Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 06 April 2023
Sec. Autism

Correlation and predictive ability of sensory characteristics and social interaction in children with autism spectrum disorder

\r\nJinhe ZhaiJinhe Zhai1Xiaoxue LiXiaoxue Li1Yong ZhouYong Zhou2Lili FanLili Fan1Wei XiaWei Xia1Xiaomin WangXiaomin Wang1Yutong LiYutong Li1Meiru HouMeiru Hou1Jia Wang*Jia Wang1*Lijie Wu*Lijie Wu1*
  • 1School of Public Health, Harbin Medical University, Harbin, China
  • 2Heilongjiang Provincial Center for Disease Control and Prevention, Harbin, China

Background: Individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) often have different social characteristics and particular sensory processing patterns, and these sensory behaviors may affect their social functioning. The objective of our study is to investigate the sensory profiles of children with ASD and their association with social behavior. Specifically, we aim to identify the predictive role of sensory processing in social functioning.

Methods: The Short Sensory Profile (SSP) was utilized to analyze sensory differences between ASD children and their peers. The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) and other clinical scales were employed to assess the social functioning of children with ASD. Additionally, the predictive ability of sensory perception on social performance was discussed using random forest and support vector machine (SVM) models.

Results: The SSP scores of ASD children were lower than those of the control group, and there was a significant negative correlation between SSP scores and clinical scale scores (P < 0.05). The random forest and SVM models, using all the features, showed higher sensitivity, while the random forest model with 7-feature factors had the highest specificity. The area under the receiver operating characteristic (ROC) curve (AUC) for all the models was higher than 0.8.

Conclusion: Autistic children in our study have different patterns of sensory processing than their peers, which are significantly related to their patterns of social functioning. Sensory features can serve as a good predictor of social functioning in individuals with ASD.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a complex neurodevelopmental disorder characterized by challenges in social communication and interaction and intense behavior or interests (1). In recent years, the global incidence of ASD has increased rapidly. According to the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention (CDC) in the United States, the prevalence of ASD has risen to 1 in 44 individuals (2). The global prevalence has reached 1−2% (3). A multicenter survey of children aged 6−12 years in China found that the prevalence of ASD was approximately 0.70% (4). The increasing prevalence has necessitated that we understand the impact of ASD.

In the past, researchers have primarily focused on social and stereotyped behaviors when studying ASD, but they have gradually discovered that many people with ASD also experience differences in sensory perception. In fact, as early as the 1940s, Kanner (5) had already described these symptoms in individuals with ASD. More recently, the fifth edition of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-5) has included sensory patterns as a diagnostic criterion for ASD, increasing the attention given to sensory features in ASD research. Studies have found that particular sensory patterns occur in ASD of all ages, with intense sensory experiences reported in as many as 60 to 90% of individuals with ASD (6).

Sensory processing is the brain’s way of recognizing and organizing external stimuli to obtain effective information from them (7). Living organisms are constantly exposed to a diverse array of information in their environment and rely on their sensory system to process information from various sources, such as sounds, visual stimuli, tactile sensations, and smells. For social creatures like humans, the environment is also rich in social information that needs to be perceived and understood. In a study by Morgan, it was suggested that the brain’s sensory and perceptual system evaluates and integrates social information and situational factors to generate appropriate social behaviors in response to social-emotional cues (8).

Individuals with ASD often experience intense sensory processing, which makes it difficult for them to adapt to the changing sensory environment. It is believed that sensory processing and socialization may originate from the same underlying mechanisms and that they may influence each other during childhood development. These sensory experiences have negative effects on attention and language development and may predict social withdrawal (911). Communication involves multiple social cues that are often combined, requiring individuals to process multiple sensory modalities simultaneously. However, existing research often focuses on a single sensory modality, and there is a lack of comprehensive evaluation of all sensory modalities in ASD and their relationship with social interaction. Therefore, investigating the relationship between multiple sensory modalities and social interaction could provide a new direction for ASD intervention and is the primary aim of this study.

Autism is mainly treated through behavior training as there is currently no drug treatment available. The effectiveness of intervention is largely dependent on the timing of its implementation: therefore, early identification of symptoms is crucial for a better prognosis. There is evidence that differential sensory behavior manifests earlier, which highlights the potential of exploring the predictive value of sensory features in estimating social levels. This not only reveals the relationship between sensory and social features but also enhances early diagnosis of autism. To this end, this study employs machine learning (ML) algorithms, such as neural networks, decision trees, rule-based classifiers, and support vector machines, which are automated data processing tools that can build accurate prediction models based on research datasets (12). The use of machine learning algorithms in autism research was initially introduced to address the challenge of diagnosing this complex disorder. In Paolo et al. (13) applied a support vector machine model and found that dyskinesia can be used as a clinical symptom to facilitate early recognition of ASD. Subsequent research has increasingly focused on the potential applications of machine learning in the field of ASD. With a deeper understanding of autism, researchers have explored a wider range of markers beyond early behavioral presentations, including neuroimaging, molecular biology, and genetic predictions, which can be combined with machine learning to provide new avenues for ASD research (1416). For example, Alivar et al. (17) utilized support vector machines and artificial neural network classification models to examine the impact of common sleep problems in ASD on daily behavior. Despite numerous successes in incorporating machine learning into ASD research, few studies have focused on individual typical symptoms, such as social interaction.

To sum up, the present study aims to achieve two objectives: (a) investigating whether children with ASD have a higher frequency of sensory reactivity differences than their peers; and (b) examining the correlation between sensory processing, social function, and clinical diagnosis in children with ASD, as well as the predictive power of sensory features for social ability. This research is expected to provide clinical physicians with auxiliary tools for diagnosis and increase the enthusiasm of rehabilitation therapists in formulating intervention strategies that emerge from knowing a person’s sensory patterns.

Materials and methods

Participants

Participants for the ASD group were recruited from the Research Center for Children’s Developmental Behavior of Harbin Medical University. Criteria for inclusion in the ASD group required a diagnosis of ASD by a psychiatrist or clinical psychologist using the DSM-5 diagnostic criteria. Participants for the control group were recruited from local kindergartens and primary schools in Harbin city. Individuals with comorbid mental development disorders, definite head trauma, nervous system diseases, major somatic diseases, or a history of epileptic seizures were excluded.

The study included a total of 489 participants divided into two groups: the ASD group, consisting of 266 individuals with autism (219 boys and 47 girls), and the control group, consisting of 223 children (170 boys and 53 girls). The children in the ASD group had a mean age of 5.31 ± 0.102 years, while those in the control group had a mean age of 5.32 ± 0.115 years. There were no significant differences between the two groups in terms of age or gender.

The Short Sensory Profile

The Short Sensory Profile (SSP) is a questionnaire completed by parents to assess children’s responses to sensory stimuli. It is commonly used to identify sensory behaviors in children with ASD between the ages of 3 and 10 years. The questionnaire consists of 38 items across seven sensory domains, each describing various sensory-related behaviors. The frequency of behaviors is scored on a scale of 1 to 5, with lower scores indicating a greater possibility of sensory differences from expected patterns. Based on the score range, the testee’s sensory profile can be categorized into three grades (expected patterns = 0, possible difference = 1, or obvious difference = 2) (18) (The specific classification criteria are shown in Supplementary Table 2). The grades of possible difference and obvious difference are regarded as unexpected patterns.

The Social Responsiveness Scale

The Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS) is used as an aid in the clinical diagnosis of ASD children between the ages of 4 and 18 years. The scale consists of 65 items divided into 5 sub-scales, each describing the social situations children may encounter in their daily lives, including behaviors related to social communication, communication skills, and repetitive and rigid behaviors associated with autism. A higher score on the scale indicates a greater degree of social difference (19).

Autism diagnostic interview-revised

The Autism Diagnostic Interview–Revised (ADI-R) is a standardized and structured diagnostic tool based on parent interviews and is widely recognized as the gold standard in autism assessment. The scale consists of four domains: social interaction, communication, repetitive behaviors, and early developmental history. Higher scores on the ADI-R indicate greater impairment in the corresponding domain (20).

Autism diagnostic schedule

The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (ADOS) is a standardized and semi-structured diagnostic tool that is widely recognized as a gold standard for ASD diagnosis. Specifically trained evaluators interact with children in a standardized manner, observing their behavior for signs of restricted and repetitive interests and behaviors during play and assessing their social communication, play, and imagination for the purpose of assisting in the diagnosis of ASD (21).

The questionnaire and scales used in this study were administered in Chinese. The authorized Chinese versions of these instruments were used to assess the participants’ autism symptoms and related behaviors.

Data preprocessing of model construction

Basic participant information and SSP data were collected as key variables, including age, sex, and eight sensory dimensions of the SSP. Participants with ASD were categorized for their social responsiveness based on their SRS raw scores, with a cut-off of 60. Children who score above 60 are more likely to be considered as having autism. A two-variable prediction model was constructed using SRS scores (0 for expected patterns, 1 for unexpected patterns) as the output variable.

Random forest model

Random forest is a type of ensemble learning method that uses decision trees as the base model, specifically, classification and regression trees (CART) (22). CART uses the Gini index as the evaluation criterion, which is defined as Gini = 1-Σ(P(i)*P(i)), where P(i) represents the proportion of type I samples in the data set at the current node. The ntree parameter is tested and adjusted during the model-building process. The Gini index is used to calculate the heterogeneity of the observed values at nodes in the classification tree caused by each variable, which allows for the comparison of variable importance. The model is re-constructed for each method by retaining the feature factors with the highest importance ranking after calculating the relationship between the error rate and the number of features through cross-validation.

Support vector machine model

Support vector machine (SVM) is a classification model that is based on the principle of structural risk minimization and VC dimension theory. This model seeks the best balance between learning ability and accuracy to obtain the best generalization ability (23). In this study, the Gaussian kernel function that demonstrated the best performance was used as the kernel function for the SVM model. Initially, all feature factors were inputted into the SVM model for training. Subsequently, based on the variable importance from the random forest model, the feature factors with high importance were selected to reduce the feature factor dimension. The selected feature factors were then inputted into the SVM model for further training.

Model construction and verification

To mitigate structural risks in the model construction process and evaluate the generalization ability of the prediction model, we adopted the K-fold cross-validation method. In each fold, 9/10 of random samples were used for model training, and the remaining 1/10 were used for model validation. By separating the data into training and validation sets, the model’s effectiveness could be better assessed. We performed a 10% discount cross-validation five times. The model’s prediction performance was evaluated based on its sensitivity and specificity in predicting the test set and the AUC under the ROC curve.

Statistical analysis

The original data were analyzed using SPSS version 26.0. The Shapiro-Wilk (S-W) method was used to test the normality of scores for each scale. The Chi-square test was used to determine whether there was a difference in gender between the ASD group and the control group. The Mann–Whitney U test was used to determine if there were differences in age and SSP scores between the two groups. The Chi-square test was used to analyze differences between the ASD and control groups in SSP score grades. In addition, Spearman rank correlation was used to analyze the correlation between SSP scores and clinical scales in the ASD group. The random forest and support vector machine models were constructed and verified using R version 3.5.3.

Results

Analysis of differences in sensory features between groups

The S-W test showed that the SSP score data of the two groups did not obey the normal distribution. According to the Mann–Whitney test, the scores of the ASD group in seven sub-fields and the total scores of multiple senses were significantly lower than those of the comparison group. The χ2 test showed that the ratio of unexpected patterns in the ASD group was higher than that of the TD group (P < 0.05) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Comparison of SSP score characteristics between ASD group and healthy control [Mid (P25, P75)/n (%)].

According to the nature of each topic in the SSP questionnaire, we divided all items into three modes: hyper-sensitivity, hyposensitivity, and seeking, and described the number of ASD people with these three patterns, respectively. The number of people with only one of the patterns was 95 [hypersensitivity = 84 (31.6%), hyposensitivity = 7 (2.63%), sensory seeking = 4 (1.5%)]. In addition, 69 people had two types of sensory reactivity differences at the same time, and 59 people showed all three types of patterns at the same time (Supplementary Table 1).

Correlation analysis between SSP score and behavior scale score in the ASD group

Spearman correlation analysis showed that the SSP gustatory/olfactory and motor sensitivity scores were negatively correlated with ADI-R social function scores, tactile scores were negatively correlated with non-verbal communication scores, but the correlation with the score of verbal communication was not statistically significant; the gustation/olfactory score was positively correlated with the ADOS communication score; and the auditory filtering score was negatively correlated with the ADOS social interaction score. Except for the correlation between motor sensitivity and all dimensions of SRS, the scores of other sensory dimensions of SSP were negatively correlated with certain domains of SRS (P < 0.05). The correlation between the SSP score and other dimensions of each scale score is not statistically significant (P > 0.05) (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Spearman correlation analysis between SSP score and SRS/ADIR/ADOS score(r).

Data dimension reduction results of the models

The varImpPlot function in random forest is used to rank the importance of all factors, reduce the dimension of characteristic factors, select the characteristic factors that have a significant influence on the prediction results, and reconstruct the prediction model. The order of importance is hyposensitivity/sensory seeking>multisensory>age>auditory filtering>low strength>vision/auditory>tactile> gustation/olfactory>motor sensitivity>gender. According to the cross-validation of the relationship between error rate and characteristic factors, when the number of characteristic factors is approximately seven, the error rate is low and stable. Therefore, the first seven characteristic factors are selected to construct the random forest model and SVM model again, and the prediction results are compared with those of the ten characteristic prediction models.

Analysis, evaluation, and comparison of random forest model and SVM model

Both the stochastic forest model and SVM model with all feature factors have higher sensitivity, specificity, and AUC than the model with data dimension reduction. The random forest model with all characteristic factors has the highest sensitivity, up to 0.916, and the stochastic forest model with seven characteristic factors has the highest specificity, reaching 0.944. The ROCs of the four curves are close to the upper left corner, and AUC is higher than 0.8. The prediction effect of the models is good (0.8 ∼ 0.9). Among them, the SVM prediction model with all feature factors has the best AUC (Figure 1 and Table 3).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. ROC curves of the models.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison of invalidation result of prediction models.

Discussion

The sensory system is complex and the differences in the performance of ASD individuals can manifest in various ways, making it difficult to distinguish and evaluate sensory behavior. As a result, there have been numerous studies on sensory symptoms in ASD utilizing many types of sensory questionnaires. For instance, Baranek et al. (24) found that 69% of ASD children had sensory differences using the Sensory Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ). Rogers et al. (25), using the SSP, found that individuals with ASD were more sensitive to tactile, auditory, and taste/smell stimuli. Tavassoli et al. (26), in a similar survey using the Sensory Profile (SP), found that 31% of children with neurodevelopmental disorders, including ASD, had auditory over-responsivity and 27% had tactile over-responsivity. However, the previous studies have not clearly distinguished the differences in performance and their severity among sensory domains. The innovation of the current study lies in its use of the SSP questionnaire to analyze evaluation results in detail and address the aforementioned issues. Our findings indicate that within the ASD group, 55.1% of total scale scores were classified as unexpected patterns, suggesting that over half of children with ASD exhibit more intense sensory behaviors in general.

During our further in-depth analysis, we noted that the highest proportion of differences in sensory behavior was related to auditory filtering (77.8%). The ability to extract and process relevant information from complex sound environments is essential in daily communication. Most parents in our study reported that their ASD children had difficulty concentrating and performing activities in noisy environments, indicating that it is not easy for ASD individuals to filter out irrelevant sounds. Previous research by Groen found that individuals with autism require a higher signal-to-noise ratio than control groups to extract disyllabic words from various auditory backgrounds, as demonstrated in their performance on sounds presented in pink noise (27). Moreover, when combined with the 30% of ASD children who exhibit sensory differences in the visual and auditory domains, our findings suggest a comprehensive difference in audio-visual integration within this group. Previous research found that ASD children’s audio-visual temporal binding window is four times larger than that of typically developing children, indicating a significant deficit in audio-visual acuity (28). Another sensory dimension with a high proportion of different behavior was related to the tactile system, with 66.2% of children with ASD displaying sensory-motor problems. Specifically, 40.8% of the children in this group were found to be sensitive to movement. Previous studies have documented a range of sensorimotor disorders in ASD, including uncoordinated movements of the upper and lower body, manifesting as difficulty with gait and reaching, respectively (2931). Of particular note in this study is the observation that individuals with ASD have difficulty with tasks requiring strong grip strength, lifting heavy objects, and maintaining balance and posture. These findings provide new evidence and insight into the sensory-motor performance of individuals with ASD. Dorothea has suggested that ineffective motor circuits may be the underlying cause of autism motor patterns (32). Although the unexpected patterns’ rates of other sensory domains in our sample of individuals with ASD were relatively low, they were still significantly higher than in comparison children and could contribute to behavioral differences in ASD. For example, their tactile sensitivity may make them uncomfortable with being approached or touched by others, while taste and smell sensitivity may lead to picky eating and anorexia, which can hinder individual development. It is clear that the sensory behavioral characteristics of ASD are varied, and a deeper understanding of them can inform targeted interventions.

In our investigation, the vast majority of individuals with ASD displayed not just one but multiple coexisting sensory reactivity differences, and they may each have their own unique effects on behavior. For instance, children with both tactile and auditory filtering difficulties may be resistant to being approached and often fail to respond to others’ calls, thus creating significant obstacles to effective communication. Furthermore, external environmental stimuli often involve a complex mix of sensory information, such as the rapid integration of auditory and visual cues in everyday conversation, which can result in speech perception deficits if not properly integrated in a timely manner (33, 34). It is clear that the presence of multiple sensory reactivity differences can exacerbate the already complex sensory issues experienced by individuals with ASD and must be taken into account when designing interventions to address these challenges. The presence of sensory responsivity in multiple dimensions makes it difficult for individuals with ASD to cope. Additionally, changes in reaction patterns play an important role in their unusual behavior, which can be classified into three categories: hyperresponsiveness, hyporesponsiveness, and sensory seeking. Our results show that among individuals with only one unusual reaction (42%), hypersensitivity was the most common (37%). However, the proportion of individuals with multiple reactions should not be ignored (57%) and is consistent with findings from Taylor’s research (35). The causes of different patterns in ASD sensory behavior have been extensively researched and may be influenced by factors such as cortical excitability, unexpected receptor function at the molecular level, and genes (3638). However, the cross-disciplinary nature of these anomalies and the complicated reaction patterns make it difficult to distinguish ASD’s sensory behavior, which can have serious implications for their other skills. In conclusion, the detailed exploration of sensory response behavior in ASD children in current research is of great significance for treatment and symptom recognition.

Effective communication and successful social interaction require individuals to fully utilize their sensory systems in order to obtain valuable information and engage in social activities in a world full of chaotic information (39). However, particular sensory responses in individuals with ASD may contribute to the development of social disorders. The current research results show a negative correlation between SSP scores and social dimension scores on clinical scales, strongly suggesting that as sensory sensitivities become more severe, social engagement becomes more challenging. Tactile perception is particularly important for infants as it is one of the primary ways they explore the external environment and is crucial to their overall development. Tactile input, particularly friendly forms of social touch like hugs, is vital for social development from infancy onward, and gentle emotional touch can help to alleviate anxiety and improve social functioning (40). Autistic individuals often exhibit hypersensitivity to gentle touch, but reduced sensitivity to painful stimulation (41, 42), leading to difficulty in detecting social cues. The current study found that the SSP tactile score was negatively correlated with the ADI-R non-verbal communication score, which is consistent with the findings of Foss-Feig et al. (43). It is evident that tactile processing can affect various forms of communication and interaction (44). The link between tactile processing and social symptoms in individuals with ASD is also apparent, given that tactile information is critical for distinguishing the relationship between oneself and others, which is the foundation of social cognition (45). As indicated in the SRS questionnaire, autistic children may struggle to comprehend the interrelation of things, unlike their peers, which can hinder their social engagement.

Alongside touch, the impact of vision and hearing on social behavior also cannot be overlooked. “Listening” and “observing” are two essential means of communication with others. Our findings suggest that unexpected audio-visual perception is negatively associated with social functioning across three dimensions: social perception, social communication, and social motivation. Emotional recognition is a crucial aspect of social behavior, encompassing the comprehension of facial expressions and emotional rhythms (46, 47), which rely on intact audio-visual processing. Up to 80% of environmental information is transmitted to the brain through vision (48), and visual processing is linked to social behaviors such as joint attention and imitation of others (49, 50). However, most individuals with ASD find maintaining eye contact challenging and tend to focus more on local details rather than the whole, which can make it challenging to track complex and subtle social cues. ASD children often fixate on lip movements during conversations with others, rather than comprehensively observing facial expressions to discern social cues such as emotions (51). Furthermore, linguistic elements play a significant role in conveying emotions and attitudes. Due to auditory sensitivities, children with ASD may fixate on certain linguistic features (such as tone of voice or intonation) during communication, making it challenging for them to filter out socially irrelevant information from others’ speech and subsequently integrate social cues, such as linguistic expressions of emotions. Moreover, excessive sensitivity to sound may lead to children experiencing heightened fear toward particular sounds, further limiting their social development. Our assessment using the SSP has shown that ASD children often struggle to concentrate in noisy environments, underscoring the obstacles they face in auditory filtering and attention.

Anorexia, picky eating, and difficulty eating are common in children with autism, indicating a possible link to heightened olfactory and taste perception. Another key finding of this study is that these sensory differences not only affect the eating behaviors of those with ASD but may also lead to social difficulties. Through our correlation analysis, we discovered that olfactory and taste expression are also linked to the manifestation of autistic behaviors. The SRS scale defines autism behaviors as unacceptable changes in daily routines, unusual perceptual interests, and behaviors that make the child seem odd or unusual to others. These behaviors not only interfere with the emotions of individuals with ASD but also make it difficult for them to fit in socially and represent a significant obstacle to integrating into their social environment. Lahera’s research highlights the close relationship between smell perception, emotional behavior, and cognitive ability (52).

Social function is a critical factor that impacts the quality of life and prognosis of individuals with ASD. Clinical evaluations of communication ability in individuals with ASD typically involve questionnaire surveys, psychological measurements, and evaluations by appraisers during gameplay (5355). However, these methods are often influenced by subjective factors to some extent. In recent years, with the advancement of computer technology, big data analysis methods are increasingly being employed in scientific research, and machine learning is increasingly being used in the field of ASD. Currently, machine learning algorithms are being applied in many areas of research, including the discovery of effective biomarkers for diagnosis, the classification of ASD based on various phenotypes, and the prediction of risk genes (5658). In this study, we have innovatively used this method to determine how much sensory behavior can predict social function. Our results significantly demonstrate that both random forest and SVM models can reliably and consistently distinguish ASD social interaction, providing a new perspective for future research. The SRS scale, which serves as an output factor in our model, is widely used in clinical practice and is a necessary tool for accurately assessing ASD diagnosis and social functioning in children. The characteristic factors used in the model construction are derived from the scores of each sensory field in the SSP questionnaire, as well as demographic characteristics such as gender and age. Our findings provide evidence for predicting the possibility of ASD diagnosis and social functioning through sensory expression.

We ranked the contribution of characteristic factors to the model using the data dimension reduction method and found that the top three factors are hyposensitivity/sensory seeking, the total multi-sensory score, and age. These factors play a significant role in predicting social function. However, the importance of scores in each sensory sub-field ranked lower than the total score, suggesting that the influence of feeling on social interaction is the result of the synthesis of various senses, and the total effect is greater than the single effect. Cross-multisensory fusion’s influence on individual behavior has attracted widespread attention as a separate field, particularly for autistic people. To improve adaptive behavior, ASD interventions and treatments have been carried out using multi-sensory-based approaches (59). Electrophysiological and neuroimaging studies have established that neural circuits and brain structure play a crucial role in multisensory integration, which provides a basis for exploring the biological links between sensation and social interaction (60). The predictive effect of demographic characteristics indicates that age is the most robust factor in the model, whereas gender is the least important among the various characteristics. Currently, the diagnosis of autism lacks effective biomarkers, mainly relying on doctors’ experience in evaluating symptoms. After diagnosis, there is no specific drug for treatment, and, therefore, behavioral intervention is the only option. The results of this study emphasize that taking unusual tactile symptoms as a new perspective and paying more attention to tactile representation can not only enhance the specificity of early recognition but also improve the tactile sense as an intervention target, which is likely to alleviate ASD symptoms. These findings provide an important reminder to researchers, doctors, and even caregivers of ASD, which is that increasing awareness and sensitivity to different sensory symptoms in autistic children can greatly benefit early identification and treatment. It also emphasizes the importance of caregivers paying attention to their children’s sensory behaviors in daily life to avoid overlooking early symptoms of ASD. Furthermore, we revealed a link between sensory and social and found that sensory symptoms can reliably predict social functioning. The establishment of the autism prediction model has provided certain clinical assistance for diagnosis. As the development of sensory behavior may precede social behavior and is easier to identify, this model can predict an individual’s social level at an earlier stage, thereby increasing the sensitivity of ASD diagnosis. In addition, during the intervention process for individuals with autism, rehabilitation therapists can use this model to understand the stage-wise progress of their social functioning. All these results can help medical professionals deepen their understanding of the relationship between these two factors and provide new avenues for developing treatment strategies from a sensory perspective.

Conclusion

This study illustrates that ASD children have a higher incidence of sensitive sensory behavior, which seems to relate to social performance, so it is important to predict social engagement based on sensory symptoms.

Data availability statement

The data analyzed in this study is subject to the following licenses/restrictions: Due to the nature of this research, participants of this study did not agree for their data to be shared publicly, so supporting data is not available. Requests to access these datasets should be directed to JW, wangjiahyd@163.com.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Harbin Medical University Ethics Committee. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author contributions

JZ conceived the study, performed the data acquisition and analysis, and drafted and revised the manuscript. XL and YZ were involved in the writing and revision of the manuscript and contributed to the acquisition of data. LF, YL, and MH were responsible for personnel recruitment and questionnaire distribution. WX and XW conducted questionnaire recovery and quality checks. JW and LW conceived the study and helped revise the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was funded by the Scientific Research Business Expenses of Heilongjiang Provincial Scientific Research Institutes (CZKYF2023-1-A010), Heilongjiang Natural Science Foundation (LH2022H051), and National Natural Science Foundation of China (81874270).

Acknowledgments

The authors express special thanks to all the children who participated and their parents.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1056051/full#supplementary-material

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 5th ed. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Baio J, Wiggins L, Christensen DL, Maenner M, Warren Z, Kurzius-Spencer M, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years - autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, United States, 2014. MMWR Surveill Summ. (2018) 67:1–23. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. World Health Organization (WHO). Autism spectrum disorders. Geneva: World Health Organization (2019).

Google Scholar

4. Zhou H, Xu X, Yan W, Zou X, Wu L, Luo X, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder in china: a nationwide multi-center population-based study among children aged 6 to 12 years. Neurosci Bull. (2020) 36:961–71. doi: 10.1007/s12264-020-00530-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kanner L. Autistic disturbances of affective contact. Acta Paedops. (1968) 35: 100–36.

Google Scholar

6. Ausderau K, Sideris J, Furlong M, Bulluck J, Baranek G. National survey of sensory features in children with ASD: factor structure of the sensory experience questionnaire (3.0). J Autism Dev Disord. (2014) 44:915–25. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1945-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Haberberger RV, Barry C, Dominguez N, Matusica D. Human dorsal root ganglia. Front Cell Neurosci. (2019) 13:271. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2019.00271

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Rogers-Carter M, Christianson J. An insular view of the social decision-making network. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2019) 103:119–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2019.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Hilton CL, Harper JD, Kueker RH, Lang A, Abbacchi A, Todorov A, et al. Sensory responsiveness as a predictor of social severity in children with high functioning autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev. Disord. (2010) 40:937–45. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0944-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Baranek G, Watson L, Boyd B, Poe M, David F, McGuire L. Hyporesponsiveness to social and nonsocial sensory stimuli in children with autism, children with developmental delays, and typically developing children. Dev Psychopathol. (2013) 25:307–20. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412001071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Gliga T, Jones E, Bedford R, Charman T, Johnson M. From early markers to neuro-developmental mechanisms of autism. Dev Rev. (2014) 34:189–207. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Rahman M, Usman O, Muniyandi R, Sahran S, Mohamed S, Razak R. A review of machine learning methods of feature selection and classification for autism spectrum disorder. Brain Sci. (2020) 10:949. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10120949

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Perego P, Forti S, Crippa A. Reach and throw movement analysis with support vector machines in early diagnosis of autism. Ann Int Conf IEEE Eng Med Biol Soc. (2009) 2009:2555–8. doi: 10.1109/IEMBS.2009.5335096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Nogay HS, Adeli H. Machine learning (ML) for the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder (ASD) using brain imaging. Rev Neurosci. (2020) 2020:43. doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2020-0043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Zhan Y, Wei J, Liang J, Xu X, He R, Robbins TW, et al. Diagnostic classification for human autism and obsessive-compulsive disorder based on machine learning from a primate genetic model. Am J Psychiatry. (2021) 178:65–76. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.19101091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Osredkar J, Gosar D, Maèek J, Kumer K, Fabjan T, Finderle P, et al. Urinary markers of oxidative stress in children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Antioxidants. (2019) 8:187. doi: 10.3390/antiox8060187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Alivar A, Carlson C, Suliman A. Smart bed based daytime behavior prediction in children with autism spectrum disorder - a pilot study. Med Eng Phys. (2020) 83:15–25. doi: 10.1016/j.medengphy.2020.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Mcintosh D, Miller L, Shyu V, Dunn W. Development and validation of the short sensory profile. Sensory Profile User’s Manual. (1999) 1999:59–73.

Google Scholar

19. Constantino J, Gruber CP. Social responsiveness scale (SRS) manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services. (2005).

Google Scholar

20. Guo Y, Xiaoling Y, Liu J. Study on diagnostic validity and reliability of autism diagnostic interview scale (revised). Chin J Psychiatry. (2002) 1:45–8.

Google Scholar

21. Tang Y, Guo Y, Rice CE. Introduction of autism diagnostic observation scale, one of the gold standards for autism diagnosis. Int J Psychiatry. (2010) 37:38–40.

Google Scholar

22. Dutschmann T, Baumann K. Evaluating high-variance leaves as uncertainty measure for random forest regression. Molecules. (2021) 26:6514. doi: 10.3390/molecules26216514

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Wu Q, Wan W, Lingzhi W. Exponential smoothing support vector classifier. J Xi’ Univ Posts Telecommun. (2014) 19:9–14.

Google Scholar

24. Baranek G, David F, Poe M, Stone WL, Watson LR. Sensory experiences questionnaire: discriminating sensory features in young children with autism, developmental delays, and typical development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2006) 47:591–601. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01546.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Rogers S, Hepburn S, Wehner E. Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2019) 33:631–42. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000006000.38991.a7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Tavassoli T, Brandes-Aitken A, Chu R, Porter L, Schoen S, Miller L, et al. Sensory over-responsivity: parent report, direct assessment measures, and neural architecture. Mol Autism. (2019) 10:4. doi: 10.1186/s13229-019-0255-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Ashburner J, Ziviani J, Rodger S. Sensory processing and classroom emotional behavioural, and education outcomes in children with autism spectrum disorder. Am J Occupat Ther. (2008) 62:564–73. doi: 10.5014/ajot.62.5.564

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Noel J, Lytle M, Cascio C, Wallace M. Disrupted integration of exteroceptive and interoceptive signaling in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2018) 11:194–205. doi: 10.1002/aur.1880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kaur M, Srinivasan S, Bhat A. Comparing motor performance, praxis, coordination, and interpersonal synchrony between children with and without autism spectrum disorder (ASD). Res Dev Disabil. (2018) 72:79–95. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Mostert-Kerckhoffs M, Willems A, Tenback D, Koning J, Van Harten P, Staal W. Motor disturbance in ASD: a pilot study showing hypokinetic behavior? J Autism Dev. (2020) 50:415–28. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04171-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Wuang Y, Huang C, Tsai H. Sensory integration and perceptual-motor profiles in school-aged children with autistic spectrum disorder. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2020) 16:1661–73. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S253337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Floris D, Barber A, Nebel M, Martinelli M, Lai M, Crocetti D, et al. Atypical lateralization of motor circuit functional connectivity in children with autism is associated with motor deficits. Mol Autism. (2016) 7:35. doi: 10.1186/s13229-016-0096-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Stevenson R, Siemann J, Schneider B, Eberly H, Woynaroski T, Camarata S, et al. Multisensory temporal integration in autism spectrum disorders. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:691–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3615-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Stevenson R, Segers M, Ncube B, Black K, Bebko J, Ferber S, et al. The cascading influence of multisensory processing on speech perception in autism. Autism. (2018) 22:609–24. doi: 10.1177/1362361317704413

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Taylor E, Holt R, Tavassoli T, Ashwin C, Baron-Cohen S. Revised scored sensory perception quotient reveals sensory hypersensitivity in women with autism. Mol Autism. (2020) 11:18. doi: 10.1186/s13229-019-0289-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Takarae Y, Sweeney J. Neural hyperexcitability in autism spectrum disorders. Brain Sci. (2012) 7:129. doi: 10.3390/brainsci7100129

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Coghlan S, Horder J, Inkster B, Mendez M, Murphy D, Nutt D, et al. GABA system dysfunction in autism and related disorders: from synapse to symptoms. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2012) 36:2044–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Loke Y, Hannan A, Craig J. The role of epigenetic change in autism spectrum disorders. Front Neurol. (2015) 6:107. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2015.00107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Baum S, Stevenson R, Wallace M. Behavioral, perceptual, and neural alterations in sensory and multisensory function in autism spectrum disorder. Prog Neurobiol. (2015) 134:140–60. doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Butts J. Outcomes of comfort touch in institutionalized elderly female residents Geriatric. Nursing. (2001) 22:180–4. doi: 10.1067/mgn.2001.117914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Pelc K, Cheron G, Dan B. Behavior and neuropsychiatric manifestations in angelman syndrome. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2008) 4:577–84. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S2749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Rattaz C, Dubois A, Michelon C, Viellard M, Poinso F, Baghdadli A, et al. How do children with autism spectrum disorders express pain? A comparison with developmentally delayed and typically developing children. Pain. (2013) 154:2007–13. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Foss-Feig J, Heacock J, Cascio C. Tactile responsiveness patterns and their association with core features in autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2012) 6:337–44. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Burgoon J, Guerrero L, Floyd K. Nonverbal communication. Boston: Allyn & Bacon (2009).

Google Scholar

45. Deschrijver E, Wiersema J, Brass M. Action-based touch observation in adults with high functioning autism: can compromised self-other distinction abilities link social and sensory everyday problems? Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2017) 12:273–82. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Bänziger T, Grandjean D, Scherer K. Emotion recognition from expressions in face, voice, and body: the multimodal emotion recognition test (MERT). Emotion. (2009) 9:691–704. doi: 10.1037/a0017088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Adolphs R. Cognitive neuroscience of human social behaviour. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2003) 4:165–78. doi: 10.1038/nrn1056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Haupt C, Huber A. How axons see their way–axonal guidance in the visual system. Front Biosci. (2008) 13:3136–49. doi: 10.2741/2915

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Iacoboni M. Imitation, empathy, and mirror neurons. Ann Rev Psychol. (2009) 60:653–70. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Leekam S, Baron-Cohen S, Perrett D. Eyedirection detection: a dissociation between geometric and joint attention skills in autism. Br J Dev Psychol. (1997) 15:77–95. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-835X.1997.tb00726.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Foxe J, Molholm S, Del Ben V, Frey H, Russo N, Blanco D, et al. Severe multisensory speech integration deficits in high-functioning schoolaged children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and their resolution during early adolescence. Cerebral Cortex. (2015) 25:298–312. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Lahera G, Ruiz-Murugarren S, Fernández-Liria A, Saiz-Ruiz J, Buck B, Penn D, et al. Relationship between olfactory function and social cognition in euthymic bipolar patients. CNS Spectr. (2016) 21:53–9. doi: 10.1017/S1092852913000382

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Najafabadi M, Sheikh M, Hemayattalab R. The effect of SPARK on social and motor skills of children with autism. Pediatr Neonatol. (2018) 59:481–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pedneo.2017.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Kidd S, Berry-Kravis E, Choo T, Chen C, Esler A, Hoffmann A, et al. Improving the diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder in fragile X Syndrome by adapting the social communication questionnaire and the social responsiveness scale-2. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50:3276–95. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04148-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Morrison K, Pinkham A, Kelsven S. Psychometric evaluation of social cognitive measures for adults with autism. Autism Res. (2019) 12:766–78. doi: 10.1002/aur.2084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Chen Q, Qiao Y, Xu X, You X, Tao Y. Urine organic acids as potential biomarkers for autism-spectrum disorder in chinese children. Front Cell Neurosci. (2019) 13:150.

Google Scholar

57. Cheng H, Yu J, Xu L, Li J. Power spectrum of spontaneous cerebral homodynamic oscillation shows a distinct pattern in autism spectrum disorder. Biomed Opt Exp. (2019) 10:1383–92. doi: 10.1364/BOE.10.001383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Wang J, Wang L. Prediction and prioritization of autism-associated long non-coding RNAs using gene expression and sequence features. BMC Bioinform. (2020) 21:505. doi: 10.1186/s12859-020-03843-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Case-Smith J, Weaver L, Fristad M. A systematic review of sensory processing interventions for children with autism spectrum disorders. Autism. (2015) 19:133–48. doi: 10.1177/1362361313517762

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Siemann J, Veenstra-VanderWeele J, Wallace M. Approaches to understanding multisensory dysfunction in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. (2020) 13:1430–49. doi: 10.1002/aur.2375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism, sensory, social, core symptom, SSP, clinical scale

Citation: Zhai J, Li X, Zhou Y, Fan L, Xia W, Wang X, Li Y, Hou M, Wang J and Wu L (2023) Correlation and predictive ability of sensory characteristics and social interaction in children with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychiatry 14:1056051. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1056051

Received: 28 September 2022; Accepted: 13 March 2023;
Published: 06 April 2023.

Edited by:

Lawrence Fung, Stanford University, United States

Reviewed by:

Winnie Dunn, University of Missouri, United States
Michael J. Morrier, Morrier Consulting LLC, United States

Copyright © 2023 Zhai, Li, Zhou, Fan, Xia, Wang, Li, Hou, Wang and Wu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jia Wang, wangjiahyd@163.com; Lijie Wu, wulijiehyd@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.