Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 25 July 2022
Sec. Addictive Disorders

Meaning in life as a mediator of dark triad with confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome among male drug abstainers

\nLiping ShiLiping Shi1Shijin SunShijin Sun1Xueli ZhuXueli Zhu2Yaoguo Geng
Yaoguo Geng3*
  • 1Department of Psychology, School of Social Development and Public Policy, Fudan University, Shanghai, China
  • 2Beijing Key Laboratory of Applied Experimental Psychology, National Demonstration Center for Experimental Psychology Education (Beijing Normal University), Faculty of Psychology, Beijing Normal University, Beijing, China
  • 3School of Education, Zhengzhou University, Zhengzhou, China

Background: Although the roles of personality in predicting substance abuse have been widely documented, few studies have investigated the relationships the dark triad (DT) personalities had with confidence in treatment (CIT) and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome (SETO) in drug abstainers.

Objective: This study examined the relationship between DT and treatment-relevant variables, and the potential effect of meaning in life (MIL) in these links.

Methods: Participants were male inpatients who started substance abuse treatment between June and December 2018 in Henan Province, China. The inclusion criteria were the diagnosis of substance use disorders. The exclusion criteria were illiteracy, comorbidity with psychopathology disorders, intellectual disability, and refusal of consent. A total of 236 men (aged 21–62 years, M = 45.30, SD = 7.72) were randomly selected and reported their DT, MIL, CIT, and SETO.

Results: Results showed that DT was negatively correlated with MIL, CIT, and SETO. MIL was positively correlated with CIT and SETO. The dark triad is associated with CIT both directly and indirectly via MIL. DT is indirectly correlated with SETO via MIL. Higher levels of DT in drug abstainers can reduce CIT and SETO by decreasing individual's MIL.

Conclusion: This study provides insights into the links between the DT and treatment-relevant variables, which can potentially impact the effectiveness of current substance abuse treatment programs.

Introduction

Substance abuse has become a serious global problem, affecting approximately 5% of the global adult population (1). Even more worrying, 29.5 million adults worldwide are suffering from drug dependence and need treatment. Substance abuse has ensued significant personal, familial, and economic consequences (1). For example, an estimated 28 million “healthy” life lost worldwide in 2015 as a result of premature death and disability caused by substance abuse. However, the effectiveness of current substance abuse treatment programs is questioned due to the high rate of relapse after treatment (2). Thus, clarifying the risk factors for the recurrence of substance abuse is imperative.

Numerous studies have consistently found that personality traits are associated with the occurrence of substance abuse behaviors, as well as recovery frustration (3, 4). For example, drug users (who use heroin and cocaine) are found to generally show higher neuroticism and lower conscientiousness of Big Five personality traits than healthy controls and smokers (5). Neuroticism may be a risk factor for the recurrence of substance abuse, while conscientiousness is a protective factor for recovery (2).

In comparison with the Big Five personality traits, the dark triad (DT) personality was considered to be part of the dark side of human nature (6). DT consists of three conceptually distinct but empirically overlapping personality traits, namely, Machiavellianism (marked by scheming and manipulative), narcissism (featured by grandiosity, vanity, and entitlement), and psychopathy (characterized by impulsiveness, callousness, and aggression). Dark triad theory has aroused much scientific interest in the past two decades. However, to our knowledge, only a small number of studies addressed the links between DT and substance abuse (5, 7).

For example, DT was found to be positively associated with the frequency of substance use (7), and narcissism and psychopathy were found to be predictors of the number of substances used among college students (5). Some specific aspects of DT, such as disagreeableness, antagonism, impulsivity/deficit in self-control, and sensation-seeking, emerged as predictors for the development of substance use disorders (811). Additionally, studies suggest that people high in DT traits have a preference for central nervous system stimulants (e.g., cocaine, caffeine, and methamphetamine), while professional athletes high in DT traits were more likely to use performance-enhancing drugs than those athletes low in DT traits (7).

Although previous studies are influential in underlining the impact of DT on the vulnerability or propensity of substance abuse, little is known about the role of DT in the process of recovery from substance abuse or drug addiction, especially for the impact on treatment-relevant variables, such as confidence in treatment (CIT) and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome (SETO) (12, 13). Confidence in treatment is an important prognostic indicator for the treatment of substance abuse disorder (14), reflecting patients' sense of self-efficacy, an expectation of cure (12), and the degree to refuse drugs. Strong confidence in refusing drugs could significantly reduce the frequency of marijuana use (15) and was a predictor of rehabilitation and long-term treatment effect (1517). Similar to confidence in treatment, subjective evaluation of treatment outcome is often used to assess the therapeutic effect in the medical field (13) and may be influenced by disease-specific (such as severity) or patient-specific characteristics (such as age, gender, and personality traits). Unfortunately, to date, no work has addressed the links between DT traits and confidence in the treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome.

Previous research has noted associations between substance abuse and meaning in life (the extent to which respondents perceive their lives as meaningful and the extent to which respondents are actively seeking meaning in their lives) (1822). For example, Frankl reported that people would feel empty if they failed to find meaning in their lives, and then reduced the sense of emptiness through drugs (19). Recently, Zhuang and Chen assessed the effectiveness of the Logotherapy group counseling in alleviating the relapse tendency in a Chinese drug abstainer sample and found that Logotherapy group counseling could effectively reduce the chance of relapse by improving their meaning of life (23). It appears that substance abuse is correlated with the absence of reasons or meaning in life (20), and search and acquisition of meaning in life is essential to successfully abstain from drugs and maintain the long-term curative effect for drug abstainers.

Recently, meaning in life has also been shown to be associated with DT. For example, Wang et al. reported that DT was negatively correlated with meaning in life, and patience fully mediated the links (24). Moreover, Geng et al. found that the sense of life meaning can buffer the influence of early childhood adversity on DT, with the higher the sense of life meaning, the weaker the effects of childhood adversity on life-history strategy and life-history strategy on DT (25). Therefore, we believe a low level of meaning in life might be a psychological marker of those captured by high DT. Based on the above literature, we presume that meaning in life mediates the impact of the dark triad on treatment-relevant variables.

In this study, using Chinese male drug abstainers as subjects, we conducted research to examine the associations between DT and confidence in treatment to fill several gaps in the literature. Based on the nature of DT (such as lower agreeableness, emotional coldness, self-promotion, and a fast life history strategy) and its impact on social-psychological functions (such as lower intimacy in personal relationships, a stronger disposition to aggression and impulsivity, cyberbullying, increased sexual harassment tendency, sleep dysfunctions, as well as disordered eating style; (6, 2628)), we first hypothesized that DT would be inversely correlated with confidence in treatment. Besides, we also sought to investigate the relationship between DT and subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes in a sample of Chinese male drug abstainers. In view of the fact that there is no relevant literature published, we make no assumptions about the relationship between DT and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome. Moreover, little research has focused on the associations among DT, meaning in life, and treatment-relevant variables so far. Thus, we aimed to examine the associations among DT, meaning in life, confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome, and expand on previous works by examining the potential mediating effect of meaning in life on the associations between DT and confidence in treatment, as well as subjective evaluation of treatment outcome. It should be noted that, as an exploratory study, the DT total score was first used to evaluate the associations among dark personalities, meaning in life, and treatment relevant variables in this study. This work is expected to provide some guidance for the formulation and implementation of drug treatment policies.

In summary, we expected to find the following: (a) negative correlations between DT and meaning in life, and confidence in treatment; (b) positive correlations between meaning in life and confidence in treatment; and (c) meaning in life serves as a mediator between DT and confidence in treatment.

Method

Participants

Participants were male inpatients who started substance abuse treatment between June and December 2018 in Henan Province, China. A total of 236 men with substance use disorders (aged 21–62 years, M = 45.30, SD = 7.72) were diagnosed according to DSM-V criteria. The diagnosis was performed or identified in the department of psychiatry at the 8th people's hospital of Zhengzhou through a non-structured interview and an extensive psychiatric assessment conducted by two qualified psychiatrists. The patients had no illiteracy, intellectual disability, and other major psychiatric co-morbidity. There were 75.74% respondents with education at or above junior high school level, 22.46% were unmarried, 42.37% were married, and 35.17% were divorced, separated, or widowed. All participants entered treatment for the first time and completed our survey during the recovery phase. Most participants received treatment for heroin addiction, but addiction to crystal methamphetamine and cannabis was also represented in the sample. This study was granted in June 2018 by the Ethical Committee of Zhengzhou University and carried out in accordance with the approved guidelines and regulations. All subjects confirmed their compliance through written informed consent.

Measures

The Short Dark Triad Scale—Chinese version (SD3-C) was adopted to measure the three aspects of the Dark Triad (29). The first nine items identify Machiavellianism (e.g.,“I like to use clever manipulation to get my way”), the second nine items focus on psychopathy (e.g., “People who mess with me always regret it”), and the last nine items measure narcissism (e.g.,“I insist on getting the respect I deserve”). Participants rated the extent to which they agreed with each item on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). We calculated each aspect by adding its corresponding items as well as composited a total score of DT by adding all items. In the current study, the scale has sound reliability: α(Fullscale) = 0.85, α(Machiavellianism) = 0.71, α(psychopathy) = 0.71, and α(narcissism) = 0.63. Only the DT total score was used to evaluate the associations among dark personalities, meaning in life, and treatment-relevant variables in this study.

The 10-item Meaning in Life Questionnaire (MLQ) is a self-reported inventory designed to measure perceived life meaning (30). In the current study, we utilized the Chinese version of MLQ (31), which comprises two subscales: presence and search. Items anchored on a 7-point scale range from one (absolutely untrue) to seven (absolutely true). Presence assesses the degree to which life is perceived as meaningful (e.g., “My life has a clear sense of purpose”), while Search assesses individual's motivation to discover meaning in life (e.g., “I am always looking to find my life's purpose”). We calculated each subscale by adding its corresponding items. In this study, the internal consistency reliability of the entire scale was 0.75 for the entire questionnaire, 0.69 for Presence subscale, and 0.77 for Search subscale.

The Confidence in Treatment (CIT) was assessed with five items derived from the Treatment Motivation Questionnaire (TMQ) (32). Items were rated from 1 (not true at all) to 4 (very true). Items were added to create an indicator of CIT. A higher score represents a higher sense of confidence in treatment. In our sample, the internal consistency reliability of the entire scale was 0.66.

Subjective evaluation of treatment outcome (SETO) was measured with one item (i.e., “I believe that the treatment provided is effective”) on a 5-point scale (1 = strongly disagree, 5 = strongly agree). When the item clears the conceptual ambiguity and reflects the concept common to participants' daily experiences, single-item measures can be appropriated (33). We had explained the meaning of this item description, so we hope we can obtain an accurate indicator of SETO. A higher value stands for a higher evaluation of SETO.

Statistical analysis

To check our hypotheses, correlation analysis was performed by SPSS 23.0 and our proposed structural equation model (SEM) was examined by the AMOS 22.0 program with DT and meaning in life as latent variables and confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome as observed variables. DT served as the predictor variables, confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome as the outcome variables, and meaning in life as the mediator. To determine whether an indirect effect or a total effect was statistically significant, we used maximum likelihood estimation and the bias-corrected bootstrap 95% confidence interval (CI) based on 5,000 bootstrapping. Several indices were considered to evaluate the adjustment of the model (i.e., the minimum discrepancy (χ2/df ; values in the range of 2–5 are indicative of an acceptable fit) (34), the Comparative Fit Index and the Incremental Fit Index (CFI and IFI; values equal to or over 0.95 indicate a good adjustment), and root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA; values below 0.08 indicate a good fit) (35).

Results

Descriptive analyses

In Table 1, we reported the descriptive statistics and correlations among study variables. The composite of DT was negatively correlated with confidence in treatment, subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes, and meaning in life. In addition, meaning in life, confidence in treatment, and subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes were positively correlated with each other.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations of study variables.

Structural equation modeling

The results of SEM indicated an acceptable fit to the data (χ2/df = 2.08, CFI = 0.96, IFI = 0.96, and RMSEA = 0.07). Analysis of parameter estimates indicated that except for the pathway from DT to subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes, standard coefficients of pathway were all significant (as shown in Figure 1), indicating that DT predicts subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes indirectly via meaning in life (β = −0.05, SE = 0.03, 95% CI [−0.11, −0.01]), and R2 = 0.24; while DT can predict confidence in treatment both directly (β = −0.13, SE = 0.07, 95% CI [−0.27, −0.03]) and indirectly (β = −0.09, SE = 0.03, 95% CI [−0.15, −0.04]), and R2 = 0.74.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The mediated model of meaning in life between DT and CIT and SETO. CIT, confidence in treatment; SETO, subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes. Numbers are standardized regression coefficients. *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

Discussion

For all we know, this is the first study that mainly focused on the relationships among the DT, treatment-relevant variables, and meaning in life. The results revealed that DT was negatively correlated with confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes. Meaning in life was positively correlated with confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes. DT can predict confidence in treatment both directly and indirectly through meaning in life, while DT predicts subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes indirectly.

In support of our hypotheses, DT was found to be negatively correlated with confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes, suggesting that drug abstainers scoring high on DT are more likely to have low confidence in treatment and low subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes. There are theoretical reasons and empirical evidence for these findings. Theoretically, DT may be an indicator of a fast Life History strategy (8, 36, 37). According to the Life History Theory (38), individuals need to trade-off reproductive efforts against somatic efforts continually, and these trade-offs will stabilize into behavioral strategies that vary in a continuum from slow to fast. A fast life strategy is reflective of reproductive efforts outweighing somatic efforts, and is mainly manifested by casual sex, short-term mating, risk-taking behaviors (such as substance use), and a lack of long-term planning and responsibility (39, 40). Individuals who (e.g., those with a high level of DT) follow a fast strategy go at the expense of psychological and physical health to solve adaptive tasks.

On the other hand, empirical studies have revealed that DT is marked by failure to self-regulate, risky behavior, dissatisfaction of the need, and reduced subjective wellbeing (4143). Therefore, drug abstainers with a high level of DT traits tend to pay less attention to improving healing and commit to treatment, and they consequently perceive less fulfillment and satisfaction, thus following worse treatment evaluation.

As expected, the present study was able to provide an insight into the underlying psychological process that may be associated with treatment-relevant variables. SEM revealed that DT indirectly lowered confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes through meaning in life. These results suggest that (1) the greater their scores on DT, the more participants endorsed a lower level of meaning in life; (2) the lower their scores on meaning in life, the less participants endorsed confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome; and (3) meaning in life mediated the effects of DT on confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome. In this sense, meaning in life may be an important basic psychological function to understand the effects of personality on relapse tendency (44). These results are largely in line with previous studies on the relationships between DT and internalizing problems (such as less appreciation of others and increased negative evaluation) (26).

There are several theoretical and practical contributions and implications of this study. First, we are interested in the relationship between DT and substance abuse treatment. For one thing, our findings enrich the literature about the relevant variables of substance abuse treatment in Chinese culture. Related studies have concentrated on the Big Five personality traits and largely ignored the impact of DT (2). Combined with previous studies, our findings provide a more complete picture by including the DT as a factor that may relate to confidence in substance abuse treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome. The results provide initial evidence of negative relations between the DT and substance abuse treatment variables. Another reason is that the negative effects of the DT on substance abuse treatment further indicate that the DT has a broader scope of impacts because the dark side of the DT has mainly been limited to the susceptibility and severity of drug misuse in previous studies (7).

Second, this study explored the possible indirect pathways involving emotional function and observed that the DT weakened meaning in life, which could then damage confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome. Substance abuse treatment is not only a personal disturbance but also a social issue. These findings could offer some guidance for the government of China and other courtiers to set and implement policies for improving drug treatment. For example, in the process of detoxification, relevant staff should pay attention to the plasticity of personality and the impact of a dark personality on life meaning. Focusing on reducing the negative and destructive characteristics of drug abstainers, and helping them to gain and strengthen meaning should be treated as ways to improve recovery and reduce relapse.

Limitation and future directions

Despite the strengths of the current study, its potential limitations should be considered. First, the cross-sectional nature of our data limits our ability to draw conclusions about causal links. Future studies should therefore make use of longitudinal study designs. Second, we must pay attention to the problem that the effect sizes are too small in predicting CIT and SETO, which means that there are other key mediating variables, and future research should explore the other internal mechanisms. Third, as a Chinese drug abstainer sample was studied, the treatment pattern of drug abstainers was different from other mental disorders. Future research would generalize our findings to a diverse clinical population. Fourth, given that Machiavellianism, narcissism, and psychopathy include their own dimensions, SD3 may be too brief to capture the whole picture of the DT. Future research may examine the associations between meaning in life, confidence in treatment, and subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes, and different types of psychopathy, narcissism. Lastly, we draw conclusions only based on the Chinese population. Future research should consider a more varied cultural populations, such as Western populations.

Conclusion

Taken together, the present study underscores the importance of the DT personality as a destructive factor of confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes, and meaning in life as a protective factor of confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethical Committee of Zhengzhou University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author contributions

LS and YG conceptualized and designed the study. SS gave directions to the study. LS and XZ analyzed and interpreted the data and wrote the manuscript. SS, XZ, and YG critically reviewed the manuscript text. All authors reviewed the manuscript and given final approval of the version to be published.

Funding

This work was supported by the Education Program of the National Social Science Fund of China (BBA170064).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the Qiliyan Substance Abuse Treatment Center for their assistance on this project.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. United Nations Office on Drugs and Crime. 2017. World Drug Report. (2017). Available online at: https://www.unodc.org/wdr2017/index.html.

2. Hanif R, Kliewer W, Riaz R. Personality traits differentiate Pakistani males in recovery versus relapse from substance use disorders. Pers Indiv Diff. (2019) 141:226–8. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.01.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Hakulinen C, Elovainio M, Batty GD, Virtanen M, Kivimäki M, Jokela M. Personality and alcohol consumption: pooled analysis of 72,949 adults from eight cohort studies. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2015) 151:110–4. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2015.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. N'goran AA, Baggio S, Deline S, Studer J, Mohler-Kuo M, Daeppen JB, et al. Association between non-medical prescription drug use and personality traits among young Swiss men. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2015) 69:228–37. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12231

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Stenason L, Vernon PA. The dark triad, reinforcement sensitivity and substance use. Pers Indiv Diff. (2016) 94:59–63. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2016.01.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Paulhus DL, Williams KM. The dark triad of personality: narcissism, machiavellianism and psychopathy. J Res Pers. (2002) 36:556–63. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(02)00505-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Gott AJ, Hetzel-Riggin MD. What did you expect? Substance use expectancies mediate the relationships between dark triad traits, substance use, and substance preference. Psychol Rep. (2018) 121:831–52. doi: 10.1177/0033294118755094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Crysel LC, Crosier BS, Webster GD. The dark triad and risk behavior. Pers Indiv Diff. (2013) 54:35–40. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.07.029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Feldman M, Boyer B, Kumar VK, Prout M. Personality, drug preference, drug use, and drug availability. J Drug Educ. (2011) 41:45–63. doi: 10.2190/DE.41.1.c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Flexon JL, Meldrum RC, Young JTN, Lehmann PS. Low self-control and the dark triad: disentangling the predictive power of personality traits on young adult substance use, offending and victimization. J Crim Justice. (2016) 46:159–69. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2016.05.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Kotov R, Gamez W, Schmidt F, Watson D. Linking “big” personality traits to anxiety, depressive, and substance use disorders: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. (2010) 136:768–821. doi: 10.1037/a0020327

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Velicer WF, Diclemente CC, Rossi JS, Prochaska JO. Relapse situations and self-efficacy: an integrative model. Addict Behav. (1990) 15:271–83. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(90)90070-E

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mercer SW, Neumann M, Wirtz M, Fitzpatrick B, Vojt G. General practitioner empathy, patient enablement, and patient-reported outcomes in primary care in an area of high socio-economic deprivation in Scotland—a pilot prospective study using structural equation modeling. Patient Educ Couns. (2008) 73:240–5. doi: 10.1016/j.pec.2008.07.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Kadden RM, Litt MD. The role of self-efficacy in the treatment of substance use disorders. Addict Behav. (2011) 36:1120–6. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2011.07.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Hayaki J, Herman DS, Hagerty CE, de Dios MA, Anderson BJ, Stein MD. Expectancies and self-efficacy mediate the effects of impulsivity on marijuana use outcomes: an application of the acquired preparedness model. Addict Behav. (2015) 36:389–96. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2010.12.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Chung T, Maisto SA, Mihalo A, Martin CS, Cornelius JR, Clark DB. Brief assessment of readiness to change tobacco use in treated youth. J Subst Abus Treat. (2011) 41:137–47. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2011.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Litt MD, Kadden RM, Kabela-Cormier E, Petry NM. Coping skills training and contingency management treatments for marijuana dependence: exploring mechanisms of behavior change. Addiction. (2010) 103:638–48. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02137.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Chen G. Natural recovery from drug and alcohol addiction among Israeli prisoners. J Offender Rehabil. (2006) 43:1–17. doi: 10.1300/J076v43n03_01

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Frankl VE. Man's Search for Ultimate Meaning. Cambridge: Perseus Publishing. (2000).

PubMed Abstract

20. Harlow LL, Newcomb MD, Bentler PM. Depression, self-derogation, substance use, and suicide ideation: lack of purpose in life as a mediational factor. J Clin Psychol. (1986) 42:5–21. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(198601)42:1<5::aid-jclp2270420102>3.0.co;2-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Heintzelman SJ, Christopher J, Trent J, King LA. Counterfactual thinking about one's birth enhances well-being judgments. J Posit Psychol. (2013) 8:44–9. doi: 10.1080/17439760.2012.754925

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Schellekens AFA, Jong CAJD, Buitelaar JK, Verkes RJ. Co-morbid anxiety disorders predict early relapse after inpatient alcohol treatment. Eur Psychiat. (2015) 30:128–36. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2013.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Zhuang YJ, Chen YQ. The effectiveness of Logotherapy group counseling in alleviating the relapse tendency of drug abstainers. J Psychol Sci. (2015) 38:468–73.

Google Scholar

24. Wang J, Li T, Wang K, Wang C. Patience as a mediator between the dark triad and meaning in life. Appl Res Qual Life. (2019) 14:527–43. doi: 10.1007/s11482-018-9627-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Geng YG, Sai XY, Jonason PK, Yang MQ, Zhu XL, Gu JJ, et al. Childhood adversity is associated with adulthood white blood cell count through narcissism. Pers Indiv Diff. (2021) 174:110662. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110662

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. PreotiucPietro D, Carpenter J, Giorgi S, Ungar L. Studying the Dark Triad of Personality using Twitter Behavior Studying the Dark Triad of personality through Twitter behavior. in Proceedings of the 25th ACM International on Conference on Information and Knowledge Management. (2017) 76:761–70. doi: 10.1145/2983323.2983822

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Yang M, Zhu X, Sai X, Zhao F, Wu H, Geng Y. The dark triad and sleep quality: mediating role of anger rumination. Pers Indiv Diff. (2019) 151:109484. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2019.06.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Shi L, Sun S, Geng Y. Differential eating behavior patterns among the dark triad. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19:7062. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19127062

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Zhang J, Ziegler M, Paulhus DL. Development and evaluation of the short dark triad - Chinese version (SD3-C). Curr Psychol. (2020) 39:1161–71. doi: 10.1007/s12144-019-00272-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Van Orden KA, Bamonti PM, King DA, Duberstein PR. Does perceived burdensomeness erode meaning in life among older adults? Aging Ment Health. (2012) 16:855–60. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2012.657156

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Liu SS, Gan YQ. Reliability and validity of the Chinese version of the meaning in life questionnaire. Chin Ment Health J. (2010) 24:478–82. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2010.06.021

CrossRef Full Text

32. Ryan RM, Plant RW, O'Malley S. Initial motivations for alcohol treatment: relations with patient characteristics, treatment involvement, and dropout. Addict Behav. (1995) 20:279–97. doi: 10.1016/0306-4603(94)00072-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wanous JP, Reichers AE. Estimating the reliability of a single-item measure. Psychol Rep. (1996) 78:631–4. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1996.78.2.631

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Marsh HW, Hocevar D. Application of confirmatory factor analysis to the study of self-concept: first- and higher-order factor models and their invariance across groups. Psychol Bull. (1985) 97:562–82. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.97.3.562

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Hu L, Bentler PM. Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure anaysis: conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct Equ Modeling. (1999) 6:1–55. doi: 10.1080/10705519909540118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Jonason PK, Foster JD, Egorova MS, Parshikova O, Csathó A, Oshio A, et al. The Dark Triad traits from a life history perspective in six countries. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:1476. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01476

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Jonason PK, Koenig BL, Tost J. Living a fast life. Hum Nat. (2010) 21:428–42. doi: 10.1007/s12110-010-9102-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Del Giudice M. An evolutionary life history framework for psychopathology. Psychol Inq. (2014) 25:261–300. doi: 10.1080/1047840X.2014.884918

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Figueredo AJ, Baca TCD, Woodley MA. The measurement of human life history strategy. Pers Indiv Diff. (2013) 55:251–5. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Figueredo AJ, Cabeza dBT, Black CJ, Garcí RA, Fernandes HB, Wolf PS, et al. Methodologically sound: evaluating the psychometric approach to the assessment of human life history. Evol Psychol. (2015) 13:299–338. doi: 10.1177/147470491501300202

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

41. Dunkel CS, Mathes E, Harbke C. Life history strategy, identity consolidation, and psychological well-being. Pers Indiv Diff. (2011) 51:34–8. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Jonason PK, Baughman HM, Carter GL, Parker P. Dorian Gray without his portrait: psychological, social, and physical health costs associated with the Dark Triad. Pers Indiv Diff. (2015) 78:5–13. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Malesza M, Ostaszewski P. Dark side of impulsivity - Associations between the Dark Triad, self-report and behavioral measures of impulsivity. Pers Indiv Diff . (2016) 88:197–201. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.09.016

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Mund M, Finn C, Hagemeyer B, Neyer FJ. Understanding dynamic transactions between personality traits and partner relationships. Curr Dir Psychol. (2016) 25:411–6. doi: 10.1177/0963721416659458

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dark triad, substance abuse, confidence in treatment, subjective evaluation of treatment outcomes, meaning in life

Citation: Shi L, Sun S, Zhu X and Geng Y (2022) Meaning in life as a mediator of dark triad with confidence in treatment and subjective evaluation of treatment outcome among male drug abstainers. Front. Psychiatry 13:928101. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.928101

Received: 25 April 2022; Accepted: 28 June 2022;
Published: 25 July 2022.

Edited by:

Béla Birkás, University of Pécs, Hungary

Reviewed by:

Jelena Dostanic, Union University, Serbia
Ling Yang, Northwest Normal University, China

Copyright © 2022 Shi, Sun, Zhu and Geng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yaoguo Geng, Z2VuZ3lnJiN4MDAwNDA7enp1LmVkdS5jbg==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.