Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 22 June 2022
Sec. Computational Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Machine Learning and Psychosis: Diagnosis, Prognosis and Treatment View all 5 articles

Accelerated Global and Local Brain Aging Differentiate Cognitively Impaired From Cognitively Spared Patients With Schizophrenia

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York City, NY, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Djavad Mowafaghian Centre for Brain Health, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada
  • 3Division of Psychiatry, University of Edinburgh, Edinburgh, United Kingdom

Background: Accelerated aging has been proposed as a mechanism underlying the clinical and cognitive presentation of schizophrenia. The current study extends the field by examining both global and regional patterns of brain aging in schizophrenia, as inferred from brain structural data, and their association with cognitive and psychotic symptoms.

Methods: Global and local brain-age-gap-estimates (G-brainAGE and L-brainAGE) were computed using a U-Net Model from T1-weighted structural neuroimaging data from 84 patients (aged 16–35 years) with early-stage schizophrenia (illness duration <5 years) and 1,169 healthy individuals (aged 16–37 years). Multidomain cognitive data from the patient sample were submitted to Heterogeneity through Discriminative Analysis (HYDRA) to identify cognitive clusters.

Results: HYDRA classified patients into a cognitively impaired cluster (n = 69) and a cognitively spared cluster (n = 15). Compared to healthy individuals, G-brainAGE was significantly higher in the cognitively impaired cluster (+11.08 years) who also showed widespread elevation in L-brainAGE, with the highest deviance observed in frontal and temporal regions. The cognitively spared cluster showed a moderate increase in G-brainAGE (+8.94 years), and higher L-brainAGE localized in the anterior cingulate cortex. Psychotic symptom severity in both clusters showed a positive but non-significant association with G-brainAGE.

Discussion: Accelerated aging in schizophrenia can be detected at the early disease stages and appears more closely associated with cognitive dysfunction rather than clinical symptoms. Future studies replicating our findings in multi-site cohorts with larger numbers of participants are warranted.

Introduction

Schizophrenia is a severe mental illness that presents with positive and negative symptoms and cognitive dysfunction (1). Cognitive abnormalities have been reported in multiple domains including executive function, processing speed, memory and attention (2). Additionally, schizophrenia is associated with structural abnormalities in brain regions that support these cognitive functions. These consist of reductions in the anterior cingulate, prefrontal and temporal cortical regions and in the volume of the hippocampus, amygdala, thalamus, and insula (36). Longitudinal studies suggest more rapid progressive decreases in brain tissue in patients with schizophrenia than in healthy individuals, primarily in frontal and temporal areas (7, 8) raising the possibility that processes related to schizophrenia may contribute to the premature development of age-related brain changes (i.e., accelerated aging). The notion of aberrant aging in schizophrenia is also supported by evidence of greater age-related cognitive decline (9), higher incidence of dementia (10), and biological markers of aging as indicated by shortened telomere length and accelerated epigenetic clocks (1113).

The investigation of accelerated aging in schizophrenia benefits from developments in machine learning methods that enable the prediction of chronological age from neuroimaging data. The discrepancy between the neuroimaging-predicted age and the chronological age, termed the “brain-age-gap estimate” (brainAGE), is an individualized estimate of the degree of deviation from typical age-related brain changes (14, 15). A positive brainAGE indicates that the biological age of an individual's brain is “older” than their actual age, and a negative brainAGE reflects the inverse. In older populations, higher brainAGE has been associated with decline in executive function, memory, and information processing speed that are also directly relevant to schizophrenia (16).

Prior studies in schizophrenia have mainly used brain structural magnetic resonance imaging (sMRI) to compute brainAGE as a global measure that captures the totality of regional age-related changes (henceforth referred to as G-brainAGE). Multiple studies have reported elevated G-brainAGE in patients with schizophrenia (1722); the largest such study reported an increase in G-brainAGE of about 4 years (Cohen's d effect size = 0.50) based on sMRI data from 2,598 healthy individuals and 2,803 patients with schizophrenia, aged 17–73 years (23). While these studies provide strong evidence for accelerated brain aging in schizophrenia, no study to date has examined the spatial variation of age-related changes which could identify brain regions that might be highly vulnerable. Additionally, the link between accelerated brain-aging and cognition remains largely unexplored despite its potential to provide mechanistic insights of therapeutic utility.

To address this gap, we applied a novel machine learning algorithm with a U-Net architecture (24) to sMRI data to compute G-brainAGE and voxel-level local brain-age (henceforth L-brainAGE) in patients with early-stage schizophrenia and healthy individuals. We used Heterogeneity through Discriminative Analysis (HYDRA) (25) to stratify patients based on their cognitive characteristics. Our working hypothesis was that accelerated aging, as inferred by G-brainAGE and L-brainAGE, will be greater in clusters of patients showing cognitive impairment and the highest deviance in terms of L-brainAGE will be observed in prefrontal and temporal regions where longitudinal changes have most consistently been reported in schizophrenia.

Materials and Methods

Samples

The data used in the current study were obtained from the 1.1 release of the Human Connectome Project Early Psychosis Study (HCP-EP; https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/human-connectome-project-for-early-psychosis/article/updated-hcp-early-psychosis-11-release) and the 1,200 subjects release of the Human Connectome Project Young Adults Study (HCP-YA; https://www.humanconnectome.org/study/hcp-young-adult) (details in Supplementary Material 1.1). These studies used harmonized protocols for sMRI data acquisition and cognitive assessment allowing data pooling. In the HCP-EP, the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5, Research Version (SCID-5-RV) (26) was used to establish the diagnostic status of patients and confirm illness onset within the past 5 years. The diagnostic status of healthy individuals was established using the SCID-5-RV in the HCP-EP and specific eligibility criteria in HCP-YA (details in Supplementary Material 1.1).

The study sample comprised 84 patients with schizophrenia (age range: 16–35 years; 30.20% female) and 1,169 healthy individuals (age range: 16–37 years; 53.3% female) (Supplementary Table 1). Data collection at the original recruitment sites complied with the ethical standards of national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. The data used for the current analyses were de-identified and accessed through the National Data Archive.

Procedures

Cognitive and Clinical Assessment

Cognitive functioning in HCP-EP and HCP-YA participants was assessed using the Penn Emotion Recognition Task (27) and the NIH Cognition Toolbox (28) which includes the following sub-tests: dimensional change card sort test, flanker inhibitory control and attention test, oral reading recognition test, picture vocabulary test, pattern comparison processing speed test, list sorting working memory test, and picture sequence memory test (details in Supplementary Table 2). In patients, the intelligence quotient (IQ) was additionally evaluated using the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence, Second Edition (WASI-II) (29), while clinical symptoms and social function were, respectively, assessed with the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) (30) and the Mental Illness Research, Education, and Clinical Center version of the Global Assessment of Functioning scale (MIRECC-GAF) (31). Lifetime antipsychotic medication dosage was recorded as Chlorpromazine (CPZ) equivalents using the Gardner approach (32) and as months of cumulative exposure.

HYDRA Cognitive Clusters

We used HYDRA to identify subgroups of patients based on the cognitive test performance measures from the NIH Cognition Toolbox and Penn Emotion Recognition Task (Supplementary Table 2) with and sex and age were modeled as covariates. The WASI-II IQ was not used in clustering but was used for external cluster validation. HYDRA is a semi-supervised machine learning tool that clusters cases based on their differences from a healthy reference sample by finding multiple linear hyperplanes, which together form a convex polytope, thus extending linear max-margin classifiers to the non-linear space (https://github.com/evarol/HYDRA). We used 5-fold cross validation to determine the clustering stability for a range of 2–5 clusters based on the adjusted Rand index (ARI) (33); the solution with the highest ARI was chosen. Statistical significance was established with permutation testing compared to a null distribution (details in Supplementary Material 1.2). In order to depict the cognitive profiles of the clusters identified using HYDRA, we standardized performance on each cognitive test by z-score transforming scores based on normative means for the NIH Toolbox, and across all individuals included in the study for the Penn Emotion Recognition Task subtests. To test algorithm-independent reproducibility, the cognitive data were submitted to k-means clustering implemented in R using version 3.0 of the NbClust package (Supplementary Material 1.2).

Neuroimaging Acquisition and Preprocessing

Whole-brain T1-weighted data in the HCP-EP and HCP-YA samples were acquired on Siemens 3T scanners using similar 3D magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MPRAGE) sequences (details in Supplementary Material 1.3). All images were processed locally with the Statistical Parametric Mapping Toolbox (SPM12; https://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm/software/spm12/) using established procedures (details in Supplementary Material 1.3). Briefly, in each participant, T1-weighted images were normalized using affine followed by non-linear registration, corrected for bias field inhomogeneities, and segmented into gray and white matter, and cerebrospinal fluid. The Diffeomorphic Anatomic Registration Through Exponentiated Lie algebra algorithm (DARTEL) (34) was applied to normalize the segmented scans into a standard MNI space (MNI-152 space).

Computation of L-brainAGE and G-brainAGE

The gray and white matter outputs generated after preprocessing served as input to a convolutional neural network (U-Net) (https://github.com/SebastianPopescu/U-NET-for-LocalBrainAge-prediction) to yield voxel-wise estimates of L-brainAGE using parameters pre-trained in an independent sample of 4,155 healthy individuals aged 18–90 years (details in Supplementary Material 1.4). Importantly, the HCP-EP and HCP-YA datasets were not used in the development of the L-brainAGE model. The resulting L-brainAGE images provide information about voxel-level differences between neuroimaging-predicted age and chronological age for each individual. Subsequently, the G-brainAGE was computed in each participant by averaging voxel-level L-brainAGE information across the brain (details in Supplementary Material 1.4). To account for residual associations with age, G-brainAGE was corrected in all study participants by regressing out chronological age using established methods (35); unless otherwise specified, the corrected G-brainAGE values were used in all subsequent analyses while analyses involving L-brainAGE included age as a covariate. For both L-brainAGE and G-brainAGE, positive values indicate an older appearing brain than what would be predicted by chronological age. Model accuracy was ascertained using the voxel-level Mean Absolute Error (MAE), which quantifies the absolute difference between the neuroimaging-predicted age and the chronological age, and by the correlation between predicted age (averaged across voxels) and chronological age (unadjusted for chronological age).

Statistical Approach

Statistical significance across all comparisons was set at PFWE < 0.05 with family-wise-error correction. Case-control differences in L-brainAGE were identified using General Linear Models in SPM12 with age, sex, and site modeled as covariates. Case-control differences in G-brainAGE were assessed using analysis of covariance with group as a fixed factor, and sex and site included as covariates. Associations between clinical symptoms, social functioning, and WASI-II IQ with G-brainAGE and L-brainAGE were, respectively, assessed using Spearman's correlation and general linear models in SPM12. Follow-up sensitivity analyses in patients were conducted to test for the effects of medication dose.

Results

Cognitive Clusters in Patients

HYDRA identified two clusters of patients based on their cognitive profiles (Table 1; Figure 1). K-means clustering of the cognitive test performance measures yielded the same results (Supplementary Material 2.1; Supplementary Figure 1). The two HYDRA clusters did not differ in terms of age, sex, years of education, PANSS positive and negative symptoms, and MIRECC-GAF scores for occupational and social functioning, but patients in cluster 1 had received higher doses of antipsychotic medication (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of the cognitive subgroups of schizophrenia patients.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Cognitive profiles of the patients with schizophrenia. Average standardized cognitive performance profiles in the impaired and spared cognitive subgroups identified using HYDRA clustering. Error bars represent standard error. For RT Correct Responses ER40, the directionality of values was reversed so that higher values denote better performance. Executive Function/Inhibition: NIH Toolbox Flanker Inhibitory Control and Attention Test; Executive Function (EF)/Cognitive Flexibility: NIH Toolbox Dimensional Change Card Sort Test; Processing Speed: NIH Toolbox Pattern Comparison Processing Speed Test; Working Memory: NIH Toolbox List Sorting Working Memory Test; Episodic Memory: NIH Toolbox Picture Sequence Memory Test; Fluid IQ: NIH Toolbox Cognition Fluid Composite; Language/Reading: NIH Toolbox Oral Reading Recognition Test; Language/Vocabulary: NIH Toolbox Picture Vocabulary Test; Crystalized IQ: NIH Toolbox Cognition Crystallized Composite; Full IQ: NIH Toolbox Cognition Total Composite Score; Penn Emotion Recognition Test: Number of Correct Anger Identifications: Anger ER40; Penn Emotion Recognition Test: Number of Correct Fear Identifications: Fear ER40; Penn Emotion Recognition Test: Number of Correct Happy Identifications: Happy ER40; Penn Emotion Recognition Test: Number of Correct Neutral Identifications: Neutral ER40; Penn Emotion Recognition Test: Number of Correct Sad Identifications: Sad ER40; Penn Emotion Recognition Test Number of Correct Responses: Correct ER40: Penn Emotion Recognition Test: Correct Responses Median Response Time: RT Correct Responses ER40.

Cluster 1 (n = 69) was considered “impaired” because the performance of patients assigned to this cluster was significantly lower compared to cluster 2 in all cognitive subtests of the NIH Toolbox (P < 0.05; Supplementary Table 3), with the exception of processing speed. Patients in cluster 1, also performed significantly worse in total correct responses (T = −2.56; P = 0.01) and identification of fearful (T = −2.00; P = 0.05) and sad faces (T = −2.36; P = 0.02) in the Penn Emotion Recognition Task. After z-score transforming cognitive performance in each of the cognitive subtests, patients in cluster 1 showed performance was decreased by 0.15–2.01 standard deviations (SD) compared to normative means (Figure 1). The greatest deviations in cognitive performance for individual subtests in cluster 1 were seen in executive function, both in inhibition (SD = −2.01) and cognitive flexibility (SD = −1.59). In contrast, cluster 2 (n = 15) was considered “spared” because the performance of patients assigned to this cluster in each of the cognitive subtests was generally within 0.5 SD of the normative means (Figure 1), with the exception of cognitive flexibility (SD = 0.64), reading decoding (SD = 1.31), vocabulary comprehension (SD = 0.66), and identification of happy faces (SD = −0.63). Confirming the validity of the clustering, the WASI-II IQ, which was not included as part of the clustering, was significantly lower in the impaired compared to the spared cluster (Cohen's d = −0.80, PFWE < 0.05; Table 1).

G-brainAGE in the Cognitively Impaired and Spared Clusters

The G-brainAGE model of healthy individuals had a MAE of 5.69 ± 3.57 (further details in Supplementary Material 2.2). The correlation between the chronological and predicted brain ages was r = 0.38 for healthy individuals. The impaired and spared clusters had numerically higher G-brainAGE [mean (SD): 11.08 (7.16) years and mean (SD): 8.94 (4.69) years respectively] compared to healthy individuals [mean (SD): 4.53 (4.73) years]. The age-corrected G-brainAGE was significantly higher in the cognitively impaired patient subgroup compared to healthy individuals (impaired: F = 29.11, p = 8.19E−8; Cohen's d = 0.58). The age-corrected G-brainAGE was comparable between healthy individuals and the cognitively spared patient subgroup (F = 2.39, P = 0.12; Cohen's d = 0.07). We did not observe any significant correlations between G-brainAGE with the PANSS positive or negative symptoms subscale scores, antipsychotic medication, MIRECC-GAF scores and WASI-II IQ within either cognitive cluster (Supplementary Table 4).

L-brainAGE in the Cognitively Impaired and Spared Cluster

The L-brainAGE model yielded MAE ranging from 3.36 to 16.67 across voxels (further details in Supplementary Material 2.2; Supplementary Figures 2, 3). A general linear model with age, sex and site as covariates showed widespread increases in L-brainAGE in the impaired cluster compared to healthy individuals with the highest values observed in the middle and inferior temporal lobe, superior and middle frontal lobe, precuneus, and postcentral gyrus (PFWE < 0.05; Figure 2). By contrast, comparison of the spared cluster to healthy individuals revealed a localized increase in L-brainAGE in the anterior cingulate cortex that was only observed at an uncorrected level of P < 0.005 (Figure 3). There was no significant association with L-brainAGE and current antipsychotic medication dose in CPZE, even at an uncorrected P < 0.05.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Differences in L-brainAGE between healthy individuals and the cognitively impaired patient subgroup. T-value overlay of statistically significant group differences in L-brainAGE based on a two-sample t-test comparing healthy individuals with the cognitive impaired patients (PFWE < 0.05). The color bar shows regions in which patients with impaired cognition have higher brainAGE compared to healthy individuals. Images are displayed in neurological orientation with MNI coordinates.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Differences in L-brainAGE between healthy individuals and the cognitively spared patient subgroup. T-value overlay of statistically significant group differences in L-brainAGE based on a two-sample t-test comparing healthy individuals with the cognitive spared patients (Puncorrected < 0.005). The color bar shows regions in which patients with spared cognition have higher brainAGE compared to healthy individuals. Images are displayed in neurological orientation with MNI coordinates.

Discussion

This is the first study to examine the regional pattern of accelerated brain aging in relation to profiles of cognitive impairment in patients with schizophrenia at the early stages of the disorder. The results show that patients with psychotic symptoms and cognitive underperformance evidence global and widespread local accelerated brain aging, while patients with psychotic symptoms and preserved cognitive functioning showed no differences in global accelerated aging and only minimal age-related acceleration in the anterior cingulate at the local level.

Application of machine learning to cognitive task performance data separated the current patient sample into a cognitively impaired (80%) and a cognitively spared (20%) cluster. Studies in patients with schizophrenia have consistently identified a cognitively spared cluster while cognitively impaired patients have been found to form up to 3 sub-clusters based on the degree of impairment (3639). The current study is mostly closely aligned with that of Wenzel and colleagues (40), who also examined patients with schizophrenia within 2 years of the onset of psychosis. Patient stratification was based on their performance in tasks of attention, processing speed, executive function and memory and yielded an impaired cluster characterized by underperformance across all tasks and a spared cluster characterized by preserved and even enhanced performance in executive function and crystallized intelligence. The impaired group in the current study and that of the Wenzel study showed significant underperformance compared to healthy individuals. The decrements in the impaired group were most pronounced in executive functioning. In terms of the spared cluster, both samples also identified individuals that seem to be performing within the normal range of cognition, and overperformance in cognitive flexibility, which falls within the domain of executive functioning. The current study further expands this field of research both methodologically and conceptually by testing the hypothesis that mechanisms related to accelerated aging, both at the global and local level may underpin the cognitive stratification of patients.

All prior studies in schizophrenia focused on sMRI-derived G-brainAGE (1723), which has been found to be increased both at the early (18, 21) and more chronic disease stages (17, 19, 20, 22, 23). This study extends the investigation of G-brainAGE by providing evidence that increases in this metric may be confined to those patients who also manifest cognitive impairment. This proposition was also supported by the findings of L-brainAGE that showed a pattern of widespread apparent brain aging that was most pronounced in frontal and temporal regions. Notably, higher L-brainAGE was noted in brain regions where accelerated age-related decreases have been found in longitudinal studies of patients with schizophrenia (7, 8).

Patients in the spared cluster showed accelerated aging confined to the anterior cingulate cortex, which is crucial for integrating cognitive and emotional processes in support of goal directed behavior (4144), and is often implicated in very early stages of psychosis (4547). The degree of age-acceleration in the anterior cingulate cortex was small and it would appear that any dysfunction in this region in the spared cluster may not be of sufficient magnitude to lead to measurable cognitive difficulties.

The two clusters did not differ significantly in symptom severity or general functioning. Additionally, there was no association between brain-aging metrics and either of these domains suggesting some degree of specificity of the G-brainAGE and L-brainAGE findings to cognition. We also did not find evidence of an association between brain aging metrics and antipsychotic medication although patients in the impaired cluster had been exposed to higher doses of antipsychotic medication, as medication exposure is often driven by clinical severity, it is not possible to comment whether medication may have had an independent effect on our findings.

The current study has several limitations. Firstly, its cross-sectional nature does not allow inferences about the temporal association between brain aging and cognition. Further longitudinal studies are therefore important particularly in light of the report by Schnack et al. (19) that G-brainAGE in patients with schizophrenia increases further particularly during the first 2 years after disease onset. Second, the study does not address the evolution of cognitive function in schizophrenia and its possible impact on the long-term stability of the clusters identified here (2, 9). Third, the sample size of this study is modest. Larger sample sizes are needed to address generalizability and more robust identification of cognitive clusters and their associations with G-brainAGE and L-brainAGE, as well as with a wider range of clinical and behavioral features. Fourth, the current study does not inform on molecular changes that may underlie brain aging in schizophrenia. The available literature on brain aging suggests a potential role mainly for metabolic dysfunction (48) and low-level neuroinflammation (49) that should be assessed in future studies.

Conclusion

In this study, we found differential patterns of accelerated brain aging in patients with schizophrenia based on their cognitive performance, with greater global accelerated brain aging, and more widespread local accelerated brain aging in patients with impaired than those with spared cognition. Further studies in longitudinal cohorts are needed to better understand progressive changes in accelerated aging and their association with the evolution of cognitive dysfunction and with molecular pathways involved in aging.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: Human Connectome Project Young Adults (http://db.humanconnectome.org) and Human Connectome Project for Early Psychosis (https://nda.nih.gov/general-query.html?q=query=featureddatasets:Connectomes%20Related%20to%20Human%20Disease).

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Human Connectome Project. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SH, RK, AR, HW, and SF contributed to conception and design of the study. SH organized the database and wrote the first draft of the manuscript. SH, RG, AM, and NS performed the statistical analysis. NS, SF, and RG wrote sections of the manuscript. All authors contributed to manuscript revision, read, and approved the submitted version.

Funding

SF received support from the National Institute of Mental Health under grant R01MH113619 and the National Institute on Aging under grant R01AG058854.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported in part through the computational resources and staff expertise provided by Scientific Computing at the Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai. Data collection and sharing for the Human Connectome Project Young Adults (HCP-YA) was provided by the MGH-USC Human Connectome Project Young Adults (NIH 1U54MH091657, Principal Investigators: Bruce Rosen, Arthur W Toga, Van J Weeden; http://www.humanconnectome.org/). HCP data are disseminated by the Laboratory of Neuro Imaging at the University of California, Los Angeles. Data collection and sharing for the Human Connectome Project for Early Psychosis was disseminated by the NIMH Data Archives under the Human Connectome Projects – Connectomes Related to Human Disease (NIMH R01MH117012 and U01MH109977; Principal Investigators: Alan Breier and Martha Shenton, https://nda.nih.gov/general-query.html?q=query=featureddatasets:Connectomes%20Related%20to%20Human%20Disease).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2022.913470/full#supplementary-material

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed, DSM-5. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Fett AJ, Reichenberg A, Velthorst E. Lifespan evolution of neurocognitive impairment in schizophrenia - a narrative review. Schizophr Res Cogn. (2022) 28:100237. doi: 10.1016/j.scog.2022.100237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Haijma SV, Van Haren N, Cahn W, Koolschijn PCM, Hulshoff Pol HE, Kahn RS. Brain volumes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis in over 18,000 subjects. Schizophr Bull. (2013) 39:1129–38. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. van Erp TG, Hibar DP, Rasmussen JM, Glahn DC, Pearlson GD, Andreassen OA, et al. Subcortical brain volume abnormalities in 2028 individuals with schizophrenia and 2540 healthy controls via the enigma consortium. Mol Psychiatry. (2016) 21:547–53. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.63

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. van Erp TG, Walton E, Hibar DP, Schmaal L, Jiang W, Glahn DC, et al. Cortical brain abnormalities in 4474 individuals with schizophrenia and 5098 control subjects via the enhancing neuro imaging genetics through meta analysis (ENIGMA) consortium. Biol Psychiatry. (2018) 84:644–54. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2018.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lewandowski KE, Bouix S, Ongur D, Shenton ME. Neuroprogression across the Early Course of Psychosis. J Psychiatr Brain Sci. (2020) 5:e200002. doi: 10.20900/jpbs.20200002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Hulshoff Pol HE, Kahn RS. What happens after the first episode? A review of progressive brain changes in chronically ill patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2008) 34:354–66. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Olabi B, Ellison-Wright I, McIntosh AM, Wood SJ, Bullmore E, Lawrie SM. Are there progressive brain changes in schizophrenia? A meta-analysis of structural magnetic resonance imaging studies. Biol Psychiatry. (2011) 70:88–96. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.01.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Fett A-KJ, Velthorst E, Reichenberg A, Ruggero CJ, Callahan JL, Fochtmann LJ, et al. Long-term changes in cognitive functioning in individuals with psychotic disorders: findings from the suffolk county mental health project. JAMA Psychiatry. (2020) 77:387–96. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2019.3993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Jonas K, Abi-Dargham A, Kotov R. Two hypotheses on the high incidence of dementia in psychotic disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. (2021) 78:1305–6. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2021.2584

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Czepielewski LS, Massuda R, Panizzutti B, da Rosa ED, de Lucena D, Macêdo D, et al. Telomere length in subjects with schizophrenia, their unaffected siblings and healthy controls: evidence of accelerated aging. Schizophr Res. (2016) 174:39–42. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2016.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Higgins-Chen AT Boks MP Vinkers CH Kahn RS Levine ME Schizophrenia and epigenetic aging biomarkers: increased mortality reduced cancer risk and unique clozapine effects. Biol Psychiatry. (2020) 88:224–35. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2020.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Nguyen TT, Eyler LT, Jeste DV. Systemic biomarkers of accelerated aging in schizophrenia: a critical review and future directions. Schizophr Bull. (2018) 44:398–408. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Franke K, Ziegler G, Klöppel S, Gaser C, Initiative AsDN. Estimating the age of healthy subjects from T1-weighted MRI scans using kernel methods: exploring the influence of various parameters. Neuroimage. (2010) 50:883–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Franke K, Gaser C. Ten years of brainage as a neuroimaging biomarker of brain aging: what insights have we gained? Front Neurol. (2019) 10:789. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00789

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Liem F, Varoquaux G, Kynast J, Beyer F, Masouleh SK, Huntenburg JM, et al. Predicting brain-age from multimodal imaging data captures cognitive impairment. Neuroimage. (2017) 148:179–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Kaufmann T, van der Meer D, Doan NT, Schwarz E, Lund MJ, Agartz I, et al. Common brain disorders are associated with heritable patterns of apparent aging of the brain. Nat Neurosci. (2019) 22:1617–23. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0471-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Koutsouleris N, Davatzikos C, Borgwardt S, Gaser C, Bottlender R, Frodl T, et al. Accelerated brain aging in schizophrenia and beyond: a neuroanatomical marker of psychiatric disorders. Schizophr Bull. (2014) 40:1140–53. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbt142

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Schnack HG, Van Haren NE, Nieuwenhuis M, Hulshoff Pol HE, Cahn W, Kahn RS. Accelerated brain aging in schizophrenia: a longitudinal pattern recognition study. Am J Psychiatry. (2016) 173:607–16. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2015.15070922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Nenadić I, Dietzek M, Langbein K, Sauer H, Gaser C. Brainage score indicates accelerated brain aging in schizophrenia, but not bipolar disorder. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. (2017) 266:86–9. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.05.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hajek T, Franke K, Kolenic M, Capkova J, Matejka M, Propper L, et al. Brain age in early stages of bipolar disorders or schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2019) 45:190–8. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx172

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Shahab S, Mulsant BH, Levesque ML, Calarco N, Nazeri A, Wheeler AL, et al. Brain structure, cognition, and brain age in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and healthy controls. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2019) 44:898–906. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0298-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Constantinides C, Han LK, Alloza C, Antonucci L, Arango C, Ayesa-Arriola R, et al. Brain ageing in schizophrenia: evidence from 26 international cohorts via the enigma schizophrenia consortium. medRxiv. (2022). doi: 10.1101/2022.01.10.21267840

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Popescu SG, Glocker B, Sharp DJ, Cole JH. Local brain-age: a U-net model. Front Aging Neurosci. (2021) 13:761954. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.761954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Varol E, Sotiras A, Davatzikos C, Initiative AsDN. Hydra: revealing heterogeneity of imaging and genetic patterns through a multiple max-margin discriminative analysis framework. Neuroimage. (2017) 145:346–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.02.041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. First MB, Williams JB, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5—Research Version (SCID-5 for DSM-5, Research Version; SCID-5-RV). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2015). p. 1–94.

Google Scholar

27. Gur RC, Richard J, Hughett P, Calkins ME, Macy L, Bilker WB, et al. A cognitive neuroscience-based computerized battery for efficient measurement of individual differences: standardization and initial construct validation. J Neurosci Methods. (2010) 187:254–62. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hodes RJ, Insel TR, Landis SC. The NIH toolbox: setting a standard for biomedical research. Neurology. (2013) 80 (11 Suppl. 3):S1. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182872e90

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence–Second Edition (WASI-II). San Antonio, TX: Pearson (2011).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

30. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. the positive and negative syndrome scale (Panss) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (1987) 13:261–76. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Niv N, Cohen AN, Sullivan G, Young AS. The MIRECC version of the global assessment of functioning scale: reliability and validity. Psychiatr Serv. (2007) 58:529–35. doi: 10.1176/ps.2007.58.4.529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Gardner DM, Murphy AL, O'Donnell H, Centorrino F, Baldessarini RJ. International consensus study of antipsychotic dosing. Am J Psychiatry. (2010) 167:686–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09060802

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hubert L, Arabie P. Comparing partitions. J. Class. (1985) 2:193–218. doi: 10.1007/BF01908075

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Ashburner J. A fast diffeomorphic image registration algorithm. Neuroimage. (2007) 38:95–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Le TT, Kuplicki RT, McKinney BA, Yeh H-W, Thompson WK, Paulus MP, et al. A nonlinear simulation framework supports adjusting for age when analyzing brainage. Front Aging Neurosci. (2018) 10:317. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00317

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Lewandowski K, Sperry S, Cohen B, Öngür D. Cognitive variability in psychotic disorders: a cross-diagnostic cluster analysis. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:3239–48. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Van Rheenen TE, Cropley V, Zalesky A, Bousman C, Wells R, Bruggemann J, et al. Widespread volumetric reductions in schizophrenia and schizoaffective patients displaying compromised cognitive abilities. Schizophr Bull. (2018) 44:560–74. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Dickinson D, Zaidman SR, Giangrande EJ, Eisenberg DP, Gregory MD, Berman KF. Distinct polygenic score profiles in schizophrenia subgroups with different trajectories of cognitive development. Am J Psychiatry. (2020) 177:298–307. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2019.19050527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Green M, Cairns M, Wu J, Dragovic M, Jablensky A, Tooney P, et al. Genome-wide supported variant MIR137 and severe negative symptoms predict membership of an impaired cognitive subtype of schizophrenia. Mol Psychiatry. (2013) 18:774–80. doi: 10.1038/mp.2012.84

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Wenzel J, Haas SS, Dwyer DB, Ruef A, Oeztuerk OF, Antonucci LA, et al. Cognitive subtypes in recent onset psychosis: distinct neurobiological fingerprints? Neuropsychopharmacology. (2021) 46:1475–83. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa031.110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Paus T. Primate anterior cingulate cortex: where motor control, drive and cognition interface. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2001) 2:417–24. doi: 10.1038/35077500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Ridderinkhof KR, Ullsperger M, Crone EA, Nieuwenhuis S. The role of the medial frontal cortex in cognitive control. Science. (2004) 306:443–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1100301

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Rushworth MF, Buckley MJ, Behrens TE, Walton ME, Bannerman DM. Functional organization of the medial frontal cortex. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2007) 17:220–7. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Walton ME, Croxson PL, Behrens TE, Kennerley SW, Rushworth MF. Adaptive decision making and value in the anterior cingulate cortex. Neuroimage. (2007) 36:T142–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Chung Y, Allswede D, Addington J, Bearden CE, Cadenhead K, Cornblatt B, et al. Cortical abnormalities in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis: findings from the NAPLS2 cohort. Neuroimage Clin. (2019) 23:101862. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Takayanagi Y, Kulason S, Sasabayashi D, Takahashi T, Katagiri N, Sakuma A, et al. Reduced thickness of the anterior cingulate cortex in individuals with an at-risk mental state who later develop psychosis. Schizophr Bull. (2017) 43:907–13. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw167

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Fornito A, Yücel M, Dean B, Wood SJ, Pantelis C. Anatomical abnormalities of the anterior cingulate cortex in schizophrenia: bridging the gap between neuroimaging and neuropathology. Schizophr Bull. (2009) 35:973–93. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. McWhinney S, Kolenic M, Franke K, Fialova M, Knytl P, Matejka M, et al. Obesity as a risk factor for accelerated brain ageing in first-episode psychosis—a longitudinal study. Schizophr Bull. (2021) 47:1772–81. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbab064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Comer AL, Carrier M, Tremblay M-È, Cruz-Martín A. The inflamed brain in schizophrenia: the convergence of genetic and environmental risk factors that lead to uncontrolled neuroinflammation. Front Cell Neurosci. (2020) 14:274. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2020.00274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognitive subtypes, brainAGE, early psychosis, clustering, machine learning

Citation: Haas SS, Ge R, Sanford N, Modabbernia A, Reichenberg A, Whalley HC, Kahn RS and Frangou S (2022) Accelerated Global and Local Brain Aging Differentiate Cognitively Impaired From Cognitively Spared Patients With Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 13:913470. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.913470

Received: 05 April 2022; Accepted: 30 May 2022;
Published: 22 June 2022.

Edited by:

Alessandro Pigoni, IRCCS Ca' Granda Foundation Maggiore Policlinico Hospital, Italy

Reviewed by:

Jason Smucny, University of California, Davis, United States
Valentina Ciullo, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy
Paolo Enrico, Università degli Studi di Milano, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Haas, Ge, Sanford, Modabbernia, Reichenberg, Whalley, Kahn and Frangou. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sophia Frangou, sophia.frangou@mssm.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.