Nigg, J, and Leibenluft, E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2014) 171:276–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Gross, JJ, and Thompson, RA. Emotion regulation. Conceptual foundations In: J Gross, editor. Handbook of Emtion regulation. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2007). 3–24.

Google Scholar

27. Powers, A, Cross, D, Fani, N, and Bradley, B. PTSD, emotion dysregulation, and dissociative symptoms in a highly traumatized sample. J Psychiatr Res. (2015) 61:174–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.12.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Aldao, A, Gee, DG, De Los, RA, and Seager, I. Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic factor in the development of internalizing and externalizing psychopathology: current and future directions. Dev Psychopathol. (2016) 28:927–46. doi: 10.1017/S0954579416000638

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Samson, AC, Phillips, JM, Parker, KJ, Shah, S, Gross, JJ, and Hardan, AY. Emotion dysregulation and the Core features of autism Spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2014) 44:1766–72. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-2022-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Morie, KP, Jackson, S, Zhai, ZW, Potenza, MN, and Dritschel, B. Mood disorders in high-functioning autism: the importance of alexithymia and emotional regulation. J Autism Dev Disord. (2019) 49:2935–45. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04020-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gormley, E, Ryan, C, and McCusker, C. Alexithymia is associated with emotion dysregulation in young people with autism spectrum disorder. J Dev Phys Disabil. (2022) 34:171–86. doi: 10.1007/s10882-021-09795-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Dell’Osso, L, Carpita, B, Cremone, IM, Muti, D, Diadema, E, Barberi, FM, et al. The mediating effect of trauma and stressor related symptoms and ruminations on the relationship between autistic traits and mood spectrum. Psychiatry Res. (2019) 279:123–9. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.10.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Dell’Osso, L, Cremone, IM, Carpita, B, Dell’Oste, V, Muti, D, Massimetti, G, et al. Rumination, posttraumatic stress disorder, and mood symptoms in borderline personality disorder. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2019) 15:1231–8. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S198616

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Peled, A. Brain “Globalopathies” cause mental disorders. Med Hypotheses. (2013) 81:1046–55. doi: 10.1016/j.mehy.2013.09.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Dell’Osso, L, Dalle Luche, R, and Maj, M. Adult autism spectrum as a transnosographic dimension. CNS Spectr. (2016) 21:131–3. doi: 10.1017/S1092852915000450

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Dell’Osso, L, Lorenzi, P, and Carpita, B. Autistic traits and illness trajectories. Clin Pract Epidemiol Ment Health. (2019) 15:94–8. doi: 10.2174/1745017901915010094

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Dell’Osso, L, and Carpita, B. What misdiagnoses do women with autism spectrum disorder receive in the DSM-5? CNS Spectr. (2022) 28:269–70. doi: 10.1017/S1092852922000037

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Wolff, JC, Thompson, E, Thomas, SA, Nesi, J, Bettis, AH, Ransford, B, et al. Emotion dysregulation and non-suicidal self-injury: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur Psychiatry. (2019) 59:25–36. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2019.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Aldao, A, Nolen-Hoeksema, S, and Schweizer, S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2010) 30:217–37. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Klonsky, ED. The functions of deliberate self-injury: a review of the evidence. Clin Psychol Rev. (2007) 27:226–39. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.08.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Selby, EA, and Joiner, TE. Cascades of emotion: the emergence of borderline personality disorder from emotional and behavioral dysregulation. Rev Gen Psychol. (2009) 13:219–29. doi: 10.1037/a0015687

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Mazefsky, CA, Yu, L, White, SW, Siegel, M, and Pilkonis, PA. The emotion dysregulation inventory: psychometric properties and item response theory calibration in an autism spectrum disorder sample. Autism Res. (2018) 11:928–41. doi: 10.1002/aur.1947

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Gadow Kenneth, D, Pinsonneault, JK, Perlman, G, and Sadee, W. Association of dopamine gene variants, emotion dysregulation and ADHD in autism spectrum disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2014) 35:1658–65. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2014.04.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bennett, RH, Somandepalli, K, Roy, AK, and Di Martino, A. The neural correlates of emotional lability in children with autism Spectrum disorder. Brain Connect. (2017) 7:281–8. doi: 10.1089/brain.2016.0472

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Samson, AC, Wells, WM, Phillips, JM, Hardan, AY, and Gross, JJ. Emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder: evidence from parent interviews and children’s daily diaries. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2014) 56:903–13. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12370

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Samson, AC, Hardan, AY, Podell, RW, Phillips, JM, and Gross, JJ. Emotion regulation in children and adolescents with autism Spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2015) 8:9–18. doi: 10.1002/aur.1387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Mazefsky, CA, Pelphrey, KA, and Dahl, RE. The need for a broader approach to emotion regulation research in autism. Child Dev Perspect. (2012) 6:92–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2011.00229.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Cai, RY, Richdale, AL, Dissanayake, C, Trollor, J, and Uljarević, M. Emotion regulation in autism: reappraisal and suppression interactions. Autism. (2019) 23:737–49. doi: 10.1177/1362361318774558

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Neuhaus, E, Webb, S, and Bernier, R. Linking social motivation with social skill: the role of emotion dysregulation in autism spectrum disorder. Dev Psychopathol. (2019) 31:931–43. doi: 10.1017/S0954579419000361

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Patel, S, Day, TN, Jones, N, and Mazefsky, CA. Association between anger rumination and autism symptom severity, depression symptoms, aggression, and general dysregulation in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. (2017) 21:181–9. doi: 10.1177/1362361316633566

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Selby, EA, Anestis, MD, and Joiner, TE. Understanding the relationship between emotional and behavioral dysregulation: emotional cascades. Behav Res Ther. (2008) 46:593–611. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2008.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Spek, AA, van Ham, NC, and Nyklíček, I. Mindfulness-based therapy in adults with an autism spectrum disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Res Dev Disabil. (2013) 34:246–53. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2012.08.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Smith, JM, and Alloy, LB. A roadmap to rumination: a review of the definition, assessment, and conceptualization of this multifaceted construct. Clin Psychol Rev. (2009) 29:116–28. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. McVey, AJ, Schiltz, HK, Coffman, M, Antezana, L, and Magnus, B. A preliminary psychometric analysis of the difficulties with emotion regulation scale (DERS) among autistic adolescents and adults: factor structure, reliability, and validity. J Autism Dev Disord. (2022) 52:1169–88. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05018-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Carleton, RN. Into the unknown: a review and synthesis of contemporary models involving uncertainty. J Anxiety Disord. (2016) 39:30–43. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.02.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Sáez-Suanes, GP, García-Villamisar, D, Del Pozo, AA, and Dattilo, J. Emotional dysregulation and uncertainty intolerance as transdiagnostic mediators of anxiety in adults with autism spectrum disorders and intellectual disability. Res Dev Disabil. (2020) 106:103784. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2020.103784

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Conner, CM, Kim, PS, White, SW, and Mazefsky, CA. The role of emotion dysregulation and intolerance of uncertainty in autism: Transdiagnostic factors influencing co-occurring conditions. Res Dev Disabil. (2022) 130:104332. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2022.104332

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Charlton, AS, Smith, IC, Mazefsky, CA, and White, SW. The role of emotion regulation on co-occurring psychopathology in emerging adults with ASD. J Autism Develop Disord. (2020) 50:2585–92. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-03983-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Boulter, C, Freeston, M, South, M, and Rodgers, J. Intolerance of uncertainty as a framework for understanding anxiety in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Develop Disord. (2014) 44:1391–402. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-2001-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Keefer, A, Kreiser, NL, Singh, V, Blakeley-Smith, A, Reaven, J, and Vasa, RA. Exploring relationships between negative cognitions and anxiety symptoms in youth with autism spectrum disorder. Behav Ther. (2018) 49:730–40. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2017.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Cai, RY, Richdale, AL, Dissanayake, C, and Uljarević, M. Brief report: inter-relationship between emotion regulation, intolerance of uncertainty, anxiety, and depression in youth with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Develop Disord. (2018) 48:316–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-017-3318-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. McEvoy, PM, and Erceg-Hurn, DM. The search for universal transdiagnostic and trans-therapy change processes: evidence for intolerance of uncertainty. J Anxiety Disord. (2016) 2016:96–107. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2016.02.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Zdankiewicz-Ścigała, E, Ścigała, D, Sikora, J, Kwaterniak, W, and Longobardi, C. Relationship between interoceptive sensibility and somatoform disorders in adults with autism spectrum traits. The mediating role of alexithymia and emotional dysregulation. PLoS One. (2021) 16:e0255460. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0255460

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Gaigg, SB, and Cornell, AS. Bird G the psychophysiological mechanisms of alexithymia in autism spectrum disorder. Autism. (2016) 22:227–31. doi: 10.1177/1362361316667062

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Craig, AD. How do you feel–now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2009) 10:59–70. doi: 10.1038/nrn2555

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Craig, AD. Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Curr Opin Neurol. (2003) 13:500505. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(03)00090-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Craig, AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2002) 3:655666. doi: 10.1038/nrn894

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Silani, G, Bird, G, Brindley, R, and Singer, T. Levels of emotional awareness and autism: an fMRI study. Soc Neurosci. (2008) 3:97–112. doi: 10.1080/17470910701577020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Dağdelen, F. Decreased theory of mind abilities and increased emotional dysregulation in adolescents with ASD and ADHD. Alpha Psych. (2021) 22:100–5. doi: 10.5455/apd.135050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Ibrahim, K, Eilbott, JA, Ventola, P, He, G, Pelphrey, KA, McCarthy, G, et al. Reduced amygdala-prefrontal functional connectivity in children with autism Spectrum disorder and co-occurring disruptive behavior. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2019) 4:1031–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.01.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Ni, HC, Lin, HY, Tseng, WI, and Gau, SS. Association of self-regulation with white matter correlates in boys with and without autism spectrum disorder. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:13811. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-70836-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Mazefsky, CA, Collier, A, Golt, J, and Siegle, GJ. Neural features of sustained emotional information processing in autism spectrum disorder. Autism. (2020) 24:941–53. doi: 10.1177/1362361320903137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Dell’Osso, L, Massoni, L, Battaglini, S, Cremone, IM, Carmassi, C, and Carpita, B. Biological correlates of altered circadian rhythms, autonomic functions and sleep problems in autism spectrum disorder. Ann General Psychiatry. (2022) 21:13. doi: 10.1186/s12991-022-00390-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Mazefsky, CA, Herrington, J, Siegel, M, Scarpa, A, Maddox, BB, Scahill, L, et al. The role of emotion regulation in autism spectrum disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2013) 52:679–88. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Mikita, N, Hollocks, MJ, Papadopoulos, AS, Aslani, A, Harrison, S, Leibenluft, E, et al. Irritability in boys with autism spectrum disorders: an investigation of physiological reactivity. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2015) 56:1118–26. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12382

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Chiu, HT, Ip, IN, Ching, FNY, Wong, BP, Lui, WH, Tse, CS, et al. Resting heart rate variability and emotion dysregulation in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2023). doi: 10.1007/s10803-022-05847-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Keluskar, J, Reicher, D, Gorecki, A, Mazefsky, C, and Crowell, JA. Understanding, assessing, and intervening with emotion dysregulation in autism Spectrum disorder: a developmental perspective. Child Adolesc Psychiatr Clin N Am. (2021) 30:335–48. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2020.10.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Torrado, JC, Gomez, J, and Montoro, G. Emotional self-regulation of individuals with autism Spectrum disorders: smartwatches for monitoring and interaction. Sensors (Basel). (2017) 17:1359. doi: 10.3390/s17061359

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Bemmouna, D, Coutelle, R, Weibel, S, and Weiner, L. Feasibility, acceptability and preliminary efficacy of dialectical behavior therapy for autistic adults without intellectual disability: a mixed methods study. J Autism Dev Disord. (2022) 52:4337–54. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-05317-w

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Huntjens, A, van den Bosch, LMCW, Sizoo, B, Kerkhof, A, Huibers, MJH, and van der Gaag, M. The effect of dialectical behaviour therapy in autism spectrum patients with suicidality and/ or self-destructive behaviour (DIASS): study protocol for a multicentre randomised controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. (2020) 20:127. doi: 10.1186/s12888-020-02531-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: emotional dysregulation, autism spectrum disorder, autistic traits, bioanatomical correlates, emotional regulation

Citation: Dell’Osso L, Massoni L, Battaglini S, De Felice C, Nardi B, Amatori G, Cremone IM and Carpita B (2023) Emotional dysregulation as a part of the autism spectrum continuum: a literature review from late childhood to adulthood. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1234518. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1234518

Received: 04 June 2023; Accepted: 30 August 2023;
Published: 18 September 2023.

Edited by:

Kleanthes K. Grohmann, University of Cyprus, Cyprus

Reviewed by:

Monica Mazza, University of L’Aquila, Italy
Camilla Gesi, ASST Fatebenefratelli Sacco, Italy

Copyright © 2023 Dell’Osso, Massoni, Battaglini, De Felice, Nardi, Amatori, Cremone and Carpita. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Barbara Carpita, YmFyYmFyYS5jYXJwaXRhMTk4NkBnbWFpbC5jb20=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.