Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 23 September 2022
Sec. Aging Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Insights in Aging Psychiatry: 2022 View all 8 articles

The complex burden of determining prevalence rates of mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review

  • 1Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology and Health Studies, “Sapienza” University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 2Department of Psychology, “Sapienza” University of Rome, Rome, Italy
  • 3Body and Action Laboratory, Istituto di Ricovero e Cura a Carattere Scientifico (IRCCS) Santa Lucia Foundation, Rome, Italy

Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a syndrome characterized by a decline in cognitive performance greater than expected for an individual's age and education level, but that does not interfere much with daily life activities. Establishing the prevalence of MCI is very important for both clinical and research fields. In fact, in a certain percentage of cases, MCI represents a prodromal condition for the development of dementia. Accordingly, it is important to identify the characteristics of MCI that allow us to predict the development of dementia. Also, initial detection of cognitive decline can allow the early implementation of prevention programs aimed at counteracting or slowing it down. To this end, it is important to have a clear picture of the prevalence of MCI and, consequently, of the diagnostic criteria used. According to these issues, this systematic review aims to analyze MCI prevalence, exploring the methods for diagnosing MCI that determine its prevalence. The review process was conducted according to the PRISMA statement. Three thousand one hundred twenty-one international articles were screened, and sixty-six were retained. In these studies, which involved 157,035 subjects, the prevalence of MCI ranged from 1.2 to 87%. The review results showed a large heterogeneity among studies due to differences in the subjects' recruitment, the diagnostic criteria, the assessed cognitive domains, and other methodological aspects that account for a higher range of MCI prevalence. This large heterogeneity prevents drawing any firm conclusion about the prevalence of MCI.

Introduction

Age is the biggest risk factor for the development of cognitive impairment. However, many other aspects could be associated with worsening cognitive functions, such as socioeconomic status, genetics, education, environmental exposure, and other comorbidities (1, 2).

In this regard, mild cognitive impairment (MCI) is a syndrome defined by a decline in cognitive performance greater than expected for an individual's age and education level, but that does not interfere much with daily life activities (1). It is distinct from dementia, characterized by more severe cognitive deficits that affect daily functions. According to Petersen (3), MCI represents an intermediate stage of a continuum that ranges from normal cognitive functioning to dementia (i.e., Alzheimer's disease). Various categories of MCI are described, including benign senescent forgetfulness (4), cognitive impairment, and no dementia [CIND; (5)]. The common characteristic is a cognitive deterioration that is insufficient to diagnose dementia (6).

There is not a consensus on the criteria for diagnosing MCI. At first, MCI patients were described as having only a memory impairment (7). However, more recently, the concept of MCI has been expanded to include other types of cognitive impairments beyond memory. Petersen (8) hypothesized a categorization in multiple subtypes based on the nature and number of impaired domains. According to this hypothesis, it is possible to distinguish: (a) amnestic MCI single domain, characterized by an impairment only in the memory domain (aMCIsd); (b) amnestic MCI multiple domains marked by impairments in memory and other neurocognitive domains (aMCImd); (c) non-amnestic MCI single domain, defined by an impairment in one single domain other than memory (naMCIsd); and, finally, (d) a non-amnestic MCI multiple domains expressed by impairments in at least two neurocognitive domains, that do not include memory (naMCImd). These subtypes can lead to different epidemiology and pathogenesis (9). In particular, individuals affected by amnestic MCI are more likely to develop Alzheimer's disease [AD; (9)]. Besides memory, a commonly impaired domain in MCI (2, 10) and AD (11) is executive functioning.

Despite some guidelines being defined (7), there is great variability in the connotation of this diagnostic category. Clinical criteria include a cognitive impairment in one or more domains with a suggested cut-off equal to 1.0/1.5 SD below normative expectations (1). In the majority of cases, MCI diagnosis is advanced for patients with (i) memory problems corroborated by an informant, (ii) objective evidence of memory impairments, (iii) normal general cognitive functioning, (iv) intact functional abilities, and (v) no dementia (12). Among the different criteria for diagnosing MCI, the operationalization criteria proposed by Petersen et al. (7) have received strong support, thus becoming the most frequently used (12). However, various aspects remain unclear, and there are debates about them. For example, it is unclear whether cognitive decline is established only referred to age-specific norms or education-specific norms. Moreover, in contrast with Petersen's criteria (7), those proposed by Jak et al. (13) require that an individual obtains scores below 1 SD in at least two tests rather than one. These criteria are strongly associated with biomarkers for diagnosing AD, involving higher diagnostic stability and earlier identification of those who will turn into dementia (14).

MCI prevalence estimates are difficult to compare because of the differences among studies in the embraced definitions and the employed methodology (15). Moreover, there are numerous confounding variables, such as the sample's age and the number of schooling years, the applied diagnostic criteria, the assessed cognitive domains, and the assessment tools adopted for the diagnosis.

Many authors agree in considering MCI associated with a greater risk of dementia. Shah et al. (16) revealed that the rate of conversion to dementia in the MCI population is equal to 10–15% per year, while this rate amounts to 1–2% per year among the control group. Many authors reported a prevalence of MCI of 15–20% in the general population (1719). However, prevalence estimates are variable and influenced by several factors: indeed, the presence or absence of MCI depends on the sensitivity and specificity of the tests used, population norms, and estimates of premorbid cognitive functioning (20). For this reason, prevalence estimates can range from 3 to 53.8% (21).

Establishing the prevalence of MCI is very important for clinical and research fields, as well as for the public health system. Indeed, the high numbers of people with dementia and cognitive disorders and their economic impact imply that an effective public health response should be a priority (20). Moreover, on a clinical level, it is important to identify the characteristics of MCI that allow us to predict the development of dementia. On the other hand, initial detection of cognitive decline can allow the early implementation of programs aimed at counteracting or slowing it down. To this end, it is important to have a clear picture of the prevalence of MCI and, consequently, of the diagnostic criteria used. Accordingly, this systematic review aims to analyze MCI prevalence and to explore the causes of its large heterogeneity further.

Method

Search strategy

This systematic review was conducted according to the PRISMA-Statement (22, 23). It has been performed by selecting articles published in international journals from January 1st, 1999, to January 20th, 2022, to include the year in which Petersen et al. (7) developed their MCI model. The databases utilized for the research were PsycInfo, PubMed, Scopus, and Web of Science. The review was based only on articles published in English and Italian. The search syntax can be found in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Search syntax.

Inclusion/exclusion criteria

According to the review aims, the following inclusion criteria were adopted: (I) controlled studies, cross-sectional and/or longitudinal, with the aim to evaluate MCI prevalence in the general population; (II) studies that specify and operationalize the diagnostic criteria and assessment tools for diagnosing MCI; (III) studies that precisely report the subject recruitment process.

The exclusion criteria were: (I) studies that included patients with head trauma, neurological disorders (e.g., epilepsy, multiple sclerosis, Parkinson's disease, etc.), metabolic diseases (e.g., diabetes or metabolic syndrome), autoimmune diseases (e.g., rheumatoid arthritis, Lupus, etc.), cardiovascular diseases (e.g., stroke or heart attack), oncological diseases, diagnosis of frontotemporal dementia, vascular dementia, dementia with Lewy bodies; (II) studies that included diagnoses of Cognitive Impairment Non Dementia-CIND, Age-Associated Memory Impairment-AAMI, Age-Associated Cognitive Decline-AACD; (III) studies that only adopted neurophysiological assessment tools, neuroimaging or biological markers for the diagnosis of MCI; (IV) studies that included only one gender in their sample (100% females or 100% males); (V) studies not containing the word “prevalence” in the title, in order to exclude articles that evaluated prevalence of MCI as a secondary goal, but that were not focused at evaluating MCI prevalence.

Two researchers independently screened the selected articles. The screening of the articles for titles and abstracts enabled the exclusion of those studies which were not relevant. This first screening resulted in 548 included articles. Subsequently, the full texts of the selected articles were read in order to evaluate their eligibility, thus resulting in 66 retained articles. The results are summarized in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA flow diagram.

Data extraction and studies quality assessment

According to the PICOS method (22), the following information has been extracted from each of the included articles: authors and publication year; country; type of study; sample size, and participants' characteristics (age, schooling years, gender); adopted diagnostic criteria, employed assessment tools and cut-off values for MCI diagnoses; assessed cognitive domains; the prevalence of MCI.

Study quality (bias) was assessed following the guidelines developed by Loney et al. (24). This method consists of eight statements, and the scoring process implies assigning one point for each criterion achieved. Therefore, the maximum score is eight points. These statements are meant to evaluate sampling, response rate, condition measurements, sample size, the precision of estimates, study design, description of participants, setting, and non-responders (25).

Publication bias

We attributed one point to each statement according to the assessment method developed by Loney et al. (24). From this process, we obtained 15 articles with eight points (maximum score), 34 with seven points, 13 studies were assigned 6 points, and only four articles received five points or less. These results are summarized in Table 2.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Bias assessment.

Results

Prevalence range

The sixty-six studies included in this systematic review involved 157,035 participants and reported a prevalence of MCI that ranges from 1.2% (26) up to 87% (27). This estimate drops to 0.6% (28) in the articles that evaluate only aMCI. This high heterogeneity is due to many reasons since these sixty-six articles vary in many aspects. These aspects are summarized in Table 3.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Selected studies' characteristics and MCI prevalence.

Type of studies

Most of these studies are cross-sectional, except for 18 articles out of 66 that are longitudinal. Moreover, 57.6% of them have a sample size larger than 1,000 participants. In the risk of bias assessment, according to the guidelines (24), we considered a sample of fewer than 300 participants affected by bias. However, only three studies have included < 300 subjects (31, 33, 78).

Age

The majority of these studies included participants over 60 years old. The authors that include younger samples [≥45, ≥50 or ≥55; (42, 49, 52, 59, 61, 68, 79, 86)] found a prevalence range that goes from 3 to 34.4%. This range is lower than the whole range, implying that a lower prevalence could be caused by sampling younger people since cognitive deficits increase with aging. However, even among authors who included young adults in their sample, it is easily noticeable that there is great variability in MCI prevalence estimates (from 3 to 34.4%). This aspect indicates that age is not the only variable that affects the prevalence results. Moreover, as can be observed in Figure 2, people aged 60–69 show a slightly higher prevalence of MCI when compared with older people. This aspect contradicts what was stated above, but many factors can be explained it. First, this prevalence estimate by age group was made considering the average age of the participants; however, the age range is generally not reported, nor is the age composition of the sample reported (e.g., if the mean age of the sample is 65 years old, it is not clear how many people aged 60 and how many of 85 contribute to determining this mean). Second, the outcome may be due to the extreme variability in diagnostic criteria and tests used to assess cognitive decline. Third, studies generally do not report whether other factors may influence cognitive decline (e.g., depression, anxiety, poor sleep quality) and whether the outcome may depend on medications (e.g., antidepressants, hypnotics, rivastigmine), which are likely to be hired more at a later age. Fourth, older people could already be progressed to dementia; therefore, MCI prevalence rates are lower when age increases. Finally, Figure 2 includes 53 out of 66 studies because 6 were excluded because of an aMCI diagnosis, and 7 studies were excluded since they did not report the mean age of the participants. However, it is not possible to draw definite conclusions.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. MCI mean prevalence rates according to age.

Gender

Gender composition varies among studies, with a female share ranging from 43 to 75.9%. Some of our studies included a vast majority of women [≥70%; (35, 36, 38, 66, 96)] and found a prevalence range that goes from 3 to 49%. This range is lower than the total range, which could mean that women present lower MCI prevalence rates than men, according to Petersen's findings (83). However, most studies did not report the prevalence separately for women and men, so it is impossible to draw definitive conclusions.

Schooling years

In our review, many studies included illiterate people in their sample (17, 28, 31, 33, 35, 36, 43, 46, 59, 65, 66, 73, 92). The prevalence range found by these studies goes from 6 to 68%, but these rates drop to 0.6% (28) in the articles that evaluate only aMCI. It is conceivable that MCI prevalence can increase by including illiterate people in the sample, but it is impossible to corroborate this hypothesis with the results of this review. Moreover, some studies have not specified the schooling years of their sample, and this aspect complicates the interpretation of our results further. In Figure 3, we synthesized schooling years into two categories: 10 or fewer years or above 10. Prevalence rates are similar in these two categories, and this information confirms that it is difficult to establish whether this aspect contributes to the development of MCI.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. MCI mean prevalence rates according to schooling years.

Geographical region

Regarding geographical distribution, 26 studies were conducted in Asia, 20 in Europe, 10 in America, 5 in Oceania, 4 in Africa, and one study included people from both America and Asia. Figure 4 reports the mean prevalence of MCI by geographic distribution. It can be seen that the standard deviations are high, and the majority of articles are from Asia or Europe, and we do not have sufficient data on the other areas. Therefore, it is impossible to draw conclusions on this matter in our review.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. MCI mean prevalence rates according to the geographical region of samples.

MCI criteria and operationalizations

There are problems linked to differences in the methodologies of the studies. Many different criteria have been used to diagnose MCI. Furthermore, researchers that utilize the same criteria often operationalize them differently and use diverse neuropsychological tasks.

Various tasks have been used to evaluate cognitive decline. Some of them measured global cognitive functioning, such as CAMCOG (Cambridge Cognitive Examination), CDR (Clinical Dementia Rating Scale), CERAD (Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease), CSI-D (Community Screening Instrument for Dementia), MMSE (Mini-Mental State Examination), MoCA (Montreal Cognitive Assessment), WAIS (Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale), SIDAM (A Structured Interview for the diagnosis of dementia of the Alzheimer type, Multi-infarct dementia and dementias of other etiology according to ICD-10 and DSM-III-R); others measure different cognitive domains (such as intelligence, memory, attention, executive functioning, language, praxia, visuospatial abilities, and processing speed). All the neuropsychological tasks used in the selected articles are reported in Table 4.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Assessed cognitive domains and neuropsychological testsa.

Concerning diagnostic criteria, half of the studies referred to Petersen's criteria. All the utilized diagnostic criteria are reported in detail in Table 5 and Figure 5.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5. Diagnostic criteriaa.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. MCI mean prevalence rates according to diagnostic criteria.

Cognitive domains

For what concerns cognitive domains, 29 articles out of 66 (44%) only measured global cognitive functioning, utilizing MMSE, MoCA, CDR, or other tasks such as SIDAM. In these studies, prevalence ranges from 3 to 68%. Five studies (7.6%) assessed one other domain (memory or executive functioning). The prevalence range in these articles goes from 6.7 to 35.3%. Six studies (9%) analyzed at least two cognitive domains, finding a prevalence that ranges from 3 to 27.6%. Lastly, 26 articles out of 66 (39.4%) evaluated at least three cognitive domains. The range goes from 1.2 up to 87% in these ones. Figure 6 and Table 6 report the MCI prevalence according to the cognitive domains assessed.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. MCI prevalence range according to the number of assessed cognitive domains. MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; GF, global functioning; GF + 1, global functioning + one domain; GF + 2, global functioning + two domains; GF + 3 (or more), global functioning + three domains (or more).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6. Prevalence range according to the assessed cognitive domainsa.

Cut-off scores

In Figure 7, mean prevalence MCI rates are reported according to the utilization of different cut-off scores for the MCI diagnosis. Three studies (27, 57, 89) have utilized multiple scores for their diagnosis. MoCA scores refer to the normative score of < 26 to diagnose MCI. MMSE cut-off scores vary between 17 and 28. Test-dependent cut-off scores refer to specific values related to the assessment tools, for example a score of 6 or 7 points to the RCS was utilized by Ruan et al. (88) to diagnose MCI. Also in this case, standard deviations are very high, and the limited and different number of studies using a single criterium prevent us from any conclusion about their relationship with MCI prevalence.

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. MCI mean prevalence rates according to cut-off scores.

Discussion

This systematic review aimed to evaluate and compare the estimated prevalence of MCI. Ten years ago, one study (103) had already tried to compare these prevalence rates, highlighting a great variability ranging from 3% (104) up to 42% (54). The authors' purpose was to identify the sources of variation in MCI prevalence rates, but they encountered many challenges while trying to investigate this topic. Although many studies have been included since 2012, we faced similar difficulties in our systematic review, where we found an even greater range compared to the previous one reported by Ward et al. (103). Indeed, the selected studies of this review rate go from 1.2% (26) to 87% (27). The greater range of MCI prevalence observed in this review compared to that of Ward et al. (103) may have been due to an exponential increase in MCI studies over the past decade.

However, it is unclear whether these great discrepancies are caused by methodological problems, such as heterogeneity in the sample characteristics (size, age, gender, schooling, and nationality), assessment tools or procedures, or limits due to the operationalization of the MCI criteria. According to this last aspect, since its first proposal (7), MCI criteria have been evolving: at first, it was used only to describe impairments in the memory domain (7), while afterward, it broadened to embrace many other domains (3). This factor has contributed to a certain confusion regarding the MCI diagnostic methodology.

We can state that the differences in the diagnostic criteria have determined the biggest problems incomparing studies in our review. This aspect is highly complicated since many studies apply the same diagnostic criteria but operationalize them differently. For example, Chuang et al. (37) utilize Albert's criteria with a cut-off of 1.5 SD, while González et al. (49) utilize the same criteria with a cut-off of 1 SD. Using a 1.5 SD cut-off is more sensitive to catching cognitive decline than a 2 SD cut-off but necessarily less specific (20). The most utilized criteria in this review are those indicated by Petersen et al. (3, 7): indeed, half of the studies included in this review refer to these criteria. These operational differences can regard the use of different tasks to assess cognitive abilities, the assessment of different cognitive domains, or different cut-off values. It is a known fact that the operationalization of MCI criteria affects MCI prevalence estimates, and assessing MCI with different criteria, even if slightly modified, can lead to very different prevalence estimates. For example, Kochan et al. (57) modified their operationalizations, finding prevalence rates ranging from 4 to 70%.

Moreover, some authors have modified the original diagnostic criteria of MCI (1, 35, 36, 45), which has caused even greater heterogeneity. Indeed, there is often a criterion regarding subjective cognitive complaints (SCC) in the original criteria, but there is no consensus on its validity. Therefore, researchers often diagnose MCI in patients who do not report SCC using modified criteria. The elimination of this criterion results in higher prevalence rates: indeed, Busse et al. (36) utilized these modified criteria and found an increase from 8.5 to 16.8%.

The boundary between MCI and subjective cognitive impairment is complex, and it could seem arbitrary since it depends on an interaction between the properties of the employed tasks and the subject's characteristics (schooling, language, and cultural factors).

Moreover, the authors utilize different assessment tools for diagnosing MCI. In this review, 29 studies out of 66 only measured global functioning using MMSE, MoCA, CDR, or other tests. However, it is known that simpler, bedside screening tests like the MMSE and MoCA can be useful for screening purposes, but they may exhibit ceiling effects in those with the mildest levels of impairment (20).

As reported in Figure 6, the evaluation of global cognitive functioning alone or the evaluation of global functioning in addition to one, two, or more domains only highlights that evaluating at least three cognitive domains in addition to global functioning returns a higher prevalence. This result is quite predictable since evaluating more cognitive domains increases the possibility of identifying a compromised cognitive domain. However, it should be noted that the prevalence is lower when assessing global cognitive functioning plus two cognitive domains compared with assessing global cognitive functioning alone. Furthermore, in all cases, the range of MCI prevalence is very high. It is evident that the results are affected by other factors, such as the characteristics of the sample (e.g., age, gender, schooling) or the tests used to evaluate cognitive functions. Obtaining different prevalence using different tests can be obvious. For example, it is very different assessing attention with the Attentional Matrix test (105) rather than by using a more complex test that evaluates various aspects of attention, such as the Attentional Network Test (106) or a variant of it more suitable for evaluating attention systems in the elderly [e.g., (107, 108)]. Therefore, we can state that the prevalence of MCI is strongly affected by the choice of neuropsychological assessment parameters. This lack of a clear, standardized diagnostic methodology is the biggest cause of the highly heterogeneous prevalence rates found in this systematic review.

Besides these differences due to the MCI criteria, there are problems regarding the methodological aspects of the studies, that is, the sample characteristics (size, age, gender, schooling, and nationality) that we analyzed in the results section.

In particular, the minimum age for inclusion varied largely. Some studies included an older sample (≥60, ≥75, or ≥90), while others embraced much younger people (≥45, ≥50, or ≥55). This aspect could have influenced our prevalence estimates. The authors that include younger samples (≥45, ≥50 or ≥55; 26–33) found a prevalence range that goes from 3 to 34.4%. This range is lower than our whole range, implying that a lower prevalence could be caused by sampling younger people since cognitive deficits increase with aging. However, even among authors who include young adults in their sample, it is easily noticeable that there is great variability in MCI prevalence estimates (from 3 to 34.4%), and this aspect indicates that age is not the only variable that affects our results. However, there is no clear difference in the MCI prevalence in the different age groups. As already indicated, this result may depend on the age of the individual components that make up the group, the criteria and tests used for diagnosing MCI, and the transition from MCI to dementia in the older groups.

Gender composition is another factor that varies substantially in these studies, with a female share ranging from 43 to 75.9%. In literature, it is not known if gender has some effect on MCI prevalence. A meta-analysis by Au et al. (109) found a higher prevalence of naMCI in women. On the other hand, Petersen et al. (83) noted that MCI prevalence is higher in men. Some of our studies included a vast majority of women (≥70%; 34–38) and found a prevalence range that goes from 3 to 49%. This range is lower than our whole range, which could suggest that women present lower MCI prevalence than men, according to Petersen's findings (83). However, there is still great variability in this range, and it is not very easy to draw definitive conclusions.

Besides gender, another important factor to take into consideration is schooling. This aspect is very diverse in the retained studies, going from illiteracy to university education. Not having completed high school can increase the possibility of developing MCI (110). Therefore, it is possible that having included illiterate people in the sample has highly increased the resulting prevalence estimates. In our review, many studies included illiterate people in their sample (17, 28, 31, 33, 35, 36, 43, 46, 59, 65, 66, 73, 92). The prevalence range found by these studies goes from 6 to 68%, but these rates drop to 0.6% (28) in the articles that evaluate only aMCI. It is conceivable that MCI prevalence can increase by including illiterate people in the sample, but it is impossible to corroborate this hypothesis with the results from this review. Moreover, some studies did not specify the sample's schooling years, and this aspect complicates the interpretation of our results even further.

As concerns sample size, in this review, we compared studies with a few hundred participants with studies with a sample of a few thousand subjects, up to 15,000. 57.6% of our articles had a sample size larger than 1,000 participants. The sample size is an important variable to consider in prevalence studies. In general, sampling for prevalence surveys requires a precise evaluation, especially when the studied condition is very rare or has a tendency for geographical clustering. Therefore, the sample size estimate for prevalence studies is a function of expected prevalence and precision for a given level of confidence expressed by the z statistics (111). Most of the included studies did not specify the calculation method for their sampling, and 42.4% had a sample with < 1,000 participants. Moreover, three studies (31, 33, 78) had < 300 participants, and we considered this a risk of bias according to the guidelines (24). This discrepancy in sample sizes does not help make comparisons among studies.

Our data are also grouped by country. A recent review conducted by Pais et al. (112) has not found any difference in MCI prevalence based on the geographical region. On the other hand, another study considered eight countries (Cuba, Repubblica Dominicana, Perù, Messico, Venezuela, Porto Rico, Cina e India) and found prevalence estimates of aMCI ranged from 0.6 to 4.6% (28). MCI is a construct that is easily influenced by the dominant culture. In some countries, illiteracy is very high and, as stated above, education represents a protective factor against MCI (113). It is impossible to draw conclusions on this matter in our review since most of our articles are from Asian or European studies, and we do not have sufficient data on the other areas. However, it would be useful to conduct reviews for analyzing studies based only on one specific area since each country has a different educational system and illiteracy rates, which could result in different MCI prevalence rates. Also in this case, it is necessary to define whether the apparent absence of difference in prevalence can be real or represent a type I error due to the numerous methodological problems already mentioned, as is also suggested by the high standard deviations. Likewise, the use of different diagnostic criteria (Figure 5), the number of cognitive domains evaluated (Figure 6), and the used cut-offs (Figure 7) return a highly variable prevalence of MCI, attributable to the afore mentioned methodological problems.

It is easily noticeable how methodological aspects characterizing these studies are insufficient to account for the high heterogeneity that we found in our results. Therefore, we could conclude that the main factors contributing to our outcomes are various diagnostic and operational issues.

Limits, implications, and suggestions for the future

This review highlighted several limitations in examining prevalence studies regarding Mild Cognitive Impairment. In particular, the main weakness is given by the great heterogeneity of the included studies. Indeed, these 66 studies have used different criteria and scales to evaluate cognitive decline and assessed different cognitive domains. Therefore, this makes it difficult to compare results across studies. Another critical point of the reviewed studies concerns the different characteristics of the sample (size, age, sex, schooling) that limit the possibility of making comparisons even more. Moreover, another possible limit is linked to the incomplete recovery of studies: selecting only studies published in English and Italian may have led to eliminating important articles, further limiting the generalizability of the results. Another main limitation of this systematic review concerns the exclusion of all studies published before 1999. This choice was made to focus only on the MCI diagnosis after Petersen's proposal (7). However, since the 1960's authors have been trying to find nosological categories to describe the presence of an isolated cognitive impairment in the elderly who are not affected by dementia [e.g., Benign Senescent Forgetfulness, Age Associated Memory Impairment, Age Associated Cognitive Decline, Cognitive Impairment No Dementia; (114)]. Therefore, our results could be strongly affected by these exclusion criteria. However, our main goal was to focus on MCI, and we chose these criteria based on other authors who adopted our same strategy. Indeed, Vanacore et al. (114) state that, by searching the term “MCI” on PubMed, there was an increase of 929% from 1999 to 2016.

Regardless of these limitations, at the end of this review emerges an effective need to develop a diagnostic system shared and standardized. Indeed, in the light of the results of this review, it would be advisable for future research to define a standardized and homogenous diagnostic system so that it will be possible to compare results across studies and ensure greater efficiency and accuracy in the diagnosis of MCI. In addition, it will be necessary to apply universal cut-off values, which have considered the different factors associated with cognitive decline, such as age, schooling, and even gender. Moreover, we need to develop a broad test protocol to assess precisely all the cognitive domains involved in MCI. Specifically, there should be a consensus on the number and the type of cognitive domains to evaluate, the cut-off scores to utilize, and the assessment tools to implement. It would also be useful to analyze the interactions between various cognitive systems and the implications of the two cerebral hemispheres, which vary with age (107) and may also present specific characteristics in MCI. It could also be interesting to analyze the association of these cognitive systems with the autonomous nervous system since they operate in close association (115, 116), and this could give us an additional biomarker for diagnosing MCI.

Conclusions

Overall, this systematic review highlights a great variability in the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment. It is impossible to establish whether this heterogeneity is due to the adoption of different diagnostic criteria or dissimilarities in the sample's characteristics since all these factors are deeply interrelated. For example, some authors adopt the same diagnostic criteria (such as Petersen's) but use different assessment tools and cut-off scores. Therefore, we tried to stress this conclusion even further. We believe these results are caused by the lack of a standardized diagnostic methodology and an incomplete neuropsychological assessment. Consequently, many difficulties in this sector regard the effective implementation of interventions and therapies and the reliability of the results in the research field. Developing criteria and operationalizations that are more precise and standardized could allow improving benefits for all, firstly patients. Moreover, there is a strong need for a comprehensive standardized neuropsychological evaluation to allow a clear delineation of the aging profile associated with mild cognitive impairment. An evaluation of this type could be inserted into pathological aging prevention programs, and it could be useful for monitoring the progression from mild cognitive impairment to dementia.

In conclusion, the most relevant results of this review are two. First, the need to provide more methodological details in published studies on the composition of the sample in terms of age (e.g., reporting not only the mean age but also the age range and age distribution within the group), gender, schooling (also in this case reporting the distribution). Second, there is an urgent need to define a standard protocol for assessing cognitive decline, specifying which tests should be used, assessing all the main cognitive domains, and clearly defining the diagnostic criteria. In addition, the need to control the extent to which cognitive decline may represent the effect of other psychological (e.g., depression and anxiety) or behavioral (e.g., sleep quality) factors may be suggested, considering the close interconnection between these factors (107) and the effects of poor sleep on cognitive decline (117).

Data availability statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author contributions

MC had the idea of the work and made substantial contributions to its conception. FA, GF, and FF revised it thoroughly in each phase of the research. GM and AG acquired, analyzed, and interpreted the data. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Albert MS, DeKosky ST, Dickson D, Dubois B, Feldman HH, Fox NC, et al. The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer's disease: recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer's Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimers Dement. (2011) 7:270–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Guarino A, Forte G, Giovannoli J, Casagrande M. Executive functions in the elderly with mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review on motor and cognitive inhibition, conflict control and cognitive flexibility. Aging Ment Health. (2020) 24:1028–45. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2019.1584785

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment as a diagnostic entity. J Intern Med. (2004) 256:183–94. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01388.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Kral VA. Senescent forgetfulness: benign and malignant. Can Med Assoc J. (1962) 86:257–60.

Google Scholar

5. Ebly EM, Hogan DB, Parhad IM. Cognitive impairment in the nondemented elderly: results from the Canadian Study of Health and Aging. Arch Neurol. (1995) 52:612–19. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1995.00540300086018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Woodard JL, Dorsett ES, Cooper JG, Hermann BP, Sager MA. Development of a brief cognitive screen for mild cognitive impairment and neurocognitive disorder. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. (2005) 12:299–315. doi: 10.1080/138255890968268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Tangalos EG, Kokmen E. Mild cognitive impairment: clinical characterization and outcome. Arch Neurol. (1999) 56:303–8. doi: 10.1001/archneur.56.3.303

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment. N Engl J Med. (2011) 364:2227–34. doi: 10.1056/NEJMcp0910237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Petersen RC, Doody R, Kurz A, Mohs RC, Morris JC, Rabins PV, et al. Current concepts in mild cognitive impairment. Arch Neurol. (2001) 58:1985–92. doi: 10.1001/archneur.58.12.1985

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Corbo I, Casagrande M. Higher-level executive functions in healthy elderly and mild cognitive impairment: a systematic review. J Clin Med. (2022) 11:1204. doi: 10.3390/jcm11051204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Guarino A, Favieri F, Boncompagni I, Agostini F, Cantone M, Casagrande M. Executive functions in Alzheimer disease: a systematic review. Front Aging Neurosci. (2019) 10:437. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00437

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Grundman M, Petersen RC, Ferris SH, Thomas RG, Aisen PS, Bennett DA, et al. Mild cognitive impairment can be distinguished from Alzheimer disease and normal aging for clinical trials. Arch Neurol. (2004) 61:59–66. doi: 10.1001/archneur.61.1.59

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Jak AJ, Bondi MW, Delano-Wood L, Wierenga C, Corey-Bloom J, Salmon DP, et al. Quantification of five neuropsychological approaches to defining mild cognitive impairment. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2009) 17:368–75. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e31819431d5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Bondi MW, Smith GE. Mild cognitive impairment: a concept and diagnostic entity in need of input from neuropsychology. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2014) 20:129–34. doi: 10.1017/S1355617714000010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Matthews FE, Stephan BC, McKeith IG, Bond J, Brayne C, Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study. Two-year progression from mild cognitive impairment to dementia: to what extent do different definitions agree? J Am Geriatr Soc. (2008) 56:1424–33. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2008.01820.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Shah Y, Tangalos EG, Petersen, RC. Mild cognitive impairment. When is it a precursor to Alzheimer's disease? Geriatr Switz. (2000) 55:62–5.

Google Scholar

17. Jia J, Zhou A, Wei C, Jia X, Wang F, Li F, et al. The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and its etiological subtypes in elderly Chinese. Alzheimers Dement. (2014) 10:439–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.09.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Manly JJ, Tang MX, Schupf N, Stern Y, Vonsattel JP, Mayeux R. Frequency and course of mild cognitive impairment in a multiethnic community. Ann Neurol. (2008) 63:494–506. doi: 10.1002/ana.21326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Shimada H, Makizako H, Doi T, Yoshida D, Tsutsumimoto K, Anan Y, et al. Combined prevalence of frailty and mild cognitive impairment in a population of elderly Japanese people. J Am Med Dir Assoc. (2013) 14:518–24. doi: 10.1016/j.jamda.2013.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Dunne RA, Aarsland D, O'Brien JT, Ballard C, Banerjee S, Fox NC, et al. Mild cognitive impairment: the Manchester consensus. Age Ageing. (2021) 50:72–80. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afaa228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Panza F, D'Introno A, Colacicco AM, Capurso C, Del Parigi A, Caselli RJ, et al. Current epidemiology of mild cognitive impairment and other predementia syndromes. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2005) 13:633–44. doi: 10.1097/00019442-200508000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Liberati A, Altman DG, Tetzlaff J, Mulrow C, Gøtzsche PC, Ioannidis JP, et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. J Clin Epidemiol. (2009) 62:e1–34. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2009.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Prisma Group. Reprint—preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. Phys Ther. (2009) 89:873–80. doi: 10.1093/ptj/89.9.873

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Loney PL, Chambers LW, Bennett KJ, Roberts JG, Stratford PW. Critical appraisal of the health research literature: prevalence or incidence of a health problem. Chronic Dis Can. (1998) 19:170–6. Retrieved from: https://www.proquest.com/scholarly-journals/critical-appraisal-health-research-literature/docview/1370199450/se-2

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

25. Migliavaca CB, Stein C, Colpani V, Munn Z, Falavigna M. Quality assessment of prevalence studies: a systematic review. J Clin Epidemiol. (2020) 127:59–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jclinepi.2020.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Teh WL, Abdin E, Vaingankar JA, Shafie S, Jeyagurunathan A, Yunjue Z, et al. Prevalence, lifestyle correlates, and psychosocial functioning among multi-ethnic older adults with mild cognitive impairment in singapore: preliminary findings from a 10/66 population study. Yale J Biol Med. (2021) 94:73–83.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

27. Robertson K, Larson EB, Crane PK, Cholerton B, Craft S, McCormick WC, et al. Using varying diagnostic criteria to examine mild cognitive impairment prevalence and predict dementia incidence in a community-based sample. J Alzheimers Dis. (2019) 68:1439–51. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180746

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sosa AL, Albanese E, Stephan BC, Dewey M, Acosta D, Ferri CP, et al. Prevalence, distribution, and impact of mild cognitive impairment in Latin America, China, and India: a 10/66 population-based study. PLoS Med. (2012) 9:e1001170. doi: 10.1371/journal.pmed.1001170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Afgin AE, Massarwa M, Schechtman E, Israeli-Korn SD, Strugatsky R, Abuful A, et al. High prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease in Arabic villages in northern Israel: impact of gender and education. J Alzheimers Dis. (2012) 29:431–9. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-111667

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Morris JC. Current vision and scoring rules the clinical dementia rating (CDR). Neurology. (1993) 43:2412–14. doi: 10.1212/WNL.43.11.2412-a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

31. Alkhunizan M, Alkhenizan A, Basudan L. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in Saudi Arabia: a community-based study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord Extra. (2018) 8:98–103. doi: 10.1159/000487231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Trzepacz PT, Hochstetler H, Wang S, Walker B, Saykin AJ. Relationship between the montreal cognitive assessment and mini-mental state examination for assessment of mild cognitive impairment in older adults. BMC Geriatr. (2015) 15:1–9. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0103-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Amer M, Mousa S, Khater M, Wahab, WA. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment among older adults living in Mansoura city, Egypt. Middle East Curr Psychiatry. (2012) 19:3–7. doi: 10.1097/01.XME.0000407821.18381.3c

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Anstey KJ, Cherbuin N, Eramudugolla R, Sargent-Cox K, Easteal S, Kumar R, et al. Characterizing mild cognitive disorders in the young-old over 8 years: prevalence, estimated incidence, stability of diagnosis, and impact on IADLs. Alzheimers Dement. (2013) 9:640–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2012.11.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Busse A, Bischkopf J, Riedel-Heller SG, Angermeyer MC. Subclassifications for mild cognitive impairment: prevalence and predictive validity. Psychol Med. (2003) 33 33:1029–38. doi: 10.1017/S0033291703007839

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Busse A, Bischkopf J, Riedel-Heller SG, Angermeyer MC. Mild cognitive impairment: prevalence and incidence according to different diagnostic criteria: Results of the Leipzig Longitudinal Study of the Aged (LEILA75+). Br J Psychiatry. (2003) 182:449–54. doi: 10.1192/bjp.182.5.449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Chuang YF, Liu YC, Tseng HY, Lin PX, Li CY, Shih MH, et al. Urban-rural differences in the prevalence and correlates of mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling older adults in Taiwan: The EMCIT study. J Formos Med Assoc. (2021) 120:1749–57. doi: 10.1016/j.jfma.2021.03.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Clark LR, Koscik RL, Nicholas CR, Okonkwo OC, Engelman CD, Bratzke LC, et al. Mild cognitive impairment in late middle age in the Wisconsin registry for Alzheimer's prevention study: prevalence and characteristics using robust and standard neuropsychological normative data. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. (2016) 31:675–88. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acw024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Dimitrov I, Tzourio C, Milanov I, Deleva N, Traykov L. Prevalence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in a Bulgarian urban population. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. (2012) 27:131–35. doi: 10.1177/1533317512442371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Portet F, Ousset PJ, Visser PJ, Frisoni GB, Nobili F, Scheltens P, et al. Mild cognitive impairment (MCI) in medical practice: a critical review of the concept and new diagnostic procedure. Report of the MCI Working Group of the European Consortium on Alzheimer's Disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2006) 77:714–18. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2005.085332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ding D, Zhao Q, Guo Q, Meng H, Wang B, Luo J, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in an urban community in China: a cross-sectional analysis of the Shanghai Aging Study. Alzheimers Dement. (2015) 11:300–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2013.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Dlugaj M, Weimar C, Wege N, Verde PE, Gerwig M, Dragano N, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and its subtypes in the Heinz Nixdorf Recall study cohort. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2010) 30:362–73. doi: 10.1159/000320988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fernández-Blázquez MA, Noriega-Ruiz B, Ávila-Villanueva M, Valentí-Soler M, Frades-Payo B, Del Ser T, et al. Impact of individual and neighborhood dimensions of socioeconomic status on the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment over seven-year follow-up. Aging Ment Health. (2020) 25:814–23. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2020.1725803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Ganguli M, Chang CC, Snitz BE, Saxton JA, Vanderbilt J, Lee CW. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment by multiple classifications: the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team (MYHAT) project. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2010) 18:674–83. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181cdee4f

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Winblad B, Palmer K, Kivipelto M, Jelic V, Fratiglioni L, Wahlund LO, et al. Mild cognitive impairment–beyond controversies, towards a consensus: report of the International Working Group on Mild Cognitive Impairment. J Intern Med. (2004) 256:240–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2796.2004.01380.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Gavrila D, Antunez C, Tormo MJ, Carles R, Garcia Santos JM, Parrilla G, et al. Prevalence of dementia and cognitive impairment in Southeastern Spain: the Ariadna study. Acta Neurol Scand. (2009) 120:300–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2009.01283.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Caracciolo B, Palmer K, Monastero R, Winblad B, Bäckman L, Fratiglioni L. Occurrence of cognitive impairment and dementia in the community: a 9-year-long prospective study. Neurology. (2008) 70:1778–85. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000288180.21984.cb

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. GjØra L, Heine Strand B, Bergh S, Borza T, Brækhus A, Engedal K, et al. Current and future prevalence estimates of mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and its subtypes in a population-based sample of people 70 years and older in Norway: the HUNT Study. J Alzheimers Dis. (2021) 79:1213–26. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. González HM, Tarraf W, Schneiderman N, Fornage M, Vásquez PM, Zeng D, et al. Prevalence and correlates of mild cognitive impairment among diverse Hispanics/Latinos: study of latinos-investigation of neurocognitive aging results. Alzheimers Dement. (2019) 15:1507–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2019.08.202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Han JW, So Y, Kim TH, Lee DY, Ryu SH, Kim SY, et al. Prevalence rates of dementia and mild cognitive impairment are affected by the diagnostic parameter changes for neurocognitive disorders in the DSM-5 in a Korean population. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2017) 43:193–203. doi: 10.1159/000458408

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Hänninen T, Hallikainen M, Tuomainen S, Vanhanen M, Soininen H. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment: a population-based study in elderly subjects. Acta Neurol Scand. (2002) 106:148–54. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0404.2002.01225.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Juncos-Rabadan O, Pereiro AX, Facal D, Rodriguez N, Lojo C, Caamaño JA, et al. Prevalence and correlates of cognitive impairment in adults with subjective memory complaints in primary care centres. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2012) 33:226–32. doi: 10.1159/000338607

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Katz MJ, Lipton RB, Hall CB, Zimmerman ME, Sanders AE, Verghese J, et al. Age and sex specific prevalence and incidence of mild cognitive impairment, dementia and Alzheimer's dementia in blacks and whites: a report from the Einstein Aging Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2012) 26:335. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0b013e31823dbcfc

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Artero S, Ancelin ML, Portet F, Dupuy A, Berr C, Dartigues JF, et al. Risk profiles for mild cognitive impairment and progression to dementia are gender specific. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (2008) 79:979–84. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.2007.136903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Khedr E, Fawi G, Abbas MA, Mohammed TA, El-Fetoh NA, Al Attar G, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and dementia among the elderly population of Qena Governorate, Upper Egypt: a community-based study. J Alzheimers Dis. (2015) 45:117–26. doi: 10.3233/JAD-142655

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Kim KW, Park JH, Kim MH, Kim MD, Kim BJ, Kim SK, et al. A nationwide survey on the prevalence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment in South Korea. J Alzheimers Dis. (2011) 23:281–91. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2010-101221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kochan NA, Slavin MJ, Brodaty H, Crawford JD, Trollor JN, Draper B, et al. Effect of different impairment criteria on prevalence of “objective” mild cognitive impairment in a community sample. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2010) 18:711–22. doi: 10.1097/JGP.0b013e3181d6b6a9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Kumar R, Dear KB, Christensen H, Ilschner S, Jorm AF, Meslin C, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in 60-to 64-year-old community-dwelling individuals: the Personality and Total Health through Life 60+ study. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2005) 19:67–74. doi: 10.1159/000082351

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Lara E, Koyanagi A, Olaya B, Lobo A, Miret M, Tyrovolas S, et al. Mild cognitive impairment in a Spanish representative sample: prevalence and associated factors. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2016) 31:858–67. doi: 10.1002/gps.4398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Lee SB, Kim KW, Youn JC, Park JH, Lee JJ, Kim MH, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and its subtypes are influenced by the application of diagnostic criteria: results from the Korean Longitudinal Study on Health and Aging (KLoSHA). Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2009) 28:23–9. doi: 10.1159/000228841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Li X, Ma C, Zhang J, Liang Y, Chen Y, Chen K, et al. Prevalence of and potential risk factors for mild cognitive impairment in community-dwelling residents of Beijing. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2013) 61:2111–19. doi: 10.1111/jgs.12552

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Limongi F, Siviero P, Noale M, Gesmundo A, Crepaldi G, Maggi S. Prevalence and conversion to dementia of mild cognitive impairment in an elderly Italian population. Aging Clin Exp Res. (2017) 29:361–70. doi: 10.1007/s40520-017-0748-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Lindgren N, Rinne JO, Palviainen T, Kaprio J, Vuoksimaa E. Prevalence and correlates of dementia and mild cognitive impairment classified with different versions of the modified Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status (TICS-m). Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2019) 34:1883–91. doi: 10.1002/gps.5205

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Knopman DS, Roberts RO, Geda YE, Pankratz VS, Christianson TJ, Petersen RC, et al. Validation of the telephone interview for cognitive status-modified in subjects with normal cognition, mild cognitive impairment, or dementia. Neuroepidemiology. (2010) 34:34–42. doi: 10.1159/000255464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Liu CC, Liu CH, Sun Y, Lee HJ, Tang LY, Chiu MJ. Rural-urban disparities in the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in Taiwan: a door-to-door nationwide study. J Epidemiol. (2021) 2021:JE20200602. doi: 10.2188/jea.JE20200602

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Liu LY, Lu Y, Shen L, Li CB, Yu JT, Yuan CR, et al. Prevalence, risk and protective factors for mild cognitive impairment in a population-based study of Singaporean elderly. J Psychiatr Res. (2022) 145:111–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2021.11.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Lopez OL, Jagust WJ, DeKosky ST, Becker JT, Fitzpatrick A, Dulberg C, et al. Prevalence and classification of mild cognitive impairment in the cardiovascular health study cognition study: part 1. Arch Neurol. (2003) 60:1385–9. doi: 10.1001/archneur.60.10.1385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Lopez-Anton R, Santabarbara J, De-la-Cámara C, Gracia-García P, Lobo E, Marcos G, et al. Mild cognitive impairment diagnosed with the new DSM-5 criteria: prevalence and associations with non-cognitive psychopathology. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2015) 131:29–39. doi: 10.1111/acps.12297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Luck T, Riedel-Heller SG, Kaduszkiewicz H, Bickel H, Jessen F, Pentzek M, et al. Mild cognitive impairment in general practice: age-specific prevalence and correlate results from the German study on ageing, cognition and dementia in primary care patients (AgeCoDe). Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2007) 24:307–16. doi: 10.1159/000108099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Ma F, Wu T, Zhao J, Ji L, Song A, Zhang M, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and its subtypes among Chinese older adults: role of vascular risk factors. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2016) 41:261–72. doi: 10.1159/000446507

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Meguro K, Ishii H, Yamaguchi S, Ishizaki J, Sato M, Hashimoto R, et al. Prevalence and cognitive performances of clinical dementia rating 0.5 and mild cognitive impairment in Japan: the Tajiri Project. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2004) 18:3–10. doi: 10.1097/00002093-200401000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Mohan D, Iype T, Varghese S, Usha A, Mohan M. A cross-sectional study to assess prevalence and factors associated with mild cognitive impairment among older adults in an urban area of Kerala, South India. BMJ open. (2019) 9:e025473. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-025473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Mooi CS, Hamid TA. Prevalence and factors associated with mild cognitive impairment on screening in older Malaysians. Düşünen Adam. (2016) 29:298. doi: 10.5350/DAJPN2016290401

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Shahar S, Omar A, Vanoh D, Hamid TA, Mukari SZ, Din NC, et al. Approaches in methodology for population-based longitudinal study on neuroprotective model for healthy longevity (TUA) among Malaysian Older Adults. Aging Clin Exp Res. (2016) 28:1089–104. doi: 10.1007/s40520-015-0511-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Moretti F, De Ronchi D, Palmer K, Forlani C, Morini V, Ferrari B, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of mild cognitive impairment in the general population. Data from an Italian population-based study: the Faenza Project. Aging Ment Health. (2013) 17:267–75. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2012.732034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Noguchi-Shinohara M, Yuki S, Dohmoto C, Ikeda Y, Samuraki M, Iwasa K, et al. Differences in the prevalence of dementia and mild cognitive impairment and cognitive functions between early and delayed responders in a community-based study of the elderly. J Alzheimers Dis. (2013) 37:691–8. doi: 10.3233/JAD-130398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Ogunniyi A, Adebiyi AO, Adediran AB, Olakehinde OO, Siwoku AA. Prevalence estimates of major neurocognitive disorders in a rural Nigerian community. Brain Behav. (2016) 6:e00481. doi: 10.1002/brb3.481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Paddick SM, Kisoli A, Samuel M, Higginson J, Gray WK, Dotchin CL, et al. Mild cognitive impairment in rural Tanzania: prevalence, profile, and outcomes at 4-year follow-up. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2015) 23:950–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2014.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Parlevliet JL, Uysal-Bozkir Ö, Goudsmit M, van Campen JP, Kok RM, ter Riet G, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and dementia in older non-western immigrants in the Netherlands: a cross-sectional study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2016) 31:1040–49. doi: 10.1002/gps.4417

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Krabbe PF, Stouthard ME, Essink-Bot ML, Bonsel GJ. The effect of adding a cognitive dimension to the EuroQol multiattribute health-status classification system. J Clin Epidemiol. (1999) 52:293–301. doi: 10.1016/S0895-4356(98)00163-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Peltz CB, Corrada MM, Berlau DJ, Kawas CH. Cognitive impairment in nondemented oldest-old: prevalence and relationship to cardiovascular risk factors. Alzheimers Dement. (2012) 8:87–94. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Petersen RC. Mild cognitive impairment: current research and clinical implications. In: Seminars in Neurology, Vol. 27. New York, NY (2007). p. 022–031.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

83. Petersen RC, Roberts RO, Knopman DS, Geda YE, Cha RH, Pankratz VS, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment is higher in men: the Mayo Clinic Study of Aging. Neurology. (2010) 75:889–97. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181f11d85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Radford K, Mack HA, Draper B, Chalkley S, Daylight G, Cumming R, et al. Prevalence of dementia in urban and regional Aboriginal Australians. Alzheimers Dement. (2015) 11:271–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Rao D, Luo X, Tang M, Shen Y, Huang R, Yu J, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and its subtypes in community-dwelling residents aged 65 years or older in Guangzhou, China. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2018) 75:70–5. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2017.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Rentería MA, Manly JJ, Vonk JM, Arango SM, Obregon AM, Samper-Ternent R, et al. Midlife vascular factors and prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in late-life in Mexico. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (2021) 28:351–61. doi: 10.1017/S1355617721000539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Trittschuh EH, Crane PK, Larson EB, Cholerton B, McCormick WC, McCurry SM, et al. Effects of varying diagnostic criteria on prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in a community based sample. J Alzheimers Dis. (2011) 25:163–73. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2011-101821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Ruan Q, Xiao F, Gong K, Zhang W, Zhang M, Ruan J, et al. Prevalence of cognitive frailty phenotypes and associated factors in a community-dwelling elderly population. J Nutr Health Aging. (2020) 24:172–80. doi: 10.1007/s12603-019-1286-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Sasaki M, Kodama C, Hidaka S, Yamashita F, Kinoshita T, Nemoto K, et al. Prevalence of four subtypes of mild cognitive impairment and APOE in a Japanese community. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2009) 24:1119–26. doi: 10.1002/gps.2234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Shahnawaz Z, Reppermund S, Brodaty H, Crawford JD, Draper B, Trollor JN, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of depression in mild cognitive impairment: the Sydney Memory and Ageing Study. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2013) 127:394–402. doi: 10.1111/acps.12008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Jungwirth S, Weissgram S, Zehetmayer S, Tragl KH, Fischer P. VITA: subtypes of mild cognitive impairment in a community-based cohort at the age of 75 years. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2005) 20:452–8. doi: 10.1002/gps.1311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Su X, Shang L, Xu Q, Li N, Chen J, Zhang L, et al. Prevalence and predictors of mild cognitive impairment in Xi'an: a community-based study among the elders. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e83217. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Tognoni G, Ceravolo R, Nucciarone B, Bianchi F, Dell'Agnello G, Ghicopulos I, et al. From mild cognitive impairment to dementia: a prevalence study in a district of Tuscany, Italy. Acta Neurol Scand. (2005) 112:65–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2005.00444.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Tsolaki M, Gkioka M, Verykouki E, Galoutzi N, Kavalou E, Pattakou-Parasyri V. Prevalence of dementia, depression, and mild cognitive impairment in a rural area of the island of Crete, Greece. Am J Alzheimers Dis Other Demen. (2017) 32:252–64. doi: 10.1177/1533317517698789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Petersen RC, Smith GE, Waring SC, Ivnik RJ, Kokmen E, Tangelos EG. Aging, memory, and mild cognitive impairment. Int. Psychogeriatr. (1997) 9:65–9. doi: 10.1017/S1041610297004717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Tsoy RT, Turuspekova ST, Klipitskaya NK, Mereke A, Cumming RG. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment among older people in Kazakhstan and potential risk factors. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2019) 33:136–41. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000298

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Vlachos GS, Kosmidis MH, Yannakoulia M, Dardiotis E, Hadjigeorgiou G, Sakka P, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in the elderly population in Greece: results from the HELIAD study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2020) 34:156–62. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Welstead M, Luciano M, Muniz-Terrera G, Taylor AM, Russ TC. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in the Lothian Birth Cohort 1936. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. (2021) 35:230. doi: 10.1097/WAD.0000000000000433

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Wu L, He Y, Jiang B, Liu M, Wang J, Yang S, et al. The association between the prevalence, treatment and control of hypertension and the risk of mild cognitive impairment in an elderly urban population in China. Hypertens Res. (2016) 39:367–75. doi: 10.1038/hr.2015.146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Xu S, Xie B, Song M, Yu L, Wang L, An C, et al. High prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in the elderly: a community-based study in four cities of the Hebei Province, China. Neuroepidemiology. (2014) 42:123–30. doi: 10.1159/000357374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Yang J, Zhao X, Sui H, Liu X. High prevalence and low awareness of mild cognitive impairment in a suburban community in Shanghai. Neurol India. (2021) 69:1693. doi: 10.4103/0028-3886.333524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Juarez-Cedillo T, Sanchez-Arenas R, Sanchez-Garcia S, Garcia-Pena C, Hsiung GY, Sepehry AA, et al. Prevalence of mild cognitive impairment and its subtypes in the Mexican population. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2012) 34:271–81. doi: 10.1159/000345251

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Ward A, Arrighi HM, Michels S, Cedarbaum JM. Mild cognitive impairment: disparity of incidence and prevalence estimates. Alzheimers Dement. (2012) 8:14–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Huang J, Meyer JS, Zhang Z, Wei J, Hong X, Wang J, et al. Progression of mild cognitive impairment to Alzheimer's or vascular dementia versus normative aging among elderly Chinese. Curr Alzheimer Res. (2005) 2:571–8. doi: 10.2174/156720505774932223

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Spinnler H, Tognoni G. Italian group on the neuropsychological study of ageing: Italian standardization and classification of neuropsychological tests. Ital J Neurol Sci. (1987) 6:1–120.

106. Fan J, McCandliss BD, Sommer T, Raz A, Posner MI. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J Cogn Neurosci. (2002) 14:340–7. doi: 10.1162/089892902317361886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Casagrande M, Agostini F, Favieri F, Forte G, Giovannoli J, Guarino A, et al. Age-related changes in hemispherical specialization for attentional networks. Brain Sci. (2021) 11:1115. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11091115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Federico F, Marotta A, Orsolini M, Casagrande M. Aging in cognitive control of social processing: evidence from the attention network test. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. (2021) 28:128–42. doi: 10.1080/13825585.2020.1715336

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Au B, Dale-McGrath S, Tierney MC. Sex differences in the prevalence and incidence of mild cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis. Ageing Res Rev. (2017) 35:176–99. doi: 10.1016/j.arr.2016.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Sachdev PS, Lipnicki DM, Kochan NA, Crawford JD, Thalamuthu A, Andrews G., et al. The prevalence of mild cognitive impairment in diverse geographical and ethnocultural regions: the COSMIC collaboration. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0142388. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Arya R, Antonisamy B, Kumar S. Sample size estimation in prevalence studies. Indian J Pediatr. (2012) 79:1482–8. doi: 10.1007/s12098-012-0763-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Pais R, Ruano L, Carvalho O, Barros H. Global cognitive impairment prevalence and incidence in community dwelling older adults—A systematic review. Geriatrics. (2020) 5:84. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics5040084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Anderson ND. State of the science on mild cognitive impairment (MCI). CNS Spectrums. (2019) 24:78–87. doi: 10.1017/S1092852918001347

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Vanacore N, Di Pucchio A, Lacorte E, Bacigalupo I, Mayer F, Grande G, et al. From mild cognitive impairment to dementia: what is the role of public health? Recenti Prog Med. (2017) 108:211–5. doi: 10.1701/2695.27557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Forte G, Favieri F, Casagrande M. Heart rate variability and cognitive function: a systematic review. Front Neurosci. (2019) 13:710. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2019.00710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Forte G, Morelli M, Casagrande M. Heart rate variability and decision-making: autonomic responses in making decisions. Brain Sci. (2021) 11:243. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11020243

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Casagrande M, Forte G, Favieri F, Corbo I. Sleep quality and aging: a systematic review on healthy older people, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer's disease. IJERPH. (2022) 19:8457. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19148457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Appendix

3MS, Modified Mini-Mental State; ACE, Addenbrooke's Cognitive Examination tool; ADAS, Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Scale; ASR, Anomalous Sentence Repetition; BCST, Brookdale Cognitive Screening Test; BD, Block Design; BDAE, Boston Diagnostic Aphasia Examination; BIMC, Blessed Information–Memory-Concentration test; BJLOT, Benton Judgment of Line Orientation Test; BNT, Boston Naming Test; B-SEVLT, Brief-Spanish English Verbal Learning Test; BSRT, Buschke Selective Reminding Test; BVMT, Brief Visuospatial Memory Test; BVRT, Benton Visual Retention Test recognition; CAMCOG, Cambridge Cognitive Assessment-Revised; CAMDEX, Cambridge Examination for Mental Disorders of the Elderly; CASI, Cognitive Abilities Screening Instrument; CCD, Cross-Cultural Dementia; CCR, Category Cued Recall test; CDR, Clinical Dementia Rating Scale; CDT, Clock Drawing Test; CERAD, Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease; CERAD-K-C, Korean version of the CERAD Clinical Assessment Battery; CERAD-K-N, Korean version of the CERAD Neuropsychological Assessment Battery; CIM, Complex Ideational Material; CSI “D”, Community Screening Instrument for Dementia; CVLT, California Verbal Learning Test; DSB, Digit Span Backward; DSF, Digit Span Forward; DSST, Digit symbol substitution test; DST, Digit Span Test; eCog12, Everyday Cognition-12; FAB, Frontal Assessment Battery; FAS, FAS Verbal Fluency Test; FCSRT, Free and Cued Selective Reminding Test; FOME, Fuld Object-Memory Evaluation; GAI, Geriatric Anxiety Inventory; GDS, Global Deterioration Scale; GVLT, Greek Verbal Learning Test; HINDI, Hindi Mental State Examination; IDEA, Intervention for Dementia in Elderly Africans; IQCODE, Informant Questionnaire on Cognitive Decline in the Elderly; IU Token Test, Indiana University Token Test; KICA, Kimberley Indigenous Cognitive Assessment; LM, Logical Memory; -M, Modified; MCG, Medical College of Georgia; MDB, Mental Deterioration Battery; MES, Memory and Executive Screening test; MIS, Memory Impairment Screen; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; MMSE-KC, Korean version of Mini-Mental State Examination; MoCA, Montreal Cognitive Assessment; MP, Motor Programming; NART, National adult reading test; NCGGFAT, National Center for Geriatrics and Gerontology-Functional Assessment Tool; NTBV, Neuropsychological Test Battery Vienna; NCT, Number Connection Test; NIHTB PVT, NIH Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Test; -R, Revised; RAVLT, Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test/ AVLT, Auditory Verbal Learning Test; RBMT, Rivermead Behavioural Memory Test; RCS, Rapid Cognitive Screen; ROCF, Rey–Osterrieth complex figure test; RUDAS, Rowland Universal Dementia Assessment Scale; SBT, Short Blessed Test; SDMT, Symbol Digit Modalities Test; SIDAM, A Structured Interview for the diagnosis of dementia of the Alzheimer type, Multi-infarct dementia and dementias of other etiology according to ICD-10 and DSM-III-R; SIS, Six-Item Screener; SKT, Syndrom Kurztest; SRT, Story Recall Test; STMS, Short Test of Mental Status; TICS-m, Telephone Interview for Cognitive Status; TMT, Trail Making Test; VF, Verbal Fluency Test; WF, word fluency; VR, Visual Reproduction; WAB, Western Aphasia Battery; WAIS, Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale; WHO-UCLA AVLT, World Health Organization- University of California–Los Angeles Auditory Verbal Learning Test; WLM, word list memory; WLR, word list recall; WMS, Wechsler Memory Scale.

Keywords: mild cognitive impairment (MCI), prevalence, epidemiology, systematic review, MCI diagnostic criteria

Citation: Casagrande M, Marselli G, Agostini F, Forte G, Favieri F and Guarino A (2022) The complex burden of determining prevalence rates of mild cognitive impairment: A systematic review. Front. Psychiatry 13:960648. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.960648

Received: 03 June 2022; Accepted: 18 August 2022;
Published: 23 September 2022.

Edited by:

Gianfranco Spalletta, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Federica Piras, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy
Johannes Pantel, Goethe University Frankfurt, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Casagrande, Marselli, Agostini, Forte, Favieri and Guarino. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Giulia Marselli, Z2l1bGlhLm1hcnNlbGxpJiN4MDAwNDA7dW5pcm9tYTEuaXQ=; Maria Casagrande, bWFyaWEuY2FzYWdyYW5kZSYjeDAwMDQwO3VuaXJvbWExLml0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.