Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 02 February 2023
Sec. Computational Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Computational Models of Brain in Cognitive Function and Mental Disorder View all 10 articles

Brain network changes in adult victims of violence

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, Faculty of Medicine, RWTH Aachen University, Aachen, Germany
  • 2JARA-BRAIN Institute Brain Structure and Function, INM-10, Institute of Neuroscience and Medicine, Jülich Research Centre, Jülich, Germany
  • 3Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition, and Behavior, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmengen, Netherlands

Introduction: Stressful experiences such as violence can affect mental health severely. The effects are associated with changes in structural and functional brain networks. The current study aimed to investigate brain network changes in four large-scale brain networks, the default mode network, the salience network, the fronto-parietal network, and the dorsal attention network in self-identified victims of violence and controls who did not identify themselves as victims.

Materials and methods: The control group (n = 32) was matched to the victim group (n = 32) by age, gender, and primary psychiatric disorder. Sparse inverse covariance maps were derived from functional resting-state measurements and from T1 weighted structural data for both groups.

Results: Our data underlined that mostly the salience network was affected in the sample of self-identified victims. In self-identified victims with a current psychiatric diagnosis, the dorsal attention network was mostly affected underlining the potential role of psychopathological alterations on attention-related processes.

Conclusion: The results showed that individuals who identify themselves as victim demonstrated significant differences in all considered networks, both within- and between-network.

1. Introduction

The link between victimization and poor mental health has been recognized in many studies (13). Severe forms of victimization include physical and sexual violence. Additionally, different forms of abuse such as threat, stalking, blackmailing are often experiences as severe harm to an individual’s life (4, 5) including consequences such as depression, anxiety, suicidal ideation, and panic attacks. The self-identification as a victim thereby does not necessarily agree with external labeling (1) and the same event might be perceived very differently between individuals. Importantly, even if not ensured by external sources, self-perceived victimization is stressful and associated with negative consequences such as self-blame, loneliness, anxiety, and low self-worth (6). Further studies suggest that executive functioning is reduced in individuals who have experienced violence during early childhood or adolescence (7, 8). Given the severe and often protracted effects of perceived victimization, it is important to determine how this can lead to mental problems. In this respect, the brain plays an important role. However, while victimization represents a severe risk factor for mental disorders, only little is known with respect to victimization as a trans-diagnostic risk factor on the neural level.

Previous studies have investigated if the exposure to violence can affect brain morphology and brain function. Such associations between violence exposures and brain structural changes have been investigated for gray (GM) and white matter (WM). In GM, changes in volume (913), cortical thickness (CT) (1216), surface area (1214, 16), and local gyrification (14) were observed in connection to the experience of childhood neglect and abuse. Mostly GM and CT reductions were observed in victims of violence [e.g., (1114)]. Nevertheless, increases were reported in female survivors of intimate partner violence (10). Additionally, GM volume reductions in the prefrontal cortex were reported (17), and these findings were recently confirmed in a trans-diagnostic sample of adult participants who reported childhood maltreatment. Another line of studies investigated neural changes in association to combat-related trauma (9, 18, 19). Interestingly these studies showed that combat exposure related volume reductions were distinct from reductions related to a PTSD and depression diagnosis (20, 21). In sum, structural abnormalities were observed in both cortical and subcortical regions in different samples in all tissues, although some findings (22) argue against a strong association of WM changes and the experience of violence. Furthermore, a recent study pointed to alterations in brain organization (23) based on the covariance of GM volume between selected areas in victims versus controls. Studies that explicitly focus on the subjective self-identification as a victim including a broad definition of violence in this field are lacking.

Changes in brain activity also have been associated with the exposure to violence (2428). The majority of fMRI studies in different populations that had been exposed to violence showed deviations in activation during cognitive or emotional tasks [for a review see (29)], and functional connectivity alterations occurred during emotion provoking tasks (26, 30, 31) and resting state fMRI (32). Functional differences between survivors of intimate partner violence (IPV) with a PTSD diagnosis and a non-traumatized group were reported in the anterior insula, which is the hub of the salience network (26). Furthermore, decreased connectivity among the anterior insula, amygdala, and anterior cingulate cortex (ACC), was reported for IPV related PTSD during a face-match task (31). Moreover, painful stimulation led to an elevated activation of the right middle insula and the right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex in IPV survivors with PTSD (33). Potential PTSD specific influence may be expected here, since a decrease in subjective pain intensity ratings over time was accompanied by attenuation of activation within the right anterior insula, which at the same time was associated with avoidance symptoms of PTSD.

Previous results in survivors of violence were often specific to a certain age group or a specific type of violence. Many studies have focused on physical, sexual or emotional abuse experienced during childhood (34), underlining the role of the hippocampus and amygdala (17, 3537). Furthermore, in populations that experienced violence in early childhood, physical forms of violence seem to be associated more strongly with changes in amygdala and anterior cingulate cortex while emotional abuse may result in changes related to reward and mood processing circuits (38). Other findings may even suggest differences in the brain networks of individuals exposed to emotion abuse versus neglect (39). While some studies have successfully shown changes in brain activation for specific victimized populations, brain changes have–to our knowledge–not been studied in transdiagnostic samples of individuals who identified themselves as victim including a broad definition of victimization.

The existing literature demonstrates the necessity to study the relationship between brain modulations and subjective victimization as a trans-diagnostic phenomenon, thereby enabling the identification of neural changes independent of a mental health diagnosis. Additionally, specific types of experienced violence have mostly been investigated in specific groups, for example combat related exposure in males and intimate partner violence in females. To our knowledge, currently there is no study that included male and female adults identifying themselves as victims independent of the type of violence, or age of the individual. Furthermore, only a few studies have so far investigated large-scale network changes simultaneously on a structural and functional level. Specifically, changes in the default mode network (DMN), the fronto-parietal network (FPN), and the salience network (SN) as well as the dorsal attention network (DAN) (4042) have been proposed as prominent characteristics of psychiatric disorders and as markers of exposure to violence. Thus, studying changes in these networks and their association with previous victimization may support the identification of neural risk factors for mental health issues, independent of a specific diagnosis.

The current study aimed to identify differences in structural and functional covariance in the DMN, FPN, SN, and DAN in two different groups: The first group (V) was characterized by self-identification as victim of violence; the second group (NV) was matched to the V group by age, gender and the primary psychiatric diagnosis. Our study did not exclude participants based on the psychiatric diagnosis and represented therefore a more realistic clinical population, which enabled the investigation of structural and functional brain network connectivity. To investigate structural and functional organization differences, we focused on pre-determined regions of interest (ROIs) in the DMN, FPN, SN, and DAN, and analyzed group differences in between and within network covariance patterns of both function and structure.

We expected to discover differences in structural and functional organization of the four large-scale brain networks between V and NV, independently of any psychiatric diagnosis. Similar changes in covariance in SN and DMN were expected in the V group. We assumed, that presence of a psychiatric diagnosis played an additional important role in the difference between V and NV. Therefore, we performed a diagnosis-specific explorative analysis. As a secondary hypothesis, we assumed that the group of V with a present acute psychiatric diagnosis (VD+) differed from the NV with a present acute psychiatric diagnosis (NVD+), and a differing structural and functional covariance pattern would be observed as compared to the trans-diagnostic consideration. As a third hypothesis, we assumed, that V and NV would differ in their psychopathology, which in turn would correlate with the differences in the structural and functional covariance.

2. Materials and methods

2.1. Sample

The sample included two groups of adults of which the first group had subjectively experienced violence (V), while the matched control group had not experienced violence before (NV). Inclusion criteria for both groups were: (i) age between 18 and 60 years, (ii) right-handedness, (iii) MRI suitability, and (iv) absence of any neurological diseases. Specific inclusion criterion for the V group was the prior experience of at least one type of the following forms of violence. The experience of violence was verified by a screening instrument and a detailed qualitative interview which were developed within the “Gender Violence” project (43, 44). The definition of violence used applied to this screening instrument and the interview based on the WHO standards (45) defining physical, emotional, and sexual violence. Orienting to previous studies, and because it is a frequent precursor or other forms of violence during intimate partnerships (46), economic violence (financial abuse) was added as a further category in the screening. Physical forms included all forms of body attacks such as hitting, kicking, shaking, spitting; sexual forms include all sexual acts without agreement such as coercion, sexual assault or rape; emotional forms included permanent insults, humiliation, bullying, stalking, threat; economic forms included robbery, passing of salary, prohibition to fulfill basic needs. The NV group included only individuals who negated any prior experience of violence at a primary screening and did not identify themselves as victimized. The V group was recruited from the participant pool of a large study in which detailed semi-structured interviews about the experience of violence were performed. Within this larger study, participants in the V group were recruited on the one hand in cooperation with an intervention center against domestic violence in Aachen, Germany (“Frauen helfen Frauen e.V.”) who asked individuals with experiences of violence if they would be willing to participate in the study. Participation in the study was voluntary and completely independent of any further consultation. On the other hand, we distributed flyers describing different forms of violence, the study aims and contact points for individuals seeking help in all departments in the university hospital Aachen, including the emergency department and the psychiatric department. Individuals who self-identified themselves as victims of violence and wanted to participate could notify study personal via phone or email. Flyers were also distributed at other public places offering consultation or therapy to potential victimized individuals such as ambulant therapists. For individuals that were in addition to study participation or independent of study participation seeking help and that were not supported otherwise a team of trained experts and psychologists offered consultation as part of the project. The fMRI study only included individuals that had undergone the qualitative interview in the main arm of the study and a further screening concerning MRI criteria if participants were interested in taking part in the fMRI study. 33.3% of all recruited participants took part in the fMRI study as well. The NV group was directly recruited via flyers and at the university hospital RWTH Aachen, specifically the Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics. All participants gave their written informed consent to participate in the study and received a compensatory payment of 85 Euros. Included participants additionally underwent the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview [MINI; (47)], which allowed us to match both groups for age, sex, and MINI diagnoses. Overall, the V group included 49 subjects and the NV group 41 individuals of which 25 in the V group and 20 in the NV group had any kind of psychiatric diagnosis.

2.2. Study protocol

The study protocol was approved by the internal Ethics Committee of the RWTH Aachen University and thus complied with the ethical principles stated in the Declaration of Helsinki. The complete study procedure consisted of an initial resting state fMRI scan, a social stress paradigm, an emotion induction paradigm, a second resting state scan, an anatomical scan, neuropsychological tests, and several self-report questionnaires.

Besides the behavioral variables, the present investigation focused on the anatomical scan and the first resting state scan. Imaging data were acquired on a whole-body Siemens 3T Trio scanner (Siemens AG; Erlangen, Germany) equipped with 12 channel head coil, located at the RWTH Aachen University hospital in Germany, whereas some subjects were measured after the scanner upgrade to Prisma. During the resting state acquisition, participants were instructed to relax and lie still with eyes opened, focusing a fixation cross presented on a black screen. Afterward, all participants assured that they had not fallen asleep.

In order to test if groups differed with regard to psychopathology severity, stress coping and neuropsychological functioning, after the MRI procedure, we quantified (i) the strength of depressive symptoms through the Beck Depression Inventory [BDI; (48)], (ii) state and trait anxiety scores through the State-Trait Anxiety Inventory [STAI; (49)], and (iii) information on stress exposure and stress symptoms through the Stress and Coping Inventory [SCI; (50)]. In the V group, we also measured perceived distress caused by violence experiences through the Impact of Event Scale [IES; (51)]. Neuropsychological tests included the digit span [ZNS, forward and backward; Hamburg Wechsler Intelligence test (HAWIE-R); (52)], the verbal fluency test [VLT; (53)], a measure for verbal intelligence [Mehrfach Wortschatztest version B, MWT_B; (54)] and a test for shared attention and executive functions/cognitive flexibility [Trail making test, TMT; (55)]. From the introduced neuropsychological tests, descriptive variables were derived: TMT comprised the difference between the acquired TMT version A and version B; VLT_1 represented the total fluency performance (i.e., phonemic fluency und semantic fluency), while VLT_2 represented switching (i.e., phonemic switching und semantic switching), and HAWIE-R (ZNS) represented the sum of the forward and backward digit-span tests.

Several participants could not be included in our analyses due to the following reasons: (i) missing anatomical (n = 4) or any resting state scans (n = 7) due to technical problems, (ii) incomplete coverage of the whole brain during structural scan (n = 10), (iii) influence of alcohol (n = 1), (iv) sudden nausea (n = 1), and (v) lack of credibility of statements due to several contradictions (n = 1). Thus, the final sample consisted of 64 participants, comprising 32 participants who experienced violence, and 32 controls. The number of V, who suffered from a current psychiatric diagnosis (VD+), was 25, and the number of V without a current diagnosis (VD–) was 7. The number of NV, who suffered from a current psychiatric diagnosis (NVD+), was 20, and the number of NV without a diagnosis (NVD–) was 12.

2.3. Voxel based morphometry

To investigate structural differences between both groups, we acquired a T1-weighed image for each participant using an MPRAGE sequence (TR = 2,300 ms, TE = 3.03 ms, flip angle = 9°, FOV = 256 × 256 mm2, 176 sagittal slices, voxel size = 1 × 1 × 1 mm3). Structural imaging data were preprocessed using the Computational Anatomy toolbox (CAT 121). First, each scan was manually reoriented to the intercommisural plane. After correction for inhomogeneities in field intensity, affine and non-linear normalization to MNI standard space was applied using the DARTEL default template within a unified segmentation model (56). Then, images were segmented into GM, WM, and cerebrospinal fluid. Additionally, the GM volumes were scaled by the amount of contraction applied during the preceding normalization. This modulation with Jacobian determinants ensured that the total volume of GM corresponded to that of the original images. Finally, the modulated GM segments were smoothed using a Gaussian kernel of 8 mm FHWM which was suggested to improve the morphometric examination of smaller and larger brain regions (57, 58). A subsequent homogeneity check did not identify any outliers. The ensuing voxel-based morphometry data were used to examine covariance in GM volumes in the sample.

2.4. Functional resting state

To compare brain function between V and NV, 250 functional images for each participant were acquired using a EPI sequence (TR = 1,600 ms, TE = 30 ms, flip angle = 67°, FOV = 192 × 192 mm2, matrix size = 64 × 64, 26 transversal slices, voxel size = 3 × 3 × 4.2 mm3, acquisition order = interleaved ascending). Functional imaging data were preprocessed using the functional connectivity toolbox (CONN 18a2). Initially, the first four scans of each participant were discarded to allow for magnetic field saturation. Then, the individual resting state time series were preprocessed according to the following steps: (i) realignment and unwarping, (ii) slice-time correction, (iii) outlier detection [97th percentiles using Artifact Detection Toolbox (ART)], (iv) segmentation and spatial normalization to MNI standard space, and (v) smoothing (Gaussian kernel of 8 mm FWHM). Subsequently, the pre-processed time series were denoised to account for potential confounding effects of (i) 6 motion parameters, (ii) their derivatives, (iii) squares of the 6 motion parameters and their derivatives, (iv) mean CSF and WM signal (v) outlier regressors from ART. Additionally, quadratic detrending and despiking before regression were applied. We did not use global signal regression. Furthermore, the time-series were band-pass filtered to retain signals between 0.01 and 0.08 Hz. This frequency range likely represented neural signal and was less susceptible to physiological noise (59, 60). The resulting resting state time series were used to investigate functional connectivity in the sample.

2.5. ROI definition

We were interested in how covariance within and between for major networks differed between V and NV. For that aim, the functional connectivity toolbox CONN was used (61). CONN’s standard network atlas was based on an independent component analysis of the functional resting state data of a large sample of healthy adults (61, 62). Although variances in the brain structure are expected in healthy controls and patient groups, applying the atlas information based on healthy adults for the investigation of patient groups is considered valid because previous studies have shown differences in the DNM, SN, and FPN based on different whole brain nodes and seeds [for a meta-analysis see Koch et al. (63)] suggesting robust group differences in these networks despite of potential structural differences. The atlas provides an established brain parcellation that divided the DMN, SN, DAN, and FPN into 19 spatially distinct network nodes, which were parts of the brain networks (Figure 1). The DMN covered the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), the bilateral lateral parietal cortex (LPCs), and the precuneus (PCUN). The SN included the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) as well as the bilateral anterior insula (AIs), the rostral prefrontal cortex (RPFCs), and the supramarginal gyrus (SMGs). The DAN consisted bilaterally of frontal eye fields (FEFs) and the intraparietal sulci (IPSs). The FPN comprised both the right and left lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFCs) and the posterior parietal cortex (PPCs). The 19 investigated network nodes served as ROIs and were used to extract structural and functional brain information from individuals in the V and NV groups. For each participant, brain data was averaged across all voxels belonging to a particular ROI. This yielded individual average GM volumes, average GM density and average functional resting state time series for each ROI. The extracted GM volumes, densities and time series were z-standardized individually. This z-standardization mainly served two purposes in the following analyses: (i) to ensure the comparability of ROIs, and (ii) to enable the interpretation of covariance measures as correlation (= normalized covariance). To avoid potential confounding effects in the brain data, we accounted for sex, age, MINI diagnosis, antidepressants, and the number of other psychotropic drugs. In the structural analyses, we also accounted for total intracranial volume. Numerical confounds were z-standardized, while scale confounds were dummy encoded. Deconfounding on the group level was performed for time series in CONN, while for GM volumes and densities it was done with NiftiMapsMasker from nilearn package (64). The extracted network information served as input for the estimation of structural covariance and functional connectivity matrices in each group.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Neuroanatomical visualization of four target networks, according to CONN’s standard atlas. The default mode network is shown in dark green and consists of the medial prefrontal cortex (MPFC), the bilateral lateral parietal cortex (LPCs), and the precuneus (PCUN). The saliency network (purple) includes the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) as well as the bilateral anterior insula (AIs), rostral prefrontal cortex (RPFCs), and supramarginal gyrus (SMGs). The dorsal attention network (turquoise) consists of frontal eye field (FEFs) and intraparietal sulcus (IPSs) bilaterally. The frontoparietal network (yellow) comprises both the right and left lateral prefrontal cortex (LPFCs) and posterior parietal cortex (PPCs). Regions of interest are plotted on an MNI standard brain in anterior, lateral, and posterior view using MRIcroGL (https://www.nitrc.org/projects/mricrogl/).

2.6. Sparse inverse covariance

To estimate covariance differences between the groups V/NV and VD+/NVD+, partial correlations between the 19 nodes were calculated on the group level. Group level partial correlations (assuming the group of subjects underlies the same functional or VBM structure) were calculated using sparse inverse covariance estimation [covariance precision from GraphicalLassoCV, from Python sklearn (64)]. By accounting for the influence of other brain regions, partial correlations as compared to full correlations yield direct, unbiased relationships between two ROIs (65, 66). Partial correlations could be estimated by sparse inverse covariance (6769). An L1 penalty automatically set less important entries in the connectivity matrix to zero which enabled a robust estimation also in smaller samples (66, 69). The sparsity degree was internally chosen via a 3-fold cross-validation that ascertained the generalizability of the model to new data (70, 71). For calculations we used Python 3.7, primarily using the neuroimaging package nilearn (72) and the machine learning package scikit-learn (64).

To investigate the differences in structural covariance between V and NV, we estimated gray matter volumes (GMV), gray matter densities (GMD) and gray matter masses (GMM) of ROIs, and performed independent t-tests, to see if there are significant group differences between the values of GMD, GMV or GMM. Further, we investigated partial correlations in the estimated GM parameters between the 19 ROIs on group level separately in the V and NV groups. For this purpose, we used sparse inverse covariance estimation to determine the partial correlation between brain volumes on group level and reported the differences in covariance between V and NV. A single value in each subject for a given GM volume in each ROI prohibited estimation of individual partial correlation matrices.

Afterward, we performed similar calculations for functional data, to investigate the differences in functional covariance between V and NV. An independent t-test probed for significant differences between the ROI time series of the V and NV groups. Again, sparse inverse covariance was used to generate partial correlations between brain volumes of each time series of resting state for each ROI. Analyses were performed separately for the V and NV groups. Described methods for GM variables and resting state are depicted in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Methods applied to the whole cohort for (A): Variables derived from gray matter (GM) such as gray matter volumes (GMV), gray matter densities (GMD), and gray matter masses (GMM) and (B): Time series of resting state fMRI. Sparse inverse covariance was estimated between the 19 nodes on the group level. Further, the covariances were compared between V and NV. The differences in covariances were correlated with the questionnaires.

The analysis steps described above for the determination of covariance differences, as well as statistical tests for group differences and brain-behavior associations, described in the following text, were repeated for a separate subgroup of subjects of the V and NV group with a current psychiatric diagnosis (D+). The subjects with a current diagnosis and an experience of violence (VD+) were compared to those with a current diagnosis but without an experience of violence (NVD+). These complementary calculations were performed to compare more homogeneous samples (see Tables 2, 3). Due to a small number of participant without a psychiatric diagnosis, we did not perform sub-analyses in these small V and NV groups.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Mean and standard deviation of the subscales on the impact of events scale, the estimated duration of exposure to violence and the age at the first exposure to violence in the V group contrasting individuals who had experiences physical or sexual forms of violence to those who exclusively experienced other forms.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Sample characteristics for victims of violence (V) and non-victims (NV): Binary variables (sex, MINI diagnosis, and antidepressants): Statistical comparison of groups performed via chi-square test of independence; Continuous variables: mean ± standard deviation, statistical comparison of groups performed via independent two-sample t-test for normally distributed features [STAI trait, TMT, VLT2, HAWIE-R (ZNS)] and Mann Whitney U-test for non-parametric cases.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Sample characteristics for victims of violence (VD+) and non-victims (NVD+) with a current diagnosis: Binary variables (sex, MINI diagnosis, and antidepressants): Statistical comparison of groups performed via chi-square test of independence; Continuous variables: mean ± standard deviation, statistical comparison of groups performed via independent two-sample t-test for normally distributed features [SCI stress exposure, TMT, VLT2, HAWIE-R (ZNS)] and Mann Whitney U-test for non-parametric cases (the rest).

2.7. Group differences in covariance

Based on partial correlation maps, we examined group differences in covariance between ROIs. Negative values described a relative decrease in covariance, or cross-talk, between regions in the V group compared to the NV group. Complementarily, positive values described a relative increase of covariance, or higher level of cross-talk between the nodes. To inspect the observed structural and functional covariance patterns differences between victims and controls, we employed non-parametric test for mean differences (73, 74). To this end, we compared the data of V to the general distribution simulated by randomly drawing 104 bootstrap samples of the NV, with replacement. Thus, every bootstrapped subsample consisted of 41 subjects for the NV group, and 20 in the NV group had any kind of psychiatric diagnosis. Structural and functional correlation matrices of the victims were compared to the bootstrapped 99.999% population intervals of the controls, which corresponds to testing for significant differences at a corrected, two-sided alpha-level of 10–5. The same analysis was performed for VD+ versus NVD+.

2.8. Summary of covariance differences across imaging modalities

To determine the convergence of network-specific covariance differences between the pairs of 171 nodes, we summed up the findings by calculating the frequency of differences in covariance between V and NV in every node across all imaging modalities. This number thus represented the total number of group differences in a network, to which these nodes belonged. The same was done separately for the VD+ versus NVD+ comparison. Since four networks were investigated, ten possible combinations for within- and between-network covariance existed. Four of those described the within-network covariance (DMN-DMN, SN-SN, DAN-DAN, and FPN-FPN), and the rest described the between-network covariance (DMN-SN, DMN-DAN, DMN-FPN, SN-DAN, SN-FPN, DAN-FPN). Additionally, we aimed to investigate the importance of differing covariance for a specific single node. To this end, the frequency of each node being involved in a different covariance comparing V versus NV and VD+ versus NVD+ was recorded.

2.9. Brain-behavior associations

Finally, significant structural and functional network aberrations were probed for their association with specific behavioral and neuropsychological variables. We used canonical correlation analysis (CCA) which investigated internal relationships between two sets of variables by seeking maximal correlations between combinations of variables in both sets (75, 76). Thus, the aim of the CCA was to test if a significant amount of variance of structural/functional network aberrations and behavioral/neuropsychological variables across subjects could be explained by pairs of canonical variates (modes of co-variation), and to discover the internal relationships between the two sets (74). The latter was done by the calculation of the correlations between each variable and the corresponding canonical variates.

For functional data, we estimated covariance between the nodes, which demonstrated significant differences between the subject groups, for each participant on the individual level. For functional data, we estimated covariance for each participant on the individual level. Statistical significance of canonical correlations was determined sequentially with a Wilks’ Lambda, using F-approximation (77). All p-values were Bonferroni-corrected to account for multiple comparisons and tested at a corrected alpha level of 0.05.

3. Results

3.1. Group characteristics

Within the V group experience of violence differed regarding the experiences type, length and age of exposure. In total, nine of 32 included participants in the V group experienced only emotional and economic violence and only one participant reported to have been exposed to economic violence as only form of violence. All other participants had experienced physical or sexual violence including multiple forms. Only 5 patients reported to have experienced physical violence without any other form of violence and only one participant reported to have been exposed to sexual violence only. Overall 22 participants reported to have been exposed to several forms of violence, while 10 reported only one form of violence. The estimated duration participants in the V group were exposed to one or more types of violence repeatedly was 8.3 years with only three individuals being exposed to violence (physical) only a single time. While the estimated mean duration did not differ significantly between individuals who had experienced physical or sexual violence (among others) and those who did not [t(30) = 1.19, p = 1.22, Table 1]. However, age of the first exposure was significantly lower individuals who experiences physical or sexual violence compared to those who did not [t(30) = 2.02, p = 0.035, Table 1]. Comparing the mean scores of the IES subscales intrusion, avoidance and hyperarousal of individuals who experienced (among others) physical or sexual violence in contrast (23) to those who did not report any of these forms (9) showed no significant differences in any scale [intrusion: t(28) = 1.36, p = 0.092; avoidance: t(28) = 0.31, p = 0.389; hyperarousal: t(28) = 0.58; p = 0.285]. Two participants (experiencing physical forms of violence) did not want to answer the IES, which is why the mean scores are reported only for a group of 30 participants.

As shown by non-significant group differences, we were able to successfully match self-identified victims and the control group in age, sex, and MINI diagnoses (Table 2). The groups did not significantly differ in any neuropsychological variable or questionnaire.

Comparison of groups only including participants with a current psychiatric diagnosis (VD+ and NVD+) were provided in Table 3. The groups did not significantly differ in any neuropsychological variable or questionnaire. Furthermore, no significant differences were discovered neither between GMM, GMV or GMD nor between the time series in ROIs of the V and NV groups and the VD+ and NVD+ groups.

3.2. Structural covariance in brain networks in all subjects, independent on the psychiatric diagnosis

Based on the structural covariance matrix, and after performing previously described bootstrapping to identify differences in covariance between V and NV, we identified 9 out of 171 node pairs, that demonstrated differences in GMM covariance between groups at a corrected alpha level of 10–4 (Figure 3A). Significantly less covariance between regions in V was observed in all significantly different covariance measures. Within-network disturbances emerged only in the SN and constituted 33% of all detected aberrations, demonstrating less covariance and therefore lower homogeneity in the structural organization of V as compared to NV. Between-network-wise, FPN, DAN, SN, and DMN revealed aberrations in half of their nodes. On the other hand, GMD (Figure 3C) demonstrated 4 aberrant connections, all of which overlapped with GMM covariance differences. These consistent aberrations were observed in SN/DAN and SN/FPN. GMV (Figure 3E) demonstrated 5 aberrant connections, which showed reduced covariance in V, same as in GMM. These aberrations were observed in GMM, and occurred within SN, in SN/FPN, SN/DMN, and SN/DAN.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Group differences in structural covariance of gray matter masses (GMM) (A), gray matter densities (GMD) (C), and gray matter volumes (GMV) (E) within and between four major brain networks for the full sample, and group differences in structural covariance of GMM (B), GMV (D), and GMD (F) within and between four major brain networks for the subsample of subjects with a current psychiatric diagnosis. Squares indicate significant differences in partial correlations between V and NV. Colors on the axes and of the nodes correlated with the networks: DMN–green, SN–purple, DAN–cyan, FPN–yellow. Nodes with within-network differences were highlighted with black squares.

3.3. Functional covariance in brain networks in all subjects, independent of psychiatric diagnosis

The comparison of functional covariance matrices yielded 2 out of 171 functional connections that significantly differed between groups at a corrected alpha-level of 10–4 (Figure 4A). Significantly higher covariance between regions in V was observed within the FPN network. Between-network-wise, less covariance was observed in V in FPN/DAN covariance.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Group differences in functional covariance of RS within and between four major brain networks for the full sample (A) and for the subsample of subjects with a current psychiatric diagnosis (B). Squares indicate significant differences in partial correlations between victims and controls. Colors on the axes and of the nodes correlated with the networks: DMN–green, SN–purple, DAN–cyan, FPN–yellow. Nodes with within-network differences were highlighted with black squares.

We furthermore investigated differences in structural and functional covariance between groups with a current MINI diagnosis (VD+ and NVD+).

3.4. Structural covariance in brain networks in subjects with a current psychiatric diagnosis

We identified 13 out of 171 pairs of nodes (Figure 3B) that differed significantly in GMM between VD+ and NVD+. Only one node (DMN/DAN) showed a slight overexpression in VD+, as compared to NVD+. Lower covariance between regions in VD+ was observed in the remaining 12 connections. Within-network disturbances emerged only in the SN. Between-network-wise, the aberrations were observed in DMN, FPN and DAN across all modalities. GMV (Figure 3D) demonstrated 10 aberrant connections, which showed less covariance in VD+. Within-network covariance aberrations were observed in SN, and in DAN. In contrast to the whole sample GMV analysis (V/NV), group differences in the VD+/NVD+ sample demonstrated a decrease in DAN covariance with DMN and FPN. Furthermore, GMD demonstrated 9 aberrant connections, with significant decrease in covariance in DAN/DMN and DAN/FPN, and with within-network covariance aberrations in SN (Figure 3F).

3.5. Functional covariance in brain networks in subjects with a current psychiatric diagnosis

The comparison of functional covariance matrices yielded 4 out of 171 functional connections, that significantly differed between the VD+ and NVD+ groups at a corrected alpha-level of 10–4 (Figure 4B). Covariance in VD+ differed from NVD+ in the same four nodes as in the V versus NV group, and in the three additional nodes. In these nodes, SN/DMN and SN/FPN demonstrated higher, and DAN/DMN lower covariance in VD+ as compared to NVD+. No within-network differences were observed.

3.6. Across-modality covariance differences

The summaries of structural and functional covariance differences between the two V and NV groups across all modalities were depicted in Figure 5A for V versus NV, and in Figure 5B for VD+ versus NVD+. The histograms demonstrated that covariance in VD+ was to a higher degree different from NVD+, than in the analogous comparison of the V versus NV. Specifically, this meant that the networks in VD+ were less covariant between each other, than in NVD+. In particular, regarding the VD+ versus NVD+ comparison, the DAN was associated with the majority of within- and between-network covariance differences (33% of all differences), followed by the SN (28%). In contrast, the main sample (V versus NV) showed mostly differences in the SN (49% of all differences), followed by the FPN (26%). Upon characterizing the affected network nodes on the individual level (Figure 6), we demonstrated that the DMN and DAN nodes were affected to a lower degree in the main sample than in the D + sample (10 versus 14% for DMN and 20 versus 27% for DAN). On the other hand, SN and FPN were affected more in the full sample (40 versus 32% for SN and 30 versus 27% for FPN).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. (A) Number of all significant group differences in covariance between networks in the main sample. (B) Differences in covariance between networks in D+ sample (subjects with a psychiatric diagnosis). Colors represent networks: DMN–green, SN–purple, DAN–cyan, FPN–yellow.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. Covariance differences across modalities in all subjects (full sample for V/NV comparison, and D+ sample for VD+/NVD+ comparison). Colors represent networks: DMN–green, SN–purple, DAN–cyan, FPN–yellow. Shaded areas represented density functions of the histograms and highlighted differences in frequency of node involvement.

3.7. Association with psychopathology

Finally, we examined if the observed group differences in structural and functional covariance were related to psychopathological symptoms (STAI trait, BDI, SCI stress exposure), as well as to neuropsychological functions [VFT1 with one, and VFT2 with two categories, TMT, MWT and HAWIE-R (ZNS)] using CCA. In the full sample (V versus NV), the analysis revealed a single highly significant CCA mode that related brain connectomes to subject measures (r = 0.94, p = 0.008). We observed that 94% of the variation in brain connectomes was explained by the variation in questionnaires. Since only the first CCA mode was significant, the first canonical variate for brain measures (CCX_1) was plotted against the first canonical variate for the questionnaires (CCY_1) in the scatter plot (Figure 7). These correlations between each variable and the corresponding canonical variate were used to interpret the first CCA mode, and the correlations with the correlation over r > 0.2 were provided in Table 4. The contributions of the variables to the CCA modes were also demonstrated in Figure 7. All correlations between the first canonical variable for brain, and the brain measures were uniformly large, and were represented by all included measures of DMN and SN. Among the psychopathological symptoms and neuropsychology variables, STAI contributed to CCY_1 to the highest proportion. Thus, CCY_1 can be considered as an anxiety measure. Thus, due to the significance of the CCA decomposition, CCX_1 and CCY_1 demonstrated high correlation, and uncovered dependence between anxiety traits and a linear combination of structural brain measures of SN and DMN. However, the CCX_1 and CCY_1 did not differ significantly between V and NV. Thus, the latent variables did not reflect the victimization status.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Correlation of the first canonical correlation analysis (CCA) mode variables for the full sample (upper right), and contributions of the variables to the first and second CCA dimensions.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Correlation between canonical correlation analysis (CCA) variates and variables in the full sample.

In the D+ groups, the number of subjects was not sufficient to estimate the modes of variance reliably for both RS and GM brain measures. However, based on the previous analysis of the full sample, we considered for the CCA analysis only GM brain measures. The analysis revealed a single highly significant CCA mode that related brain connectomes to subject measures (r = 0.99, p < 10–5). Again, the first canonical variate for brain measures (CCX_1) was plotted against the first canonical variate for the questionnaires (CCY_1) in the scatter plot (Figure 8). The highest correlations between each variable and the corresponding canonical variate were provided in Table 5. The contributions of the variables to the CCA modes were also demonstrated in Figure 8. The correlations between the first canonical variable for brain, and the brain measures were uniformly large, and were again represented by the measures of DMN and SN. Therefore, the canonical variate CCX_1 could again be considered as an overall measure across all brain measures. On the other hand, TMT and ZNS contributed to CCY_1 to the highest proportion. Thus, the dependence between TMT and ZNS, and a linear combination of structural brain measures of SN and DMN, was discovered.

FIGURE 8
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 8. Correlation of the first canonical correlation analysis (CCA) mode variables for the sample of subjects with a current psychiatric diagnosis (upper right), and contributions of the variables to the first and second CCA dimensions.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Correlation between canonical correlation analysis (CCA) variates and variables for the sample of subjects with a current psychiatric diagnosis.

4. Discussion

The current study aimed to identify differences in structural and functional covariance in the DNM, FPN, SN, and DAN in a trans-diagnostic sample of individuals who identified themselves as victims of violence. This sample was compared to individuals who did not indicate any prior experience of violence but had a similar history of mental disorders. To further limit the influence of different psychopathologies on the differences in network covariance in both groups, two comparisons were made: a comparison of victims and non-victims in the whole sample (V versus NV), and in a subsample with the present psychiatric diagnosis (D+ group: VD+ versus NVD+). Applying multiple comparisons correction, the only differences between V and NV was discovered in BDI and SCI stress exposure, both higher in V. On the other hand, the only difference between VD+ versus NVD+ was discovered in VLT_1, which was higher in NV. These differences may be linked to the exposure to violence indirectly as the patients in this study who mostly had experienced violence over a long time and with multiple incidents may have had an increased severity of emotional and cognitive symptoms compared to other patients who may not have had any traumatic experiences.

While no differences between the V and NV group nor between VD+ and NVD+ group were observed neither in GMM/GMV/GMD nor in the time series in ROIs, the relative organization of the brain seemed to be different between groups. Specifically, differences in structural and functional covariance within and between the four selected networks were discovered. Sparse inverse covariance of the GM parameters and RS time series between the regions showed both positive and negative partial covariance differences within and between networks in both the full sample comparisons, and in the D+ sample comparisons.

Differences in the covariance in all four investigated networks, detected in V versus NV, may reflect organizational differences in the brain of victimized individuals related to specific characteristics of the group. However, we did not find any correlation between observed neural differences and the self-reports. There may be different explanations: on the one hand, self-reports may have not reliably reflected well-being and psychopathological symptoms due to the influence of self-perceptual abilities and social desirability. On the other hand, differences in network organization may have had heterogeneous sources or were related to further variables not specifically assessed in this study. While single values of psychopathological symptoms and neuropsychology did not differ in comparisons of V versus NV and VD+ versus NVD+, CCA analysis discovered significant CCA modes in both cases. This way, the questionnaires of the full sample, mainly represented by anxiety, related to the brain measures of DMN and SN. On the other hand, neural measures of the DMN and SN in the D+ sample explained the variability in shared attention and executive functions/cognitive flexibility as well as working memory, based on the uncovered single significant CCA mode. Evidence was found for the relationship between working memory and executive functioning, which might point to the common executive attention construct (78). While no direct evidence was found, the strongly affected covariance of the DAN might underlie the observed CCA modality in the D+ sample. Nevertheless, the latent variables did not reflect the victimization status, since the CCX_1 and CCY_1 did not differ between VD+ and NVD+.

Structural and functional differences did not show a large overlap, which may further support the heterogeneous sources of variance in the self-identified victims and non-victims. In patients with major depression that experienced childhood trauma disturbances in functional brain networks similar to those investigated in our study have been associated with trauma severity (79). Higher childhood trauma severity moreover predicted symptoms of anxiety which may show some similarity to the association of the anxiety related component and covariance measures of gray matter in SN and DMN. In addition to factors that directly relate to the exposure of and severity to violence, cultural influences, personality, genetics, and for the patients in both groups also access and success of mental health treatment, may contribute to the reorganization of brain structure and function. These different sources of variance may impact structure and function differently thereby concealing or enhancing organizational changes in structure or function. Our results underline what has been summarized in a systematic review on subtypes of violence and associated functional and structural alterations; deviations occur in different brain regions not only depending on the subtype of violence but also with regard to structure and function activity and connectivity or integrity (39). The biological pathway the authors suggest may be one reason for such differences. Early-life stress (exposure to violence) is expected to affect brain organization which then may result in functional network changes either directly or rather indirectly accompanying pathology. Instead of originally affected stress-related brain regions, regions that are associated with other cognitive processes affected by dysfunctional stress systems may show functional disturbances in later life. Observing such a mismatch of structural and functional covariance measures may thus support the independence or asymmetry of structural and functional network organization. The SN was affected in both the full sample and the subjects with a current diagnosis (D+), with it being the most affected network in the full sample. Structurally, in the full sample and in the D+ sample, the SN demonstrated reduced within- and between-network covariance. Functionally, however, no differences relative to NV were observed in the full sample, while in VD+ SN demonstrated increased covariance in SN/DMN and SN/FPN. Thus, VD+ in our study demonstrated functional hyperconnectivity of SN, which was not observed in victims if the group also included healthy, potentially more resilient individuals. It could be therefore hypothesized, that especially those victims with a present diagnosis exhibited a functionally disturbed SN. Nevertheless, the victimized group proved to suffer from structural aberrations in the SN covariance. In healthy populations, the SN has been recognized as necessary for the efficient regulation of activity in the DMN. Thus, the failure of this regulation would lead to inefficient cognitive control and weaker performance on cognitive control tasks (80). Correspondingly, although in our sample patients in the V and NV group were mostly not free of a psychiatric diagnosis, possibly especially the victim group suffered from loss of control. Similarly, in Bogliacino et al. (81), a reduction of cognitive control in victims of urban violence and warfare was reported. Furthermore, differences involving SN in the D+ sample were most frequent in DAN/SN, while in the full sample this was the case for the FPN/SN covariance. The latter network communication seems to be responsible for the externally directed cognition (80). Finding altered communication between FPN/SN in the whole V sample may thus underline that these regions are affected independent of the severity or violence or the mental health consequences.

Node connections of the DMN also demonstrated major differences between V and NV in both structure and function. The DMN in healthy subjects is responsible for a self-referential introspective state (82). Structurally, both the full sample and the D+ sample demonstrated reduced between-network covariance in DMN/FPN. DMN/FPN connection was shown to be responsible for introspective processes and executive function (83). Thus, the reduced structural covariance between DMN and FPN might point to possible decreased introspective processes, often observed in victims elsewhere (83). While this would have to be confirmed in future studies, such a deficit might be more pronounced in participants in the VD+ group due to an active psychiatric diagnosis. Functionally, the inverse interplay between psychopathology and large-scale brain networks of the DMN/FPN has been demonstrated before (41) for a number of psychiatric diagnoses. This might explain the observed higher number of differences in DMN/FPN structural covariance as compared to the full sample in our study. Functionally, however, we could not support this finding. While no differences were discovered in the full sample, VD+ demonstrated reduced covariance in DMN/DAN (i.e., the connection DMN.PCC/DAN.IPS), and increased covariance in DMN/SN. DMN/DAN covariance has been related to perceptual attention in healthy populations (83), while in anxiety and PTSD patients, functional covariance impairments were observed in dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (84). While DMN/DAN covariance seemed to be similar between V and NV, we cannot make a definitive statement in this regard in the D+ sample due to opposite covariance between different nodes. More research into the single nodes is required at this point.

The DAN is engaged during externally directed attentional tasks (85). In V and VD+, the DAN demonstrated reduced within-and between-network covariance both in structure and function. Additionally, to the DAN/SN and DAN/DMN differences in covariance, described above, D+ sample demonstrated a higher number of differences in structural covariance in DAN/FPN. FPN regulates DAN in accordance with goals and task demands, and it is involved in the regulation of perceptual attention (83). Recent data showed negatively associated network connectivity between DAN and FPN in subjects with depression, anxiety and suicidality (41). Thus, based on our findings and in line with others, the higher proportion of DAN/FPN covariance differences in D+ sample might be a sign of the less efficient attention processes as compared to the full sample.

In interpreting these findings, several limitations have to be taken into account. First, only selected networks were investigated, therefore differences in other networks cannot be excluded. These networks were anatomically defined which may introduce a larger bias than extracting data-driven time series as in other studies (86). The victims were self-identified victims of violence, which is a highly subjective measure, and it cannot be quantified, since no correlation between aberrant network nodes and behavioral variables were discovered. Nevertheless, it is important to investigate neural alterations related to the subjective perception as this perception may be strongly connected to mental health problems (6). Despite of attempts to account for a large heterogeneity with regard to psychopathology by matching both groups with regard to the primary psychiatric diagnosis, the total sample was too small to test different subgroups with specific disorders and subgroups without any mental disorder. In addition, the heterogeneity of the type of violence individuals were exposed to was large and the size of the sample did not allow us to test in network changes may differ depending on specific forms of violence such as (exclusively social) or non-social forms of violence. As pointed out in a recent review (38) physical and sexual violence in early childhood may seems to be associated with higher risks of PTSD and personality disorders while emotional violence more often associated with developing major depression. Animal models of physical versus non-physical abuse even suggest that brain circuit changes associated with abuse may differ. The current results, referring to the in changes of brain connectivity across all different types of violence may therefore conceal more specific changes associated with physical or non-physical violence. Further research in single nodes and in subgroups must be performed, while the study sample is to be extended. Finally, the upgrade of the scanner to Prisma while the study was carried may have introduced data variance which can reduce the classification accuracy in the data as shown in projects applying classifiers on fMRI data in multi-side projects (87).

In a nutshell, differences in functional and structural covariance between self-identified victims and people who never experienced violence or did not identify themselves as victims were observed, with a primary role of the SN. In the group with heightened pathologies and various mental disorders, most differences between victims and non-victims occurred in DAN. When the sample was controlled for psychiatric disorders, less covariance differences were observed, indicating that a major part of the network variance may reflect differences in the pathological status of two groups.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Internal Ethics Committee of the RWTH Aachen University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LW, NK, and UH designed the study. LW collected the data. AS and LW preprocessed and analyzed the data and wrote the manuscript. All authors provided feedback and approved the final manuscript version.

Funding

This work was funded by the State of North Rhine-Westphalia (NRW, Germany), the European Union through the “NRW Ziel2 Program” as a part of the European Fund for Regional Development, and by Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation)–269953372/GRK2150.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Brain Imaging Facility of the Interdisciplinary Centre for Clinical Research (IZKF) Aachen within the Faculty of Medicine at RWTH Aachen University.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Footnotes

  1. ^ www.neuro.uni-jena.de/cat
  2. ^ www.nitrc.org/projects/conn

References

1. Arseneault L, Bowes L, Shakoor S. Bullying victimization in youths and mental health problems: ‘much ado about nothing’? Psychol Med. (2010) 40:717–29. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Hedtke KA, Ruggiero KJ, Fitzgerald MM, Zinzow HM, Saunders BE, Resnick HS, et al. A longitudinal investigation of interpersonal violence in relation to mental health and substance use. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2008) 76:633–47. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.76.4.633

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Lacey KK, McPherson MD, Samuel PS, Powell Sears K, Head D. The impact of different types of intimate partner violence on the mental and physical health of women in different ethnic groups. J Interpers Violence. (2013) 28:359–85. doi: 10.1177/0886260512454743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Dokkedahl S, Kok RN, Murphy S, Kristensen TR, Bech-Hansen D, Elklit A. The psychological subtype of intimate partner violence and its effect on mental health: protocol for a systematic review and meta-analysis. Syst Rev. (2019) 8:198. doi: 10.1186/s13643-019-1118-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Stevens F, Nurse JRC, Arief B. Cyber stalking, cyber harassment, and adult mental health: a systematic review. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2021) 24:367–76. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2020.0253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Graham S, Juvonen J. Self-blame and peer victimization in middle school: an attributional analysis. Dev Psychol. (1998) 34:587–99. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.3.587

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Daly BP, Hildenbrand AK, Turner E, Berkowitz S, Tarazi RA. Executive functioning among college students with and without history of childhood maltreatment. J Aggress Maltreat Trauma. (2017) 26:717–35. doi: 10.1080/10926771.2017.1317685

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Li Y, Dong F, Cao F, Cui N, Li J, Long Z. Poly-victimization and executive functions in junior college students. Scand J Psychol. (2013) 54:485–92. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Clausen AN, Billinger SA, Sisante J-FV, Suzuki H, Aupperle RL. Preliminary evidence for the impact of combat experiences on gray matter volume of the posterior insula. Front Psychol. (2017) 0:2151. doi: 10.3389/FPSYG.2017.02151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Daugherty J, Verdejo-Román J, Pérez-García M, Hidalgo-Ruzzante N. Structural brain alterations in female survivors of intimate partner violence. J Interpers Violence. (2020) 37:1–34. doi: 10.1177/0886260520959621

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Fonzo GA, Flagan TM, Sullivan S, Allard CB, Grimes EM, Simmons AN, et al. Neural functional and structural correlates of childhood maltreatment in women with intimate-partner violence-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2013) 211:93–103. doi: 10.1016/J.PSCYCHRESNS.2012.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Lim L, Hart H, Mehta M, Worker A, Simmons A, Mirza K, et al. Grey matter volume and thickness abnormalities in young people with a history of childhood abuse. Psychol Med. (2018) 48:1034–46. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717002392

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Price M, Albaugh M, Hahn S, Juliano AC, Fani N, Brier ZMF, et al. Examination of the association between exposure to childhood maltreatment and brain structure in young adults: a machine learning analysis. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2021) 46:1888–94. doi: 10.1038/s41386-021-00987-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kelly PA, Viding E, Wallace GL, Schaer M, De Brito SA, Robustelli B, et al. Cortical thickness, surface area, and gyrification abnormalities in children exposed to maltreatment: neural markers of vulnerability? Biol Psychiatry. (2013) 74:845–52. doi: 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2013.06.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ross MC, Sartin-Tarm AS, Letkiewicz AM, Crombie KM, Cisler JM. Distinct cortical thickness correlates of early life trauma exposure and posttraumatic stress disorder are shared among adolescent and adult females with interpersonal violence exposure. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2020) 46:741–9. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-00918-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Tozzi L, Garczarek L, Janowitz D, Stein DJ, Wittfeld K, Dobrowolny H, et al. Interactive impact of childhood maltreatment, depression, and age on cortical brain structure: mega-analytic findings from a large multi-site cohort. Psychol Med. (2020) 50:1020–31. doi: 10.1017/S003329171900093X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Paquola C, Bennett MR, Lagopoulos J. Understanding heterogeneity in grey matter research of adults with childhood maltreatment–a meta-analysis and review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 69:299–312. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2016.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Averill LA, Abdallah CG, Pietrzak RH, Averill CL, Southwick SM, Krystal JH, et al. Combat Exposure severity is associated with reduced cortical thickness in combat veterans: a preliminary report. Chron Stress. (2017) 1. doi: 10.1177/2470547017724714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Bremner JD, Randall P, Scott TM, Bronen RA, Seibyl JP, Southwick SM, et al. MRI-based measurement of hippocampal volume in patients with combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (1995) 152:973. doi: 10.1176/AJP.152.7.973

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Averill CL, Satodiya RM, Scott JC, Wrocklage KM, Schweinsburg B, Averill LA, et al. Posttraumatic stress disorder and depression symptom severities are differentially associated with hippocampal subfield volume loss in combat veterans. Chron Stress. (2017) 1:2017. doi: 10.1177/2470547017744538

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Wrocklage KM, Averill LA, Cobb Scott J, Averill CL, Schweinsburg B, Trejo M, et al. Cortical thickness reduction in combat exposed U.S. veterans with and without PTSD. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2017) 27:515–25. doi: 10.1016/J.EURONEURO.2017.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Fennema-Notestine C, Stein MB, Kennedy CM, Archibald SL, Jernigan TL. Brain morphometry in female victims of intimate partner violence with and without posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2002) 52:1089–101. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(02)01413-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Roos A, Fouche J-P, Stein DJ. Brain network connectivity in women exposed to intimate partner violence: a graph theory analysis study. Brain Imaging Behav. (2017) 11:1629–39. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9644-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Elton A, Tripathi SP, Mletzko T, Young J, Cisler JM, James GA, et al. Childhood maltreatment is associated with a sex-dependent functional reorganization of a brain inhibitory control network. Hum Brain Mapp. (2014) 35:1654–67. doi: 10.1002/HBM.22280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Ethridge P, Sandre A, Dirks MA, Weinberg A. Past-year relational victimization is associated with a blunted neural response to rewards in emerging adults. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2018) 13:1259–67. doi: 10.1093/SCAN/NSY091

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Simmons AN, Paulus MP, Thorp SR, Matthews SC, Norman SB, Stein MB. Functional activation and neural networks in women with posttraumatic stress disorder related to intimate partner violence. Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 64:681–90. doi: 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2008.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Strigo IA, Simmons AN, Matthews SC, Grimes EM, Allard CB, Reinhardt LE, et al. Neural correlates of altered pain response in women with posttraumatic stress disorder from intimate partner violence. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 68:442–50. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Weissman DG, Jenness JL, Colich NL, Miller AB, Sambrook KA, Sheridan MA, et al. Altered neural processing of threat-related information in children and adolescents exposed to violence: a transdiagnostic mechanism contributing to the emergence of psychopathology. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2020) 59:1274–84. doi: 10.1016/J.JAAC.2019.08.471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Hein TC, Monk CS. Research review: neural response to threat in children, adolescents, and adults after child maltreatment–a quantitative meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2017) 58:222–30.

Google Scholar

30. Cisler JM, Scott Steele J, Smitherman S, Lenow JK, Kilts CD. Neural processing correlates of assaultive violence exposure and PTSD symptoms during implicit threat processing: a network-level analysis among adolescent girls. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2013) 214:238–46. doi: 10.1016/J.PSCYCHRESNS.2013.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Fonzo GA, Simmons AN, Thorp SR, Norman SB, Paulus MP, Stein MB. Exaggerated and disconnected insular-amygdalar blood oxygenation level-dependent response to threat-related emotional faces in women with intimate-partner violence posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 68:433–41. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Boccadoro S, Siugzdaite R, Hudson AR, Maeyens L, Van Hamme C, Mueller SC. Women with early maltreatment experience show increased resting-state functional connectivity in the theory of mind (ToM) network. Eur J Psychotraumatol. (2019) 10:1647044. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2019.1647044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Strigo IA, Simmons AN, Matthews SC, Grimes EM, Allard CB, Reinhardt LE, et al. Neural correlates of altered pain response in women with posttraumatic stress disorder from intimate partner violence. Biol Psychiatry. (2010) 68:442–50.

Google Scholar

34. McCrory EJ, Gerin MI, Viding E. Annual research review: childhood maltreatment, latent vulnerability and the shift to preventative psychiatry – the contribution of functional brain imaging. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2017) 58:338–57. doi: 10.1111/JCPP.12713

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Lim L, Howells H, Radua J, Rubia K. Aberrant structural connectivity in childhood maltreatment: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 116:406–14. doi: 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2020.07.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Yuan M, Rubin-Falcone H, Lin X, Rizk MM, Miller JM, Sublette ME, et al. Smaller left hippocampal subfield CA1 volume is associated with reported childhood physical and/or sexual abuse in major depression: a pilot study. J Affect Disord. (2020) 272:348–54. doi: 10.1016/J.JAD.2020.03.169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. van Rooij SJH, Smith RD, Stenson AF, Ely TD, Yang X, Tottenham N, et al. Increased activation of the fear neurocircuitry in children exposed to violence. Depress Anxiety. (2020) 37:303–12. doi: 10.1002/DA.22994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Waters RC, Gould E. Early life adversity and neuropsychiatric disease: differential outcomes and translational relevance of rodent models. Front Syst Neurosci. (2022) 16:860847. doi: 10.3389/fnsys.2022.860847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Cassiers LL, Sabbe BG, Schmaal L, Veltman DJ, Penninx BW, Van Den Eede F. Structural and functional brain abnormalities associated with exposure to different childhood trauma subtypes: a systematic review of neuroimaging findings. Front Psychiatry. (2018) 9:329. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00329

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Sha Z, Wager TD, Mechelli A, He Y. Common dysfunction of large-scale neurocognitive networks across psychiatric disorders. Biol Psychiatry. (2019) 85:379–88.

Google Scholar

41. Yu M, Linn KA, Shinohara RT, Oathes DJ, Cook PA, Duprat R, et al. Childhood trauma history is linked to abnormal brain connectivity in major depression. Proc Natl Acad Sci U.S.A. (2019) 116:8582–90. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1900801116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Lebois LA, Li M, Baker JT, Wolff JD, Wang D, Lambros AM, et al. Large-scale functional brain network architecture changes associated with trauma-related dissociation. Am J Psychiatry. (2021) 178:165–73.

Google Scholar

43. Evler A, Scheller M, Wagels L, Bergs R, Clemens B, Kohn N, et al. Gender-inclusive care of victims of violence: the model project “gender Gewaltkonzept” at the university hospital Aachen. Der Nervenarzt. (2016) 87:746–52. doi: 10.1007/s00115-015-0024-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Habel U, Wagels L, Ellendt S, Scheller M, Evler A, Bergs R, et al. Violence and health. Symptoms, consequences and treatment of victimized patients. Bundesgesundheitsblatt Gesundheitsforschung Gesundheitsschutz. (2016) 59:17–27. doi: 10.1007/s00103-015-2258-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Garcia-Moreno C, Jansen HA, Ellsberg M, Heise L, Watts CH. Prevalence of intimate partner violence: findings from the WHO multi-country study on women’s health and domestic violence. Lancet. (2006) 368:1260–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(06)69523-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hing N, O’Mullan C, Nuske E, Breen H, Mainey L, Taylor A, et al. Gambling-related intimate partner violence against women: a grounded theory model of individual and relationship determinants. J Interpers Violence. (2022) 37:N18639–65. doi: 10.1177/08862605211037425

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. (1998) 59(Suppl. 20):quiz34–57.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

48. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch General Psychiatry. (1961) 4:561–71. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene RE. The State-Trait Anxiety Inventory (Test Manual). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1970).

Google Scholar

50. Satow L. Stress–und Coping-Inventar (SCI): Test–und Skalendokumentation. (2012). Available online at: http://www.drsatow.de/ (accessed February 15, 2013).

Google Scholar

51. Horowitz M, Wilner N, Alvarez W. Impact of event scale: a measure of participantive stress. Psychosom Med. (1979) 41:209–18. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197905000-00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Erzberger CS, Engel RR. Zur äquivalenz der normen des wechsler-intelligenztests für erwachsene (WIE) mit denen des Hamburg-wechsler-intelligenztests für erwachsene – revision (HAWIE-R). Z Neuropsychol. (2010) 21:25–37. doi: 10.1024/1016-264X/a000002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Troyer AK, Moscovitch M, Winocur G. Clustering and switching as two components of verbal fluency: evidence from younger and older healthy adults. Neuropsychology. (1997) 11:138–46. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.11.1.138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Lehrl S. Mehrfachwahl-Wortschatz-Intelligenztest MWT-B. Manual zum MWTB. Balingen: Spitta-Verl (1995).

Google Scholar

55. Tischler L, Petermann F. Trail making test (TMT). Z Psychiatrie Psychol Psychother. (2010) 58:79–81. doi: 10.1024/1661-4747.a000009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Ashburner J, Friston KJ. Unified segmentation. Neuroimage. (2005) 26:839–51.

Google Scholar

57. Honea R, Crow TJ, Passingham D, Mackay CE. Regional deficits in brain volume in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis of voxel-based morphometry studies. Am J Psychiatry. (2005) 162:2233–45. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. White T, OLeary D, Magnotta V, Arndt S, Flaum M, Andreasen NC. Anatomic and functional variability: the effects of filter size in group fMRI data analysis. Neuroimage. (2001) 13:577–88. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0716

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Fox DF, Raichle ME. Spontaneous fluctuations in brain activity observed with functional magnetic resonance imaging. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2007) 8:700–11. doi: 10.1038/nrn2201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Lu H, Zuo Y, Gu H, Waltz JA, Zhan W, Scholl CA, et al. Synchronized delta oscillations correlate with the resting-state functional MRI signal. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2007) 104:18265–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0705791104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Whitfield-Gabrieli S, Nieto-Castanon A. Conn: a functional connectivity toolbox for correlated and anticorrelated brain networks. Brain Connectiv. (2012) 2:125–41. doi: 10.1089/brain.2012.0073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Rademacher J, Galaburda AM, Kennedy DN, Filipek PA, Caviness VS. Human cerebral cortex: localization, parcellation, and morphometry with magnetic resonance imaging. J Cogn Neurosci. (1992) 4:352–74. doi: 10.1162/jocn.1992.4.4.352

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Koch SB, van Zuiden M, Nawijn L, Frijling JL, Veltman DJ, Olff M. Aberrant resting-state brain activity in posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Depress Anxiety. (2016) 33:592–605. doi: 10.1002/da.22478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Pedregosa F, Varoquaux G, Gramfort A, Michel V, Thirion B, Grisel O, et al. Scikit-learn: machine learning in Python. J Mach Learn Res. (2011) 12:2825–30.

Google Scholar

65. Marrelec G, Horwitz B, Kim J, Pelegrini-Issac M, Benali H, Doyon J. Using partial correlation to enhance structural equation modeling of functional MRI data. Magnet Reson Imaging. (2007) 25:1181–9. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2007.02.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Varoquaux G, Craddock RC. Learning and comparing functional connectomes across participants. Neuroimage. (2013) 80:405–15.

Google Scholar

67. Friedman J, Hastie T, Tibshirani R. Sparse inverse covariance estimation with the graphical lasso. Biostatistics. (2008) 9:432–41. doi: 10.1093/biostatistics/kxm045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Smith SM, Miller KL, Salimi-Khorshidi G, Webster M, Beckmann CF, Nichols TE, et al. Network modelling methods for FMRI. Neuroimage. (2011) 54:875–91.

Google Scholar

69. Varoquaux G, Gramfort A, Poline J-B, Thirion B. Brain covariance selection: better individual functional connectivity models using population prior. Proceedings of the Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems. Vancouver, BC: (2010). p. 2334–42.

Google Scholar

70. Stone M. Cross-validatory choice and assessment of statistical predictions. J R Stat Soc Ser B (Methodol). (1974) 36:111–47. doi: 10.1111/j.2517-6161.1974.tb00994.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Stone M. Cross-validation: a review. Statistics. (1978) 9:127–39.

Google Scholar

72. Abraham A, Pedregosa F, Eickenberg M, Gervais P, Mueller A, Kossaifi J, et al. Machine learning for neuroimaging with scikit-learn. Front Neuroinformatics. (2014) 8:14. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2014.00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Miller KL, Alfaro-Almagro F, Bangerter NK, Thomas DL, Yacoub E, Xu J, et al. Multimodal population brain imaging in the UK Biobank prospective epidemiological study. Nat Neurosci. (2016) 19:1523–36.

Google Scholar

74. Smith SM, Nichols TE, Vidaurre D, Winkler AM, Behrens TEJ, Glasser MF, et al. A positive-negative mode of population covariation links brain connectivity, demographics and behavior. Nat Neurosci. (2015) 18:1565–7. doi: 10.1038/nn.4125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Wang H-T, Smallwood J, Mourao-Miranda J, Xia CH, Satterthwaite TD, Bassett DS, et al. Finding the needle in a high-dimensional haystack: canonical correlation analysis for neuroscientists. Neuroimage. (2020) 216:116745. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.116745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Zhuang X, Yang Z, Cordes D. A technical review of canonical correlation analysis for neuroscience applications. Hum Brain Mapp. (2020) 41:3807–33. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Mardia KV, Kent JT, Bibby JM. Multivariate Analysis. Cambridge, MA: Academic Press (1979).

Google Scholar

78. McCabe DP, Roediger HL, McDaniel MA, Balota DA, Hambrick DZ. The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology. (2010) 24:222–43. doi: 10.1037/a0017619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Schirmer ST, Beckmann FE, Gruber H, Schlaaff K, Scheermann D, Seidenbecher S, et al. Decreased functional connectivity in patients with major depressive disorder and a history of childhood traumatization through experiences of abuse. Behav Brain Res. (2023) 437:114098. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2022.114098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Menon V, Uddin LQ. Saliency, switching, attention and control: a network model of insula function. Brain Struct Funct. (2010) 214:655–67. doi: 10.1007/s00429-010-0262-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Bogliacino F, Grimalda G, Ortoleva P, Ring P. Exposure to and recall of violence reduce short-term memory and cognitive control. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2017) 114:8505–10. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1704651114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Mak LE, Minuzzi L, MacQueen G, Hall G, Kennedy SH, Milev R. The default mode network in healthy individuals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Connectiv. (2017) 7:25–33. doi: 10.1089/brain.2016.0438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Dixon ML, De La Vega A, Mills C, Andrews-Hanna J, Spreng RN, Cole MW, et al. Heterogeneity within the frontoparietal control network and its relationship to the default and dorsal attention networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2018) 115:E1598–607. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1715766115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Wen Z, Seo J, Pace-Schott EF, Milad MR. Abnormal dynamic functional connectivity during fear extinction learning in PTSD and anxiety disorders. Mol Psychiatry. (2022) 27:2216–24. doi: 10.1038/s41380-022-01462-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Spreng RN, Shoemaker L, Turner GR. Executive functions and neurocognitive aging. In: E Goldberg editor. Executive Functions in Health and Disease. Amsterdam: Elsevier (2017). p. 169–96. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-803676-1.00008-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Nicholson AA, Harricharan S, Densmore M, Neufeld RW, Ros T, McKinnon MC, et al. Classifying heterogeneous presentations of PTSD via the default mode, central executive, and salience networks with machine learning. Neuroimage Clin. (2020) 27:102262. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Kang L, Chen J, Huang J, Jiang J. Autism spectrum disorder recognition based on multi-view ensemble learning with multi-site fMRI. Cogn Neurodyn. (2022).

Google Scholar

Keywords: victims of violence, neuroimaging, structural covariance, functional connectivity, partial correlation, sparse inverse covariance

Citation: Shymanskaya A, Kohn N, Habel U and Wagels L (2023) Brain network changes in adult victims of violence. Front. Psychiatry 14:1040861. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2023.1040861

Received: 09 September 2022; Accepted: 16 January 2023;
Published: 02 February 2023.

Edited by:

Jianzhong Su, University of Texas at Arlington, United States

Reviewed by:

Yuanxi Li, East China University of Science and Technology, China
Songting Li, Shanghai Jiao Tong University, China

Copyright © 2023 Shymanskaya, Kohn, Habel and Wagels. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Lisa Wagels, www.frontiersin.org lwagels@ukaachen.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.