Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 24 January 2023
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Causes and Consequences of Solitude in Children and Adolescents View all 10 articles

Unsociability and social adjustment of Chinese preschool migrant children: The moderating role of resilience

\r\nJingjing ZhuJingjing Zhu1Zhenzhen ZhangZhenzhen Zhang1Pin XuPin Xu2Kaiyu HuangKaiyu Huang3Yan Li*Yan Li1*
  • 1Early Childhood Education College, Shanghai Normal University, Shanghai, China
  • 2Hongkou District Education College, Shanghai, China
  • 3Qingpu District Education College, Shanghai, China

Objectives: The present study examined the moderating effect of children’s resilience on the relations between unsociability and social adjustment (i.e., prosocial behaviors, peer exclusion, interpersonal skills, internalizing problems) in Chinese preschool migrant children.

Methods: Participants were N = 148 children (82 boys, Mage = 62.32 months, SD = 6.76) attending two public kindergartens in Shanghai, People’s Republic of China. Mothers provided ratings of children’s unsociability and resilience; teachers assessed children’s social adjustment outcomes, and children reported their receptive vocabulary.

Results: Unsociability was positively associated with peer exclusion and internalizing problems, and negatively associated with prosocial behaviors and interpersonal skills among Chinese preschool migrant children. Moreover, children’s resilience significantly moderated the relationship between unsociability and social adjustment. Specifically, among children with lower levels of resilience, unsociability was significantly and positively associated with peer exclusion and internalizing problems, while among children with higher levels of resilience, unsociability was not associated with social adjustment difficulties.

Conclusion: The current findings inform us of the importance of improving children’s resilience to buffer the negative adjustment among Chinese migrant unsociable young children. The findings also highlight the importance of considering the meaning and implication of unsociability for preschool migrant children in Chinese culture.

Introduction

In early childhood, peer relationship plays an essential and unique role to children’s social, emotional, and cognitive development (1). Children who do not engage in peer interactions (e.g., socially withdrawn children) may miss out on the potential benefits of interactive experiences, peer support, and other benefits that come from such social situations, which cause a host of behavior problems and adjustment difficulties, exposing those children to additional psychological developmental risks and dilemmas (2). Social withdrawal is defined as the behavior of children who inhibit themselves from participating in peer interactions and exhibit solitary pastimes in social contexts (2, 3). According to the conceptual model proposed by Asendorpf, children’s social withdrawal behavior is determined by a combination of social approach motivations (i.e., desire to seek social interactions) and social avoidance motivations (i.e., desire to avoid social interactions), which can be further classified as shyness, unsociability, and social avoidance (4, 5). Unsociability and shyness are based on different levels of internal approach and avoidance motivations in challenging social situations (6). Shyness is characterized by approach-avoidance motivation conflict, in which shy children want to play with their peers but are simultaneously inhibited by fear and anxiety (5). In contrast, unsociability is characterized as reflecting a combination of low social approach motivation and low social avoidance motivation, referring to behaviors in which children are not interested in social activities and do not actively resist the interaction of others (5). However, shyness and unsociability are positively associated with withdrawal behaviors, as well as negatively associated with social initiations and psychological engagement during peer interactions (7). Shyness and unsociability are characterized as related but distinct constructs and, therefore, it has become common practice to control for any shared variance in order to explore their unique effects and implications (7).

Unlike shyness, unsociability has been considered a relatively benign form of social withdrawal, particularly in early childhood, this might be due to the endorsement of preferred solitary behavior in individualistic societies (7, 8). In support of this notion, results from a study among young children suggested that unsociability is not necessarily associated with perceived social and physiological adjustment (i.e., peer problems, internalizing problems, social anxiety) (7). Because collectivistic cultures society such as China places a high emphasis on social interdependence and group affiliation, parents teach children to form a sense of family affiliation and responsibility from an early age (9). In this regard, unsociable children who pursue their preferences rather than stay in groups may be considered anti-collective and selfish, which may cause adverse reactions from others and increase their risk of negative social adjustment (10). For example, results from previous studies showed that unsociability in Chinese adolescents and preschool children was uniquely associated with peer problems (i.e., peer victimization, peer exclusion), internalizing problems (i.e., loneliness, depression), and academic problems (11, 12).

In recent years, with the accelerated development of the economy and urbanization process in China, rural-to-urban migration has gradually become one of the most salient contextual factors shaping Chinese family life in the 21st century (13). According to the “Annual Report on China’s Education for Migrant Children (2019–2020),” the number of migrant children under 17 years old in China was 34.26 million, among which the size of preschool migrant children under 5 years old had reached 10.53 million, which is accounting for 30.74% of the total number of migrant children, ranking first among all age groups and the most significant increase (14). Furthermore, compared to school-age migrant children, preschool migrant children face enormous challenges, such as the upbringing setting transition from family to kindergarten, the rapid development period of physics and physiology, as well as the adaptation of increasingly complex external environment, which all have caused certain impacts and challenges to cognitive development and interpersonal interactions (15). Thus, we cannot ignore the social adjustment of preschool migrant children.

To our knowledge, the relations between Chinese children’s migrant experience and psychological well-being have been extensive study in the past decade. For example, previous studies indicated that compared with non-migrant children, migrant children are more likely to experience higher prevalence of mental health problems, including depression, social anxiety, and behavior problems (16, 17). Both residential and school mobility represent significant ecological transitions for children, which is a major challenge for the positive adjustment of migrant children. The migrant children’s social adjustment is also influenced by the hukou system, which allocates residency rights to the birth population, linking their rights and benefits to their hukou status and location. Previous evidence revealed that the preservation of the current hukou system of household registration might expose urban migrants to unfair maltreatment and make migrants a vulnerable social group (18). Because migrant parents and children do not have legal registration status in the city. As a result, migrant children do not have the same privileges as urban children, which do not have access to the same quality of education in public schools. In such cases, migrant children may not be able to access stable support from the city, which perhaps leads to their social maladjustment. In addition, it has been found that the screen rate for social withdrawal is higher in migrant children than in non-migrant children in China (19). Thus, migrant children may be more likely to exhibit unsociable behavior. Accordingly, in the present study, we sought to examine the relations of unsociability with social adjustment in Chinese migrant children. To our knowledge, the only existing study related to this topic is the one conducted by Ding et al. (20) on relations between unsociability and adjustment in migrant children in China. The researchers found in that study that unsociability is more evidently associated with social adjustment problems in Chinese migrant children than in non-migrant children.

Notwithstanding, not all unsociable children undergo social maladjustment, implying that some potential risk or protective factors may affect adjustment outcomes for withdrawn children in China (21, 22). The previous study has found that some migrant children follow a “disadvantaged—resilient—well-developed” trajectory, resulting in positive physiological adjustment (23). Resilience, as “the process of, capacity for, or outcome of successful adjustment despite challenging or threatening circumstances,” is regarded as an important protective factor for emotional and behavior problems of children (24). Accordingly, we would focus on unsociability in the current study, which has drawn noticeably less consideration than shyness among Chinese young children (12). More precisely, we explored the potential moderating role of resilience in relations of unsociability with social adjustment in a sample of preschool migrant children in China.

Unsociability and social adjustment

Cultural contexts play an important role in the development of children’s social behavior and adjustment functioning (9). The attitudes of peers, parents and teachers to specific social behaviors vary across cultures, which influence children’s social, emotional, and school adjustments (25). In western societies, unsociability does not imply low social skills and is sometimes viewed as an expression of personal choice, autonomy, or self-oriented action (8). Thus, unsociability is considered to be a relatively benign form of social withdrawal. In this regard, previous evidence consistently revealed that unsociability is not associated with internalizing problems, peer problems, and social anxiety in children from early childhood through early adolescence (7, 26), and peers also report greater acceptance of unsociable children than shy peers (27). However, the Chinese Confucian culture emphasizes children’s obedience to the expectations and standards of authoritative parents, but unsociable behavior is notably characterized by solitary action, and unsociable children do not inhibit their willingness to continue to indulge in solitary play because of the needs of the surrounding environment, which is clearly at odds with the demands of a collectivist culture (28). Thus, unsociability is more likely to cause negative outcomes on children’s social, psychological, and school adjustment in Chinese society. For example, Liu et al. found that unsociability was associated with adjustment difficulties more strongly in Chinese children than in their Canadian counterparts (29). Furthermore, in a sample of Chinese early adolescents, research evidence suggested that unsociability was associated with peer difficulties, school maladjustment, and internalizing problems (11, 30), even in early childhood (12, 31).

Given that unsociable children experience increased adjustment difficulties, not to mention preschool migrant children in China, it is crucial to identify factors that “protect” or “buffer” against negative outcomes. One area of interest involves the role of resilience, which has been found to alleviate children’s internalizing problems, peer problems, and prosocial behaviors (32). Additionally, previous evidence also indicated that higher levels of resilience help reduce social withdrawal behaviors among Chinese left-behind preschool children (33), but the effect on unsociability remains unclear. While previous study has identified the protective influence of resilience on social adjustment among Chinese children, little research examined the resilience in moderating the relations between unsociability and social adjustment, particularly among rural-to-urban migrant preschoolers in China. Therefore, this study would examine the relations between unsociability and social maladjustment and further explore the moderating role of resilience in these relations among Chinese migrant preschoolers.

Resilience: A moderator

Resilience is defined as an individual’s flexibility to cope with different difficulties and challenges in life and the ability to recover from adversities and misfortunes (34). Miller found that among students with learning disabilities, compared to non-resilience, those with higher levels of resilience could gain successful experiences, begin to identify areas of strength, and engage in activities with peers (35). This further suggested that when adverse situations or risk factors hinder children’s development, children’s resilience could serve as a protective factor, stimulating their inherent qualities, reducing the harmful effects of adverse situations on children, and making them more adaptable (36). Moreover, resilience has been considered an essential element in positive psychology, and it has been found to be associated with children’s subjective well-being and positive cognitions during childhood (37, 38). For example, children with higher levels of resilience perceive less distress and stress, and are less likely to suffer from anxiety and depression (39). In conclusion, resilience can contribute to positive developmental outcomes for disadvantaged children.

As discussed above, because of the increased risks faced by migrant children, there is a need to identify protective factors that can help them successfully adapt to new environments, and manage the accompanying psychological and adjustment challenges that acculturation encompasses. According to the protective factor model, when certain positive internal characteristics are present, individuals will have an immunity to stress, which reduces the negative impact of stress on the individual’s adjustment functioning (40). Thus, resilience could act as a positive inner characteristic that reduces or counteracts the negative impact of risk factors on individual development outcomes. Previous studies found that the protective effects of resilience for disadvantaged children (4143). For example, Martinez-Torteya et al. (41) have found that in early childhood, resilience played a protective role in maintaining positive adaptive and easy temperamental characteristics in children exposed to domestic violence, compared to their non-resilient counterparts (41). Furthermore, previous research suggested that resilient children enable positive adaptation despite maltreatment (44). Sattler and Gershoff also found that children in poverty who reached higher levels of resilience at entry to kindergarten exhibit similar academic achievement throughout elementary school as children not in poverty (45). Similarly, the findings were found in a study sample of Chinese children. For example, Fan and Fan found that higher levels of resilience could reduce the psychological adjustment difficulties (e.g., depression, loneliness, self-esteem) among Chinese left-behind children (42). They identified resilience as a quality necessary for the growth of children in adversity. It has been found that resilience moderated the relationship between peer victimization and depression among migrant school-age children in China (43). Specifically, the negative impact of peer victimization on depressive symptoms decreased with the increased levels of resilience.

In conclusion, most of the existing literature generally points to the positive development of disadvantaged children from a resilience perspective, and little work has yet examined the moderating role of resilience in the relationship between unsociability and social adjustment among unsociable migrant preschoolers in the context of Chinese culture.

The present study

As mentioned above, unsociability is incompatible with the traditional Chinese values of social interdependence and group affiliation (10). As such, unsociability has been found to cause a range of children’s adjustment difficulties (12, 31), and it could be inferred that migrant unsociable children would face greater adjustment difficulties. According to the stress-buffer model (46, 47), although migrant unsociable children experience double distress of life environment and psychological problems, considering resilience as the potential quality for children could buffer the effects of “migration” and “unsociability,” then they would show the positive social adjustment. Indeed, existing studies have indicated that resilience could reduce migrant children’s adjustment problems (23, 32, 48). Previous studies have focused on the impact of relative unsociability and social adjustment difficulties among Chinese non-migrant children (12, 31). However, the underlying mechanism of resilience in the relations between unsociability and social adjustment is unclear among Chinese preschool migrant children (20). Furthermore, previous research has found that compared to unsociable girls, unsociable boys would be less accepted by their peers, experience more peer problems, and have poorer quality of friendships (49). Thus, we examined the gender differences in the subsequent analysis. In addition, children’s social communicative competence plays a vital role in children’s peer experiences (50). It has been indicated that children’s receptive vocabulary is associated with socioemotional adjustment (51). Therefore, it is critical to control for migrant children’s receptive vocabulary in the study.

In summary, the primary purpose of the present study was to examine the moderating role of resilience between unsociability and social adjustment among preschool migrant children in China. To be consistent with the previous research (e.g., 11, 12), we focused on four main aspects of social adjustment: prosocial behaviors, peer exclusion, interpersonal skills, and internalizing problems. Based on the extant literature, we expected that unsociability was significantly associated with adjustment difficulties. Furthermore, it was hypothesized that resilience would moderate the relations between unsociability and social adjustment. While at higher levels of resilience, it would serve to buffer unsociable migrant preschoolers from experiencing social maladjustment (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The hypotheses model.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants were N = 148 preschool migrant children (82 boys, Mage = 62.32 months, SD = 6.76) recruited from two public kindergartens in Shanghai, People’s Republic of China. All children were of Han ethnicity, which represents over 97% of China’s population.

Nearly 22% of the mothers and 24% of the fathers had completed high school; 40% of the mothers and 27% of the fathers had completed junior college; 35% of the mothers and 41% of the fathers had earned a bachelor’s degree, and 3% of the mothers and 8% of the fathers had earned a postgraduate degree. Maternal and paternal scores were averaged to create a broader measure of parental education (with higher scores representing higher education).

Procedure

The present study was reviewed and approved by the ethics review board of Shanghai Normal University. Written consent was obtained from parents of all migrant preschoolers. The participation rate was 98%. Mothers rated their children’s unsociability and resilience. During children’s testing sessions, we assessed children’s receptive vocabulary. Teachers completed measures of children’s social adjustment.

Measures

Maternal ratings

Mothers completed the Chinese version of Child Social Preference Scale (CSPS) (31, 52). Of particular interest was the subscale assessing unsociability, which comprises 4 items (e.g., “My child is just as happy to play quietly by his/herself than to play with a group of children,” α = 0.65). Given the common conceptual overlap and similar patterns of adjustment among Chinese youth, it is important to control for any common variation with shyness when exploring the implications of unsociability among Chinese migrant children (29, 52). As such, mothers also completed the shyness subscale, which comprises 7 items (e.g., “My child seems to want to play with other children, but is sometimes nervous to,” α = 0.86). Items were rated on a five-point scale (from 1 = “not at all” to 5 = “a lot”). These items were aggregated to create the unsociability and shyness score, with higher scores indicating higher levels of unsociability and shyness.

Mothers also completed the Chinese version of Ego-Resiliency Scale (ERS) (53). The ERS scale comprises 11 items (e.g., “Freezes up when things are stressful, or else keeps doing the same thing over and over again”; α = 0.89). Items were rated on a nine-point scale (from 1 = “not at all” to nine = “a lot”), with higher scores indicating higher levels of resilience.

Teacher ratings

Teachers were asked to completed the Chinese version of Child Behavior Scale (CBS) (54, 55). Of particular interest were subscales assessing prosocial behaviors (6 items, e.g., “Help other children”; α = 0.90) and peer exclusion (7 items, e.g., “Not welcomed by other children”; α = 0.86). Items were rated on a three-point scale (from 1 = “doesn’t apply” to 3 = “certainly applies”), with higher scores indicating higher levels of prosocial behaviors and peer exclusion. The CBS has been shown to be reliable and valid in young Chinese children (55).

Teachers also completed the Chinese version of Social Skills Teacher Rating System (SSTRS) (56, 57). We were particularly interested in the subscales assessing interpersonal skills (11 items, e.g., “Make friends easily”; α = 0.90) and internalizing problems (4 items, e.g., “Looks lonely”; α = 0.66). Items were rated on a three-point scale (from 0 = “never” to 2 = “always”), with higher scores indicating higher levels of interpersonal skills and internalizing problems. The SSTRS has been shown to be reliable and valid in young Chinese children (57).

Children assessments

Children’ receptive vocabulary was assessed using the Chinese version of the Peabody Pictures Vocabulary Test (PPVT-III) (58). The scale consists of 204-items, for every item include four pictures, a picture was shown on a quadrant, and the child was asked to identify the picture that best fit the word that was read to the child. Testing took place in blocks of eight items. If a child made more than six errors within a single block of eight items, testing was discontinued. The final receptive-vocabulary scores (range from 0 to 204) were computed by subtracting all the incorrect and missed answers from the total number of items (i.e., 204), and with higher scores indicating higher levels of receptive-language skills (59). The PPVT-III has been shown to be reliable and valid in Chinese children (12).

Statistical analysis

We used SPSS 26.0 software for data analysis. Preliminary analyses included a series of t-tests to explore gender differences and correlations among study variables. Next, we used the PROCESS macro (Model 1) with non-parametric bootstrapping with 1,000 resamples to explore moderating effect (60). The significant effects were probed with a 95% bias-corrected confidence interval (CI) (61). Finally, the Johnson-Neyman (J-N) technique was used to probe significant interactions (62), as suggested by other researchers (49).

Results

Preliminary analyses

Descriptive statistics and correlations for all study variables are displayed in Table 1. The results of the t-tests indicated that there were significant gender differences in prosocial behaviors (Mboy = 2.24, SD = 0.57; Mgirl = 2.45, SD = 0.52, t = −2.27, p = 0.02), peer exclusion (Mboy = 1.23, SD = 0.43; Mgirl = 1.07, SD = 0.20, t = 2.72, p = 0.007), and interpersonal skills (Mboy = 1.36, SD = 0.44; Mgirl = 1.57, SD = 0.36, t = −3.11, p = 0.002). There were no gender differences in internalizing problems (Mboy = 0.23, SD = 0.33; Mgirl = 0.14, SD = 0.25, t = 1.66, p = 0.10), resilience (Mboy = 6.36, SD = 1.02; Mgirl = 6.62, SD = 1.02, t = −1.55, p = 0.12), and unsociability (Mboy = 1.76, SD = 0.57; Mgirl = 1.67, SD = 0.58, t = 0.93, p = 0.35).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and inter-correlations for all study variables (N = 148).

As indicated in Table 1, Unsociability was significantly and positively associated with peer exclusion and internalizing problems (marginal significant), and was significantly and negatively associated with prosocial behaviors and interpersonal skills. Resilience was not significantly associated with indices of social adjustment. Children’s age was significantly and positively associated with prosocial behaviors and interpersonal skills. Children’s receptive vocabulary was significantly and positively associated with prosocial behaviors and interpersonal skills, and significantly and negatively associated with unsociability, peer exclusion, and internalizing problems. Parental education was significantly and positively associated with resilience. Accordingly, we controlled for child gender, age, receptive vocabulary, parental education, shyness in the subsequently analyses.

Unsociability, resilience, and social adjustment

The primary goal of current study was to examine the moderating effects of resilience in the relations between unsociability and social adjustment among preschool migrant children. To accomplish this goal, we tested the effects of unsociability and resilience (and their interaction) in relation to the outcome variables (i.e., prosocial behaviors, peer exclusion, interpersonal skills, internalizing problems), while controlling for children’s gender,1 age, receptive vocabulary, shyness, and parental education. Analyses were conducted using the SPSS macro PROCESS (60). Results are displayed in Table 2. These findings were largely consistent with the correlational analyses (despite the additional control variables). Of particular interest, there were significant interaction effects between unsociability and resilience were also found to be related to peer exclusion and internalizing problems.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Main and moderating effects of unsociability and resilience on indices of social adjustment.

Following suggestions by Hayes and Matthes (63), we used the Johnson–Neyman (J-N) technique to further probe the significant interactions (all the predictors were standardized for the analyses) (62). This technique allowed us to estimate a region of significance for the simple slope of a predictor conditioned on the value of the continuous moderator. The results found that for the prediction of peer exclusion and internalizing problems (see Figures 2, 3), when resilience level was lower than −0.46 SD and 1.22 SD, separately, unsociability was significantly and positively associated with peer exclusion and internalizing problems. However, when resilience level was higher than −0.46 SD and 1.22 SD, separately, unsociability was no longer associated with peer exclusion and internalizing problems.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Johnson–Neyman regions of significance and confidence bands for mother-rated unsociability along resilience in relation to peer exclusion. Solid diagonal line represents the regression coefficient for unsociability along resilience. Dashed diagonal blue lines are confidence bands–upper and lower bounds of 95% confidence interval for unsociability regression coefficient along resilience. The vertical blue line indicates the point along resilience at which the unsociability regression coefficient transitions from statistical significance (left of dashed vertical line) to non-significance (right of dashed vertical line). The value of the dashed vertical line is −0.46.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Johnson–Neyman regions of significance and confidence bands for mother-rated unsociability along resilience in relation to internalizing problems. Solid diagonal line represents the regression coefficient for unsociability along resilience. Dashed diagonal blue lines are confidence bands–upper and lower bounds of 95% confidence interval for unsociability regression coefficient along resilience. The vertical blue line indicates the point along resilience at which the unsociability regression coefficient transitions from non-significance (left of dashed vertical line) to statistical significance (right of dashed vertical line). The value of the dashed vertical line is 1.22.

Discussion

The goal of the present study was to explore the relations between unsociability and social adjustment and the moderating role of resilience in a sample of preschool migrant children in China. Our results indicated that unsociability was associated with prosocial behaviors, interpersonal skills, peer exclusion, and internalizing problems. Additionally, resilience could be viewed as a protective factor that buffers the negative effects of unsociability on peer exclusion and internalizing problems. To our knowledge, this was the first study revealing the moderate role of resilience in the relations of unsociability with social adjustment, which constitutes significant contribution to our understanding of the mechanism between social behaviors and adjustment in different contexts.

Association between unsociability and social adjustment

Results from the current study suggested that unsociability was significantly and negatively associated with prosocial behaviors and interpersonal skills, and significantly and positively associated with peer exclusion and internalizing problems (marginal significance) among Chinese preschool migrant children. The findings were similar to the results of Liu et al. (11), who reported that unsociability in Chinese non-migrant children and adolescents was significantly related to peer preference, peer victimization, internalizing problems, learning problems, and loneliness (11). Zhu et al. (12) also reported that unsociability was related to peer exclusion, asocial behaviors, and anxious-fearful among Chinese preschool non-migrant children (12). However, as indicated by recent research, among non-migrant preschoolers in urban settings, unsociability was unrelated to interpersonal skills (64). For migrant children, the life changes and stressful events caused by migrating from rural-to-urban areas may expose children to challenges that affect various aspects of their development, such as their family relationships, peer relationships, and social competence. This study also supported the argument that compared with non-migrant unsociable preschoolers, migrant unsociable preschoolers have poor social skills and may suffer more social adjustment difficulties.

Chinese society appears to be in flux. It has been argued that social change has positively impacted the valuation of unsociability, shifting attitudes closer to Western society’s prevalent view (28). However, Chinese culture emphasizes interdependence and maintaining group harmonization (3). The solitary, non-interacting behaviors have been perceived as undesirable for social regulation and valuation, contrary to the goal of social integration. Therefore, unsociable children are more likely to be negatively evaluated by peers, parents, and teachers, which affects children’s social, emotional, and school adjustment (10). In particular, previous evidence revealed that traditional Chinese values may be maintained to a greater extent in families and children with a rural background in China (20). Accordingly, the conflicts between social change and cultural traditions constitutes a more undesirable environment for unsociable children in social adjustment. Overall, unsociability was associated with social adjustment problems in migrant unsociable children in the present study. The results suggested that migrant unsociable preschoolers in urban settings are more likely to experience adjustment difficulties.

Moderating effect of resilience

To be consistent with our hypothesis, this study further verified the moderating role of resilience. After controlling for gender, age, receptive language, parental education, and shyness, the results indicated that resilience moderated the association between unsociability and peer exclusion and internalizing problems in Chinese migrant preschool children. Specifically, unsociability had a significant negative predictive effect on peer exclusion and internalizing problems when children reported lower levels of resilience. However, that unsociability had a non-significant predictive effect on peer exclusion and internalizing problems when migrant children reported higher levels of resilience.

Such results support the protective factor model of resilience, which suggests that resilience serves as an individual protective characteristic that could buffer the deleterious effects of personal and/or contextual factors (40). In this regard, resilience, which refers to the capacity of individuals to sustain competent functioning or cope successfully with significant change, adversity, or risk may play a vital role in helping unsociable migrant preschoolers to reduce their risk of social maladjustment (34, 43). Accordingly, Michele et al. also found that children with higher levels of resilience displayed more positive emotions, which plays an important role in the individual’s ability to cope with stress (65). Previous evidence revealed that socially avoidant preschoolers with higher levels of emotion regulation would experience fewer internalizing problems (66). Zhao et al. also found that higher levels of resilience mitigated the negative effect of less social support on migrant children’s depression and loneliness (23). Thus, Chinese migrant preschoolers with higher levels of resilience may exhibit fewer internalizing problems. Due to their status as “newcomers” in the urban setting (e.g., school, neighborhood), migrant preschoolers are often discriminated against because they are regarded to have undesirable characteristics (e.g., unfamiliar customs, undesirable behaviors) that mark them as different and then lead them to be rejected, victimized, or excluded by peers (67). Of note, children with higher levels of resilience are more curious, enjoy exploring, and exhibit stronger creativity in playing, which can be accepted and appreciated by peers (54). Previous evidence also indicated that higher levels of resilience could buffer the negative effects of peer victimization among rural-to-urban migrant school-age children in China (43). Consequently, Chinese migrant preschoolers with higher levels of resilience may be less excluded by peers.

Moreover, social integration is one of the disadvantages faced by preschool migrant children in the process of “migration,” which makes the screen rate of social withdrawal of migrant children higher than non-migrant children (19). Thus, migrant children may be more affected by unsociable behavior. Previous study suggested that unsociability was associated with school and psychological problems more evidently in migrant children than non-migrant children (20). According to the stress-buffer model (46, 47), higher levels of resilience may be a protective factor that buffers against the negative impact of “migration” and “unsociability” on social adjustment among migrant preschool children. The previous evidence also revealed that resilience could be a critical “shield” for migrant children, reducing the adverse effect of peer discrimination and improving their social adjustment (48). Accordingly, findings from the present study suggested we should highlight the positive effect of resilience on social adjustment among unsociable migrant preschoolers in China.

Unexpected but interesting, results were that resilience non-significantly moderated the relations of unsociability with prosocial behaviors and interpersonal skills in Chinese preschool migrant children. Unsociability denotes non-fearful preference for solitude and less interest in initiating peer interactions in childhood (5). Previous studies revealed that preschoolers are also more self-focused and ego-centric, and may not pay as much attention to peers (68, 69). In this regard, unsociable children inherently exhibit less prosocial behaviors and poor interpersonal skills, and are more likely to be negatively evaluated by parents and teachers. Moreover, migrant preschoolers constantly face unfamiliar and challenging settings (15), and there may be some other factors related to prosocial behaviors and interpersonal skills. For example, for migrant preschoolers, the parent-child interactions were characterized as lower frequency, and less interactions time, resulting in lower levels of parent-child closeness and mothers responding to children’s needs in negative and insensitive ways (70). In this case, migrant children could not learn positive interpersonal skills and exhibit less prosocial behaviors (71). Thus, the moderating role of resilience in the relations between unsociability and prosocial behaviors and interpersonal skills may not be demonstrated in migrant preschoolers.

To summarize, for children with higher levels of resilience, the influence of unsociability on social adjustment was more non-significant. For this result, we can suggest that parents, educators, and others are concerned about migrant children’s unsociable behavior. The resilience of migrant children also needs to be developed when providing appropriate interventions to improve the children’s social adjustment.

Limitations and future direction

This study makes a novel contribution to the extant literature by providing initial evidence to suggest unsociability associated with a unique pattern of social adjustment difficulties in Chinese preschool migrant children, in which resilience was found to be particularly protective about these relations. Thus, the present study provided valuable information about the role of context in preschool migrant children’s social adjustment, and has implications for prevention and intervention. The findings from this research concerning the relations of unsociability with social adjustment in preschool migrant children suggested that certain vulnerabilities associated with migrant status may indicate appropriate targets for prevention and intervention. For example, parents and professionals should pay particular attention to social adjustment difficulties (e.g., internalizing problems, peer exclusion, poor interpersonal skills, less prosocial behaviors) of migrant unsociable children in various domains (e.g., family, school). Teachers should also help migrant unsociable children to actively engage in social interaction in the kindergarten to reduce their adjustment difficulties. In addition, this study found that resilience could be particularly protective factor in reducing social adjustment problems (i.e., internalizing problems, peer exclusion) of unsociable preschool migrant children. Further research will be critical to the development of specialized prevention and invention programs to improve resilience to reduce internalizing problems and peer exclusion, with practical strategies that are effective for preschool migrant children.

Notwithstanding, some caveats should be considered in interpreting the results, with an eye toward future directions. First, this study only examined preschool migrant children in one of the most developed cities in China (i.e., Shanghai), which is substantial regional differences in social and economic development. Therefore, whether the study results are generalizable remains to be proven. Thus, we can conduct research in other cities with different economic and cultural backgrounds in the future. Second, as the preschoolers can have difficulties reporting their motivations and cognitions (72), mother provided ratings of unsociability as well as assessment of resilience of migrant preschoolers. Mothers’ reports can, to some extent, avoid the subjectivity of children’s self-reports and thus assess children’s social preferences relatively objectively (31). Future research should continue to seek to demonstrate associations between mother-rated unsociability and relevant constructs assessed via other means, including direct observation and children’s self-reports. Third, this study was a cross-sectional survey, significantly limiting our ability to make causal inference and to establish the direction of effects. In addition, we only examined the development of unsociability, social adjustment, and resilience in current preschool migrant children, which is far from sufficient. In future, a long-term survey is needed to explore the developmental mechanisms in preschool migrant children. Although more difficult to operationalize, future research is worthwhile to fill this gap.

Conclusion

This study focuses on the relations between unsociability and social adjustment (i.e., prosocial behaviors, peer exclusion, internalizing problems, interpersonal skills), and the moderating effect of resilience in preschool migrant children in China. The results indicated that unsociability is positively associated with social adjustment difficulties. In addition, consistent with the hypothesis, resilience moderated the relations of unsociability with peer exclusion and internalizing problems among Chinese migrant preschoolers. Specifically, the relations between unsociability and peer exclusion and internalizing problems were more negative among children with lower levels of resilience, but not significant for children with higher levels of resilience. Therefore, this study suggested that unsociable children with higher levels of resilience could mitigate their social adjustment difficulties. The results provided some implications for exploring the occurrence, development, and intervention of unsociability in disadvantaged migrant preschoolers. For migrant unsociable children who are more willing to be alone and seek less outside support, perhaps helping them improve resilience could be considered by parents and educators.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Shanghai Normal University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

JZ and ZZ managed the literature search and analyses. PX and KH participated in data collection. YL designed the study. All authors contributed to the article and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by funding from the National Social Science Fund of China (21CSH048).

Acknowledgments

We are really grateful to the participating children, parents, and teachers.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ There were no significant unsociability × gender interaction effects.

References

1. Rubin K, Bukowski W, Bowker J. Children in peer groups. In: Lerner RM, Bornstein MH, Leventhal T editors. Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: ecological settings and processes in developmental systems. 7th ed. New York, NY: Wiley Blackwell Press (2015). p. 175–222.

Google Scholar

2. Rubin K, Coplan R, Bowker J. Social withdrawal in childhood. Annu Rev Psychol. (2009) 60:141–71. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ding X, Zhou T, Zhang RZ, Zhou N. Social avoidance in childhood: predictors, measurements, and adjustment functioning. J Psychol Sci. (2019) 42:604–11. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20190314

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Asendorpf J. Beyond social withdrawal: shyness, unsociability, and peer avoidance. Hum Dev. (1990) 33:250–9. doi: 10.1159/000276522

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Coplan RJ, Armer M. A “multitude” of solitude: a closer look at social withdrawal and nonsocial play in early childhood. Child Dev Perspect. (2007) 1:26–32. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2007.00006.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Coplan R, Ooi L, Nocita G. When one is company and two is a crowd: why some children prefer solitude. Child Dev Perspect. (2015) 9:133–7. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12131

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Coplan R, Ooi L, Xiao B, Rose-Krasnor L. Assessment and implications of social withdrawal in early childhood: a first look at social avoidance. Soc Dev. (2018) 27:125–39. doi: 10.1111/sode.12258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Coplan R, Ooi L, Baldwin D. Does it matter when we want to be alone? Exploring developmental timing effects in the implications of unsociability. New Ideas Psychol. (2019) 53:47–57. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2018.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Chen X, French D. Children’s social competence in cultural context. Annu Rev Psychol. (2008) 59:591–616. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Chen X, Wang L, Cao R. Shyness-sensitivity and unsociability in rural Chinese children: relations with social, school, and psychological adjustment. Child Dev. (2011) 82:1531–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01616.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Liu J, Chen X, Zhou Y, Li D, Fu R, Coplan R. Relations of shyness–sensitivity and unsociability with adjustment in middle childhood and early adolescence in suburban Chinese children. Int J Behav Dev. (2017) 41:681–7. doi: 10.1177/0165025416664195

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Zhu J, Ooi L, Wu M, Li Y, Coplan R, Cao Y. Exploring the links between unsociability, parenting behaviors, and socio-emotional functioning in young children in suburban China. Early Educ Dev. (2021) 32:963–80. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2020.1821538

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Wang L, Mesman J. Child development in the face of rural-to-urban migration in China: a meta-analytic review. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2015) 10:813–31. doi: 10.1177/1745691615600145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Han J, Zhu L, Liu Y. Annual report on China’s education for migrant children (2019-2020). Beijing: Social Sciences Academic Press (2020).

Google Scholar

15. Huang H, Wang X. The effect of family socioeconomic status on migrant preschool children’s problem behaviors: the chain mediating role of family resilience and child-parent relationship. J Psychol Sci. (2022) 45:315–22. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.20220207

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Li X, Zou H, Wang R, Dou D. The developmental characteristics of temporary migrant children’s self-esteem and its relation to learning behavior and teacher-student relationship in Beijing. J Psychol Sci. (2008) 31:909–13. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2008.04.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lin X, Fang X, Liu Y, Lan J. The effect mechanism of stigma perception on mental health among migrant children in Beijing. Acta Psychol Sin. (2009) 41:967–79. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2009.00967

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kuang L, Liu L. Discrimination against rural-to-urban migrants: the role of the hukou system in China. PLoS One. (2012) 7:e46932. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046932

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Zhang L, Zhang J, Luan J, Yang Y. Behavior problem characteristics of migrant children in Baiyun district of Guangzhou. Chin Gen Pract. (2012) 15:2950–2. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1007-9572.2012.09.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Ding X, Chen X, Fu R, Li D, Liu J. Relations of shyness and unsociability with adjustment in migrant and non-migrant children in urban China. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2020) 48:289–300. doi: 10.1007/s10802-019-00583-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Bullock A, Xiao B, Liu J, Coplan R, Chen X. Shyness, parent–child relationships, and peer difficulties during the middle school transition. J Child Fam Stud. (2022) 31:86–98. doi: 10.1007/s10826-021-01979-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Zhu J, Fu R, Li Y, Wu M, Yang T. Shyness and adjustment in early childhood in Southeast China: the moderating role of conflict resolution skills. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:644652. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.644652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Zhao Y, Zhang X, Du J, Zheng X. Relationship between social support and depression, loneliness of migrant children: resilience as a moderator and mediator. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2014) 22:512–21. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2014.03.077

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Masten A, Best K, Garmezy N. Resilience and development: contributions from the study of children who overcome adversity. Dev Psychopathol. (1990) 2:425–44. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400005812

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Chen X. Shyness-inhibition in childhood and adolescence: a cross-cultural perspective. In: KRubin KH, Coplan RJ editors. The development of shyness and social withdrawal. New York, NY: Guilford Press (2010). p. 213–35.

Google Scholar

26. Sette S, Zava F, Baumgartner E, Laghi F, Coplan R. Examining links between social withdrawal subtypes and internalizing problems among Italian primary school children. Eur J Dev Psychol. (2022) 1–19. doi: 10.1080/17405629.2022.2086118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Wood K, Coplan R, Hipson W, Bowker J. Normative beliefs about social withdrawal in adolescence. J Res Adolesc. (2022) 32:372–81. doi: 10.1111/jora.12617

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Ding X, Zhang T, Deng X, Sang B, Fang L, Chen C. “Alone but not lonely” or “Alone and also lonely”: cultural differences on relations between unsociability and adjustment functioning. Adv Psychol Sci. (2015) 23:439–47. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2015.00439

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Liu J, Chen X, Coplan R, Ding X, Zarbatany L, Ellis W. Shyness and unsociability and their relations with adjustment in Chinese and Canadian children. J Cross Cult Psychol. (2015) 46:371–86. doi: 10.1177/0022022114567537

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Sang B, Ding X, Coplan R, Liu J, Pan T, Feng X. Assessment and implications of social avoidance in Chinese early adolescents. J Early Adolesc. (2018) 38:554–73. doi: 10.1177/0272431616678988

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Li Y, Zhu J, Coplan R, Gao Z, Xu P, Li L, et al. Assessment and implications of social withdrawal subtypes in young Chinese children: the Chinese version of the child social preference scale. J Genet Psychol. (2016) 177:97–101. doi: 10.1080/00221325.2016.1174100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Sun S, Guan Y, Qin Y, Zhang L, Fan F. Social support and emotional-behavioral problems: resilience as a mediator and moderator. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2013) 21:114–8. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2013.01.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Qi Y, Yang N. The effect of self-concept on the left-behind preschool children’s social withdrawal behavior: mediating and moderating role of resilience. Stud Early Child Educ. (2020) 2:41–56. doi: 10.13861/j.cnki.sece.2020.02.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Dent R, Cameron R. Developing resilience in children who are in public care: the educational psychology perspective. Educ Psychol Pract. (2003) 19:3–19. doi: 10.1080/0266736032000061170

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Miller M. Resilience elements in students with learning disabilities. J Clin Psychol. (2002) 58:291–8. doi: 10.1002/jclp.10018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Alvord M, Grados J. Enhancing resilience in children: a proactive approach. Prof Psychol Res Pract. (2005) 36:238–45. doi: 10.1037/0735-7028.36.3.238

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Fredrickson B. The broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. In: Huppert FA, Baylis N, Keverne B editors. The science of well-being. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2005). p. 217238. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198567523.003.0008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Mak W, Ng I, Wong C. Resilience: enhancing well-being through the positive cognitive triad. J Couns Psychol. (2011) 58:610–7. doi: 10.1037/a0025195

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Zhang G, Wang J, Liang Z, Deng H. The relation between adolescents’ resilience and school adaptation. Psychol Dev Educ. (2017) 33:11–20. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2017.01.02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Garmezy N, Masten A, Tellegen A. The study of stress and competence in children: a building block for developmental psychopathology. Child Dev. (1984) 55:97–111. doi: 10.2307/1129837

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Martinez-Torteya C, Bogat G, von Eye A, Levendosky A. Resilience among children exposed to domestic violence: the role of risk and protective factors. Child Dev. (2009) 80:562–77. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01279.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Fan Z, Fan X. Effect of social support on the psychological adjustment of Chinese left-behind rural children: a moderated mediation model. Front Psychol. (2021) 11:604397. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.604397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Ye Z, Chen L, Harrison S, Guo H, Li X, Lin D. Peer victimization and depressive symptoms among rural-to-urban migrant children in China: the protective role of resilience. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:1542. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01542

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Yoon S, Helsabeck N, Wang X, Logan J, Pei F, Hamby S, et al. Profiles of resilience among children exposed to non-maltreatment adverse childhood experiences. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2021) 18:10600. doi: 10.3390/ijerph182010600

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Sattler K, Gershoff E. Thresholds of resilience and within- and cross-domain academic achievement among children in poverty. Early Child Res Q. (2019) 46:87–96. doi: 10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. (1985) 98:310–57. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Masten A, Wright M. Cumulative risk and protection models of child maltreatment. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma. (1998) 2:7–30. doi: 10.1300/J146v02n01_02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Zhu Q, Guo H, Pan J, Lin D. Perceived discrimination and problem behaviors among rural-to-urban migrant children: the moderating role of resilience. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2015) 23:529–33. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2015.03.032

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bullock A, Liu J, Cheah C, Coplan R, Chen X, Li D. The role of adolescents’ perceived parental psychological control in the links between shyness and socio-emotional adjustment among youth. J Adolesc. (2018) 68:117–26. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2018.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Kaczmarek L. Assessment of social-communicative competence: an interdisciplinary model. In: Goldstein H, Kaczmarek LA, English KM editors. Promoting social communication: children with developmental disabilities from birth to adolescence. Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes (2002). p. 55–115.

Google Scholar

51. Zhu J, Li Y, Wood K, Coplan R, Chen X. Shyness and socioemotional functioning in young Chinese children: the moderating role of receptive vocabulary. Early Educ Dev. (2019) 30:590–7. doi: 10.1080/10409289.2019.1572481

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Coplan R, Prakash K, O’Neil K, Armer M. Do you” want” to play? Distinguishing between conflicted shyness and social disinterest in early childhood. Dev Psychol. (2004) 40:244–58. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.40.2.244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Eisenberg N, Chang L, Ma Y, Huang X. Relations of parenting style to Chinese children’s effortful control, ego resilience, and maladjustment. Dev Psychopathol. (2009) 21:455–77. doi: 10.1017/S095457940900025X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ladd G, Profilet S. The child behavior scale: a teacher-report measure of young children’s aggressive, withdrawn, and prosocial behaviors. Dev Psychol. (1996) 32:1008–24. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.32.6.1008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Zhu J, Wang Y, Li Y. Reliability and validity of the child behavior scale in Chinese preschoolers. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2018) 26:910–3. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2018.05.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gresham F, Elliott S. Social skills rating system. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service (1990).

Google Scholar

57. Zhu J, Li Y, Zhang Y, Xie Q, Xu P. Reliability and validity of the child social skills rating scale to Chinese preschoolers. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2017) 25:855–8. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2017.05.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Dunn L, Dunn L. PPVT-III: peabody picture vocabulary test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service (1997).

Google Scholar

59. Blankson A, O’Brien M, Leerkes E, Marcovitch S, Calkins S. Shyness and vocabulary: the roles of executive functioning and home environmental stimulation. Merrill Palmer Q. (2011) 57:105–28. doi: 10.1353/mpq.2011.0007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Hayes A. PROCESS: a Versatile Computational Tool for Observed Variable Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Modeling. (2012). Available online at: http://www.afhayes.com/public/process2012.pdf

Google Scholar

61. Preacher K, Hayes A. Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav Res Methods. (2008) 40:879–91. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Johnson P, Fay L. The Johnson-Neyman technique, its theory and application. Psychometrika. (1950) 15:349–67. doi: 10.1007/BF02288864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Hayes A, Matthes J. Computational procedures for probing interactions in OLS and logistic regression: SPSS and SAS implementations. Behav Res Methods. (2009) 41:924–36. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.3.924

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Xie Q, Wang Y, Hu F, Liu X, Li Y. Inhibitory control and social skills in 4 years old children: the moderation role of social withdrawal. Chin J Clin Psychol. (2020) 28:1033–7. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2020.05.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Michele M, Barbara L, Lisa F. Psychological resilience and positive emotional granularity: examining the benefits of positive emotions on coping and health. J Pers. (2004) 72:1161–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2004.00294.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Zhu J, Xiao B, Hipson W, Yan C, Coplan R, Li Y. Social avoidance and social adjustment: the moderating role of emotion regulation and emotion lability/negativity among Chinese preschool children. Front Psychol. (2021) 12:618670. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.618670

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Chen L, Su S, Li X, Tam C, Lin D. Perceived discrimination, schooling arrangements and psychological adjustments of rural-to-urban migrant children in Beijing, China. Health Psychol Behav Med. (2014) 2:713–22. doi: 10.1080/21642850.2014.919865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Lee H, Cranford J. Does resilience moderate the associations between parental problem drinking and adolescents’ internalizing and externalizing behaviors? A study of Korean adolescents. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2008) 96:213–21. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2008.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Piaget J. Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York, NY: Norton (1962).

Google Scholar

70. Chen L, Liu Y. The characteristics of migrant children’s communication with their parents and its relationship with migrant children’s mental health. Chin J Spec Educ. (2012) 58–63.

Google Scholar

71. Li Y, Xu L, Lu Y, Liu L, Wang Y. The associations between mother-child relationship, teacher-child relationship and social adaptive behaviors for preschool migrant children. Psychol Dev Educ. (2014) 30:624–34. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2014.06.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Thompson R, Goodvin R, Meyer S. Social development: psychological understanding, self-understanding, & relationships. In: Luby JL editor. Handbook of preschool mental health: development, disorders, and treatment. New York, NY: Guilford (2006). p. 3–22.

Google Scholar

Keywords: preschool migrant children, resilience, unsociability, social adjustment, China

Citation: Zhu J, Zhang Z, Xu P, Huang K and Li Y (2023) Unsociability and social adjustment of Chinese preschool migrant children: The moderating role of resilience. Front. Psychiatry 14:1074217. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1074217

Received: 19 October 2022; Accepted: 02 January 2023;
Published: 24 January 2023.

Edited by:

Junsheng Liu, East China Normal University, China

Reviewed by:

Amanda Bullock, Carleton University, Canada
Xi Chen, East China Normal University, China

Copyright © 2023 Zhu, Zhang, Xu, Huang and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yan Li, www.frontiersin.org liyan@shnu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.