Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 22 July 2022
Sec. Neuroimaging

Neural correlates of repetitive negative thinking: Dimensional evidence across the psychopathological continuum

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Cognitive Neuroscience, Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 3Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behavior, Radboud University and Radboud University Medical Center, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 4Pro Persona Mental Health Care, Depression Expertise Center, Nijmegen, Netherlands

Repetitive negative thinking (RNT) captures an important transdiagnostic factor that predisposes to a maladaptive stress response and contributes to diverse psychiatric disorders. Although RNT can best be seen as a continuous symptom dimension that cuts across boundaries from health to various psychiatric disorders, the neural mechanisms underlying RNT have almost exclusively been studied in health and stress-related disorders, such as depression and anxiety disorders. We set out to study RNT from a large-scale brain network perspective in a diverse population consisting of healthy subjects and patients with a broader range of psychiatric disorders. We studied 46 healthy subjects along with 153 patients with a stress-related and/or neurodevelopmental disorder. We focused on three networks, that are associated with RNT and diverse psychiatric disorders: the salience network, default mode network (DMN) and frontoparietal network (FPN). We investigated the relationship of RNT with both network connectivity strength at rest and with the stress-induced changes in connectivity. Across our whole sample, the level of RNT was positively associated with the connectivity strength of the left FPN at rest, but negatively associated with stress-induced changes in DMN connectivity. These findings may reflect an upregulation of the FPN in an attempt to divert attention away from RNT, while the DMN result may reflect a less flexible adaptation to stress, related to RNT. Additionally, we discuss how our findings fit into the non-invasive neurostimulation literature. Taken together, our results provide initial insight in the neural mechanisms of RNT across the spectrum from health to diverse psychiatric disorders.

Introduction

Stress-vulnerability models provide a transdiagnostic framework to explain how psychopathology arises from specific interactions between stressors and individual vulnerabilities (1). An individual's tendency to Repetitive Negative Thinking (RNT) captures an important transdiagnostic cognitive vulnerability, that predisposes to a maladaptive stress response, and thereby constitutes a risk factor for a broad range of psychiatric disorders (2, 3). RNT entails a way of thinking about problems or negative experiences that is repetitive, intrusive, and difficult to disengage from (4). Initially, research on RNT started in disorder-specific research, using the concept ‘rumination' in relation to depression and ‘worry' when studying anxiety disorders. Recently, however, it has been recognized that worry and rumination are in fact both characterized by the same core elements that are mentioned above (5).

RNT can best be seen as a continuous symptom dimension that not only cuts across diagnostic boundaries (6), but also from health to disorder (7). In health, it is an important risk factor for developing stress-related disorders (i.e., depression and anxiety disorders) (7). In psychopathology, it reflects a crucial transdiagnostic factor in the onset, maintenance, and recurrence of especially (comorbid) stress-related disorders (6, 8, 9). There are however clear indications that RNT is present in most psychiatric disorders (4) with also a high prevalence in neurodevelopmental disorders [i.e., autism spectrum disorder (ASD) and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)] (3, 10, 11).

At the psychological level, RNT may be explained by the impaired disengagement theory, which states that RNT results from impaired cognitive control to divert attention away from one's negative thoughts (8). In health, negative information about oneself is often in discord with positive self-views and leads to cognitive conflict and disengagement from these thoughts. Pre-existent negative cognitive schemas may predispose someone to RNT and the development of a stress-related disorder, by preventing the employment of these self-regulatory strategies (3). Patients with neurodevelopmental disorders may have specific predispositions, which make them vulnerable to RNT. Patients with ADHD are prone to worrisome, intrusive thoughts due to cognitive control deficits (11) and common traits in ASD, like heightened self-focus, and cognitive inflexibility in attention switching, may predispose these patients to RNT (3). This vulnerability for RNT in neurodevelopmental disorders may be a key factor explaining the high comorbidity with stress-related disorders (3).

Despite the important role of RNT as a transdiagnostic factor in psychopathology, the mechanistic underpinnings at the brain level are still unclear. Three large-scale functional brain networks, revealed by functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) (1214), have been implied in the underlying pathophysiology of RNT, and are largely consistent with the impaired disengagement theory (2, 3, 8). The frontoparietal network (FPN) due to its role in top-down cognitive control (15, 16), the salience network (SN) for its role in flexible attention shifting (14) and the default mode network (DMN) as the neural correlate of self-referential processing (17).

Interestingly, the resting-state literature has shown a key role of these three networks in depression and generalized anxiety disorder (GAD), in which rumination and worry are core symptoms (5, 14). The meta-analysis of Kaiser and colleagues (18) and various other studies have shown decreased connectivity within the FPN in depression (19, 20). While the meta-analysis of Kaiser and colleagues showed increased connectivity between DMN regions (18), the meta-analysis of Tozzi and colleagues revealed reduced connectivity within the midline core subsystem of the DMN (21). Reviews have suggested that depression is characterized by increased connectivity within the anterior DMN and by a dissociation between the anterior and posterior DMN (19, 20). Furthermore, there is evidence for decreased within DMN connectivity in GAD (22). Although there is more evidence for decreased connectivity within the SN in depression, there is also some conflicting evidence suggesting increased connectivity between SN regions in depression and GAD (19, 23). While these studies underscore the key role of these three networks in depression and GAD, there are also sometimes contradictory findings with respect to the exact role of these networks. This may be related to limitations in the performed studies, like small sample sizes (19), but also to the heterogeneous nature of these traditional psychiatric classifications (24). This emphasizes the importance of specifically studying the relationship between these networks and core symptom dimensions like RNT.

Importantly, the resting-state studies that specifically investigated RNT across the population, from health to psychiatric disorders, have also highlighted the importance of our networks of interest in this symptom dimension (3, 2527). In their landmark study, Hamilton and colleagues (25) showed that in depression RNT, in the form of rumination, was associated with a relative dominance of DMN over FPN activity during rest. They also showed the importance of the SN in dynamically switching between the DMN and FPN (25, 28). Since then, resting-state studies have shown a negative relationship between RNT and connectivity strength within the DMN in generalized anxiety disorder, depression and health (29, 30).

Although these networks also have a central role in the acute stress response (31), only few studies have investigated the relationship between RNT and stress-induced network changes. Rosenbaum and colleagues (7) have shown that in healthy subjects there was a higher increase in stress-induced FPN activity in low ruminators compared to high ruminators. Lydon-Staley and colleagues (2) showed that higher connectivity between the DMN and FPN during sad mood induction, in health and remitted depression, predicted increased RNT. In short, there is ample evidence for the importance of these networks in RNT, both at rest and in response to stress/mood induction, but the literature is largely restricted to specific populations (mainly health and stress-related disorders) (26, 27).

In the present study, we, therefore, set out to investigate RNT as a transdiagnostic factor in a population with both healthy subjects and patients with stress-related and/or neurodevelopmental disorders from a functional network perspective. For our primary question, we studied how the connectivity strength of our networks of interest, both at rest and with respect to stress-induced changes in connectivity strength, underlies (content-independent) RNT across the whole sample. Based on the literature described above, we hypothesized that RNT would be associated with decreased connectivity within all three our networks of interest. Secondarily, we investigated whether there were specific differences between subgroups (healthy subjects, stress-related disorders, neurodevelopmental disorders and a comorbidity group) with regard to the relationship between RNT and the connectivity strength.

Material and methods

Subjects

This study is part of the MIND-Set study (Measuring Integrated Novel Dimensions in Neurodevelopmental and Stress-related Mental Disorders) (32). The MIND-Set cohort includes adult patients with a neurodevelopmental (ASD and/or ADHD) and/or stress-related disorder (mood and/or anxiety disorder). A psychiatrically healthy control group is also included. Patients were diagnosed by a trained clinician according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM) (33) using semi-structured interviews (i.e., the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders (SCID-I) (34), the Diagnostic Interview for ADHD in Adults (35) and the Dutch Interview for ASD in Adults (36)). The MIND-Set study has been approved by the Ethical Review Board of the Radboudumc (Nijmegen, the Netherlands) and all participants signed informed consent before participation. In the present study, we included the subjects from our earlier study (37), for which the RNT score was available. We refer to this earlier study for an elaborate description of the study procedure.

While the primary analyses were performed in the combined subject group, for additional secondary analyses, related to potential differences across the diagnostic spectrum, this group can be considered as consisting of four subgroups:

1. Healthy control group: subjects without a present or past psychiatric disorder.

2. Stress-related group: patients with a current depressive episode, dysthymia, and/or anxiety disorder, but no ADHD or ASD.

3. Neurodevelopmental group: patients with ASD and/or ADHD, but no stress-related disorder.

4. Comorbidity group: patients with both a stress-related and neurodevelopmental disorder.

Procedure

To ensure a relaxed baseline state, the subjects had a 45-min pre-scanning acclimatization period. For the primary analysis, we investigated the brain's functional connectivity during a baseline resting-state scan. This is the first of three resting-state scans in our scan protocol (i.e., resting-state scan 1). Additionally, we studied the brain's response to a mild psychological stressor using an experimentally well-controlled paradigm in the form of an aversive movie clip (38). This stressor consisted of a movie clip (140 s) from the movie ‘Irréversible' (2002), by Gaspar Noé, and showed extreme male-to-male aggression. Participants were asked to constantly and attentively watch the movie clip after a short introductory text put them in the scene from an eye-witness perspective, in order to maximally involve them in the experience (38, 39). A neutral movie clip served as a control condition. We studied the brain's stress response by using the resting-state scans directly after these movie clips (Figure 1). The neutral movie clip and its subsequent resting-state scan (i.e., resting-state scan 2) always preceded the aversive movie clip and its ensuing resting-state scan (i.e., resting-state scan 3).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental design. The whole protocol consists of a series of scans of which we used the structural scan and the three resting-state scans (8:30 minutes each) for our present study. Resting-state scan 1 is used to study the brain at rest, and is referred to as the baseline resting-state scan. Stress induction took place with a highly aversive movie clip (2:20 minutes), with a self-referential eyewitness instruction. The neutral movie clip (2:20 minutes) served as a control condition. Stress-induced changes in connectivity strength were investigated using the resting-state scans directly after these movie clips (stress minus neutral: resting-state 3 minus resting-state 2). Subjective stress levels were assessed with an 11-point rating scale. Heart rate (in beats per minute) was measured continuously during scanning.

Questionnaires

Demographic information (age, sex, and level of education) was acquired using a questionnaire from the Dutch Helius study (40). The level of education was measured as an ordinal variable with four levels (41).

The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ) was used to measure trait RNT. The PTQ is a 15-item self-report questionnaire that assesses trait RNT from a transdiagnostic perspective (i.e., independent from disorder specific content), in both clinical and general populations (4). Items are rated on a 5-point scale, from 0: “never” to 4: “almost always.” We used the PTQ sum score for our primary analyses. Secondarily, we also used the following three subscales of this questionnaire: core characteristics, unproductiveness, and capturing mental capacity. These subscales have been identified in previous factor analyses (4) and capture different aspects of RNT. The core characteristics subscale measures the repetitiveness, intrusiveness, and difficulties with disengaging from these thoughts (e.g., “The same thoughts keep going through my mind again and again”). The unproductiveness subscale assesses to what extent someone perceives these thoughts as not being helpful for solving one's problems (e.g., “I keep asking myself questions without finding an answer”). Finally, the capturing mental capacity subscale measures to what extent the repetitive thoughts occupy someone's attention and if they prevent someone from focusing on other things than the problems (e.g., “My thoughts prevent me from focusing on other things”). High internal consistencies were found for as well the PTQ sum score (α = 0.94–0.95) as all three subscales (Core Characteristics of RNT: α = 0.92–0.94; Unproductiveness of RNT: α = 0.77–0.87; RNT Capturing Mental Capacity: α = 0.82–0.90) (4). Finally, results have also shown a satisfactory test-retest reliability for the PTQ sum score (rtt = 0.69; p < 0.001) and all three subscales (Core Characteristics of RNT (rtt = 0.66; p < 0.001), Unproductiveness of RNT (rtt = 0.68; p < 0.001), RNT Capturing Mental Capacity (rtt = 0.69; p < 0.001)) (4).

The Inventory of Depressive Symptomatology self-report (IDS-SR) questionnaire (42), consisting of 30 items, was used to assess the severity of depressive symptoms in the past 7 days.

fMRI acquisition and preprocessing

All images were collected using a 3T Siemens Magnetom Prisma MRI scanner (Erlangen, Germany). High-resolution structural images (1.0 mm isotropic) were acquired using a T1-weighted MP-RAGE sequence (TE/TR = 3.03/2,300 ms). T2*weighted EPI BOLD-fMRI images were acquired for the resting-state scans, using a multi-band 6 protocol (TR = 1,000 ms, voxel size = 2.0 mm isotropic). Preprocessing and statistical analyses were performed on the three resting-state scans (each 500 volumes) using FSL 5.0.11 (FMRIB, Oxford, UK). These scans were preprocessed using the FMRI Expert Analysis Tool (FEAT), which is part of the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) (43). To allow for T2* equilibration effects, the first five images of each resting-state scan were discarded. Furthermore, the preprocessing steps included brain extraction, motion correction, bias field correction, high-pass temporal filtering with a cut-off of 100 s, spatial smoothing with a 4 mm full width at half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel, registration of functional images to high-resolution T1 using boundary-based registration and non-linear registration to standard space (MNI152). The final voxel size for group analysis was 2 mm isotropic. ICA-based Advanced Removal of Motion Artifacts (ICA-AROMA) was used for further single-subject denoising (44). Subjects were excluded from analyses if motion resulted in more than 2 mm sudden relative mean displacement or translation [see also van Oort et al. (37)].

fMRI analyses

We identified our networks of interest (i.e., SN, DMN, right FPN, and left FPN) in the baseline resting-state scan (i.e., resting-state scan 1). Group independent component analysis (ICA) (12) was used to decompose the data of this baseline resting-state scan of all subjects together into 20 components. Spatial cross-correlation of the unthreshholded statistical maps of our group ICA with the unthresholded statistical maps of templates from the literature was used to select the respective group-specific network components [see Smith and colleagues (13) for the DMN and FPN, and Shirer and colleagues (45) for the anterior SN template]. We selected the ICA derived components that showed the highest spatial cross-correlation with the independent network templates from the literature (13, 45). Visual inspection by two authors (JvO and PvE), confirmed the selection of the best fitting networks and the inclusion of all areas that are typically considered as core regions of these networks (i.e., for the SN: dorsal anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula; for the DMN: the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (PFC), posterior cingulate cortex and precuneus, for both FPNs: dorsolateral PFC and posterior parietal cortex; see Figure 2 for the selected networks of interest). Next, we thresholded (Z ≥ 3) the statistical network masks of our networks of interest and extracted averaged network connectivity strength values for each network, from the individual spatial maps generated with dual regression (46). This approach results for each of our three resting-state scans in one value per subject and network, which represents an aggregate measure of mean within network connectivity strength, with higher values representing stronger within network connectivity [see van Oort et al. (37) for a detailed description of this procedure].

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Networks of interest. The networks of interest [i.e., the salience network (SN), default mode network (DMN) and frontoparietal network (FPN)] were identified with group independent component analysis (ICA) in resting-state scan 1. We identified two FPNs: the left FPN (light blue) and right FPN (dark blue). The selected networks included all areas that are typically considered as core regions in these networks. The SN included the bilateral anterior insula (AI) and dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC). The DMN included the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC) and precuneus (PCU). Additionally, the DMN included the hippocampus (HC), middle temporal gyrus (MTG) and lateral occipital cortex (lOcc). Both FPNs included the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) and posterior parietal cortex (PPC). Abbreviation: R: right. [Figure adapted from van Oort et al. (37)].

Our aim was to get a better understanding of RNT from a neuroscience perspective by investigating the relationship between the PTQ sum score and the connectivity strength of our networks of interest in the baseline resting-state scan (resting-state scan 1) in the combined subject group. We opted for a hierarchical regression model to model the effect of different variables (47). In line with earlier studies we selected the PTQ score as the dependent variable and the brain level (connectivity strength) as the predictor/explaining variable for this psychological construct (4850). We made four separate hierarchical regression models, one for each of our four networks. In model 1 of the hierarchical regression model we included the following covariates, as these factors may be potential confounders: age (5153), sex (52, 5456), level of education (57, 58), IDS-SR score (25) and subject subgroup (i.e., healthy, stress-related, neurodevelopmental, and comorbidity group) (25, 59). In model 2 we included the variables from model 1 and added the network connectivity strength. In model 3, we included all variables from model 2 and added interaction terms (subject subgroup x network connectivity strength) in order to investigate potential interaction effects (for our secondary analyses). Model 2 was tested against model 1 to see if the inclusion of the network connectivity strength significantly improved model fit over and above model 1, which may provide more insight into transdiagnostic mechanisms of RNT across the combined subject group. Next, model 3 was tested against model 2 to specifically test whether the inclusion of the interaction terms significantly improved the model, compared to only the addition of network connectivity strength. This analysis may provide further insight into potential subgroup specific patterns. For the interaction analysis (model 3 vs. model 2), we decided in advance that we would use a p-value < 0.10 as a threshold for performing the following additional analyses: (1) test for all possible interaction effects between the four subject subgroups and network connectivity strength and (2) perform the regression model for each of the four subgroups separately. Finally, if the inclusion of the network connectivity strength significantly improved model fit (model 2 vs. model 1) or if there was a significant interaction effect (model 3 vs. model 2), then we also performed post-hoc, hierarchical regression models, for each of the three PTQ subscales separately. These models followed the same hierarchical structure as the regression analyses described above, in which nested models were compared against each other.

For the analyses of the relationship between the PTQ sum score and the stress-induced changes in connectivity strength, we used the same design for the hierarchical regression models as we used for our baseline resting-state scan. The only difference was that for the connectivity strength, we now used the difference score between the stress and neutral condition (resting-state 3 minus resting-state 2; Figure 1).

Finally, we would like to note that while we performed separate regression models for our different networks of interest, we did not perform a multiple comparison correction. Therefore, when interpreting the results, it is important to keep in mind the more exploratory nature of these analyses.

Statistical analysis

We used the following two measures to assess stress levels during scanning: heart rate (beats per minute) and subjective stress (11-point rating scale: 0 = no stress, 10 = maximal stress). To assess the baseline stress level, we determined the heart rate during the baseline resting-state scan (i.e., resting-state scan 1) and subjective stress directly after this scan. To detect potential differences between the four subject subgroups, separate ANCOVA's (alpha = 0.05) were performed for both these stress measures (dependent variable), while controlling for age, sex, and level of education as covariates.

For these two stress measures, the effects of stress were assessed using a mixed model repeated measures ANCOVA (alpha = 0.05), again with age, sex, and level of education as covariates. Subjective stress was assessed directly after the aversive and neutral movie and the heart rate was measured during these two movies.

To investigate potential differences in PTQ and IDS-SR scores between the four subject subgroups an ANOVA (alpha = 0.05) was performed. Furthermore, correlations were performed between the PTQ and IDS-SR.

We performed ANOVAs to test for potential differences in the within network connectivity strength for our networks of interest (dependent variable: within network connectivity strength, fixed factor: subject subgroup). Finally, for each of the three resting state-scans, ANOVAs were performed to test for potential group differences regarding mean relative framewise displacement between successive images (dependent variable: mean relative framewise displacement, fixed factor: subject subgroup).

Results

Study population

In the present study we included 46 healthy subjects and 153 patients [stress-related group (n = 57), neurodevelopmental group (n = 46), comorbidity group (n = 50)]. While there was no significant difference between the subject subgroups with respect to sex (χ2 = 1.94, p = 0.586), there was a trend for a difference regarding age (F(3, 195) = 2.47, p = 0.063), and level of education (χ2 = 16.88, p = 0.051; Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographics and clinical characteristics.

As expected, the scores for the IDS-SR and PTQ were higher in all patient subgroups compared to the healthy group. Furthermore, there were several differences between the different patient subgroups, with in general higher scores on the IDS-SR and PTQ (subscales) for the stress-related and comorbidity group compared to the neurodevelopmental group. The stress-related group scored higher on the PTQ “unproductiveness” subscale than the neurodevelopmental group (p = 0.004; see also Table 1). Finally, the PTQ sum score correlated with the IDS-SR in the combined subject group (r = 0.696, p < 0.001; Supplemental Table A).

Behavioral and physiological results

Although there were no differences in heart rate between subject groups during resting-state scan 1 (F(3, 183) = 1.16, p = 0.327), the subjective stress level was higher directly after resting-state scan 1 in all three patient subgroups compared to the healthy subjects [stress-related > healthy (p < 0.001), neurodevelopmental > healthy (p = 0.002), comorbidity > healthy (p < 0.001)]. In addition, analyses showed a stress-induced increase in subjective stress (F(1, 190) = 13.71, p < 0.001) and heart rate (F(1, 180) = 10.91, p = 0.001) for the combined subject group. While the subjective stress levels were higher in all three patient subgroups compared to the healthy subjects [stress-related > healthy (p < 0.001), neurodevelopmental > healthy (p = 0.002), comorbidity > healthy (p < 0.001)] , there were no group differences in heart rate related to stress induction (F(3, 180) = 0.90, p = 0.441; Supplemental Table B).

Functional MRI results

Networks of interest and results related to movement

The four networks of interest with the highest cross-correlation were selected (for the SN: r = 0.76, DMN: r = 0.54, left FPN: r = 0.60 and right FPN: r = 0.57). The selected networks included all areas that are typically considered as core regions in these networks. There were no significant differences between the subject subgroups related to the within network connectivity strength for our networks of interest, nor at baseline (SN: F(3, 195) = 2.25, p = 0.084; DMN: F(3, 195) = 1.76, p = 0.156; left FPN: F(3, 195) = 1.31, p = 0.273; right FPN: F(3, 195) = 1.82, p = 0.145) nor related to stress induced changes in connectivity strength (SN: F(3, 195) = 0.82, p = 0.484; DMN: F(3, 195) = 1.47, p = 0.223; left FPN: F(3, 195) = 0.57, p = 0.636; right FPN: F(3, 195) = 0.57, p = 0.636). There were no significant differences between the subject subgroups with respect to the mean relative framewise displacement in none of the three resting-state scans (resting-state scan 1: F(3, 195) = 1.13, p = 0.338, resting-state scan 2: F(3, 195) = 1.13, p = 0.339, resting-state scan 3: F(3, 195) = 0.72, p = 0.541; Table 1).

Relationship between RNT and baseline connectivity

The hierarchical regression model in the combined subject group showed that the addition of the left FPN connectivity during resting-state scan 1 significantly improved the model fit (model 2 vs. model 1), and revealed a positive association between the left FPN connectivity strength and the PTQ sum score (β = 0.11, p = 0.042; Table 2, Figure 3A). This pattern did not differ between the different subject subgroups (p = 0.955) and post-hoc tests indicated that this effect was not driven by any specific PTQ subscale [“core characteristics” (β = 0.11, p = 0.050) “unproductiveness” (β = 0.11, p = 0.091), “capturing mental capacity” (β = 0.09, p = 0.120); Supplemental Table C]. RNT was not significantly associated with baseline connectivity of the SN (β = 0.00, p = 0.974) or DMN (β = −0.01, p = 0.913).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Regression models: relationship between repetitive negative thinking (RNT) and baseline network connectivity strength.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Relationship between repetitive negative thinking and network connectivity strength. These scatterplots display the relationship between repetitive negative thinking (RNT), measured with the PTQ sum score, and network connectivity strength. This relationship is displayed for: (A) The left frontoparietal network (FPN) in the combined subject group during the baseline resting-state scan. (B) The right FPN for the different subject subgroups during the baseline resting-state scan. (C) The stress induced changes in default mode network (DMN) connectivity in the combined subject group (stress minus neutral condition).

Although the regression model that included the right FPN connectivity during resting-state scan 1 was not significantly improved by the addition of the right FPN connectivity strength (β = 0.07, p = 0.165; model 2 vs. model 1), the addition of the interaction terms did improve the model (p = 0.043; model 3 vs. model 2). This interaction was mainly driven by a difference in association with the PTQ sum score between the stress-related and neurodevelopmental group (β = 0.59, p = 0.006; Table 2, Figure 3B). The construction of the separate regression models for the four subgroups showed a positive relationship between the right FPN connectivity and the PTQ sum score for the stress-related group (β = 0.28, p = 0.042), which was absent in the neurodevelopmental group (β = −0.16, p = 0.182). Post-hoc tests revealed that the interaction effect between the stress-related and neurodevelopmental group was mainly driven by the PTQ subscales “unproductiveness” (β = 0.642, p = 0.005) and “capturing mental capacity” (β = 0.67, p = 0.003; Supplemental Table D).

Relationship between RNT and stress-induced connectivity changes

The hierarchical regression model in the combined subject group showed that the addition of stress-induced changes in DMN connectivity significantly improved the model fit (model 2 vs. model 1). The regression model revealed a negative association between the PTQ sum score and changes in DMN connectivity strength (β = −0.10, p = 0.029; Table 3, Figure 3C), indicating that a higher PTQ score was associated with a decrease in DMN connectivity after stress induction. This pattern did not differ between subject subgroups and post-hoc tests indicated that the main effect was strongest for the subscales “unproductiveness” (β = −0.11, p = 0.044) and “capturing mental capacity” (β = −0.11, p = 0.027; Supplemental Table E). There were no significant associations of RNT with stress-induced changes of the SN (β = 0.07, p = 0.162) or FPN (left FPN: β = −0.01, p = 0.916; right FPN: β = 0.08, p = 0.110).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Regression models: relationship between repetitive negative thinking (RNT) and stress induced changes in network connectivity strength.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated the relationship between RNT and network connectivity strength both at rest and in the aftermath of stress induction from a transdiagnostic perspective, by examining subjects across the psychopathological continuum from health to diverse non-psychotic psychiatric disorders. Our results showed a positive association between the level of RNT and FPN connectivity strength at rest. While this positive association was found across our whole sample for the left FPN, for the right FPN this relationship was only found in the stress-related disorders group. In addition, RNT was negatively associated with stress-induced changes in DMN connectivity. Below, we will discuss the significance of these findings. First, we will discuss the (transdiagnostic) associations at rest, before turning to the response to our experimentally well-controlled stressor.

Recently, Rosenbaum and colleagues (60) have shown that increased connectivity within the FPN at rest was related to high levels of rumination in healthy subjects. Our study extends these findings by showing transdiagnostic and disorder specific associations between RNT and the whole FPN. For the left FPN we found a positive association with RNT along the continuum from health to psychiatric disorders. For the right FPN this positive relationship was only present in the stress-related group and differed significantly from the neurodevelopmental group, which may relate to specific psychopathological patterns in the stress-related group. Notably, this pattern of the right FPN was driven by the “unproductiveness” and “capturing mental capacity” characteristics of RNT. Moreover, the analyses showed a higher unproductiveness-score on the RNT questionnaire for the stress-related group, than for the neurodevelopmental group. Interestingly, the unproductiveness of repetitive thinking is positively associated with the severity of psychopathology over and above the pure frequency of RNT (61).

There are several possible explanations for the positive association between the FPN connectivity strength and RNT. First, the FPN has been implicated in emotion regulation and top-down control (15). Thus, subjects who have a stronger (bottom-up) drive for negative thinking (higher trait RNT), may upregulate the FPN in an attempt to achieve top-down control (3, 8). Second, according to the impaired disengagement theory, subjects prone to RNT have impairments in exercising (top-down) attentional control to disengage from negative thoughts (8). Therefore, they may compensate for these impairments with stronger FPN connectivity. Third, the FPN may be involved more intrinsically in the higher-order cognitive aspects of RNT itself, e.g., in a (unproductive) problem-solving attempt (16, 29, 62). There may also be a lateralization effect, with a differential involvement of the left and right FPN, as is suggested by previous research (63).

Interestingly, neurostimulation studies provide evidence for a lateralization effect regarding the left and right FPN in RNT. In health, experimental stimulation of the left FPN with transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) led to increased left prefrontal activity and a decrease in state RNT (64). Furthermore, the clinical non-invasive neurostimulation literature has mainly studied worry in generalized anxiety disorder (GAD) (65) and rumination in depression (6). Treatment studies in depression show that depression and rumination can be relieved by both low-frequency repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (rTMS) applied over the right dorsolateral PFC (FPN), which is known to inhibit this area, and high frequency rTMS applied over the left dorsolateral PFC, that activates this region (66). Interestingly, also in GAD the inhibition of core regions of the right FPN (65, 67), with low-frequency rTMS, led to a decrease in worry severity and increased remission rates. Together with our results, these findings may support a lateralization effect with adaptive and maladaptive involvement of the left and right FPN in RNT respectively.

Although earlier studies found a negative association between RNT and connectivity within the DMN at rest (29, 30), we did not find this relationship. This difference in findings, may be related to differences in analysis methods, since these earlier studies used seed-based methods (29, 30), while we investigated the connectivity strength within the DMN as a whole. Another important difference is that we studied content-independent RNT in a transdiagnostic sample, while these earlier studies investigated worry in GAD (30) and rumination in depression and health (29). Interestingly, while we did not find significant results for the DMN at rest, our experimental stressor did reveal a negative relationship between the DMN and RNT.

Our DMN result emphasizes that stress induction may unmask specific vulnerabilities to stress. The negative relationship between RNT and stress-induced changes in DMN connectivity is in line with a recent study (68), showing a negative association between RNT and within DMN connectivity in response to sad-mood induction in health and remitted depression. These results may be related to the fact that stronger DMN connectivity is associated with a more flexible pattern of mind wandering, while RNT is characterized by an inflexible and repetitive thinking style (29, 69).

Although previous studies have shown an important role for the SN in RNT (2, 25), we did not find any significant results for this network. The absence of significant results for the SN possibly results from the static measure of mean within network connectivity that we investigate. The role of the SN in RNT can possibly be better studied by investigating the between network connectivity with other networks (2) or the more dynamic and flexible nature in which it facilitates switching between the DMN and FPN (14, 25).

While in clinical practice patients are generally classified, using heterogeneous psychiatric classifications (70), here we want to illustrate how our understanding of psychopathology may be improved by integrating a dimensional approach to RNT, with the impaired disengagement theory and the stress-vulnerability model. From a dimensional perspective, common traits of neurodevelopmental disorders can be seen as a vulnerability factor for developing RNT under stress. For example, the degree of cognitive inhibition of a person can be seen as a continuous trait, with impaired cognitive inhibition (common in ADHD) at the extreme of this spectrum (11). When confronted with a stressor, impaired cognitive inhibition serves as a vulnerability factor for developing RNT, due to impaired disengagement from negative thoughts (8), which in turn may lead to more stress-related symptoms (5). Our understanding of psychopathology could be improved by future studies applying such a dimensional, transdiagnostic conceptualization of how symptoms result from the interaction between stressors and specific traits.

The main strength of our study is that this is the first study investigating the neurobiological mechanisms of RNT transdiagnostically both at rest and related to an experimentally well-controlled stress induction procedure. However, our study has to be interpreted in light of some limitations. First, although we hypothesize that there may be a lateralization effect regarding the adaptive and maladaptive roles of the FPNs in RNT, the cross-sectional nature of our study does not allow us to make causal inferences about this with certainty. Future prospective and MRI neurostimulation studies should further investigate this hypothesis. Second, the PTQ-score measures trait RNT and we did not measure state RNT during the MRI scan itself. However, a recent study found that state and trait rumination were only moderately correlated (29). As trait RNT has been identified as an important risk factor for various psychiatric disorders, we think it is important to study this construct in relation to the resting-state and stress.

Taken together, our results provide initial insight in the neural mechanisms of RNT across the psychopathological continuum. The positive association between left FPN connectivity and RNT at rest, across the entire sample, may reflect the need for upregulation of the FPN for top-down control of negative thoughts. The negative association between RNT and changes in DMN connectivity, which were revealed by stress, may reflect a less flexible and more repetitive thinking style under stress. Importantly, RNT is a promising treatment target (71), with non-invasive neurostimulation treatments directed at the FPN showing positive effects in psychiatric disorders in which RNT is a core feature (65, 72). A better understanding of the neural mechanisms of RNT may improve future treatments, by helping to develop neural circuit-guided personalized treatments (73).

Data availability statement

The preprocessed MRI data is available on request in line with the institutional ethics guidelines. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Jasper van Oort, amFzcGVyLnZhbm9vcnQmI3gwMDA0MDtyYWRib3VkdW1jLm5s.

Ethics statement

The MIND-Set study has been approved by the Ethical Review Board of the Radboudumc (Nijmegen, the Netherlands) and all participants signed informed consent forms before participation.

Author contributions

All authors listed have made a substantial, direct, and intellectual contribution to the work and approved it for publication.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.915316/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Ingram RE, Luxton DD. Dvelopment of psychopathology: a vulnerability-stress perspective. In: Hankin BL, Abela JRZ, editors. Vulnerability-stress models. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage (2005), p. 32–46. doi: 10.4135/9781452231655.n2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Lydon-Staley DM, Kuehner C, Zamoscik V, Huffziger S, Kirsch P, Bassett DS. Repetitive negative thinking in daily life and functional connectivity among default mode, fronto-parietal, and salience networks. Transl Psychiatry. (2019) 9:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41398-019-0560-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Burrows CA, Timpano KR, Uddin LQ. Putative brain networks underlying repetitive negative thinking and comorbid internalizing problems in autism. Clin Psychol Sci. (2017) 5:522–36. doi: 10.1177/2167702616683506

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Ehring T, Zetsche U, Weidacker K, Wahl K, Schönfeld S, Ehlers A. The Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ): validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2011) 42:225–32. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Ehring T, Watkins ER. Repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic process. Int J Cogn Ther. (2008) 1:192–205. doi: 10.1521/ijct.2008.1.3.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Woody ML, Gibb BE. Integrating NIMH Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) into depression research. Curr Opin Psychol. (2015) 4:6–12. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Rosenbaum D, Thomas M, Hilsendegen P, Metzger FG, Haeussinger FB, Nuerk H, et al. Stress-related dysfunction of the right inferior frontal cortex in high ruminators: an fNIRS study. NeuroImage Clin. (2018) 18:510–7. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2018.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Koster EHW, De Lissnyder E, Derakshan N, De Raedt R. Understanding depressive rumination from a cognitive science perspective: the impaired disengagement hypothesis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2011) 31:138–45. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2010.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kaplan DM, Palitsky R, Carey AL, Crane TE, Havens CM, Medrano MR, et al. Maladaptive repetitive thought as a transdiagnostic phenomenon and treatment target: an integrative review. J Clin Psychol. (2018) 74:1126–36. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Gotham K, Bishop SL, Brunwasser S, Lord C, States U, Plains W, et al. Rumination and perceived impairment associated with depressive symptoms in a verbal adolescent-adult ASD sample. Autism Res. (2014) 7:381–91. doi: 10.1002/aur.1377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Abramovitch A, Schweiger A. Unwanted intrusive and worrisome thoughts in adults with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2009) 168:230–3. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Beckmann CF, DeLuca M, Devlin JT, Smith SM. Investigations into resting-state connectivity using independent component analysis. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. (2005) 360:1001–13. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1634

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Smith SM, Fox PT, Miller KL, Glahn DC, Fox PM, Mackay CE, et al. Correspondence of the brain's functional architecture during activation and rest. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2009) 106:13040–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0905267106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Menon V. Large-scale brain networks and psychopathology: a unifying triple network model. Trends Cogn Sci. (2011) 15:483–506. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H, et al. Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cereb Cortex. (2014) 24:2981–90. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Corbetta M, Shulman GL. control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2002) 3:201–15. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Buckner RL, Andrews-Hanna JR, Schacter DL. The brain' s default network anatomy, function, and relevance to disease. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2008) 1124:1−38. doi: 10.1196/annals.1440.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kaiser RH., Andrews-hanna JR, Wager TD, Pizzagalli DA, Hospital M. Large-scale network dysfunction in major depressive disorder: meta-analysis of resting-state functional connectivity. JAMA Psychiatry. (2015) 72:603–11. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.0071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Mulders PC, van Eijndhoven PF, Schene AH, Beckmann CF, Tendolkar I. Resting-state functional connectivity in major depressive disorder: a review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 56:330–44. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Brakowski J, Spinelli S, Dörig N, Bosch OG, Manoliu A, Holtforth MG, et al. Resting state brain network function in major depression – depression symptomatology, antidepressant treatment effects, future research. J Psychiatr Res. (2017) 92:147–59. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Tozzi L, Zhang X, Chesnut M, Holt-Gosselin B, Ramirez CA, Williams LM. Reduced functional connectivity of default mode network subsystems in depression: meta-analytic evidence and relationship with trait rumination. NeuroImage Clin. (2021) 30:102570. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2021.102570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Yang F, Fan L, Zhai T, Lin Y, Wang Y, Ma J, et al. Decreased intrinsic functional connectivity in first-episode, drug-naive adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Front Hum Neurosci. (2019) 12:1–11. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Li W, Cui H, Zhu Z, Kong L, Guo Q, Zhu Y, et al. Aberrant functional connectivity between the amygdala and the temporal pole in drug-free generalized anxiety disorder. Front Hum Neurosci. (2016) 10:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kotov R, Krueger RF, Watson D, Bagby M, Carpenter WT, Caspi A. The hierarchical taxonomy of psychopathology (HiTOP): a dimensional alternative to traditional nosologies. J Abnorm Psychol. (2017) 126:1–48. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/zaadn

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hamilton JP, Furman DJ, Chang C, Thomason ME, Dennis E, Gotlib IH. Default-mode and task-positive network activity in major depressive disorder: implications for adaptive and maladaptive rumination. Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 70:327–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Makovac E, Fagioli S, Rae CL, Critchley HD, Ottaviani C. Can't get it off my brain: Meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on perseverative cognition. Psychiatry Res - Neuroimaging. (2020) 295:111020. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2019.111020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Zhou HX, Chen X, Shen YQ, Li L, Chen NX, Zhu ZC, et al. Rumination and the default mode network: meta-analysis of brain imaging studies and implications for depression. Neuroimage. (2020) 206:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sridharan D, Levitin DJ, Menon V. A critical role for the right fronto-insular cortex in switching between central-executive and default-mode networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2008) 105:12569–74. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0800005105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Rosenbaum D, Haipt A, Fuhr K, Haeussinger FB, Metzger FG, Nuerk HC, et al. Aberrant functional connectivity in depression as an index of state and trait rumination. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:1–12. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-02277-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Andreescu C, Sheu LK, Tudorascu D, Walker S, Aizenstein H. The ages of anxiety - differences across the lifespan in the default mode network functional connectivity in generalized anxiety disorder. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2014) 29:704–12. doi: 10.1002/gps.4051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. van Oort J, Tendolkar I, Hermans EJ, Mulders PC, Beckmann CF, Schene AH, et al. How the brain connects in response to acute stress: a review at the human brain systems level. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 83:281–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. van Eijndhoven PFP, Collard RM, Vrijsen JN, Geurts DGM, Arias-Vasquez A, Schellekens AFA, et al. Measuring integrated novel dimensions in neurodevelopmental and stress-related mental disorders (MIND-Set): a cross-sectional comorbidity study from an RDoC perspective. medRxiv. (2021) 1–39. doi: 10.1101/2021.06.05.21256695

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV-TR. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2000).

Google Scholar

34. First MB, Spitzer RL, Gibbon M, Williams JBW. Structured clinical interview for DSM-IV axis I disorders, clinician version (SCID-CV). Washington, DC: Press AP (1996). doi: 10.1037/t07827-000

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Ramos-Quiroga JA, Nasillo V, Richarte V, Corrales M, Palma F, Ibáñez P, et al. Criteria and concurrent validity of DIVA 2.0: a semi-structured diagnostic interview for adult ADHD. J Atten Disord. (2016) 23:1126–35. doi: 10.1177/1087054716646451

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Vuijk R. Diagnostic Interview for ASD in Adults (Dutch: Nederlands Interview ten behoeve van Diagnostiek Autismespectrumstoornis bij Volwassenen (NIDA)). Amsterdam (2014).

Google Scholar

37. van Oort J, Kohn N, Vrijsen JN, Collard R, Duyser FA, Brolsma SCAA, et al. Absence of default mode downregulation in response to a mild psychological stressor marks stress-vulnerability across diverse psychiatric disorders. NeuroImage Clin. (2020) 25:102176. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2020.102176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Qin S, Hermans EJ, van Marle HJF, Luo J, Fernández G. Acute psychological stress reduces working memory-related activity in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Biol Psychiatry. (2009) 66:25–32. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hermans EJ, Marle HJF., Van, Ossewaarde L, Henckens MJAG, Qin S, Van Kesteren MTR, et al. Stress-related noradrenergic activity prompts large-scale neural network reconfiguration. Science. (2011) 334:1151–3. doi: 10.1126/science.1209603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Stronks K, Snijder MB, Peters RJ, Prins M, Schene AH, Zwinderman AH. Unravelling the impact of ethnicity on health in Europe: the HELIUS study. BMC Public Health. (2013) 13:402. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Ikram UZ, Snijder MB, Fassaert TJL, Schene AH, Kunst AE. The contribution of perceived ethnic discrimination to the prevalence of depression. Eur J Public Health. (2014) 25:243–8. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/cku180

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Rush AJ, Gullion CM, Basco MR, Jarrett RB, Trivedi MH. The inventory of depressive symptomatology (IDS): psychometric properties. Psychol Med. (1996) 26:477–86. doi: 10.1017/S0033291700035558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Jenkinson M, Beckmann CF, Behrens TEJ, Woolrich MW, Smith SM. FSL. Neuroimage. (2012) 62:782–90. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Pruim RHR, Mennes M, Rooij D. Van, Llera A, Buitelaar JK, Beckmann CF. ICA-AROMA: a robust ICA-based strategy for removing motion artifacts from fMRI data. Neuroimage. (2015) 112:267–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Shirer WR, Ryali S, Rykhlevskaia E, Menon V, Greicius MD. Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cereb Cortex. (2012) 22:158–65. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Filippini N, Macintosh BJ, Hough MG, Goodwin GM, Frisoni GB, Smith SM, et al. Distinct patterns of brain activity in young carriers of the APOE-ε4 allele. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2009) 106:7209–14. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0811879106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Bowerman BL, O'Connell RT. Linear Statistical Models: An Applied Approach. 2nd ed. Belmont, CA: Duxbury (1990).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

48. Jiao B, Zhang D, Liang A, Liang B, Wang Z, Li J, et al. Association between resting-state brain network topological organization and creative ability: evidence from a multiple linear regression model. Biol Psychol. (2017) 129:165–77. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Munro CE, Donovan NJ, Guercio BJ, Wigman SE, Schultz AP, Amariglio RE, et al. Neuropsychiatric symptoms and functional connectivity in mild cognitive impairment. Handb Depress Alzheimer's Dis. (2015) 46:263–71. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Mamah D, Barch DM, Repovš G. Resting state functional connectivity of five neural networks in bipolar disorder and schizophrenia. J Affect Disord. (2013) 150:601–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.01.051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Gonçalves DC, Byrne GJ. Who worries most? Worry prevalence and patterns across the lifespan. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2013) 28:41–9. doi: 10.1002/gps.3788

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Ricarte Trives JJ, Navarro Bravo B, Latorre Postigo JM, Ros Segura L, Watkins E. Age and gender differences in emotion regulation strategies: autobiographical memory, rumination, problem solving and distraction. Span J Psychol. (2016) 19:1–9. doi: 10.1017/sjp.2016.46

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Everaerd D, Klumpers F, Voshaar RO, Fernández G, Tendolkar I. Acute stress enhances emotional face processing in the aging brain. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2017) 2:591–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Boehlen FH, Herzog W, Schellberg D, Maatouk I, Schoettker B, Brenner H, et al. Gender-specific predictors of generalized anxiety disorder symptoms in older adults: results of a large population-based study. J Affect Disord. (2020) 262:174–81. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.10.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Graham BM, Weiner S, Li SH. Gender differences in avoidance and repetitive negative thinking following symptom provocation in men and women with spider phobia. Br J Clin Psychol. (2020) 59:565–77. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12267

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Mak LE, Minuzzi L, MacQueen G, Hall G, Kennedy SH, Milev R. The default mode network in healthy individuals: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Brain Connect. (2017) 7:25–33. doi: 10.1089/brain.2016.0438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Ansseau M, Fischler B, Dierick M, Albert A, Leyman S, Mignon A. Socioeconomic correlates of generalized anxiety disorder and major depression in primary care: the GADIS II study (Generalized Anxiety and Depression Impact Survey II). Depress Anxiety. (2008) 25:506–13. doi: 10.1002/da.20306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Dubois J, Galdi P, Paul LK, Adolphs R. A distributed brain network predicts general intelligence from resting-state human neuroimaging data. Philos Trans R Soc B Biol Sci. (2018) 373:1–13. doi: 10.1101/257865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. McEvoy PM, Watson H, Watkins ER, Nathan P. The relationship between worry, rumination, and comorbidity: evidence for repetitive negative thinking as a transdiagnostic construct. J Affect Disord. (2013) 151:313–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2013.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Rosenbaum D, Hilsendegen P, Thomas M, Haeussinger FB, Nuerk HC, Fallgatter AJ, et al. Disrupted prefrontal functional connectivity during post-stress adaption in high ruminators. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:1–9. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-33777-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Michael T, Halligan SL, Clark DM, Ehlers A. Rumination in posttraumatic stress disorder. Depress Anxiety. (2007) 24:307–17. doi: 10.1002/da.20228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Ruocco AC, Rodrigo AH, Lam J, Di Domenico SI, Graves B, Ayaz H, et al. problem-solving task specialized for functional neuroimaging: validation of the Scarborough adaptation of the Tower of London (S-TOL) using near-infrared spectroscopy. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:1–13. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Bruder GE, Stewart JW, McGrath PJ. Right brain, left brain in depressive disorders: Clinical and theoretical implications of behavioral, electrophysiological and neuroimaging findings. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 78:178–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Baeken C, Remue J, Vanderhasselt MA, Brunoni AR, De Witte S, Duprat R, et al. Increased left prefrontal brain perfusion after MRI compatible tDCS attenuates momentary ruminative self-referential thoughts. Brain Stimul. (2017) 10:1088–95. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Diefenbach GJ, Bragdon LB, Zertuche L, Hyatt CJ, Hallion LS, Tolin DF, et al. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for generalised anxiety disorder: a pilot randomised, double-blind, sham-controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry. (2016) 209:222–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.115.168203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Lefaucheur JP, Aleman A, Baeken C, Benninger DH, Brunelin J, Di Lazzaro V, et al. Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): an update (2014–2018). Clin Neurophysiol. (2020) 131:474–528. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Huang Z, Li Y, Bianchi MT, Zhan S, Jiang F, Li N, et al. Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation of the right parietal cortex for comorbid generalized anxiety disorder and insomnia: a randomized, double-blind, sham-controlled pilot study. Brain Stimul. (2018) 11:1103–9. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2018.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Figueroa CA, Mocking RJT, Wingen G Van, Martens S, Ruhe HG, Schene AH. Aberrant default-mode network-hippocampus connectivity after sad memory-recall in remitted-depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2017) 12:1803–13. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsx108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Christoff K, Irving ZC, Fox KCR, Spreng RN, Andrews-Hanna JR. Mind-wandering as spontaneous thought: a dynamic framework. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2016) 17:718–31. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2016.113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Cuthbert BN. The RDoC framework: facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry. (2014) 13:28–35. doi: 10.1002/wps.20087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Spinhoven P, Klein N, Kennis M, Cramer AOJ, Siegle G, Cuijpers P, et al. The effects of cognitive-behavior therapy for depression on repetitive negative thinking: a meta-analysis. Behav Res Ther. (2018) 106:71–85. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2018.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Arns M, Bervoets C, van Eijndhoven P, Baeken C, van den Heuvel OA, Aleman A, et al. Consensusverklaring voor de toepassing van rTMS bij depressie in Nederland en België. Tijdschr Psychiatr. (2019) 61:411–20.

Google Scholar

73. Williams LM, Goldstein-Piekarski AN, Chowdhry N, Grisanzio KA, Haug NA, Samara Z, et al. Developing a clinical translational neuroscience taxonomy for anxiety and mood disorder: protocol for the baseline-follow up Research domain criteria Anxiety and Depression (“RAD”) project. BMC Psychiatry. (2016) 16:1–14. doi: 10.1186/s12888-016-0771-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: fMRI, network, connectivity, RNT, rumination, worry

Citation: van Oort J, Tendolkar I, Collard R, Geurts DEM, Vrijsen JN, Duyser FA, Kohn N, Fernández G, Schene AH and van Eijndhoven PFP (2022) Neural correlates of repetitive negative thinking: Dimensional evidence across the psychopathological continuum. Front. Psychiatry 13:915316. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.915316

Received: 07 April 2022; Accepted: 27 June 2022;
Published: 22 July 2022.

Edited by:

Henry W. Chase, University of Pittsburgh, United States

Reviewed by:

Rachael G. Grazioplene, Yale University, United States
Lucy Denise Vanes, King's College London, United Kingdom
Sangcheon Choi, Massachusetts General Hospital and Harvard Medical School, United States

Copyright © 2022 van Oort, Tendolkar, Collard, Geurts, Vrijsen, Duyser, Kohn, Fernández, Schene and van Eijndhoven. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jasper van Oort, amFzcGVyLnZhbm9vcnQmI3gwMDA0MDtyYWRib3VkdW1jLm5s

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.