Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 17 October 2022
Sec. Autism
This article is part of the Research Topic Emotion Regulation in Neurodevelopmental Disorders: Current Understanding and Treatments View all 10 articles

Cognitive reappraisal training for parents of children with autism spectrum disorder

  • 1Department of Counseling and Human Development, University of Haifa, Haifa, Israel
  • 2Department of Psychology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, United States

Parents of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) experience higher stress levels than those of typically developing children. The goal of the current study was to examine whether a mentalization-based intervention would enhance parental cognitive reappraisal, an adaptive form of emotion regulation associated with lower levels of stress. Findings from 27 parents who completed a short training indicated an improvement in cognitive reappraisal. In exploratory analyses, two different types of reappraisal were examined. The intervention-related improvement was found mainly with one type of reappraisal, namely reflective reappraisal that consist of cognitive reappraisal with mentalization characteristics. In light of the evidence indicating that high cognitive reappraisal and high reflective functioning are associated with quality caregiving, findings from the current study suggesting that a brief mentalization-based intervention supports ASD parents' cognitive reappraisal with mentalization characteristics are promising and warrant further investigation.

Introduction

Parents of children with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) experience higher stress levels than parents of children with other neurodevelopmental disorders or children who are typically developing (15). Therefore, intervention programs may need to address parental stress, which in turn will benefit the child and the family. Effective emotion regulation (ER) strategies may reduce stress and other negative emotions and increase high-quality parent-child interactions (6, 7).

Cognitive reappraisal

Because different forms of ER have different consequences, it is important to be specific about the types of ER that are (or are not) used in a particular family context. One particularly adaptive form of ER is cognitive reappraisal, a type of cognitive change ER (8, 9). Cognitive reappraisal has been found to change emotions in a sustained manner, and has been associated with lower levels of negative affect (10, 11). Successful cognitive reappraisal leads to better interpersonal functioning along with physical and psychological well-being (12, 13) and fewer mental health problems (14). In light of its positive effects, there has been a growing interest in the processes that support cognitive reappraisal.

Mentalization and cognitive reappraisal

Mentalizing (also known as reflective functioning) refers to the creation of explanations of one's own or others' mental states, including thoughts, feelings, and intentions during an act of imagining and wondering (15). Mentalizing the experiences of self and others supports emotion regulation (16) and helps transform initial maladaptive thoughts into adaptive ones (15, 17). While mentalizing and wondering about others' states of mind, we imagine their perspective. Considering another person's point of view requires us to pay attention to and actively imagine that person's perspective (18), and this, in turn, promotes the understanding that each mind works differently and helps us become regulated and empathetic (19, 20). Mentalization may play a particularly central role in families of children with ASD, as it is hard to understand the children's thoughts and intentions, and often mentalization lacks a positive response from the child (21).

One reason mentalization may facilitate cognitive reappraisal is that reflective thinking initiates the development of complex representations and symbols (22, 23), leading to reappraisals with reflective characteristics (i.e., reflective appraisals) (24, 25). Reflective reappraisal is the process of reinterpretation of an event's meaning in order to down-regulate the experience of negative emotions that includes reflective characteristics (26, 27). In contrast, non-reflective reappraisals involve reinterpretations of an event's meaning that do not include reflective characteristics. For example: David, a 13-year-old, comes home from school angry and frustrated because he failed his math exam. When his dad asks for help setting the table for dinner, he shouts “Do it yourself” and slams his door. The first thought in his father's mind is: “He is so rude.” After focusing on his son's mind and imagining his thoughts, feelings, and intentions, a reappraisal might be: “I know how hard he studied. It must be so frustrating for him to fail the math exam. He is not a rude child.” In contrast, a non-reflective reappraisal could be: “He will calm down soon.”

The scientific literature has increasingly recognized the relationship between mentalization and cognitive reappraisal (24, 28, 29). For instance, Baylin (28) found mentalization initiated cognitive reappraisal. In a related vein, Fonagy et al. (24) argued mentalization plays a role in appraising an event in a way that promotes resilience. Moreover, in aversive situations, the automatic response is usually a negative thought, and the individual needs to reappraise it in a way that involves reflective, cognitive mentalizing (30). Similarly, Sharp et al. (29) presented a model for trauma intervention that integrated mentalization and cognitive reappraisal.

The current study

This study is part of a larger study (31) that assessed the effect of mentalization based workshop on parental efficacy, parental belief in mailability of emotions and child's symptoms in parents of children with ASD. The goal of the current study was to examine whether and to what extent a mentalization-based intervention, bringing together aspects of psychodynamic and cognitive-behavioral focused on emotion regulation would affect parental cognitive reappraisal (the subjective interpretation made by the parent to an emotional stimuli) in parents of children with ASD. It was hypothesized that the intervention would lead to increased use of cognitive reappraisal, particularly reflective reappraisal.

Method

Participants

Parents of children with ASD were recruited to participate in a 4-week Reflective Parenting Workshop focused on emotion regulation (31). The present report is in an extension of a previous research, in the current investigation, the focus is on the effect of intervention on cognitive reappraisal.

Recruitment for the workshop occurred through distribution of fliers at clinics around the San Francisco Bay Area that provide services to families of children with autism. Twenty-seven intervention-group parents who completed an Emotion Interaction Questionnaire (EIQ) and ERQ (Emotion Regulation Questionnaire) before and after the workshop were included in the current study. Other inclusion criteria for the study included English proficiency, having a child with a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder between the ages of 3 and 18 years, and completion of the workshop. This study was approved by the university's Institutional Review Board and registered in the Clinical Trials database. Table 1 presents participant demographics. As can be viewed from the table, all participating parents were married, and most were Caucasian women with two children. Over 80% of participants had academic degrees. One third were employed full-time, another one third were employed half-time, and the rest were either self-employed or homemakers. Approximately half of the children were between the ages 5 to 9, and half were between the ages 10 to 17. Most of the ASD children were high functioning.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Frequency distribution on participants' demographic characteristics.

Procedures

After signing consent forms, participants were screened for meeting the study inclusions criteria. Eligible participants then completed a 4-week face to face group workshop, one and a half hours once a week each session. Parenting workshop focused on reflective functioning and emotion regulation skills of parents of children with ASD based on the literature that associate high parental reflective functioning with high emotional regulation and both are related to high quality caregiving and parent-child relationship (32, 33). The intervention flow and key concepts: Session 1 - emotion, emotion regulation, different emotion regulation strategies with focus on cognitive reappraisal. Session 2 – The effect of parental emotion regulation on children, reflective functioning and holding the mind in mind, the challenges and opportunities in raising children with ASD. Session 3 – Participants are invited to share their emotional scenarios from the past week with the group. Role play of different parent-child emotional scenarios. Session 4 - Participants are invited to share their emotional scenarios from the past week with the group. Participants are invited to share anything about their participation in the workshop with an emphasis on next steps, what they took from the workshop, and anything that was particularly helpful. The participants filled out pre and post questionnaires. Participants were asked to complete emotion interaction questionnaire (EIQ) and an emotion regulation questionnaire (ERQ) in its trait version. The ERQ can be used in two different versions: one version is asking about using cognitive reappraisal during a specific time frame that can be considered as more situational. The other version is asking about using cognitive reappraisal generally, in life, without asking about a specific timeframe. Being non-related to a certain period and or situation, but to a general truth, this version can be considered as more trait related.

Measures

For demographics, the following information was collected: age, gender, race, marital status, education status, and employment status. In addition, participants were asked to specify the number of children they have.

The Emotion Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ); (12) is self-report widely used measure of emotion regulation (34) and it was administered to assess parents' emotion regulation in trait format. It has good psychometric properties with above average internal consistency (Cronbach's α for reappraisal 0.75–0.82, and suppression 0.68–0.76, test–retest reliability across 3 months = 0.69), (12). The ERQ comprises 10 items divided into 2 subscales cognitive reappraisal and suppression. In this study we used the reappraisal subscale that consists of six items (e.g., “I control my emotions by changing the way I think about the situation I'm in”). Participants rated the degree to which they agreed with each statement on a 7-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 7 = strongly agree).

The Emotion Interaction Questionnaire (EIQ, was developed by the research team) instructed parents first to describe a situation from the past week when they felt their negative emotions had an effect on their behavior, then to describe their reactions and thoughts during the situation and lastly to describe their thoughts after some time had passed.

Data reduction

Two judges were trained to assess cognitive reappraisal, as well as its two subtypes, namely reflective reappraisal and non-reflective reappraisal. Each judge got a detailed explanation with the definition of each subtype, examples of situations that were assessed and coded by the research team and demonstrated each subtype and five situations that the judges were asked to assess and code as part of the training. Cognitive reappraisal was defined as reinterpreting the meaning of an emotional event or stimulus with the goal of influencing one's emotional response (35). Reflective reappraisal was scored as being present when the criteria for cognitive reappraisal were met, and the statement showed one of the following: (1). Awareness of the nature of mental states (22); (2). Recognition of developmental aspects of mental states (36); and (3). The effort to understand behavior based on mental states (37). An example that received a high score on reflective reappraisal is the following: “We had a doctor appointment for our son during school hours. I prepped him before school so it is not a surprise. He whined for an hour because he was missing recess.” After a time passed, “I was thinking how he felt, I gave him time for himself and I was happy that he loves school, I was thinking how change in routine is hard for him.” Non-reflective reappraisal was scored as being present when the criteria for cognitive reappraisal were met, but the statement did not meet the criteria for reflective reappraisal (there are no mentalization characteristics). Two judges got parental vignettes and had to assess on 1 to 7 Likert scale how much they saw evidence for cognitive reappraisal based on the vignette, where 1 = there is no evidence and 7 = there is strongly evidence. The judges were blind to the time point of assessment, i.e., whether the parents' description comes from the pre-or post assessment. The correlation between independent raters' scores was high for both reflective reappraisals (pre-intervention: r = 0.97, p < 0.001; post intervention: r = 0.93, p < 0.001) and non-reflective reappraisals (pre-intervention: r = 0.92, p < 0.001; post intervention: r = 0.91, p < 0.001). For this reason, the ratings were averaged across raters for each reappraisal type, at each time point (pre and post-intervention).

Results

Change in cognitive reappraisal pre- to post-intervention

To examine whether cognitive reappraisal scores, measured by the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire, increased with the intervention, we employed a paired samples t-test analysis. Results indicated that there was indeed such increase [pre-intervention cognitive reappraisal: M = 4.71, SE = 0.21; post-intervention cognitive reappraisal: M = 5.15, SE = 0.19; t(26) = 2.24, p < 0.05, Cohen's D = 0.43].

Next, we examined the increase in cognitive reappraisal scores as measured by narrative scoring. Cognitive reappraisal (CR) at each time point was computed as the sum of the two reappraisal types (CR = NR+RR). The increase in the computed CR measure was then examined using a paired samples t-test analysis. Results again indicated an increase between the two measurements [t(26) = −4.46, p < 0.001, Cohen's D = 0.86; pre-intervention cognitive reappraisal: M = 4.57, SE = 0.53; post-intervention cognitive reappraisal: M = 7.89, SE = 0.46]. Figure 1A presents the results for the ERQ scoring, and Figure 1B presents the results for the narrative scoring. Interestingly, changes in the two indicators of cognitive reappraisal were not correlated (r = −0.17, ns), suggesting the difference between trait and situation-based assessments approaches.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. (A) Cognitive reappraisal scores measured by ERQ. Results indicate an increase in cognitive reappraisal, as measured by the emotion regulation questionnaire, after intervention. (B) Cognitive reappraisal scores measured by narrative ratings. Results indicate an increase in cognitive reappraisal, as measured by narrative ratings (situation-based cognitive reappraisal), after intervention. ***p < 0.001, **p < 0.01.

Change in reflective and non-reflective reappraisal pre- to post-intervention

Following the primary analyses presented above, we proceeded to some broader, exploratory analyses, that examined change between pre and post-intervention in the two types of cognitive reappraisal, namely, reflective and non-reflective reappraisal (RR and NR, respectively). To this end, we employed a Repeated Measures ANOVA, with two within-subjects variables: time (pre and post) and reappraisal type (RR and NR). The analysis revealed three significant effects. The time effect was significant [F(1,26) = 19.88, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.43], so that pre-intervention scores (M = 2.29, SE = 0.27) were significantly lower than post-intervention scores (M = 3.94, SE = 0.23). The reappraisal type effect was also significant [F(1,26) = 16.75, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.39], so that NR scores (M = 2.26, SE = 0.24) were significantly lower than RR scores (M = 3.97, SE = 0.29). Finally, the time-reappraisal type interaction was significant, [F(1,26) = 10.90, p < 0.01, ηp2 = 0.30], so that in NR scores there wasn't a significant difference between the two time points (pre-intervention: M = 2.02, SE = 0.33; post-intervention: M = 2.50, SE = 0.41; n.s.), while in RR scores there was a significant difference between the two time points (pre-intervention: M = 2.56, SE = 0.38; post-intervention: M = 5.39, SE = 0.34). Figure 2 presents the time*reappraisal type interaction.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Non-reflective and reflective reappraisal scores, pre and post-intervention. Results indicate a significant difference between the two time points in reflective reappraisal scores. The difference in non-reflective reappraisal scores was non-significant. ***p < 0.001.

Discussion

Our finding of an improvement in cognitive reappraisal after a mentalization-based intervention is in alignment with previous studies that demonstrate positive association between mentalization and successful emotion regulation (16). In particular, studies have shown that higher levels of mentalization are coupled with transformation from non-adaptive thoughts to balanced and adaptive ones (15, 17, 36) similar to the change we found in cognitive reappraisal. The possible improvement in emotion regulation after a short mentalization intervention is especially significant for parents of children with autism as they report higher levels of stress and prolong negative emotions compared to parents of typically developing children (2, 3, 5).

The increase in cognitive reappraisal is especially notable as it was evident using two different assessment methods. One is based on self-report that was given in a trait format. The second is based on a narrative scoring that was given in a state format. The improvement in cognitive reappraisal in a trait format after a short intervention is especially remarkable and might point to a possible underestimation of the true change. The improvement in state format includes ecological characteristics, based on daily situations. The fact that we did not see a correlation between the two methods may be due to the fact that correlations between trait and state measures are often weak (38, 39).

The increase in cognitive reappraisal post intervention was evidenced with reflective reappraisal but not with non-reflective reappraisal. One possible explanation for this specificity in effects could be related to the content of the intervention that is focused on mentalization. As mentalization facilitates reflective thinking, it makes sense that mentalization based intervention is leading to reappraisals with reflective characteristics (i.e., reflective reappraisals). This finding is consistent with previous studies that connect between mentalization and cognitive reappraisal with reflective characteristics. In particular studies present that the capacity to reflect on internal mental states of oneself and others is leading to cognitive appraisals that integrate qualities of mentalization (22, 24).

The improvement in reflective reappraisal following a mentalization-based intervention is important for parents of children with autism. They often have to work harder than parents of typically developing children to understand their children's behavior often in an absence of positive response from the child (21). As there is mounting evidence that high parental mentalization is associated with higher quality caregiving, attachment security, and successful emotion regulation (32, 33, 40) the possibility that short mentalization based intervention supports both parents' mentalization and emotion regulation is encouraging and support using both mentalization and emotion regulation principles in practice with parents of children with ASD.

While the current findings are presenting a potential positive impact of the mentalization-based workshop intervention for parents of children with ASD, this study has several limitations, which should be addressed in the future. First, the investigation does not have a control group, which limits our ability to determine whether change was related to the active intervention. Second, parents of children with ASD were included in the study but the diagnosis of ASD was not confirmed directly by study investigators. Instead, clinicians' reports were used to determine eligibility of parents. Third, the sample size of this study is relatively modest. Fourth, the measurement in this study were based on parents' reports. Finally, the children's ages ranged from 3 to 18, and we were underpowered to detect potential moderation by the children's age and sex.

Author contributions

YE has lead the project and the paper. AH was a mentor and consultant from the Autism perspective. JG was a mentor and consultant from Emotion Regulation perspective. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank Dana Erhard-Weiss, Mirit Kopleman, Despina Petsagourakis and Jay Webber for their help with this research project.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Davis RF III, Kiang L. Parental stress and religious coping by mothers of children with autism. Psycholog Relig Spiritual. (2020) 12:137–48. doi: 10.1037/rel0000183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. DesChamps TD, Ibañez LV, Edmunds SR, Dick CC, Stone WL. Parenting stress in caregivers of young children with ASD concerns prior to a formal diagnosis. Autism Res. (2020) 13:82–92. doi: 10.1002/aur.2213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Estes A, Olson E, Sullivan K, Greenson J, Winter J, Dawson G, et al. Parenting-related stress and psychological distress in mothers of toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Dev. (2013) 35:133–8. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2012.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Olson L, Chen B, Ibarra C, Wang T, Mash L, Linke A, et al. Behaviors are associated with increased parenting stress in caregivers of young children with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2022) 52:975–86. doi: 10.1007/s10803-021-04995-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Zablotsky B, Bradshaw CP. The association between mental health, stress, and coping supports in mothers of children with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:1380–93. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1693-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hughes EK. Reciprocal relationships between parent and adolescent internalizing symptoms. J Fam Psychol. (2010) 24:115. doi: 10.1037/a0018788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Morelen D, Shaffer A. Maternal emotion regulation: links to emotion parenting and child emotion regulation. J Fam Issues. (2014) 37:1891–916. doi: 10.1177/0192513X14546720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Gross JJ. Emotion regulation: conceptual and empirical foundations.? In:Gross JJ, editors. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. The Guilford Press (2014). p. 3–20.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

9. Urry HL. Emotion regulation in older age. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2010) 19:352–7. doi: 10.1177/0963721410388395

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, et al. Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. (2013) 24:2981–90. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Morawetz C, Bode S, Baudewig J. Effective amygdala-prefrontal connectivity predicts individual differences in successful emotion regulation. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2017) 12:569–85. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Gross JJ. Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2003) 85:348. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Picó-Pérez M, Radua J, Steward T, Menchón JM. Emotion regulation in mood and anxiety disorders: a meta-analysis of fMRI cognitive reappraisal studies. Prog Neuro Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatr. (2017) 79:96–104. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Troy AS, Wilhelm FH, Shallcross AJ. Seeing the silver lining: cognitive reappraisal ability moderates the relationship between stress and depressive symptoms. Emotion. (2010) 10:783. doi: 10.1037/a0020262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Fonagy P. Progress in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. Br J Psychiatr. (2006) 188:1–3. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.105.012088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Kivity Y, Levy KN, Kelly KM. In-session reflective functioning in psychotherapies for borderline personality disorder: the emotion regulatory role of reflective functioning. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2021) 89:751. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Beaulieu-Pelletier G, Bouchard MA, Philippe FL. Mental States Task (MST): development, validation, and correlates of a self-report measure of mentalization. J Clin Psychol. (2013) 69:671–95. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Allen JG, Fonagy P. Mentalizing in Clinical Practice. American Psychiatric Pub (2008).

Google Scholar

19. Hooker CI, Verosky SC, Germine LT, Knight RT. Mentalizing about emotion and its relationship to empathy. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2008) 3:204–17. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsn019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Swan P. Social connection: empathy and mentalization for teachers. Pastor Care Educ. (2015) 33:220–33. doi: 10.1080/02643944.2015.1094120

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Slade A. Mentalizing the unmentalizable: parenting children on the spectrum. J Infant Child Adolesc Psychother. (2009) 8:7–21. doi: 10.1080/15289160802683054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Fonagy P, Gergely G, Jurist EL. Affect Regulation, Mentalization, and the Development of the Self. Routledge (2018).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

23. Jurist EL. Mentalized affectivity. Psychoanal Psychol. (2005) 22:426. doi: 10.1037/0736-9735.22.3.426

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Allison E. What we have changed our minds about: part 2. Borderline personality disorder, epistemic trust and the developmental significance of social communication. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. (2017) 4:1–12. doi: 10.1186/s40479-017-0062-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Lecours S. Dimensions of mentalisation: outlining levels of psychic transformation. Int J Psycho Anal. (1997) 78:855–75.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

26. Katznelson H. Reflective functioning: a review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2014) 34:107–17. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Levy KN, Meehan KB, Kelly KM, Reynoso JS, Weber M, Clarkin JF, et al. Change in attachment patterns and reflective function in a randomized control trial of transference-focused psychotherapy for borderline personality disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2006) 74:1027. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.74.6.1027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Baylin J. Reflective functioning and parenting. In:Alper J, Howe D, editors. Assessing Adoptive Parents, Foster Carers, and Kinship Carers: Improving Analysis and Understanding of Parenting Capacity. London and Philadelphia: Jessica Kingsley Publishers (2017). p. 175–92.?

Google Scholar

29. Sharp C, Fonagy P. Posttraumatic stress disorder: a social-cognitive perspective. Clin Psychol Sci Pract. (2012) 19:229. doi: 10.1111/cpsp.12002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Fonagy P, Luyten P, Allison E, Campbell C. What we have changed our minds about: part 1. Borderline personality disorder as a limitation of resilience. Borderline Personal Disord Emot Dysregul. (2017) 4:1–11. doi: 10.1186/s40479-017-0061-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Enav Y, Erhard-Weiss D, Kopelman M, Samson AC, Mehta S, Gross JJ, et al. A non-randomized mentalization intervention for parents of children with autism. Autism Res. (2019) 12:1077–86. doi: 10.1002/aur.2108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Heron-Delaney M, Kenardy JA, Brown EA, Jardine C, Bogossian F, Neuman L, et al. Early maternal reflective functioning and infant emotional regulation in a preterm infant sample at 6 months corrected age. J Pediatr Psychol. (2016) 41:906–14. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsv169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Smaling HJ, Huijbregts SC, Suurland J, van der Heijden KB, Mesman J, van Goozen SH, et al. Prenatal reflective functioning and accumulated risk as predictors of maternal interactive behavior during free play, the still-face paradigm, and two teaching tasks. Infancy. (2016) 21:766–84. doi: 10.1111/infa.12137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Preece DA, Becerra R, Robinson K, Gross JJ. The emotion regulation questionnaire: psychometric properties in general community samples. J. Pers. Assess. (2020) 102:348–56. doi: 10.1080/00223891.2018.1564319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Gross JJ. Handbook of Emotion Regulation. Guilford publications (2013).

Google Scholar

36. Slade A. Parental reflective functioning: an introduction. Attach Hum Dev. (2005) 7:269–81. doi: 10.1080/14616730500245906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Allen JG. Mentalizing in practice. In:Allen JG, Fonagy P, editors. Handbook of Mentalization-Based Treatment. John Wiley & Sons Inc (2006). p. 3–30.

Google Scholar

38. Brockman R, Ciarrochi J, Parker P. Emotion regulation strategies in daily life: mindfulness, cognitive reappraisal and emotion suppression. Cogn Behav Ther. (2017) 46:91–113. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2016.1218926

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Nezlek JB. A multilevel framework for understanding relationships among traits, states, situations and behaviours. Euro J Pers. (2007) 21:789–810. doi: 10.1002/per.640

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Ensink K, Bégin M, Normandin L. Maternal and child reflective functioning in the context of child sexual abuse: pathways to depression and externalising difficulties. Euro J Psychotraumatol. (2016) 7:30611. doi: 10.3402/ejpt.v7.30611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism, parenting, cognitive reappraisal, reflective functioning, intervention

Citation: Enav Y, Hardan AY and Gross JJ (2022) Cognitive reappraisal training for parents of children with autism spectrum disorder. Front. Psychiatry 13:995669. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.995669

Received: 16 July 2022; Accepted: 20 September 2022;
Published: 17 October 2022.

Edited by:

Andrea C. Samson, Unidistance Suisse, Switzerland

Reviewed by:

Ulrike Rimmele, Université de Genève, Switzerland
Carolyn Shivers, Virginia Tech, United States

Copyright © 2022 Enav, Hardan and Gross. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yael Enav, yaelenav12@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.