Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 24 June 2022
Sec. Public Mental Health
This article is part of the Research Topic Mental Health of Disadvantaged Children View all 16 articles

Inhibitory Control of Emotional Interference in Deaf Children: Evidence From Event-Related Potentials and Event-Related Spectral Perturbation Analysis

  • 1Shaanxi Provincial Key Research Center for Children Mental and Behavioral Health, School of Psychology, Shaanxi Normal University, Xi'an, China
  • 2Institute of Behavior and Psychology, School of Psychology, Henan University, Kaifeng, China
  • 3Department of Health Promotion, Education, and Behavior, University of South Carolina, Columbia, SC, United States

Background: Impairment of interference control ability may reflect a more general deficit in executive functioning, and lead to an increase in internal-externalized problems such as impulsivity, which has been reported in deaf children. However, few researches have examined the neural mechanism of this impairment.

Methods: This study applied the electroencephalogram (EEG) technique to investigate the interference control ability in 31 deaf children and 28 hearing controls with emotional face-word stroop task.

Results: Results from behavioral task showed that deaf children exhibited lower accuracy compared to hearing controls. As for EEG analysis, reduced activation of ERP components in N1 and enhanced activation of ERP components in N450 have been found in deaf children. Besides, incongruent condition elicited larger N450 than congruent condition. Furthermore, for brain oscillation, alpha band (600–800 ms) revealed a reduced desynchronization in deaf children, while theta band (200–400 ms) revealed an enhanced synchronization in deaf children and incongruent condition, which were in line with ERP components.

Conclusion: The present findings seem to indicate that the deficit during emotional interference control ability among deaf children might be due to the impaired attention allocation ability and emotional cognitive monitoring function during emotional conflict detection process. Consequently, reduced N1 and enhanced N450 might be due to early attention impairment causing more effort of deaf children later in emotional cognitive monitoring.

Introduction

The World Health Organization (WHO) estimates that there are approximately 360 million people with hearing impairment in the world and almost one-tenth of the affected population are children (1). Previous research has shown that hearing loss not only affects the normal development of language skills, but also affects other neurocognitive functions among deaf children, such as interference control ability (2, 3). However, most of these previous studies were done with questionnaires or behavioral experiments (46) and focused on the performance of working memory, attention, inhibitory control and other executive functions of deaf children. Few of them ever implied electroencephalogram (EEG) technique to investigate the neural mechanisms of this interference control impairment among deaf children. However, EEG signals, with their millisecond temporal resolution, are excellent at tracking rapid changes in brain function, and techniques to acquire these signals are relatively simple and non-invasive, providing more accurate and detailed information to help estimate inhibitory control. Therefore, this study selected it to identify the inhibitory control of emotional interference in deaf children (7, 8).

In addition to the commonly studied interference control ability, various emotion skills are also believed to be impaired in deaf children. For instance, facial emotion processing, as one of the most studied aspects of social cognitive function, is reported being impaired in deaf children (4, 9, 10). Moreover, dozens of studies also showed that deaf children are more challenged in terms of emotion identification, emotion understanding, and the expression of emotion compared with hearing controls (1116). Such challenges can be attributed to delayed language acquisition or lack of personal experience opportunities to talk with others, as well as the long-term stress environment in which they are trapped in emotional states such as anxiety, depression and subjective anxiety.

Moreover, Gray (17, 18) believed that cognition and emotion are strongly integrated and inseparable in the process of information processing (19). A meta-analysis of inhibitory control demonstrated that several brain areas have been associated with the mechanisms underlying inhibitory control, with a network involving left and right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex (dlPFC), anterior cingulate (ACC) (20). Specifically, the role of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex (DLPFC) regions have been shown to be components of a neural network which plays a critical role in the completion of tasks requiring self-monitoring and inhibition (21, 22). In addition, several meta-analyses of emotion regulation reported activations in the bilateral dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex (dlPFC), ventrolateral pre-frontal cortex (vlPFC), dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC) (2325), which largely overlaps with the classic frontoparietal cognitive control network (26).

According to the above evidence analysis (24, 9, 10, 20, 2326), we can know that emotional and inhibitory control disorders both exist in deaf children, and the activation brain regions of cognitive and emotional networks are highly overlapped. Therefore, the present study combined two aspects and employed the face-word emotional stroop task to investigate the emotional inhibitory control of deaf children (2729). In this task, “happy” and “fear” words with red color are superimposed across facial expressions of happy and fear, conflict effects occur when emotional words and facial expressions are incongruent, which has been widely used to examine the inhibitory control of emotional interference (28, 29). Previous studies have observed two important ERP components that were related to emotional interference control processing: N1 and N450 (28, 3039). The N1 component of the ERP reflect brain activation in the early perceptual stages (38). It was hypothesized that larger amplitudes of the sensory components (N1) to emotional words indicated an increased attention-related cerebral processing during relatively early perceptual stages of information processing (31). The N450 is a popular index of conflict detection in emotional conflict control tasks which shows larger negative amplitude in the incongruent condition compared to congruent condition (3437).

Furthermore, time-frequency analysis (TFA) can provide complementary information on neural processing dynamics that is distinctive from traditional phase-locked ERP method. Therefore, according to previous studies, theta and alpha band are used for analysis to explore the characteristics of emotional suppression control in deaf children (4046). The frontal-central distribution of the evoked theta (4–7 Hz) response is suggested to be related with central executive and working memory processes, and reflects initiation of the central executive processes to detect interference and to inhibit the response for task-irrelevant features (40, 42, 43, 47). Alpha desynchronization (8–14 Hz) which reflects attentional processes, processing of sensory–semantic information and the difficulty of the task, that is to say, the more demanding a task, the stronger the amount of event-related alpha desynchronization (40, 4446).

Taken together, inhibitory control of emotional interference is vital not only for good behavior and cognitive function, but also for adequate emotional control and social interaction (48, 49). In the current study, we applied EEG technique to investigate both emotion and cognitive abilities using face-word emotional stroop task, and further explore the potential neural markers of emotional interference control deficit among deaf children. On the basis of previous research (10, 48, 5052), our hypothesis was that compared to hearing controls, deaf children would show worse performance in both behavioral and EEG measures during emotional stroop task.

Materials and Methods

Participants

We performed a sample size calculation on the basis of G*Power 3.1, using an alpha level of 0.05 with 95% power to detect a large effect size (f = 0.4). Results showed that a sample size of 12 would be needed to assure the adequate statistical power. Therefore, a total of 31 deaf children aged 9–13 were recruited from the Kaifeng Special Education School in central China and 28 matched hearing controls were recruited from the same geography area. Parent and teacher reports established that all children were born to hearing parents and had no apparent mental health disorders such as ADHD or autism spectrum disorders. Main demographic characteristics of both deaf children and hearing controls were shown in Table 1. Participants were required to complete a computer version of emotional conflict task while recording EEG. Each participant received an age-appropriate gift at the completion of the experiment as token of appreciation. The study protocol was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Henan University, and all participants provided written informed consent prior to data collection.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of deaf children and hearing controls.

Stimuli and Procedure

Twenty face pictures were selected from Chinese affective picture system (53), including 10 happy face pictures (5 female, 5 male) and 10 fearful face pictures (5 female, 5 male). Two Chinese characters, “愉快” (which means “happy”) or “恐惧” (which means “fear”) were superimposed on the faces in red. The words and facial expressions were either congruent (e.g., character meaning fear superimposed onto a fear face picture, see Figure 1) or incongruent (e.g., character meaning happy superimposed onto a fear face picture, see Figure 1). The stimuli were programmed by E-Prime 2.0 software, and they were presented on a Dell 19 -in monitor.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Procedures for emotional stroop task.

Participants performed modified face-word stroop task (judging facial expression) while sitting in quiet room with dim light, participants had to identify the facial expression of the target faces while ignoring the meaning of the words. They were instructed to respond by pressing a button, corresponding to “fear” faces (right index finger) or “happy” faces (right middle finger), as quickly and accurately as possible. The order of performing the experimental task was counterbalanced across participants.

The face-word stroop task consisted of 240 trials that were presented over 4 blocks (60 trials per block). Each block in each task consisted of an equal amount of congruent and incongruent trials. Stimuli were presented in random order within each block. Participants performed in a 24-trial practice block prior to the experiment. The timing and order of each trial was the same for each block: a fixation dot was presented for a specific duration (500 ms) followed by a blank screen of variable duration (300–500 ms). Then, the target face appeared for 1000 ms at the center of the screen. Participants had to respond within 1500 ms. The inter-trial interval (ITI) varied randomly between 800 ms and 1200 ms, with a mean of 1000 ms (Figure 1).

EEG Recording

The electroencephalogram (EEG) was recorded from a 32 scalp standard channel cap (10/20 system; Brain Products, Munich, Germany) (Figure 2). Electrooculogram (EOG) was recorded from electrodes placed above the right eye. All electrode recordings were online referenced to FCz. All inter-electrode impedance was maintained below 5 kΩ. The EEG and EOG signals were amplified using a 0.01–100 Hz band pass filter and continuously sampled at 500 Hz/channel for offline analysis.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Standard electrode map, illustrating the commonly deployed 10-20 System. F refers to Frontal lobe, T refers to Temporal lobe, C refers to Central lobe, P refers to Parietal lobe, O refers to Occipital lobe, z refers to an electrode placed on the mid-line.

After data acquisition, EEG data were transferred into the EEGLAB and Letswave toolboxes, which are open-source Matlab toolboxes for neurophysiologic data analysis (54, 55). EEG were re-referenced to the average of the two mastoids and filtered with a band pass of 0.1–30 Hz. Epochs were extracted between the 200 ms pre-stimulus and 1000 ms post-stimulus time points, and the baseline correction was performed in 200 ms pre-stimulus interval. Eye movement artifacts were removed with ICA. Finally, data were inspected and cleansed manually for any obvious remaining artifacts.

ERP Analysis

This study analyzed the potentials of the ERP components N1 and N450. The electrodes for further analysis were chosen according to ERP topographical distribution and previous studies (56, 57). Specifically, the amplitudes of the N1 (100-200 ms) were analyzed at F3, F4, Fz, and the N450 (330–400 ms) at C3, C4, Cz, P3, P4, Pz, and were measured as mean values. The time windows were determined through visual detection in the grand-averaged ERPs.

Time–Frequency Analyses

An estimate of the oscillatory power as a function of time and frequency (time–frequency representation) was obtained from single-trial EEG epochs using the continuous wavelet transform (CWT) (55). The time–frequency representations were explored between 1 Hz and 30 Hz in steps of 0.29 Hz. Epochs were extracted between the 400 ms pre-stimulus and 1000 ms post-stimulus time points. To avoid edge effects when performing CWT, the pre-stimulus time interval (−400 ms to −200 ms) was used as a baseline interval. Based on average condition contrast maps and previous studies (58), 2 clusters were tested in this study: 4–7 Hz at 200–400 ms for theta (F3, F4, Fz, C3, C4, and Cz), 8–14 Hz at 600–800 ms for alpha (P3, P4 and Pz). Each of these oscillatory components was quantified as the mean amplitude within these time windows of each participant.

Statistical Analysis

SPSS 20.0 was used to perform ANOVA or the chi-square test to investigate whether the demographic factors (including age and gender) showed significant differences between groups (deaf children and hearing controls). Furthermore, repeated measures ANOVA was conducted on behavioral and ERP data with group (deaf children vs. hearing controls) as a between-subject factor, while stimulus type (congruent, and incongruent), Hemisphere (Hemi) (only in EEG data: Left, Midline and Right) and antero-posterior distribution (AP) (only in EEG data: Frontal, Central, Parietal and Occipital) were considered as within-subject factors. For all the analyses in this study, the p-values were corrected by Greenhouse-Geisser correction when appropriate.

Results

Behavioral Data

Accuracy

ANOVA showed significant main effect of group on response accuracy (F1, 57 = 11.705, p = 0.002, η2 = 0.163), with overall lower accuracy in deaf children compared to hearing controls (see Table 2), which indicated that deaf children had difficulties in suppressing irrelevant information and suffered from deficient cognitive control mechanisms (4, 5, 50). The main effect of condition was also significant (F1, 57 = 63.094, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.525), with incongruent condition (0.872 ± 0.010) being significantly lower than congruent condition (0.927 ± 0.009), which indicated that in the presence of a conflict effect, the incongruent condition invested more cognitive resources than the congruent condition (28, 29, 59).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean accuracy and reaction time (M ± SD) of deaf children and hearing controls, and results of repeated measures ANOVA for conditions.

Reaction Time

The significant main effect of condition was found that congruent condition (759.818 ± 14.833) was significantly faster than incongruent condition (816.231 ± 14.481) (F1, 57 = 135.774, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.704), which is consistent with the response accuracy, indicating that the conflict condition requires more cognitive resources and that participants need longer reaction times to make judgments. The interaction between group and condition was significant (F1, 57 = 24.981, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.305). Further analysis indicated that a significant interference effect with the congruent condition (745.278 ± 20.437) (774.358 ± 21.504) being faster than the incongruent condition (777.492 ± 19.952) (854.969 ± 20.994) in deaf children (F1, 57 = 23.325, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.290) and hearing controls (F1, 57 = 131.910, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.698).

ERP Amplitude Analysis

For the consideration of space, we only included significant results in this part. Figure 3 showed the grand-averaged waveforms of deaf children and hearing controls. The Means and SEs for each component were displayed in Table 3, and the Means and SDs of amplitudes of each electrode N1 and N450 were displayed in Table 4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Waveforms of N1 and N450 components in emotional stroop task of deaf children and hearing controls.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. ERP amplitudes (M ± SD) of deaf children and hearing controls, and results of repeated measures ANOVA for conditions.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. The average amplitudes (μV) of the ERP components (M ± SD) between deaf children and hearing controls.

N1

A repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) was applied in this procedure with N1 amplitude as dependent variable, with stimulus type (congruent, and incongruent), Hemisphere (Hemi: Left, Midline and Right) and AP [frontal(F) (electrodes: F3, Fz, F4), central (C) (electrodes: C3, Cz, C4), and parietal (P) (electrodes: P3, Pz, P4)] as within-subject factors, and group (deaf children vs. hearing controls) as a between-subject factor. Results showed significant main effect of group on N1 amplitude (F1, 57 = 4.517, p = 0.038, η2 = 0.073), with hearing controls (−7.913 ± 0.734 μV) eliciting overall larger N1 compared to deaf children (−5.761 ± 0.697 μV), which suggested that the smaller N1 amplitudes were neurophysiological reflex of deficient inhibition for emotional stroop task in deaf children (60). The interaction effect of Group × Hemisphere was also significant for N1 amplitude (F2, 56 = 3.197, p = 0.046, η2 = 0.053). Further analysis indicated that midline region (Fz) elicited larger N1 activation in hearing controls (−8.360 ± 0.75 6μV) compared to deaf children (−5.692 ± 0.718 μV), while there was no significant group difference in response to right (F4) and left hemispheres (F3).

N450

Analysis of N450 amplitude showed a significant main effect of group (F1, 57 = 5.883, p = 0.018, η2 = 0.094), with deaf children (4.578 ± 1.263 μV) eliciting overall larger N450 compared to hearing controls (9.026 ± 1.329 μV). There also was a significant main effect of condition (F1, 57 = 5.090, p = 0.028, η2 = 0.082), incongruent condition (6.319 ± 0.945 μV) eliciting larger N450 than congruent condition (7.285 ± 0.938μV). The results of the N450 amplitudes were consistent with behavioral outcomes, reflecting deficits in inhibitory control in deaf children and inconsistent conditions requiring more effort to complete. The results showed a significant main effect of AP (F1, 57 = 203.155, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.781), with central area (1.574 ± 1.031 μV) eliciting larger N450 than parietal area (12.030 ± 0.942 μV). There was also a significant Group × AP interaction effect (F1, 57 = 6.798, p = 0.012, η2 = 0.107). According to further analysis, this interaction indicated that the central area (0.306 ± 1.420 μV) (2.842 ± 1.495 μV) elicited larger N450 than parietal area (8.849 ± 1.298 μV) (15.210 ± 1.366 μV) in both deaf children (F1, 57 = 71.447, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.556) and hearing controls (F1, 57 = 135.262, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.704).

Time-Frequency Results

Theta Activity

Figure 4 presented the Time-frequency (TF) analysis results. Theta synchronization (200–400 ms) showed significant main effect of condition (F1, 57 = 5.022, p = 0.029, η2 = 0.081), with incongruent condition (7.135 ± 0.467) eliciting larger theta synchronization than congruent condition (6.624 ± 0.460). Theta synchronization also showed significant main effect of group (F1, 57 = 5.348, p = 0.024, η2 = 0.086), with deaf children (7.920 ± 0.620) eliciting larger theta synchronization than hearing controls (5.840 ± 0.652), which were consistent with the results of N450. There also was a significant main effect of emotional background (F1, 57 = 7.659, p = 0.008, η2 = 0.118), with fear background (7.189 ± 0.458) eliciting larger theta synchronization than happy background (6.571 ± 0.469). The results also showed significant main effect of AP (F1, 57 = 44.420, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.438) and hemisphere (F2, 56 = 12.970, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.185), with frontal area (7.674 ± 0.490) eliciting larger theta synchronization than central area (6.085 ± 0.439), midline region (Fz, Cz) (7.408 ± 0.494) eliciting larger theta synchronization than left hemispheres (F3, C3) (6.857 ± 0.476) and right hemispheres (F4, C4) (5.840 ± 0.652). A significant interaction was found between the emotional background and group (F1, 57 = 7.832, p = 0.007, η2 = 0.121), further analysis indicated that deaf children (8.541 ± 0.631) elicited larger theta synchronization in fear background compared to hearing controls (5.836 ± 0.663), while there was no significant group difference in response to happy background.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Group-averaged time-frequency spectrogram during facial emotion recognition. Time (in ms) is denoted on the x-axis, with 0 ms defined as the onset of the stimuli. Frequency (in Hz) is shown on the y-axis. A represent the theta band (200–400 ms), and B represent the alpha band (600–800 ms).

Alpha Activity

Alpha desynchronization (600–800 ms) showed significant main effect of group (F1, 57 = 6.868, p = 0.011, η2 = 0.108), with hearing controls (−6.376 ± 1.003) eliciting larger alpha desynchronization than deaf children (−2.750 ± 0.953), which were in line with the results of N1. Alpha desynchronization also showed significant main effect of emotional background (F1, 57 = 6.939, p = 0.011, η2 = 0.109), with fear background (−4.882 ± 0.773) eliciting larger alpha desynchronization than happy background (−4.244 ± 0.624). There also was a significant main effect of hemisphere (F2, 56 = 10.817, p = 0.000, η2 = 0.160), with midline region (Pz) (−3.665 ± 0.540) eliciting smaller alpha desynchronization than left hemispheres (P3) (−4.823 ± 0.798) and right hemispheres (P4) (−5.190 ± 0.790).

Discussion

The present study explored the emotional interference effect among deaf children. Behavioral results showed that the main effect of condition was significant not only for the accuracy data but also for the reaction time data, which indicated that in the presence of a conflict effect, the incongruent condition invested more cognitive resources than the congruent condition (28, 29, 59). In addition, it was also found that deaf children demonstrated significantly lower accuracy rate in emotional stroop task than hearing controls, which is consistent with previous findings that deaf children had difficulties in suppressing irrelevant information and suffered from deficient cognitive control mechanisms (4, 5, 50).

These behavioral results were further explored by ERP analysis. Deaf children showed diminished activation in the emotional interference processing components compared to hearing controls. Given that the N1 components reflect the attentional focus on the target and a discrimination process within the focus of attention (31, 60), the smaller N1 amplitudes suggest two important points. First, although the emotional stroop task needed participants to concentrate on the facial expression of the picture, the deaf children paid less attention on the task-relevant information (the words of the picture) because of the interference of the meaning of the words. Therefore, a weaker allocation for target information was obtained. Second, for task-irrelevant information (the words of the picture), the deaf children also automatically put attentional resources on it; consequently, the attentional resources for task completing were deficient, and the time needed to complete the task was prolonged or the task was poorly performed. So the smaller N1 amplitudes might be correlated with a slower response to emotional interference stimuli, which suggested that the smaller N1 amplitudes were neurophysiological reflex of deficient inhibition for emotional stroop task (61). This finding is in accordance with the evidence of similar alterations during stroop task in individuals with schizophrenia, amblyopic, obsessive compulsive disorder, or depression (31, 38, 62, 63).

In contrast to N1, deaf children showed enhanced activation in N450 compared to hearing controls. According to previous studies, the N450 is a valid index of conflict monitoring in emotional conflict control tasks and shows larger negative amplitude in the incongruent condition compared to congruent condition (3437, 64). The role of the anterior cingulate cortex (ACC) and the dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex (DLPFC) regions have been shown to be components of a neural network which plays a critical role in the completion of tasks requiring self-monitoring and inhibition (21). The N450 component demonstrated enhanced amplitudes in deaf children, suggesting that deaf children may require a greater recruitment of cognitive resources from the ACC and DLPFC to achieve the performance levels of hearing controls during emotional stroop task (22, 64). In addition, compared to healthy controls, major depressive disorder (MDD) patients showed enhanced N450 amplitude (31, 65). Patients with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (AHDH), nocturnal enuresis (NE) and developmental coordination disorder (DCD) showed increased activation in the bilateral temporoparietal junctions, bilateral dorsolateral pre-frontal cortex, and bilateral anterior cingulate cortex (6668). There is a causal relationship between the decreased N1 and the increased N450, which is similar to previous studies of person with depression (31). The correlation may be due to reduced early attention requiring more effort later in emotional stroop task. Combined with the analysis of behavioral results which showed that accuracy rate of deaf children in emotional stroop task was significantly lower than hearing controls, it can be found that although deaf children made more efforts and showed more activation on N450 than hearing controls, they did not reach the same level as hearing controls, which revealed the emotional impairment of cognitive monitoring function of the deaf children, meanwhile indicated that the emotional cognitive resources for monitoring conflict information and inhibition irrelevant information in the inhibition control process of the deaf children are very limited.

Besides ERP analysis, the current study employed time–frequency measures in alpha and theta band which showed significantly more desynchronization in hearing controls and significantly more synchronization in deaf children and incongruent condition. A number of studies have found that alpha oscillation was a reliable marker of attention (69, 70), theta band was related with central executive and working memory processes, and reflected initiation of the central executive processes to detect interference and to inhibit the response for task-irrelevant features (40, 42, 43). Alpha desynchronization was similar to the results from ERP components in N1. Previous studies have showed diminished alpha suppression in the predominantly inattentive (IA) and absent alpha oscillation in ADHD (71, 72). Therefore, the diminished alpha desynchronization might suggest impaired attention distribution ability during emotional interference processing of deaf children. Theta synchronization also showed a conflict effect which was in line with the previous studies (73, 74). In addition, consistent with ERP results, deaf children showed enhanced theta synchronization which might suggest impaired cognitive monitoring function during emotional interference processing of them.

The negative impact poor inhibitory control has on a range of outcomes for deaf children, but considering that after a period of training, children showed great improvements in their inhibition skills (49, 75). Therefore, we can train the inhibitory control of emotional interference ability of deaf children to help them form a healthy personality and better integrate into the society. Specifically, school education can strengthen the training of deaf children's inhibitory control of emotional interference ability through flexible and diverse classroom forms. Based on the fact that deaf children are more inclined to visual images when receiving information, they have stronger perception and memory for actions, expressions or visualized pictures. Teachers can use multimedia animation and small games in teaching to mobilize students to participate in various emotional situations, help them improve the problems in emotional control, let them understand the way of emotional expression, enhance their ability to understand the behavior intention of others, and have a certain ability to predict the behavior consequences, so as to improve their ability of the inhibitory control of emotional interference.

Contributions, Limitations, and Future Directions

Combining the ERP and TFA data analysis methods simultaneously can not only enhance the energy of ERP components by using the high temporal resolution ERP technology, but also greatly reduce the amplitude and noise of the spontaneous EEG, so as to reveal the time process of individuals in the process of inhibitory control of emotional interference (76). Moreover, time-frequency analysis can be used to simultaneously extract the temporal and spectral domains of event-related brain activity, improving the detectability of ERP and allowing characterization of non-phase-locked components that cannot be identified by traditional time-domain averaging in healthy and deaf children (77, 78).

The limitation of this study is that it does not use nuclear magnetic technology with high spatial resolution, but only ERP technology with high temporal resolution. Future research should use the combination of ERP and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to measure the neural mechanism of deaf children in emotional stroop task, and use the advantages of the combination of high temporal resolution and high spatial resolution to explore the damage degree of inhibitory control of emotional interference and the corresponding impaired brain regions of deaf children.

Conclusion

In conclusion, the current study revealed major deficits in deaf children during emotion-related conflict control, with overall worse behavioral performance and reduced activation of N1 and alpha desynchronization, and enhanced activation of N450 and theta synchronization compared to the hearing controls, which might suggest impaired attention allocation ability and cognitive monitoring function during conflict detection process in deaf children. The Findings enriched the understanding of impaired inhibitory control of emotional interference in deaf children, and helped educators to take timely and appropriate intervention measures to promote the optimal neuropsychological development of deaf children.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of Henan University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

JZ contributed to the conception of the study. HG contributed significantly to analysis and manuscript preparation. QC performed the data analyses and wrote the manuscript. XL helped revise the manuscript. QC contributed to the interpretation and discussion of the results of the analysis. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Science and Technology Research Project of Henan Provincial Department of Science and Technology [212102310985], the Humanities and Social Science Research Project of Henan Provincial Department of Education [2020-ZDJH-026], and the Social Science Planning Project of Henan Province [2021CJY051].

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Lam AMK, Stringer P, Toizumi M, Dang DA. An international partnership analysis of a cohort of Vietnamese children with hearing impairment. Speech, Language Hearing. (2016) 19:27–35. doi: 10.1080/2050571X.2015.1108066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Cupples L, Ching TYC, Button L, Leigh G, Marnane V, Whitfield J, et al. Language and speech outcomes of children with hearing loss and additional disabilities: identifying the variables that influence performance at five years of age. Int J Audiol. (2016) 57:1–48. doi: 10.1080/14992027.2016.1228127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Zupan B. The Role of Audition in Audiovisual Perception of Speech and Emotion in Children with Hearing Loss. New York, NY: Springer (2013).

Google Scholar

4. Aubuchon AM, Pisoni DB. Verbal processing speed and executive functioning in long-term cochlear implant users. J Speech Lang Hear Res. (2015) 58:151–62. doi: 10.1044/2014_JSLHR-H-13-0259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Mani RA. The executive functions' deficits in children with hearing loss. In: 6th International Congress on Child and Adolescent Psychiatry. Tabriz: Tabtiz University of Medical Sciences (2013).

Google Scholar

6. Temurova G. Using hearing aids in determining the level of speech development in children with hearing impairment. Ment Enlightenment Sci-Methodol J. (2020) 1:111–7.

Google Scholar

7. Bell MA. Using EEG to study cognitive development: issues and practices. J Cogn Dev. (2012) 13:281–94. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2012.691143

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Luo Y, Fu Q, Xie J, Qin Y, Wu G., Liu J, Ding X. EEG-based emotion classification using spiking neural networks IEEE Access. (2020) 8:46007–16. doi: 10.1109/ACCESS.2020.2978163

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Sidera F, Amadó A. Influences on facial emotion recognition in deaf children. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2017) 22:164–77. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enw072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Gu H, Chen Q, Xing XL, Zhao JF. Facial emotion recognition in deaf children: Evidence from event-related potentials and event-related spectral perturbation analysis. Neurosci Letters. (2019) 703:198–204. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2019.01.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Denmark T., Atkinson J., Campbell R. How do typically developing deaf children and deaf children with autism spectrum disorder use the face when comprehending emotional facial expressions in british sign language? J Autism Dev Dis. (2014) 44:2584–92. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2130-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Peterson CC. The mind behind the message: advancing theory-of-mind scales for typically developing children, and those with deafness, autism, or Asperger syndrome. Child Dev. (2012) 83:469–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01728.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Tsou YT, Li B, Kret ME, Frijns JH. Hearing status affects children's emotion understanding in dynamic social situations: An eye-tracking study. Ear Hear. (2021) 42:1024. doi: 10.1097/AUD.0000000000000994

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Tsou YT, Li B, Kret ME, Sabino da Costa I, Rieffe C. Reading emotional faces in deaf and hard-of-hearing and typically hearing children. Emotion. (2020). doi: 10.1037/emo0000863. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Wang Y, Su Y. Facial expression recognition in children with cochlear implants and hearing aids. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:1–6. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Wiefferink CH, Carolien R, Lizet K, Leo DR. Emotion understanding in deaf children with a cochlear implant. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2013) 18:175–86. doi: 10.1093/deafed/ens042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Gray. JRI. of emotion and cognitive control. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2004) 13:46–8. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00272.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Gray JR, Braver TS. Integration of emotion and cognition in the lateral prefrontal cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2002) 99:4115–20. doi: 10.1073/pnas.062381899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Battaglia S, Serio G, Scarpazza C, D'Ausilio A, Borgomaneri S. Frozen in (e) motion: How reactive motor inhibition is influenced by the emotional content of stimuli in healthy and psychiatric populations. Behav Res Ther. (2021) 146:103963. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2021.103963

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Borgomaneri S, Serio G. Please, don't do it! fifteen years of progress of non-invasive brain stimulation in action inhibition. Cortex. (2020) 132:404–22. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2020.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gruber SA, Rogowska J. Decreased activation of the anterior cingulate in bipolar patients: an fMRI study. J Affect Disord. (2004) 82:191–201. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2003.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Gu H, Fan RL, Zhao JF, Chen YN, Chen Q. Inhibitory control of emotional interference in children with learning disorders: evidence from event-related potentials and event-related spectral perturbation analysis. Neurosci Letters. (2019) 1718:252–8. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2019.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Kohn N, Eickhoff SB, Scheller M. Laird a R Fox PT, Habel U neural network of cognitive emotion regulation - an ALE meta-analysis and MACM analysis. NeuroImage. (2014) 87:345–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Frank DW, Dewitt M, Hudgens-Haney M, Schaeffer DJ, Ball BH, Schwarz NF. Emotion regulation: quantitative meta-analysis of functional activation and deactivation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 45:202–11. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Buhle JT, Silvers JA, Wager TD, Lopez R, Onyemekwu C, Kober H. Cognitive reappraisal of emotion: a meta-analysis of human neuroimaging studies. Cerebral Cortex. (2014) 24:2981–90. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Zilverstand A, Parvaz MA. Neuroimaging cognitive reappraisal in clinical populations to define neural targets for enhancing emotion regulation. a systematic review. Neuroimage. (2017) 151:105–116. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Tobias E, Amit E, Seth G. Dissociable neural systems resolve conflict from emotional versus nonemotional distracters. Cerebral Cortex. (2008) 18:1475–84. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Xue S, Li Y, Kong X, He Q, Liu J. The dissociable neural dynamics of cognitive conflict and emotional conflict control: an ERP study. Neurosci Letters. (2016) 619:149–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2016.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Zhaoa X, Lia X. Influence of inhibitory tagging (IT) on emotional and cognitive conflict processing: Evidence from event-related potentials. Neurosci Letters. (2017) 657:120–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2017.08.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Chu CH, Kramer AF, Song TF, Wu CH, Hung TM. Acute exercise and neurocognitive development in preadolescents and young adults: an ERP study. Neural Plast. (2017) 2017:1–13. doi: 10.1155/2017/2631909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Dai Q. Deficient interference inhibition for negative stimuli in depression: an event-related potential study. Clin Neurophysiology. (2011) 122:52–61. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Greg H, Annmarie MN. Event-related potentials, emotion, and emotion regulation: an integrative review. Dev Neuropsychol. (2010) 35:129–55. doi: 10.1080/87565640903526504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Giller F, Aggensteiner PM, Banaschewski T, Döpfner M, Brandeis D, Roessner V. Affective dysregulation in children is associated with difficulties in response control in emotional ambiguous situations. Biol Psychiatry Cogn Neurosci Neuroimaging. (2022) 7:66–75. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Imbir KK, Spustek T, Duda J, Bernatowicz G. & J, Z. N450 and LPC event-related potential correlates of an emotional stroop task with words differing in valence and emotional origin. Front Psychol. (2017) 8:1–14. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00880

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Liotti M, Woldorff MG, Iii RP. An ERP study of the temporal course of the Stroop color-word interference effect. Neuropsychologia. (2000) 38:701–11. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(99)00106-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Shen YM, Xue S, Wang KC. Neural time course of emotional conflict control: an ERP study. Neuroscience Letters. (2013) 541:34–38. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2013.02.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Szucs D. Functional definition of the N450 event-related brain potential marker of conflict processing: A numerical Stroop study. BMC Neurosci. (2012) 13:35–49. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-13-35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Zhou A, Jiang Y, Chen J, Wei J, Dang B, Li S. Neural mechanisms of selective attention in children with amblyopia. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:1–17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0125370

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Huang Z. Processing of emotional information in working memory in major depressive disorder. Advance Psychol Sci. (2021) 29:252–67. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1042.2021.00252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Ergen M, Saban S, Kirmizi-Alsan E, Uslu A, Keskin-Ergen Y. Time–frequency analysis of the event-related potentials associated with the Stroop test. Int J Psychophysiol. (2014) 94: 463–72. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.08.177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Gu H, Zhao Q, Liu J, Zhao J, Ji L, Chi P. EEG oscillation evidences of altered resting-state brain activity in children orphaned by parental HIV/AIDS. AIDS Care. (2020) 32:177–82. doi: 10.1080/09540121.2020.1739211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Paul S, Wolfgang K, Manuel S. Fronto-parietal EEG coherence in theta and upper alpha reflect central executive functions of working memory. Int J Psychophysiol. (2005) 57:97–103. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Tesche CD. Theta oscillations index human hippocampal activation during a working memory task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2000) 97:919–24. doi: 10.1073/pnas.97.2.919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Compton RJ, Arnstein D, Freedman G, Dainer-Best J. Cognitive control in the intertrial interval: evidence from EEG alpha power. Psychophysiology. (2011) 48:583–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2010.01124.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Jiang J, Zhang Q. EEG neural oscillatory dynamics reveal semantic and response conflict at difference levels of conflict awareness. Sci Rep. (2015) 5:1–12. doi: 10.1038/srep12008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Kamaradova D, Hajda M, Prasko J, Taborsky J, Grambal A, Latalova K, et al. Cognitive deficits in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder - electroencephalography correlates. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2016) 12:1119–25. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S93040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Becske M, Marosi C, Molnár H, Fodor Z, Tombor L. Distractor filtering and its electrophysiological correlates in schizophrenia. Clin Neurophysiol. (2022) 133:71–82. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2021.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Merchán A, García LF, Maurno NG, Castañeda PR. Executive functions in deaf and hearing children: the mediating role of language skills in inhibitory control. J Exp Child Psychol. (2022) 218:1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2022.105374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Mason K, Marshall CR. Executive function training for deaf children: impact of a music intervention. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2021) 26:490–500. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enab026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Botting N, Jones A, Marshall C, Denmark T, Atkinson J. Nonverbal executive function is mediated by language: a study of deaf and hearing children. Child Dev. (2017) 88:1689–700. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kronenberger WG, Colson BG, Henning SC. Executive functioning and speech-language skills following long-term use of cochlear implants. J Deaf Stud Deaf Educ. (2014) 19:456–70. doi: 10.1093/deafed/enu011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Kronenberger WG, Pisoni DB, Henning SC. Executive functioning skills in long-term users of cochlear implants: a case control study. J Pediatr Psychol. (2013) 38:902–14. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jst034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Lu B, Hui M. The development of native Chinese affective picture system–a pretest in 46 college students. Chinese Ment Health J. (2005) 19:719–22.

Google Scholar

54. Delorme A. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. (2004) 134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Mouraux A. Across-trial averaging of event-related EEG responses and beyond. Magn Reson Imaging. (2008) 26:1041–54. doi: 10.1016/j.mri.2008.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Liu C, Yao R, Wang Z. N450 as a candidate neural marker for interference control deficits in children with learning disabilities. Int J Psychophysiology. (2014) 93:70–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.05.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Sopov M, Ostapenko M. Distractor familiarity in picture-word interference paradigm: An ERP study. Paper presented at the Conference of Experimental Psychologists. (2016) https://www.researchgate.net/publication/316660425

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

58. Wang L. Oscillatory brain dynamics associated with the automatic processing of emotion in words. Brain Lang. (2014) 137:120–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2014.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Bosworth RG, Binder EM, Tyler SC. Automaticity of lexical access in deaf and hearing bilinguals: Cross-linguistic evidence from the color Stroop task across five languages. Cognition. (2021) 212:104659. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2021.104659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Frings C. Electrophysiological correlates of visual identity negative priming. Brain Res. (2007) 1176:82–91. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.07.093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Perez-Edgar K. Individual differences in children's performance during an emotional stroop task. Brain Cogn. (2003) 52:33–51. doi: 10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00007-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Mathis KI, Wynn JK, Jahshan C, Hellemann G, Darque A. An electrophysiological investigation of attentional blink in schizophrenia: separating perceptual and attentional processes. Int J Psychophysiol. (2012) 86:108–13. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.06.052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Ozcan H, Ozer S. Neuropsychological, electrophysiological and neurological impairments in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder, their healthy siblings and healthy controls: Identifying potential endophenotype(s). Psychiatry Res. (2016) 240:110–17. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Larson MJ, Clayson PE. Making sense of all the conflict: a theoretical review and critique of conflict-related ERPs. Int J Psychophysiol. (2014) 93:283–97. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Mcneely HE, Lau MA, Christensen BK. Neurophysiological evidence of cognitive inhibition anomalies in persons with major depressive disorder. Clin Neurophysiol. (2008) 119:1578–89. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.03.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Fan LY, Gau SS. Neural correlates of inhibitory control and visual processing in youths with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a counting Stroop functional MRI study. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:2661–71. doi: 10.1017/S0033291714000038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Koch JKL, Miguel H. Prefrontal activation during Stroop and Wisconsin card sort tasks in children with developmental coordination disorder: a NIRS study. Exp Brain Res. (2018) 236:3053–64. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5358-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Wang M, Zhang K, Zhang J, Dong G, Zhang H. Abnormal neural responses to emotional stimuli but not Go/NoGo and stroop tasks in adults with a history of childhood nocturnal enuresis. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:1–10. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0142957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Urzua A, Domic M, Ramos M, Cerda A. Psychometric properties of three rating scales for attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in Chilean students. Revista Panamericana de Salud Publica. (2010) 27:157–67. doi: 10.1590/S1020-49892010000300002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Janssens C, De Loof E, Boehler CN, Pourtois G, Verguts T. Occipital alpha power reveals fast attentional inhibition of incongruent distractors. Psychophysiology. (2018) 55:1–11. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Mazaheri A, Fassbender C, Coffey-Corina S, Hartanto TA, Schweitzer JB. Differential oscillatory electroencephalogram between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder subtypes and typically developing adolescents. Biol Psychiatry. (2014) 76:422–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.08.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Fassbender C, Zhang H, Buzy WM, Cortes CR, Mizuiri D, Beckett L. A lack of default network suppression is linked to increased distractibility in ADHD. Brain Res. (2009) 1273:114–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Sanja K, Sheeva A, Andrei I, Jason S, Eric H. Theta oscillations are sensitive to both early and late conflict processing stages: effects of alcohol intoxication. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:1–13. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0043957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Tang D, Hu L. The neural oscillations of conflict adaptation in the human frontal region. Biological Psychol. (2013) 93: 364–372. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Jones A, Atkinson J, Marshall C, Botting N, St Clair MC. Expressive vocabulary predicts nonverbal executive function: a 2-year longitudinal study of deaf and hearing children. Child Dev. (2020) 91:400–14. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13226

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Zhang G, Zhang C, Cao S, Xia X, Tan X, Si L, et al. Multi-domain features of the non-phase-locked component of interest extracted from ERP data by tensor decomposition. Brain Topograph. (2020) 33:37–47. doi: 10.1007/s10548-019-00750-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Schriever VA, Han P, Weise S, Hösel F, Pellegrino R. Time frequency analysis of olfactory induced EEG-power change. PLoS ONE. (2017) 12:1–11. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0185596

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Cohen MX. Analyzing Neural Time Series Data: Theory and Practice. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2014). doi: 10.7551/mitpress/9609.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: deaf children, interference control, emotional stroop, event-related potentials, time-frequency analysis

Citation: Chen Q, Zhao J, Gu H and Li X (2022) Inhibitory Control of Emotional Interference in Deaf Children: Evidence From Event-Related Potentials and Event-Related Spectral Perturbation Analysis. Front. Psychiatry 13:897595. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.897595

Received: 16 March 2022; Accepted: 26 May 2022;
Published: 24 June 2022.

Edited by:

Giorgio Di Lorenzo, University of Rome Tor Vergata, Italy

Reviewed by:

Wenhai Zhang, Hengyang Normal University, China
Simone Battaglia, University of Bologna, Italy

Copyright © 2022 Chen, Zhao, Gu and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Junfeng Zhao, amZ6aGFvNjMmI3gwMDA0MDtob3RtYWlsLmNvbQ==; Huang Gu, aHVhbmdndTEwMTcmI3gwMDA0MDtob3RtYWlsLmNvbQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.