Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 23 March 2023
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Addictive Behaviors Among Youth and Adolescents in the Digital Age View all 10 articles

The effect of mindfulness on social media addiction among Chinese college students: A serial mediation model

Hongming ChangHongming Chang1Xiaolu Meng,Xiaolu Meng1,2Yaqi LiYaqi Li1Jiaxi LiuJiaxi Liu3Wen YuanWen Yuan3Jian NiJian Ni3Chunlu Li,
Chunlu Li1,2*
  • 1Department of Psychology, School of Medical Humanitarians, Guizhou Medical University, Guiyang, China
  • 2Guizhou Health Development Research Center, Guiyang, China
  • 3Department of Applied Psychology, Hunan University of Chinese Medicine, Changsha, China

Introduction: The COVID-19 pandemic has exacerbated social media addiction (SMA), making it urgent to find effective interventions for social media addiction. Evidence has shown that mindfulness might be an effective intervention for social media addiction. However, psychological mechanisms by which mindfulness reduce social media use remain unclear. Here, we further addressed this issue to examine whether attentional control and fear of missing out (FOMO) mediate the relationship between mindfulness and SMA.

Methods: We recruited 446 college students from two universities in China and analyzed the data.

Results: The results suggest that there are mediation effects of attentional control and FOMO between mindfulness and SMA through 3 paths: path 1, mindfulness → attention control → SMA (−0.04); path 2, mindfulness → FOMO → SMA (−0.22); and path 3, mindfulness → attention control → FOMO → SMA (−0.05).

Discussion: Therefore, mindfulness-based interventions may be an effective way to alleviate social media addiction, especially mindfulness-based interventions targeting FOMO. At the end of the article, we also discussed the limitations of this study.

Introduction

Social media use has increased exponentially. The number of social media users is estimated to have reached 3 billion worldwide (1). Social media facilitates social interaction and lowers stress for at least some users, but overuse can lead to serious negative consequences, such as social media addiction (SMA), poor sleep quality, less offline social interaction, and more depression and anxiety symptoms (25). The estimated SMA prevalence of college students is around 23% (6). To make matters worse, the COVID-19 outbreak exacerbated social media addiction and its negative consequences, such as sleep problems (7, 8), the negative impact on the well-being of excessive social media users (9), increased daily stress, and an increase in suicide-related outcomes (10, 11). Given its high prevalence and severe negative consequences, the question of how to alleviate social media addiction and reduce the adverse consequences of social media addiction has become very urgent.

Many antecedents leading to social media addiction have been investigated, such as attachment styles (12), self-esteem (13, 14), the need to socialize (15), and social anxiety (13). However, how social media addiction can be systematically controlled or regulated remained unsolved in these studies.

Existing evidence suggests that mindfulness-based interventions may be an effective intervention for social media addiction. Mindfulness helps us to be aware of where we are and what we are doing rather than to be overreacting or being overwhelmed by what is happening around us (16). It is a basic human ability to be fully present and prevalent even among ordinary people who have not been systematically trained in mindfulness (17). The ability to sustain mindfulness is called trait mindfulness (TM) (18), which can be improved by mindfulness training (19). Research interested in mindfulness-based interventions (MBI) has exploded over the past few decades (20). Mindfulness has been shown to be negatively associated with social media addiction (2124), and has an indirect effect on social media addiction through mediation (25, 26) or moderation (2729). However, the psychological mechanisms by which mindfulness reduces social media addiction remain unclear. These psychological mechanisms can help us design mindfulness-based interventions more effectively.

Two studies have shown that the influence of mindfulness to reduce social media addiction is mediated by the ability of social pressure self-efficacy (29), positive effects on self-esteem, and negative effects on social anxiety (13). In the present study, based on core features of mindfulness and social media addiction, we focused on the potential role of attentional control and social media-related fear of missing out (FOMO).

The mediating effect of fear of missing out

Currently, the COVID-19 pandemic remains a global challenge. Restrictions on the movement of people are one of the government policies aimed at reducing the spread of COVID-19. In this context, the pandemic dramatically changed one’s usual routine and decreased social contacts with each other, ultimately leading to fearful experiences that are particularly relevant to interpersonal interactions, especially the fear of missing out (FOMO). FOMO is defined as a widespread anxiety arising when individuals cannot get the experiences they want to know about, mainly manifested by a persistent desire to know what others are doing (30). In fact, it has been shown that the COVID-19 pandemic increased the fear of FOMO in China (31). During lockdowns, social media use may be one of the most effective and available ways to ease FOMO. The latter has been highlighted in previous literature as an important influencing factor of social media use (32, 33), including problematic social media use (34, 35). Therefore, FOMO may be a significant contributor to social media addiction, especially under the COVID-19 pandemic.

FOMO is likely to distract people from their experiences of in-the-moment. In fact, when one is worrying about what one is missing out, it will become especially difficult to focus on what one is doing now. Conversely, when a person is deeply involved in what they are doing at the moment, they may be less likely to think of alternatives and then will feel less FOMO (36). Mindfulness practice places a strong emphasis on focusing on the here and now. This suggests that the fear of missing out is an important target for mindfulness interventions. In fact, related research has provided direct evidence for the relationship between mindfulness and fear of missing out. For example, less mindful attention was associated with higher levels of FOMO (37). Mindfulness practice has reduced levels of fear of missing out (38). Based on this evidence, we hypothesized that FOMO mediates the effect of mindfulness on social media addiction (H1). To be specific, mindfulness may decrease FOMO of Chinese college students, which, in turn, may decrease social media addiction.

The serial mediating role of attention control and FOMO

The psychological mechanisms by which mindfulness reduces fear of missing out in the context of social media addiction require further exploration. Theoretically, attention is widely regarded as a core aspect of mindfulness (39, 40). And the practice of self-regulation of attention is one common component among various types of mindfulness-based interventions (41). For example, mindfulness meditation typically involves focusing attention on a selected target object and redirecting attention on the same target object in the event of distraction (39). In fact, attentional enhancement is one of the most common outcomes of different types of mindfulness-based interventions (40). For example, 8 weeks of mindfulness practice on novice college students has been shown to improve their alertness (39), attentional orienting (39, 42), and attention executive control (42).

According to the process model of emotion regulation, individuals regulate their emotion in the situation-attention-appraisal-response sequence, and attentional deployment is a cardinal emotion regulation strategy (43, 44). Human attention is usually deployed voluntarily based on observers’ goals (top-down) or shifted automatically according to noticeable features of stimuli in the environment (bottom-up). The former is usually called attentional control and is regulated by the anterior system, while the latter is usually called attention orientation, such as attention bias, which is regulated by the posterior system (45). However, a recent study found that there is no attentional bias toward relevant information in social media addiction (46). These evidences suggested that attentional control might be central to FOMO regulation.

Besides, a number of studies have found that reduced attention control ability affected individuals’ social media use. For example, attention deficit among college students significantly and positively predicted the severity of social media addiction (47, 48). Cognitive flexibility and sustained attention have been significantly and negatively associated with social media addiction in college women (49). Moreover, attention control deficits have become a risk marker for the anxiety onset and persistence (50, 51). Impaired attention control makes individuals more likely to skew attentional resources toward threatening information in the presence of anxiety (52). As an anxious emotion, the negative association between FOMO and attention control has been verified by previous studies (26, 38, 53).

Furthermore, according to the Interaction of Person-Effect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model proposed by Brand (54), when executive functions (e.g., attention control) are weak, the conditioned learning effects of negative emotions, such as anxiety, can cause social media use to gradually get out of control and deteriorate into compulsive use (55, 56). Thus, we hypothesized that mindfulness may indirectly influence social media addiction through the paths of attention control (H2) and attention control → FOMO (H3).

The present study

Based on the aforementioned literatures, the present study aimed to explore the psychological mechanisms underlying the effect of mindfulness on social media addiction. First, we recruited Chinese college students and examined the correlation between their mindfulness and social media addiction. Then, we further tested whether attention control and FOMO independently mediate this association. At the end, we investigated whether attentional control and FOMO mediate the relationship between mindfulness and social media addiction in a serial way. In conclusion, we aimed to fill in the gaps in the relationship between these variables, with the hope of providing a theoretical basis for intervention research in this context. We showed the hypothetical model in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The serial mediation model hypothesized in the present study.

Materials and methods

Participants

We conducted online and offline questionnaire surveys at two universities in China: Guizhou and Hunan, respectively. A total of 502 students were enrolled by stratified random sampling. All participants voluntarily filled out the questionnaire to participate in the study, without receiving rewards. All the participants were asked to sign informed consent forms and were told that they were participating in the study voluntarily and could terminate the questionnaire freely throughout the process. To ensure data quality, we excluded the data from two subject groups: (1) those who chose the same answer for greater than or equal to half the length of the total scale (57) and (2) those who did not sign the informed consent form. Finally, data from 446 participants (120 male and 326 female) ranging in age from 18 to 26 years (M = 20.36, SD = 1.40) were analyzed. This research was approved by the Ethics Committee of Guizhou Medical University.

Demographics are shown in Table 1. 26.9% and 73.1% of the participants were males and females, respectively. The ages of the participants ranged from 17 to 26 years (mean = 20.36, SD = 1.40). 22% of the participants came from cities, 27.6% came from towns, and 50.4% came from villages. Among all the participants, “only-child” (i.e., participants who were an only-child) accounted for 18.2%, while “non-only child” accounted for 81.8%; 44.4% were “left-behind child” (i.e., participants who experienced both or one parent relocating elsewhere to work for more than 6 months while the participant was growing up), while the rest, 55.6%, were not “left-behind child.”

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics.

Measures

Social media addiction

To measure the social media addiction of participants, the Bergen Facebook Addiction Scale (BFAS) was used (58), which was translated into the Chinese version by Yubo Hou (59). These items relate to experiences that have occurred in the past year and are rated on a 5-point scale from 1 (Very rarely) to 5 (Very often; example item: “ignored your partner, family members, or friends because of social media?”). The scale consists of 6 dimensions: salience, tolerance, mood changes, relapse, withdrawal, and conflict. In view of the characteristics of social networking sites in mainland China, we replaced the original scale, namely Facebook, Twitter, Instagram, etc., with those social networking sites that are popular in China: QQ, Weibo, WeChat, etc., in the instructions. The scores of the participant for all the items were added to form a social media addiction score, with higher scores indicating higher levels of social media addiction. Cronbach’s α of the whole scale was 0.92 in the current sample.

Mindfulness

To measure mindfulness, the Chinese version of the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale (60, 61) was used. The scale consists of 15 self-rating items on everyday experiences. An example is the item “I tend not to notice feelings grab my attention.” Responds rated each item on a 6-point Likert-type scale, with a range from 1 (almost always) to 6 (almost never). Total scores can range from 15 to 90, with higher scores indicating a greater level of mindfulness. In this study, Cronbach’s alpha was 0.86.

Attention control

To measure attention control, we used the Attention Control Scale (62), which was translated into the Chinese version by Siying He (63). The questionnaire has two dimensions: focusing (9 items) and shifting (11 items). Each item of this scale was rated on a 4-point scale from 1(almost never) to 4 (almost ever; i.e., almost always). In the present study, the Cronbach’s alpha of the Attention control Scale was 0.76.

The fear of missing out

To measure FOMO, we used the Fear of Missing Out in the Mobile Social Media Environment Measurement Scale, compiled by Song Xiaokang (64). The scale measures individual FOMO in the context of social media use, and consists of four dimensions: psychological motivation (e.g., “On mobile social media, as soon as I see a hint of what’s new, I’m eager to click on it right away.”), cognitive motivation (e.g., “Using mobile social media, I was able to get the news, business, or expertise I wanted”), behavioral performance (e.g., “As soon as I have time, such as waiting for the bus or recess, I am used to turning on mobile social media to check for new news or updates”), and emotional dependence (e.g., “If I cannot use mobile social media for a few days, I feel lost”). This scale has 16 items. Each item was rated on a 5-point Likert scale (1 = not at all true to 5 = absolutely true). The total score was between 16 and 80, and the higher the total score, the greater the fear of missing out. The Cronbach’s alpha was 0.87, in this study.

Statistical analysis

In the present study, data were analyzed according to how Hayes (65) described the process in his book on the SPSS 26.0. First, descriptive statistics of the investigated variables were calculated. Then, to examine the bivariate correlations between mindfulness, social media addiction, FOMO, and attention control, Pearson’s correlation analysis was used. Additionally, we ran PROCESS Macro Model 6 to examine simultaneously all the study hypotheses, as presented in Figure 1. To determine the magnitude of these effects, the bootstrapping method based on 5,000 re-samples of the data was used to produce 95% bias corrected confidence intervals (Cis). When Cis do not overlap with zero, the effect is significant at α = 0.05.

Results

Common method biases tests

To exclude common method bias, (66) single-factor test and an exploratory factor analysis were performed, where all items including four variables were included. We found that the first factor accounted for 20.7% of the total variance, which was below the 40% threshold proposed by Podsakoff et al. (67). Therefore, common method biases are unlikely to confuse the interpretation of the data analysis.

Descriptive analysis and correlations between overall variables

Table 2 presents basic descriptive data on mindfulness, attention control, FOMO, and social media addiction. The mean total scores of mindfulness were 57.43 ± 10.62 (range = 25 to 87), the meant total scores for attention control were 49.87 ± 6.05 (range = 28 to 72), the mean total scores for FOMO were 54.8 ± 9.44 (range = 27 to 78), and the mean total scores for social media addiction were 46.35 ± 12.51 (range = 20 to 83).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Descriptive statistics and results of correlational analysis of variables (N = 446).

To test the bivariate correlations of all the variables, we conducted a Pearson’s correlation analysis where standard scores for each variable were used. As shown in Table 2, all the variables were significantly correlated with each other, p < 0.01. Mindfulness was significantly and positively correlated with attention control (r = 0.40, p < 0.01), while significantly negatively correlated with FOMO and SMA (r = −0.44, p < 0.01; r = −0.52, p < 0.01), respectively. Attention control was significantly negatively correlated with FOMO and SMA (r = −0.37, p < 0.01; r = −0.37, p < 0.01), respectively. FOMO was significantly positively correlated with SMA (r = 0.67, p < 0.01).

Testing the serial mediating effects

Mindfulness, attentional control, FOMO, and social media addiction are all significantly associated, meeting the statistical requirements for a further mediation analysis of mindfulness and SMA (68). With gender, age, place of residence, only-child, and left-behind child as covariates, the mediating role of attention control and FOMO in the relationship between mindfulness and SMA was analyzed by using PROCESS Model 6 in SPSS 26.0 compiled by Hayes (65). The main results are shown in Tables 3, 4. As is shown in Table 3, Mindfulness was a significant predictor of attention control (β = 0.22, SE = 0.03, p < 0.001). Mindfulness and attention control are also significant predictors of FOMO (β = −0.31, SE = 0.04, p < 0.001; β = −0.31, SE = 0.07, p < 0.001). Finally, trait mindfulness and attention control, as well as FOMO, significantly predicted social media addiction (β = −0.30, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001; β = −0.19, SE = 0.08, p = 0.01; β = 0.70, SE = 0.05, p < 0.001). The results of the path coefficient test in the hypothetical model are shown in Figure 2.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression coefficients, standard errors, and model summary information for the influence of mindfulness in a model of social media addiction.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Direct and indirect effects of mindfulness on social media addiction.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Theoretical research model with standard coefficients. Regression coefficients were obtained with gender, age, place of residence, only-child, and left-behind child, in covariates in PROCESS Procedure for SPSS, where *** denotes that p < 0.001, N = 446.

The mediating effect sizes of attention control and FOMO in the relationship between mindfulness and SMA are shown in Table 4. As shown in Table 4; Figure 2, the mediating role between the mindfulness and SMA of attention control and FOMO is significant. The total effect value of mindfulness on SMA was −0.61, and the direct effect value of mindfulness on SMA was −0.29, and the total mediating effect accounted for 51.40% of the total effect. The mediating effect consists of three indirect effects: Path 1: Mindfulness → FOMO → SMA (−0.22), Path 2: Mindfulness → Attention Control → SMA (0.04), and Path 3: Mindfulness → Attention Control → FOMO → Social Media Addiction (−0.05). For pathways 1, 2, and 3, the ratios of the three indirect effects to the total effect were 36.68%, 6.88%, and 7.85%, respectively. All three indirect effects reached a significance level, since CIs of the above indirect effects did not include the zero value. Thus, hypotheses 1, 2, and 3 were confirmed.

The above results suggest that mindfulness not only indirectly predicts SMA through the single mediating effects of attentional control and FOMO, but also indirectly predicts SMA through the serial mediating effects of attentional control and FOMO.

Discussion

Mindfulness might be a protective factor, for college students, from social media addiction under the COVID-19 pandemic. However, psychological mechanisms by which mindfulness may reduce social media use remain unclear. The present study analyzed the relationship between mindfulness and social media addiction among Chinese college students via a serial mediation model. The results of this study suggest that attention control and fear of missing out partially mediate the relationship between mindfulness and social media addiction. Consequently, in this study, we confirmed a direct link between mindfulness and social media addiction in Chinese college students (38). We found that mindfulness, through three paths, influence social media addiction, and that these three paths are: attention control, fear of missing out, and attention control → fear of missing out. These results help us further our understanding of the relationship between mindfulness and social media addiction, and provide theoretical support for future mindfulness interventions for social media addiction.

Firstly, we demonstrated the mediating role of fear of missing out in mindfulness and social media addiction. Specifically, college students with higher levels of mindfulness had lower FOMO in the mobile social media environment, further reducing their possible SMA. According to the mechanisms of mindfulness (40), college students with higher levels of mindfulness might reduce SMA by allowing FOMO to settle, by identifying and accepting it. SMA and FOMO were found to be negatively related to mindfulness in the southeastern United States (26), India (69), and the Middle East (70) in previous researches. Consistently, similar relationships among SMA, FOMO, and mindfulness were also found in Chinese sample in our present study. Moreover, the effect of mindfulness on social media addiction through FOMO was the highest in this study, up to 36.68%. This percentage underscores the role of FOMO in how mindfulness affects the SMA process. This result suggests that reducing FOMO might be an important mechanism by which mindfulness changes social media addiction. However, no effect of a mindfulness intervention on the FOMO in the Middle East sample was found (70). This may be attributed to the effectiveness of mindfulness intervention program design and implementation. Because they did not design mindfulness interventions specifically for FOMO, and implemented them in the form of psychoeducation rather than group psychotherapy. Social Media Mindfulness Practice had been proposed recently (38).

On the other hand, the correlation between FOMO and SMA in this study is greater than that in previous studies (37, 71). This discrepancy may stem from the COVID-19 pandemic, which indicates that the relationship between FOMO and social media addiction might have become stronger under the COVID-19 pandemic. Whether this pathway is dependent on the COVID-19 scenario deserves further exploration in the future.

Secondly, this study found a significant pathway of mindfulness → attention control → fear of missing out on social media addiction. This model suggests that the serial relationship between attention control and fear of missing out mediates the relationship between mindfulness and social media addiction. Studies have shown that increased mindfulness improves attentional function (39, 72). In our results, the negative correlation between attention control and FOMO is similar to previous results (50, 73). That is to say, in general, the higher the mindfulness of college students, the higher their attention control function might be, which might potentially lead to them producing less FOMO, and their reduced FOMO may further reduce their potential social media addiction. Some evidence from neuroscience suggests that selective attention and the activation of its inhibitory attention-related networks are associated with the suppression of emotions (74). We hypothesize that this may be a continuum: i.e., higher levels of mindfulness may improve individuals’ attentional functions such as vigilance, sustained attention, and conflict monitoring, which may help identify some emotional precursors closely related to addictive behaviors and thus reduce the occurrence of addictive behaviors.

In terms of effect size, FOMO alone as a mediating role had the largest effect value, while attention control and FOMO as serial mediating roles has a small effect value. Therefore, the pathway of mindfulness → attention control → fear of missing out on social media addiction appears to play a limited role in mindfulness-based amelioration of social media addiction. On the other hand, these data suggest that mindfulness may reduce fear of missing out through other pathway, such as the non-judgment facet of mindfulness which was strongly and inversely related to negative affect and anxiety (75). Further research is needed in the future.

Finally, we verified the mediating effect of attention control on mindfulness and social media addiction. The results suggest that if college students have higher levels of mindfulness, their ability to control attention may also be higher. Such college students might effectively be able to control their attention, stay away from threatening information related to social media, and consequently reduce possible social media addiction (26). Previous studies have indicated that everyone can be distracted by excessive social media information, which can easily lead to attention fixation on social media information (76). However, a high level of mindfulness enables an individual to spot situations in which attention cannot be taken away in time (16). It seems that college students could also actively improve attention control ability to reduce the risk and behavior of social media addiction through Social Media Mindfulness practice. Because attention to current experiences and states is a core concept of mindfulness and can significantly enhance focused attention, which is one of the components of attention (77). However, the mediating effect size for attention control was small, and such results suggest that there might be more powerful mediating pathways for the effect of mindfulness on social media addiction. Therefore, when we design mindfulness for social media, we should not focus too much on attentional control.

This research has made several theoretical and practical contributions. On the one hand, we revealed that attention control and FOMO mediated the relationship between mindfulness and SMA by three paths, with an emphasis on the mediation of FOMO. On the other hand, our research suggests that mindfulness-based interventions might be an effective intervention to alleviate social media addiction, especially mindfulness-based interventions targeting FOMO.

Limitations

Our study may have some limitations as follows. The first limitation is related to our sample. We only carried out the survey in Guizhou and Hunan provinces in southern China, and therefore, follow-up research should expand the scope of the survey and utilize more representative population data. The proportion of women in this study reached 73.1%. Although we included gender as a covariate to control, future research should avoid imbalances in the gender ratio. Secondly, the present study is a cross-sectional study, only reflecting the degree of correlation between studies. In the future, longitudinal comparison will explain the causal relationship between variables. Thirdly, we only examined the mediating role of attentional control, and other facets of mindfulness will need to be further explored in the future, such as non-judgment, non-reactivity, and acting with awareness. Finally, we did not do any mindfulness intervention, although the conclusions have practical implications, they need to be confirmed by future intervention studies.

Conclusion

Mindfulness was significantly and negatively associated with social media addiction. Attention control and FOMO played mediating roles on the effect of mindfulness on social media addiction in parallel, with a large indirect effect value of FOMO, and a small indirect effect value of attention control. Attention control and FOMO also played mediating roles on the effect of mindfulness on social media addiction serially, but the indirect effect of serial mediating is small. Therefore, special emphasis should be placed on FOMO when designing social media mindfulness interventions.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Guizhou Medical University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

CL and HC designed the study. HC, XM, YL, JL, WY, and JN collected and analyzed the data. CL and HC wrote the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was funded by the Guizhou Medical University start-up fund for doctoral talent (J-[2021]050; J-[2021]051) and the National Natural Science Foundation of China (32260199).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Iwamoto, D, and Chun, H. The emotional impact of social media in higher education. Int J High Educ. (2020) 9:239–47. doi: 10.5430/ijhe.v9n2p239

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Jiang, Y. Problematic social media usage and anxiety among university students during the COVID-19 pandemic: the mediating role of psychological capital and the moderating role of academic burnout. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:612007. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.612007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Malaeb, D, Salameh, P, Barbar, S, Awad, E, Haddad, C, Hallit, R, et al. Problematic social media use and mental health (depression, anxiety, and insomnia) among Lebanese adults: any mediating effect of stress? Perspect Psychiatr Care. (2021) 57:539–49. doi: 10.1111/ppc.12576

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Marino, C, Gini, G, Vieno, A, and Spada, M. The associations between problematic facebook use, psychological distress and well-being among adolescents and young adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2018) 226:274–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Siste, K, Hanafi, E, Sen, L, Christian, H, Adrian Siswidiani, LP, Limawan, AP, et al. The impact of physical distancing and associated factors towards internet addiction among adults in Indonesia during COVID-19 pandemic: a nationwide web-based study. Front Psych. (2020) 11:580977. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.580977

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Cheng, C, Lau, Y, Chan, L, and Luk, J. Prevalence of social media addiction across 32 nations: meta-analysis with subgroup analysis of classification schemes and cultural values. Addict Behav. (2021) 117:106845. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.106845

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Fernandes, B, Biswas, U, Tan-Mansukhani, R, Vallejo, A, and Essau, C. The impact of COVID-19 lockdown on internet use and escapism in adolescents. Revista de Psicologia Clinica Con Niños Y Adolescentes. (2020) 7:59–65. doi: 10.21134/rpcna.2020.mon.2056

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lin, C, Brostrom, A, Griffiths, M, and Pakpour, A. Investigating mediated effects of fear of COVID-19 and COVID-19 misunderstanding in the association between problematic social media use, psychological distress, and insomnia. Internet Interv. (2020) 21:100345. doi: 10.1016/j.invent.2020.100345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Twenge, JM, and Campbell, WK. Media use is linked to lower psychological well-being: evidence from three datasets. Psychiatry Q. (2019) 90:311–31. doi: 10.1007/s11126-019-09630-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Brailovskaia, J, Schillack, H, and Margraf, J. Tell me why are you using social media (SM)! Relationship between reasons for use of SM, SM flow, daily stress, depression, anxiety, and addictive SM use-an exploratory investigation of young adults in Germany. Comput Hum Behav. (2020) 113:106511. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2020.106511

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Twenge, JM, Joiner, TE, Rogers, ML, and Martin, GN. Increases in depressive symptoms, suicide-related outcomes, and suicide rates among US adolescents after 2010 and links to increased new media screen time. Clin Psychol Sci. (2018) 6:3–17. doi: 10.1177/2167702617723376

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. D’Arienzo, M, Boursier, V, and Griffiths, M. Addiction to social media and attachment styles: a systematic literature review. Int J Ment Heal Addict. (2019) 17:1094–118. doi: 10.1007/s11469-019-00082-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Apaolaza, V, Hartmann, P, D’Souza, C, and Gilsanz, A. Mindfulness, compulsive mobile social media use, and derived stress: the mediating roles of self-esteem and social anxiety. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2019) 22:388–96. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2018.0681

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kose, O, and Dogan, A. The relationship between social media addiction and self-esteem among turkish university students. Addicta. (2019) 6:175–90. doi: 10.15805/addicta.2019.6.1.0036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Emin, M. A qualitative study on the reasons for social media addiction. Eurasian J Educ Res. (2018) 7:861–5. doi: 10.12973/eu-jer.7.4.861

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Kabat-Zinn, J. Wherever you go, there you are: Mindfulness meditation in everyday life. New York: Hyperion (1994).

Google Scholar

17. Carmody, J, and Baer, R. Relationships between mindfulness practice and levels of mindfulness, medical and psychological symptoms and well-being in a mindfulness-based stress reduction program. J Behav Med. (2008) 31:23–33. doi: 10.1007/s10865-007-9130-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Garland, E, Schwarz, N, Kelly, A, Whitt, A, and Howard, M. Mindfulness-oriented recovery enhancement for alcohol dependence: therapeutic mechanisms and intervention acceptability. J Soc Work Pract Addict. (2012) 12:242–63. doi: 10.1080/1533256X.2012.702638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kiken, LG, Garland, EL, Bluth, K, Palsson, OS, and Gaylord, SA. From a state to a trait: trajectories of state mindfulness in meditation during intervention predict changes in trait mindfulness. Personal Individ Differ. (2015) 81:41–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2014.12.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Shapiro, SL, and Carlson, LE. The art and science of mindfulness: Integrating mindfulness into psychology and the helping professions. Washington DC: American Psychology Press (2009).

Google Scholar

21. Eşkisu, M, Çam, Z, Gelibolu, S, and Rasmussen, KR. Trait mindfulness as a protective factor in connections between psychological issues and Facebook addiction among Turkish university students. Stud Psychol. (2020) 62:213–31. doi: 10.31577/sp.2020.03.801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Hassan, Y, and Pandey, J. Demystifying the dark side of social networking sites through mindfulness. Australas J Inf Syst. (2021) 25:1–33. doi: 10.3127/ajis.v25i0.2923

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Mazzoni, E, Cannata, D, and Baiocco, L. Focused, not lost: the mediating role of temporal dissociation and focused immersion on problematic internet use. Behav Inform Technol. (2017) 36:1–10. doi: 10.1080/0144929X.2016.1159249

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Sriwilai, K, and Charoensukmongkol, P. Face it, don’t Facebook it: impacts of social media addiction on mindfulness, coping strategies and the consequence on emotional exhaustion. Stress Health. (2016) 32:427–34. doi: 10.1002/smi.2637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kircaburun, K, Griffiths, MD, and Billieux, J. Trait emotional intelligence and problematic online behaviors among adolescents: the mediating role of mindfulness, rumination, and depression. Personal Individ Differ. (2019) 139:208–13. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.11.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Weaver, JL, and Swank, JM. An examination of college students’ social media use, fear of missing out, and mindful attention. J Coll Couns. (2021) 24:132–45. doi: 10.1002/jocc.12181

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Chi, X, Liu, X, Guo, T, Wu, M, and Chen, X. Internet addiction and depression in Chinese adolescents: a moderated mediation model. Front Psych. (2019) 10:816. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00816

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Majeed, M, Irshad, M, Fatima, T, Khan, J, and Hassan, MM. Relationship between problematic social media usage and employee depression: a moderated mediation model of mindfulness and fear of COVID-19. Front Psychol. (2020) 11:557987. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.557987

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Turel, O, and Osatuyi, B. A peer-influence perspective on compulsive social networking site use: trait mindfulness as a double-edged sword. Comput Hum Behav. (2017) 77:47–53. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.08.022

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Przybylski, AK, Murayama, K, DeHaan, CR, and Gladwell, V. Motivational, emotional, and behavioral correlates of fear of missing out. Comput Hum Behav. (2013) 29:1841–8. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.02.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Zhao, X, Hu, T, Qiao, G, Li, C, Wu, M, Yang, F, et al. Psychometric properties of the smartphone distraction scale in Chinese college students: validity, reliability and influencing factors. Front. Psychiatry, (2022) 13:e859640. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.859640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Alt, D. College students’ academic motivation, media engagement and fear of missing out. Comput Hum Behav. (2015) 49:111–9. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2015.02.057

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Buglass, S, Binder, J, Betts, L, and Underwood, J. Motivators of online vulnerability: the impact of social network site use and FOMO. Comput Hum Behav. (2017) 66:248–55. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.09.055

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Beyens, I, Frison, E, and Eggermont, S. “I don’t want to miss a thing”: adolescents’ fear of missing out and its relationship to adolescents’ social needs, Facebook use, and Facebook related stress. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 64:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.05.083

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Fabris, M, Marengo, D, Longobardi, C, and Settanni, M. Investigating the links between fear of missing out, social media addiction, and emotional symptoms in adolescence: the role of stress associated with neglect and negative reactions on social media. Addict Behav. (2020) 106:106364. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Milyavskaya, M, Saffran, M, Hope, N, and Koestner, R. Fear of missing out: prevalence, dynamics, and consequences of experiencing FOMO. Motiv Emot. (2018) 42:725–37. doi: 10.1007/s11031-018-9683-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Baker, ZG, Krieger, H, and LeRoy, AS. Fear of missing out: relationships with depression, mindfulness, and physical symptoms. Translat Iss Psychol Sci. (2016) 2:275–82. doi: 10.1037/tps0000075

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Chan, S, Van Solt, M, Cruz, R, Philp, M, Bahl, S, Serin, N, et al. Social media and mindfulness: from the fear of missing out (FOMO) to the joy of missing out (JOMO). J Consum Aff. (2022) 56:1312–31. doi: 10.1111/joca.12476

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Jha, A, Krompinger, J, and Baime, M. Mindfulness training modifies subsystems of attention. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2007) 7:109–19. doi: 10.3758/CABN.7.2.109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Shapiro, S, Carlson, L, Astin, J, and Freedman, B. Mechanisms of mindfulness. J Clin Psychol. (2006) 62:373–86. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Lee, CSC, Ma, MT, Ho, HY, Tsang, KK, Zheng, YY, and Wu, ZY. The effectiveness of mindfulness-based intervention in attention on individuals with ADHD: a systematic review. Hong Kong J Occup Ther. (2017) 30:33–41. doi: 10.1016/j.hkjot.2017.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Becerra, R, Dandrade, C, and Harms, C. Can specific attentional skills be modified with mindfulness training for novice practitioners? Curr Psychol. (2017) 36:657–64. doi: 10.1007/s12144-016-9454-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Gross, JJ. (ed.). “Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations,” in Handbook of Emotion Regulation. New York: Guilford Press (2014). 3–20.

Google Scholar

44. Najmi, S, Amir, N, Frosio, KE, and Ayers, C. The effects of cognitive load on attention control in subclinical anxiety and generalised anxiety disorder. Cognit Emot. (2015) 29:1210–23. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2014.975188

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Derryberry, D. Attention and voluntary self-control. Self Identity. (2002) 1:105–11. doi: 10.1080/152988602317319276

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Thomson, K, Hunter, SC, Butler, SH, and Robertson, DJ. Social media ‘addiction’: the absence of an attentional bias to social media stimuli. J Behav Addict. (2021) 10:302–13. doi: 10.1556/2006.2021.00011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Yilbas, B, and Karadeniz, P. The relationship between chronotype and impulsivity, attention-deficit disorder, internet, social media, and smartphone addiction. Alpha Psychiatry. (2022) 23:203–9. doi: 10.5152/alphapsychiatry.2022.21656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Boer, M, Stevens, G, Finkenauer, C, and van den Eijnden, R. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder-symptoms, social media use intensity, and social media use problems in adolescents: investigating directionality. Child Dev. (2020) 91:e853–65. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. He, Z, Li, M, Liu, C, and Ma, X. Common predictive factors of social media addiction and eating disorder symptoms in female college students: state anxiety and the mediating role of cognitive flexibility/sustained attention. Front Psychol. (2022) 12:647126. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.647126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Al-Furaih, S, and Al-Awidi, H. Fear of missing out (FoMO) among undergraduate students in relation to attention distraction and learning disengagement in lectures. Educ Inf Technol. (2021) 26:2355–73. doi: 10.1007/s10639-020-10361-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Eysenck, M, Derakshan, N, Santos, R, and Calvo, M. Anxiety and cognitive performance: attentional control theory. Emotion. (2007) 7:336–53. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.2.336

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Williams, J, Mathews, A, and MacLeod, C. The emotional stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol Bull. (1996) 120:3–24. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.120.1.3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Zheng, A, Duff, BRL, Vargas, P, and Yao, MZ. Self-presentation on social media: when self-enhancement confronts self-verification. J Interact Advert. (2020) 20:289–302. doi: 10.1080/15252019.2020.1841048

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Brand, M, Young, KS, Laier, C, Wölfling, K, and Potenza, MN. Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific internet-use disorders: an interaction of person-affect-cognition-execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 71:252–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Turel, O, and Qahri-Saremi, H. Problematic use of social networking sites: antecedents and consequence from a dual-system theory perspective. J Manag Inf Syst. (2016) 33:1087–116. doi: 10.1080/07421222.2016.1267529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Turel, O, and Qahri-Saremi, H. Explaining unplanned online media behaviors: dual system theory models of impulsive use and swearing on social networking sites. New Media Soc. (2018) 20:3050–67. doi: 10.1177/1461444817740755

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Curran, PG. Methods for the detection of carelessly invalid responses in survey data. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2016) 66:4–19. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2015.07.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Andreassen, CS, Pallesen, S, and Griffiths, MD. The relationship between addictive use of social media, narcissism, and self-esteem: findings from a large national survey. Addict Behav. (2017) 64:287–93. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2016.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Hou, Y, Xiong, D, Jiang, T, Song, L, and Wang, Q. Social media addiction: its impact, mediation, and intervention. Cyberpsychology. (2019) 13:4. doi: 10.5817/CP2019-1-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Brown, K, and Ryan, R. The benefits of being present: mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2003) 84:822–48. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.84.4.822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Deng, Y-Q, Li, S, Tang, Y-Y, Zhu, L-H, Ryan, R, and Brown, K. Psychometric properties of the Chinese translation of the mindful attention awareness scale (MAAS). Mindfulness. (2012) 3:10–4. doi: 10.1007/s12671-011-0074-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Derryberry, D, and Reed, M. Anxiety-related attentional biases and their regulation by attentional control. J Abnorm Psychol. (2002) 111:225–36. doi: 10.1037//0021-843X.111.2.225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. He, SY. (2020). Attentional control scale factor structure and empirical validity. Master’s thesis. Hunan: Hunan Normal University.

Google Scholar

64. Song Xiaokang, ZY. Developing a fear of missing out (FoMO) measurement scale in the mobile social media environment. Library Informat Serv. (2017) 61:96. doi: 10.13266/j.issn.0252-3116.2017.11.012

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Hayes, AF. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis: A regression-based approach. New York: Guilford Press (2017).

Google Scholar

66. Harman, HH. Modern factor analysi.: Chicago: University of Chicago press (1976).

Google Scholar

67. Podsakoff, PM, MacKenzie, SB, Lee, J-Y, and Podsakoff, NP. Common method biases in behavioral research: a critical review of the literature and recommended remedies. J Appl Psychol. (2003) 88:879–903. doi: 10.1037/0021-9010.88.5.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Wen, Z, and Ye, B. Mediating effect analysis: method and model development. Adv Psychol Sci. (2014) 22:731–45. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2014.00731

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Vp, N, and Veronica, N. The relationship between fear of missing out, mindfulness, and social media usage among adolescence. Int J Indian Psychol. (2020) 8:1948–2960. doi: 10.25215/0803.203

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. O'Connell, C. How FOMO (fear of missing out), the smartphone, and social media may be affecting university students in the middle east. N Am J Psychol. (2020) 22:83–102.

Google Scholar

71. Yin, L, Wang, P, Nie, J, Guo, J, Feng, J, and Lei, L. Social networking sites addiction and FoMO: the mediating role of envy and the moderating role of need to belong. Curr Psychol. (2021) 40:3879–87. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00344-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Chambers, R, Lo, BCY, and Allen, NB. The impact of intensive mindfulness training on attentional control, cognitive style, and affect. Cogn Ther Res. (2008) 32:303–22. doi: 10.1007/s10608-007-9119-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Li, J, Xue, W, Zhao, J, and Tan, L. Cognitive bias and fear of missing out (FOMO) among Chinese college students: the mediating effects of attentional control, need to belong and self-construal. Curr Psychol. (2022) 1-10:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s12144-022-03435-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Schuman-Olivier, Z, Trombka, M, Lovas, DA, Brewer, JA, Vago, DR, Gawande, R, et al. Mindfulness and behavior change. Harv Rev Psychiatry. (2020) 28:371–94. doi: 10.1097/HRP.0000000000000277

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Medvedev, ON, Cervin, M, Barcaccia, B, Siegert, RJ, Roemer, A, and Krägeloh, CU. Network analysis of mindfulness facets, affect, compassion, and distress. Mindfulness. (2021) 12:911–22. doi: 10.1007/s12671-020-01555-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Koessmeier, C, and Büttner, OB. Why are we distracted by social media? Distraction situations and strategies, reasons for distraction, and individual differences. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:711416. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.711416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Bishop, SR, Lau, M, Shapiro, S, Carlson, L, Anderson, ND, Carmody, J, et al. Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. (2004) 11:230–41. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bph077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: mindfulness, social media addiction, attention control, fear of missing out, college students

Citation: Chang H, Meng X, Li Y, Liu J, Yuan W, Ni J and Li C (2023) The effect of mindfulness on social media addiction among Chinese college students: A serial mediation model. Front. Psychiatry. 14:1087909. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1087909

Received: 02 November 2022; Accepted: 02 March 2023;
Published: 23 March 2023.

Edited by:

Lu Yu, Hong Kong Polytechnic University, Hong Kong SAR, China

Reviewed by:

Elliot Benjamin, Capella University, United States
Wahyu Rahardjo, Gunadarma University, Indonesia

Copyright © 2023 Chang, Meng, Li, Liu, Yuan, Ni and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chunlu Li, bGlndWFuMTk4MEAxNjMuY29t

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.