Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 28 April 2022
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

Associations Between Chronic Irritability and Sensory Processing Difficulties in Children and Adolescents

\r\nYuji HarimaYuji Harima1Dai Miyawaki*Dai Miyawaki1*Ayako GotoAyako Goto1Kaoru Hirai,Kaoru Hirai1,2Shoko SakamotoShoko Sakamoto1Hiroki HamaHiroki Hama1Shin KadonoShin Kadono1Sayaka NishiuraSayaka Nishiura1Koki Inoue\r\nKoki Inoue1
  • 1Department of Neuropsychiatry, Osaka Metropolitan University Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka, Japan
  • 2Department of Pediatrics, Osaka Metropolitan University Graduate School of Medicine, Osaka, Japan

Irritability is one of the most common reasons for which children and adolescents are referred for psychiatric evaluation and care. However, clinical irritability is difficult to define; thus, its prevalence varies widely. Chronic irritability may be associated with sensory processing difficulties (SPD), but little is known about the relationship between these two factors in clinical populations. In this study, we examined the prevalence of chronic irritability and its association with SPD in 166 children aged 5–16 years who were referred to the psychiatric outpatient clinic of the Osaka City University Hospital. Chronic irritability and parent-reported scores for the Short Sensory Profile, Infant Behavior Checklist-Revised, Child Behavior Checklist, and Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children (Present and Lifetime version) questionnaires were used for assessment. A total of 22 children (13.2%) presented with chronic irritability (i.e., the irritability group) and were more likely to have oppositional defiant disorder, externalizing problems, and attention issues than those without chronic irritability (i.e., the control group). SPD were reported in eight (36%) patients in the irritability group and in 21 (15%) in the control group (p = 0.029). Moreover, compared to the control group, the irritability group showed a significant difference in almost all items of the Short Sensory Profile. Chronic irritability was associated with more severe overall SPD, even after adjusting for possible confounding factors (internalizing and externalizing problems, age, sex, and low income). We provide evidence to support our hypothesis that chronic irritability is associated with SPD in children and adolescents. Therefore, SPD should be assessed to provide appropriate interventions in children and adolescents with chronic irritability.

Introduction

Irritability is one of the most common reasons for which children and adolescents are referred for psychiatric evaluation and care (1). Irritability is present in numerous disorders, including autism spectrum disorder (ASD), oppositional defiant disorder (ODD), attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), anxiety disorders, major depressive disorder, and bipolar disorder (28). Although several questionnaires exist to assess irritability (9), its definition is difficult, and thus its prevalence varies widely. A recent community-based evaluation showed that 99.3% of individuals experienced at least one incident of phasic or chronic irritability through their lifetime, indicating that irritability is a common psychiatric symptom (10). Contrastingly, Althoff et al. found that only 0.1% of adolescents had severe irritability (though this finding was based on the strictest frequency criteria) (11). In the International Classification of Diseases 11th Revision (ICD-11), chronic irritability is listed as a symptom of disruptive behavior or dissocial disorders, while in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth Edition (DSM-5), it is included in both the disruptive, impulse control, and conduct disorder category as well as the depressive disorders section (12). This discrepancy between the ICD-11 and the DSM-5 may reflect the difficulty in classifying chronic irritability (12). On the other hand, the validity of distinguishing between phasic and chronic irritability has been empirically supported (1315). Children and adolescents with chronic irritability not only have externalizing problems (13, 14), but are also at increased risk for academic struggles, poverty, suicidality (16), unipolar depression, and anxiety disorders later in life (13, 14, 1621).

Chronic irritability has recently been suggested to be associated with sensory processing difficulties (SPD) (22). Namely, Benarous et al. reported that children with chronic irritability and temper outbursts exhibited more severe SPD than did typically developing children (22). Meanwhile, SPD is included in the diagnostic criteria for ASD and is prevalent in neurodevelopmental disorders, including ASD and ADHD (2326). Previous studies have also shown that SPD is associated with internalizing (27) and externalizing problems (28, 29); co-occurring disorders such as schizophrenia (30, 31), anxiety disorders, stress-related disorders (32, 33), sleep disturbances (34), and anorexia nervosa (35); and lower age, lower baseline socioeconomic status, and male sex (36, 37). A study in a recent epidemiological survey found that an estimated 8% of school-aged children have experienced SPD (38). These studies suggest that SPD is associated with a variety of mental disorders and not just with ASD. However, little is known about the relationship between chronic irritability and SPD in clinical populations. Moreover, no studies have confirmed this relationship after adjusting for comorbidities and socioeconomic variables, which may act as confounders.

The aims of our study were: (a) to examine the prevalence of chronic irritability as defined by the degree, duration, and frequency of symptoms, and its correlation with comorbidities and psychopathologies in a clinical psychiatric population and (b) to test our hypothesis that chronic irritability is associated with SPD.

Materials and Methods

Participants

A summary of the study enrollment process is presented in Figure 1. The eligible study subjects were 244 children aged 5–17 years who were consecutively referred to the psychiatric outpatient clinic of the Osaka City University Hospital (Osaka, Japan) between April 2018 and April 2021. The participants visited the clinic for at least 3 months and were assessed by a multidisciplinary team that included expert child psychiatrists, psychologists, and psychiatric social workers. We excluded children whose parents were chronically absent or unavailable (n = 8) and those whose parents did not consent to their children participating in our study (n = 10). We also excluded children who refused to participate in semi-structured interviews or who did complete all assessments (n = 45). Children with intellectual disabilities (n = 5) (IQ < 70 based on the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Third or Fourth Edition; WISC-III or -IV) in whom symptom evaluation was difficult, those experiencing acute psychotic states (n = 4), those with severe neurological impairments or refractory epilepsy (n = 4), and those with bipolar disorder (n = 2) were also excluded. The remaining 166 children (aged 5–16 years) were divided into two groups: 22 with and 144 without chronic irritability.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow chart of the patient selection process.

Socioeconomic status has been shown to affect a variety of brain regions, including those associated with language, executive function, and attention, and is also strongly associated with health status (39, 40). Therefore, we conducted interviews to collect information on (1) parental absence (i.e., information on the absence of either the father or the mother), (2) family income, and (3) years of parental education. With regard to family income, we categorized households receiving public assistance or with an annual income of <3 million yen as low income.

We obtained written informed consent from all children and their parents before participation. The research protocol was reviewed and accepted by the Ethical Committee of Osaka City University Graduate School of Medicine. The research was performed according to the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and its later amendments.

Measurement

A multidisciplinary team converged on a diagnosis by integrating the information from the following diagnostic interviews and questionnaires.

Definition and Assessment of Chronic Irritability

For this study, we developed a new semi-structured interview that was administered to the children and their parents separately to assess present chronic irritability. The interview includes the following questions: (1) “Do you feel irritable?” (2) “Have you felt this way almost every day for the past year?” (3) “Is there a period of time, as long as a couple of months, when you didn’t feel this way?” (4) “Do you feel irritable throughout most of the day?” (5) “In how many situations do you feel irritable, at home, at school, with other children?” (6) “Do your parents, teachers, or other children notice your feelings?” We defined chronic irritability as a chronic, persistently irritable mood lasting for at least 1 year in at least two settings. Moreover, this irritability must be present for most of the day nearly every day, and must be noticeable by others in the child’s environment. Our definition of chronic irritability is consistent with the definition of irritable mood within the diagnostic criteria for disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (DMDD) in the DSM-5 (41). When there were discrepancies between a child’s and parent’s report, we queried the child and parent about the incongruent information. When the disagreement was not resolved, we obtained additional information from the teachers. We examined the test–retest and inter-rater reliability of the first battery of interviews by re-obtaining participant answers two months later.

Child Behavior Checklist

In this study, the standard Japanese version of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) was administered to the parents of the affected children as a broad psychopathological assessment. The CBCL is a parent-reported rating scale developed by Achenbach and Dumenci consisting of 113 questions that assess childhood psychopathology (42). The response scale for each question is as follows: 0, not true (as far as the proband knows); 1, somewhat or sometimes true; and 2, very often or often true. The scale is composed of three domains (total, internalizing, and externalizing scores) and eight subscales (withdrawal, somatic complaints, anxiety/depression, social problems, thought problems, attention problems, delinquent behavior, and aggressive behavior).

Short Sensory Profile and Sensory Processing Difficulties

To evaluate SPD, we used the standardized Japanese version of the Short Sensory Profile (SSP), which was administered by the children’s parents. The SSP is a 38-item parent-completed questionnaire created to determine the functional behaviors associated with SPD in children aged 3–10 years within seven sections: tactile sensitivity, taste/smell sensitivity, movement sensitivity, underresponsive/seeks sensation, auditory filtering, low energy/weak, and visual/auditory sensitivity. The SSP asks parents to respond to behavioral descriptions of various sensory-laden events using a 5-point Likert scale (43). The Japanese version of the SSP is an almost faithful translation of the original, although some changes have been made to account for cultural differences. In the Japanese version, the target age range was expanded to over 3 years, with higher scores indicating lower adaptive functioning and a higher frequency of responses.

When a child presents with an SSP total score in the “definite difference” range (scores > 2 standard deviations above the mean), it indicates that the child’s performance is among the lowest 2% of the research sample when compared to the sample of children without disabilities. Based on a previous study (44), we defined a child as having SPD if their SSP total score was within the “definite difference” range.

Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia

The Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorder and Schizophrenia (Present and Lifetime version; K-SADS-PL), a semi-structured diagnostic interview, was administered to all parents by a single study investigator. The K-SADS-PL psychometric properties have been estimated as excellent in prior work, with high inter-rater reliability as indicated by a κ of 0.93 and high test–retest reliability as indicated by intraclass correlation coefficients ranging from 0.74–0.90 (45). Each item is determined as present, absent, or unknown. The diagnostic algorithm was implemented following international guidelines. This diagnostic interview has consistently demonstrated good inter-rater reliability and high concurrent validity across studies (46).

Infant Behavior Checklist-Revised

The Infant Behavior Checklist-Revised (IBC-R) is a 24-item parent-completed questionnaire used to assess traits of autism in infancy, and it is often used for children and adolescents older than two years of age (47, 48). All 24 items are evaluated on a two-point scale and higher scores indicate more problematic behavior. The IBC-R was developed by Kanai et al. (49). They administered the scale to children referred to a psychiatry outpatient clinic (autism sample, N = 68; non-autism sample, N = 63; males, N = 97; females, N = 34; mean age, 4.1 years; standard deviation, 2.2; age range, 0.8–13.7 years). They found that a cutoff score of 7 or more had a 16.9% false negative rate and 15% false positive rate. This analysis suggests that the optimal cutoff score is 7. A total score of 7 or more is indicative of clinically significant autism traits. Psychometric analysis indicated adequate content validity and concurrent validity. Internal consistency was moderate (49).

Statistics

Comparisons were conducted between participants who did or did not present with chronic irritability (n = 22 vs. n = 144). Descriptive statistics (means, standard deviations, medians, ranges, and proportions) were calculated for demographic and clinical variables. Student’s t-tests or Mann–Whitney U tests were used to compare continuous variables as appropriate (i.e., depending on whether the variables followed a normal distribution). Pearson’s χ2 tests and Fisher’s exact tests (implemented when expected values were <5) were used to compare categorical variables. To examine the correlation between chronic irritability and SPD, a forced entry multiple regression analysis was performed with total SSP scores as the dependent variables. The presence of chronic irritability was used as the primary independent variable, and we adjusted for covariates (low income, sex, age at consultation, IBC-R score, CBCL internalizing, and externalizing scores) that have been reported to be associated with SPD and were considered potential confounders herein. Categorical variables (including chronic irritability, low income, and sex) were converted to dummy variables by creating vectors of ones and zeros (one if the characteristic existed or to indicate male sex, and zero otherwise). All data were analyzed with the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) statistical software (version 26.0.0; SPSS Japan, Inc., Tokyo, Japan). A two-sided p-value of <0.05 was considered to be statistically significant for all tests.

Results

Concerning the semi-structured interview that we created to assess chronic irritability, the test–retest reliability was high, with a kappa coefficient of 0.818 (n = 66) within an average 57-day interval and an inter-rater reliability of 0.806 (n = 13) within an average 63-day interval. A total of 22 children (13.3%) displayed chronic irritability. Table 1 presents the comparison of the sociodemographic characteristics, clinical features, and comorbidities of the two groups. No significant difference was found between the groups in terms of gender, years of parental education, parental absence, and low income. There were no significant differences in the IBC-R scores or percentage of children above the cutoff threshold between the two groups. The rates of disruptive behavior disorder (DBD) and ODD were significantly higher in the chronic irritability group than in the clinical control group (45.5 vs. 22.2%, p = 0.020; 45.5 vs. 20.1%, p = 0.009).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant sociodemographic and clinical characteristics.

Table 2 shows the CBCL scores (total, internalizing, and externalizing scores as well as the scores for the eight subscales) in the chronic irritability and clinical control groups. The total and externalizing scores were significantly higher in the former than in the latter group (69.1 vs. 64.5, p = 0.046; 66.4 vs. 58.6, p = 0.001). Regarding the CBCL subscales, three of the eight subscale scores (attention problems, delinquent behavior, and aggressive behavior) were significantly higher in the chronic irritability group than in the clinical control group (66.6 vs. 61.4, p = 0.027; 63.4 vs. 58.5, p = 0.005; 66.0 vs. 59.2, p = 0.000).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. CBCL T scores for the chronic irritability and clinical control groups.

Table 3 shows the comparative prevalence of SPD and the SSP scores (total scores as well as the sub-scores for the seven sections) in the chronic irritability and clinical control groups. The prevalence of SPD and the total SSP scores were significantly higher in the chronic irritability group than in the clinical control group (36.4 vs. 14.6%, p = 0.029; 76.5 vs. 59.7, p = 0.010). With regard to the SSP sections, five of the seven section scores (tactile sensitivity, movement sensitivity, underresponsive/seeks sensation, auditory filtering, and visual/auditory sensitivity) were significantly higher in the chronic irritability group than in the clinical control group (13.5 vs. 10.0, p = 0.004; 5.0 vs. 3.9, p = 0.015; 13.2 vs. 9.8, p = 0.002; 15.1 vs. 11.8, p = 0.016; 9.8 vs. 6.8, p = 0.001).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparison of the prevalence of SPD and Short Sensory Profile scores between the chronic irritability and clinical control groups.

Table 4 shows the results of the forced entry multiple regression analysis. The total SSP scores were predicted by chronic irritability (β = 0.175; p = 0.007), age at consultation (β = -0.166; p = 0.012), low income (β = 0.202; p = 0.002), and internalizing CBCL scores (β = 0.400; p < 0.001).

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Results of multiple regression analysis showing predictors of SSP total scores in the chronic irritability group.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, our study is the first to demonstrate an association between chronic irritability and SPD in clinically referred children and adolescents. Our results suggest two points. First, approximately 13% of the enrolled children presented with chronic irritability. We note that children with chronic irritability often have ODD as well as externalizing and attention problems. Second, children with chronic irritability are more likely to have severe SPD.

As mentioned above, irritability is one of the most common reasons for children and adolescents to be referred for psychiatric evaluation and care (1). Prior studies have reported that the prevalence of irritability varies widely depending on the definition of irritability and the target population. For example, Toohey et al. reported an extremely high prevalence of irritability (99.3%). However, this value was obtained from asking adults in a community population if they had ever felt irritable during their lifetime (10). In contrast, Althoff et al. reported a very low prevalence of irritability (0.12%) when defining irritability based on a combination of frequency, severity, and period data in a community population of children aged 13–18 years (11, 20). This considerable variation in estimates might result from differences within the studies in terms of the frequency of outbursts, duration of irritability, and extent to which these outbursts and moods adhere to the DSM-5 criteria for dysfunction (excluding individuals with mania or hypomania) (50, 51).

To our knowledge, few studies have investigated the prevalence of irritability in clinical samples. The prevalence of six-month chronic irritability presenting with temper outbursts was 26–31% among the evaluated 6–12-year-old children who had been referred to a child psychiatric outpatient clinic (52, 53). This study defined chronic irritability as a severe form of irritability that persisted for a minimum of 1 year in at least two settings. Considering the duration (1 year of irritability) and the inclusion of adolescents with less severe irritability, our primary result (which was only slightly lower than that produced in the abovementioned report) is generally comparable to that reported in previous studies conducted in a clinical setting.

We showed that children with chronic irritability are more likely to have ODD and externalizing problems but are not more likely to have depression, anxiety disorders, or internalizing problems. With regard to DBD, ODD, and externalizing problems, our results are consistent with those of previous studies showing that irritability may be a dimension of ODD (4) and that childhood irritability significantly predicts DBD (54, 55). However, our results concerning internalizing problems, including depression and anxiety, are inconsistent with those of some prior studies conducted in adolescents that have shown associations between irritability and internalizing symptoms (5, 7). One of the potential reasons for this inconsistency is that our participants were younger than those enrolled in prior studies. For example, Copeland et al. have shown that irritability in childhood predicts depression in adulthood more strongly than the presence of a childhood depressive disorder does (56). Other studies have also reported that children with irritability are more likely to have problems with externalization, depression, and anxiety when evaluated longitudinally (21, 55, 57). Thus, our participants might have developed internalizing disorders over the course of several years. In addition, our study showed an association between chronic irritability and attention problems, consistent with the findings of previous research demonstrating that children and adolescents with chronic irritability have attentional impairments in the context of frustrative non-rewards (58).

To the best of our knowledge, this study is the first to show that children with chronic irritability are more likely to develop severe SPD. However, only a few studies have examined the association between chronic irritability and SPD. Benarous et al. (22) showed that children with DMDD, which presents with both chronic irritability and temper outbursts (as based on the diagnostic criteria), scored higher than typically developing children in standardized tests provided by the Sensory Profile manual, thus suggesting an association between chronic irritability and SPD. However, these studies reported no differences in Sensory Profile scores between children with and without DMDD in clinically referred children and adolescents. Therefore, the effect of chronic irritability on SPD remains unclear.

Contrastingly, the present study suggests that children with chronic irritability have more severe SPD in clinical settings. One potential reason for this discrepancy is the differences in participant characteristics and presentations between studies. For example, temper outbursts (one of the two core features of DMDD) may present rapidly increasing tantrums with episodic characteristics. This is in contrast to the tonic characteristics of chronic irritability. Cardinale (15) suggested that temper outbursts and chronic irritability might be distinct, although highly related, irritability dimensions. Second, the participants in the study by Benarous displayed suicidal behaviors, possibly making it difficult to obtain information on the effects of irritability from these subjects. Chronic irritability may have a more direct effect on SPD than temper outbursts. Further, this study demonstrated that chronic irritability is associated with more severe overall SPD, even after adjusting for IBC-R scores, internalizing and externalizing problems, age, sex, and low income. Some previous studies have found that SPD is more prevalent in children with internalizing (27) and externalizing problems (29) and in those of lower age, male sex, and lower socioeconomic status (2729, 36, 59). Based on these studies, we conclude that the association between chronic irritability and SPD, which was detected in this study after adjusting for possible confounding factors, is reliable. Although the causal relationship between chronic irritability and SPD remains unknown, we conclude that SPD may facilitate emotional dysregulation, thus leading to the manifestation of chronic irritability in children (22). In this study, only the low energy/weak section showed no association with chronic irritability; this finding is consistent with the findings reported by Benarous (22). As the low energy/weak section is associated with poor muscle development and postural control (60), it is less strongly associated with irritability than the other sections. Irritability and SPD may share a common neurobiological basis. The pathophysiology of irritability comprises a background of aberrant reward processing, deficits of reward learning and prediction error, and cognitive control deficits (58, 6164), indicating dysfunction in the prefrontal cortex, striatum, anterior cingulate, amygdala, and middle frontal gyrus (6466). On the other hand, the integration of information from different sensory modalities is termed multisensory processing (67). Multisensory processing is associated with functions of the anterior cingulate, middle and inferior frontal gyri, and hippocampus (68, 69). Although no neurobiological studies in humans have examined the association between irritability and SPD, this association may be explained in future studies with reference to a common neurobiological basis as described above.

Our study has several limitations. First, we did not exclude participants with ASD, who often have sensory abnormalities (37). Moreover, we employed the K-SADS-PL DSM-IV-TR because the K-SADS-PL DSM-5 has yet to be translated to Japanese. ASD as a comorbidity may be a confounding factor that could influence the observed associations between chronic irritability and SPD. However, this association remained significant even after adjusting for IBC-R scores, which reflect the severity of ASD traits, thus reducing the possibility of confounding. We highly recommend that future studies use diagnostic tools for ASD, such as the Autism Diagnostic Interview (Revised). Second, this study included clinically referred children from a single university hospital, thereby warranting careful interpretation with regard to the generalization of our results to all children with chronic irritability. Third, the semi-structured interview for chronic irritability we developed was not completely validated. It was only assessed as a categorical variable and not as a continuous variable. Further, this study did not have a large sample of children with chronic irritability. However, we examined the inter-rater and test-retest reliability as well as the content validity by integrating information obtained from the children’s teachers. In fact, the current definitions of irritability and its related constructs have often been used interchangeably and non-rigorously in past studies (7072). Unfortunately, there are no well-validated Japanese versions of continuous measures of irritability such as the Affective Reactivity Index. In the future, it is recommended that diagnostic tools for irritability such as the Affective Reactivity Index be used (3, 9). Fourth, our sample size is small, leading to the possibility of sampling bias and lack of statistical power. As noted above, this sample included children referred to the psychiatric outpatient clinic of the university hospital. Regarding the strength and robustness of our results, comorbidities were assessed in this study based on definite criteria using validated semi-structured interviews. Since we performed the Short Sensory Profile and assessed SPD as continuous variable, there were certain contributions to the final analysis.

In conclusion, this study provides evidence supporting our initial hypothesis that chronic irritability is associated with SPD in both children and adolescents. Since children with chronic irritability may often have SPD, it is necessary to assess SPD in order to administer appropriate interventions to children with chronic irritability. Additional longitudinal studies are needed to clarify the underlying mechanisms and to determine whether SPD develops into chronic irritability. We believe that our findings will guide future research directions and, if confirmed, ultimately inform clinical guidelines.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Ethical Committee of Osaka City University Graduate School of Medicine. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

DM and YH were involved in the conception and design of the study and wrote the original draft. DM, YH, AG, KH, SS, HH, SK, and SN conducted data curation. DM, YH, AG, KH, SS, HH, SK, and KI were involved in the analysis, validation, or interpretation of data. All authors substantially revised the report for important intellectual content, improved the manuscript drafts, approved the final report, and accepted responsibility for all parts of this study to ensure that any questions regarding their accuracy or completeness are properly investigated and resolved.

Funding

This work was supported by JSPS KAKENHI (Grant Number JP20K03002).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Peterson BS, Zhang HP, Santa Lucia RS, King RA, Lewis M. Risk factors for presenting problems in child psychiatric emergencies. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1996) 35:1162–73. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199609000-00014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Ambrosini PJ, Bennett DS, Elia J. Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder characteristics: II. Clinical correlates of irritable mood. J Affect Disord. (2013) 145:70–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.07.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Evans SC, Abel MR, Doyle RL, Skov H, Harmon SL. Measurement and correlates of irritability in clinically referred youth: further examination of the affective reactivity index. J Affect Disord. (2021) 283:420–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Burke JD, Boylan K, Rowe R, Duku E, Stepp SD, Hipwell AE, et al. Identifying the irritability dimension of ODD: application of a modified bifactor model across five large community samples of children. J Abnorm Psychol. (2014) 123:841–51. doi: 10.1037/a0037898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Jensen PS, Youngstrom EA, Steiner H, Findling RL, Meyer RE, Malone RP, et al. Consensus report on impulsive aggression as a symptom across diagnostic categories in child psychiatry: implications for medication studies. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2007) 46:309–22. doi: 10.1097/chi.0b013e31802f1454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2014) 171:276–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Stoddard J, Stringaris A, Brotman MA, Montville D, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Irritability in child and adolescent anxiety disorders. Depress Anxiety. (2014) 31:566–73. doi: 10.1002/da.22151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Orri M, Galera C, Turecki G, Forte A, Renaud J, Boivin M, et al. Association of childhood irritability and depressive/anxious mood profiles with adolescent suicidal ideation and attempts. JAMA Psychiatry. (2018) 75:465–73. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2018.0174

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Stringaris A, Vidal-Ribas P, Brotman MA, Leibenluft E. Practitioner review: definition, recognition, and treatment challenges of irritability in young people. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2018) 59:721–39. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12823

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Toohey MJ. Irritability characteristics and parameters in an international sample. J Affect Disord. (2020) 263:558–67. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2019.11.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Althoff RR, Crehan ET, He JP, Burstein M, Hudziak JJ, Merikangas KR. Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder at ages 13–18: results from the national comorbidity survey—adolescent supplement. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. (2016) 26:107–13. doi: 10.1089/cap.2015.0038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Evans SC, Roberts MC, Keeley JW, Rebello TJ, de la Peña F, Lochman JE, et al. Diagnostic classification of irritability and oppositionality in youth: a global field study comparing ICD-11 with ICD-10 and DSM-5. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2021) 62:303–12. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.13244

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Leibenluft E, Cohen P, Gorrindo T, Brook JS, Pine DS. Chronic versus episodic irritability in youth: a community-based, longitudinal study of clinical and diagnostic associations. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. (2006) 16:456–66. doi: 10.1089/cap.2006.16.456

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Stringaris A, Cohen P, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Adult outcomes of youth irritability: a 20-year prospective community-based study. Am J Psychiatry. (2009) 166:1048–54. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08121849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Cardinale EM, Freitag GF, Brotman MA, Pine DS, Leibenluft E, Kircanski K. Phasic versus tonic irritability: differential associations with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2021) 60:1513–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Conner KR, Meldrum S, Wieczorek WF, Duberstein PR, Welte JW. The association of irritability and impulsivity with suicidal ideation among 15- to 20-year-old males. Suicide Life Threat Behav. (2004) 34:363–73. doi: 10.1521/suli.34.4.363.53745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Egger H, Angold A, Costello EJ. Adult diagnostic and functional outcomes of DSM-5 disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2014) 171:668–74. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.13091213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Cornacchio D, Crum KI, Coxe S, Pincus DB, Comer JS. Irritability and severity of anxious symptomatology among youth with anxiety disorders. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2016) 55:54–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2015.10.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Nock MK, Kazdin AE, Hiripi E, Kessler RC. Lifetime prevalence, correlates, and persistence of oppositional defiant disorder: results from the national comorbidity survey replication. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2007) 48:703–13. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01733.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Pickles A, Aglan A, Collishaw S, Messer J, Rutter M, Maughan B. Predictors of suicidality across the life span: the isle of wight study. Psychol Med. (2010) 40:1453–66. doi: 10.1017/S0033291709991905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Brotman MA, Schmajuk M, Rich BA, Dickstein DP, Guyer AE, Costello EJ, et al. Prevalence, clinical correlates, and longitudinal course of severe mood dysregulation in children. Biol Psychiatry. (2006) 60:991–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.08.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Benarous X, Bury V, Lahaye H, Desrosiers L, Cohen D, Guilé JM. Sensory processing difficulties in youths with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:164. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Rogers SJ, Ozonoff S. Annotation: what do we know about sensory dysfunction in autism? A critical review of the empirical evidence. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2005) 46:1255–68. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01431.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Allen S, Casey J. Developmental coordination disorders and sensory processing and integration: incidence, associations and co-morbidities. Br J Occup Ther. (2017) 80:549–57. doi: 10.1177/0308022617709183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Parush S, Sohmer H, Steinberg A, Kaitz M. Somatosensory functioning in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. (1997) 39:464–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1997.tb07466.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Lane SJ, Reynolds S. Sensory over-responsivity as an added dimension in ADHD. Front Integr Neurosci. (2019) 13:40. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2019.00040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Goldsmith HH, Van Hulle CA, Arneson CL, Schreiber JE, Gernsbacher MA. A population-based twin study of parentally reported tactile and auditory defensiveness in young children. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2006) 34:393–407. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9024-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mangeot SD, Miller LJ, McIntosh DN, McGrath-Clarke J, Simon J, Hagerman RJ, et al. Sensory modulation dysfunction in children with attention-deficit-hyperactivity disorder. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2001) 43:399–406. doi: 10.1017/s0012162201000743

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Gunn TE, Tavegia BD, Houskamp BM, Mcdonald LB, Bustrum JM, Welsh RK, et al. Relationship between sensory deficits and externalizing behaviors in an urban, latino preschool population. J Child Fam Stud. (2009) 18:653–61. doi: 10.1007/s10826-009-9266-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. McGhie A, Chapman J. Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia. Br J Med Psychol. (1961) 34:103–16. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8341.1961.tb00936.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Brown C, Cromwell RL, Filion D, Dunn W, Tollefson N. Sensory processing in schizophrenia: missing and avoiding information. Schizophr Res. (2002) 55:187–95. doi: 10.1016/s0920-9964(01)00255-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Liss M, Mailloux J, Erchull MJ. The relationships between sensory processing sensitivity, alexithymia, autism, depression, and anxiety. Pers Individ Dif. (2008) 45:255–9. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2008.04.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Yano K, Kase T, Oishi K. The effects of sensory-processing sensitivity and sense of coherence on depressive symptoms in university students. Health Psychol Open. (2019) 6:2055102919871638. doi: 10.1177/2055102919871638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Mazurek MO, Petroski GF. Sleep problems in children with autism spectrum disorder: examining the contributions of sensory over-responsivity and anxiety. Sleep Med. (2015) 16:270–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2014.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Merwin RM, Moskovich AA, Wagner HR, Ritschel LA, Craighead LW, Zucker NL. Emotion regulation difficulties in anorexia nervosa: relationship to self-perceived sensory sensitivity. Cogn Emot. (2013) 27:441–52. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2012.719003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Gouze KR, Hopkins J, Lebailly SA, Lavigne JV. Re-examining the epidemiology of sensory regulation dysfunction and comorbid psychopathology. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2009) 37:1077–87. doi: 10.1007/s10802-009-9333-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Baranek GT, Boyd BA, Poe MD, David FJ, Watson LR. Hyperresponsive sensory patterns in young children with autism, developmental delay, and typical development. Am J Ment Retard. (2007) 112:233–45. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2007)112[233:HSPIYC]2.0.CO;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Jussila K, Junttila M, Kielinen M, Ebeling H, Joskitt L, Moilanen I, et al. Sensory abnormality and quantitative autism traits in children with and without autism spectrum disorder in an epidemiological population. J Autism Dev Disord. (2020) 50:180–8. doi: 10.1007/s10803-019-04237-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cogn Sci. (2009) 13:65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Kondo N, Sembajwe G, Kawachi I, Van Dam RM, Subramanian SV, Yamagata Z. Income inequality, mortality, and self rated health: meta-analysis of multilevel studies. BMJ. (2009) 339:b4471. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b4471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Association D-AP. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Arlington, TX: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013).

Google Scholar

42. Achenbach TM, Dumenci L. Advances in empirically based assessment: revised cross-informant syndromes and new DSM-oriented scales for the CBCL, YSR, and TRF: comment on Lengua, Sadowksi, Friedrich, and Fischer (2001). J Consult Clin Psychol. (2001) 69:699–702. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.69.4.699

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Ahn RR, Miller LJ, Milberger S, McIntosh DN. Prevalence of parents’ perceptions of sensory processing disorders among kindergarten children. Am J Occup Ther. (2004) 58:287–93. doi: 10.5014/ajot.58.3.287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Gourley L, Wind C, Henninger EM, Chinitz S. Sensory processing difficulties, behavioral problems, and parental stress in a clinical population of young children. J Child Fam Stud. (2013) 22:912–21. doi: 10.1007/s10826-012-9650-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, et al. Schedule for affective disorders and schizophrenia for school-age children-present and lifetime version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (1997) 36:980–8. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199707000-00021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Takahashi K, Miyawaki D, Suzuki F, Mamoto A, Matsushima N, Tsuji H, et al. Hyperactivity and comorbidity in Japanese children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2007) 61:255–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01651.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Kawabe K, Kondo S, Matsumoto M, Seo K, Ochi M, Oka Y, et al. Developmental quotient to estimate intelligence in autism spectrum disorder. Pediatr Int. (2016) 58:963–6. doi: 10.1111/ped.12969

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Azuma H, Mohri I, Tachibana M, Ohno Y, Taniike M. An analysis of the developmental trajectory of children with autism spectrum disorders. No To Hattatsu. (2014) 46:429–37. doi: 10.11251/ojjscn.46.429

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Kanai C, Osada H, Koyama T, Kurita H. A study of utility of infant behavior checklist revised (IBC-R) as a screening scale for pervasive developmental disorders. Rinsho Seishinigaku. (2004) 33:313–21.

Google Scholar

50. Leibenluft E. Severe mood dysregulation, irritability, and the diagnostic boundaries of bipolar disorder in youths. Am J Psychiatry. (2011) 168:129–42. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.10050766

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Evans CA, Nelson LJ, Porter CL. Making sense of their world: sensory reactivity and novelty awareness as aspects of temperament and correlates of social behaviours in early childhood. Inf Child Dev. (2012) 21:503–20. doi: 10.1002/icd.1754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Axelson D, Findling RL, Fristad MA, Kowatch RA, Youngstrom EA, Horwitz SM, et al. Examining the proposed disruptive mood dysregulation disorder diagnosis in children in the longitudinal assessment of manic symptoms study. J Clin Psychiatry. (2012) 73:1342–50. doi: 10.4088/JCP.12m07674

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Freeman AJ, Youngstrom EA, Youngstrom JK, Findling RL. Disruptive mood dysregulation disorder in a community mental health clinic: prevalence, comorbidity and correlates. J Child Adolesc Psychopharmacol. (2016) 26:123–30. doi: 10.1089/cap.2015.0061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Burke JD, Johnston OG, Butler EJ. The irritable and oppositional dimensions of oppositional defiant disorder integral factors in the explanation of affective and behavioral psychopathology. Child Adolesc Psychiatr N Am. (2021) 30:637–47. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2021.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Dougherty LR, Smith VC, Bufferd SJ, Kessel E, Carlson GA, Klein DN. Preschool irritability predicts child psychopathology, functional impairment, and service use at age nine. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2015) 56:999–1007. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Copeland WE, Shanahan L, Costello EJ, Angold A. Childhood and adolescent psychiatric disorders as predictors of young adult disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2009) 66:764–72. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.85

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Stringaris A, Goodman R. Longitudinal outcome of youth oppositionality: irritable, headstrong, and hurtful behaviors have distinctive predictions. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2009) 48:404–12. doi: 10.1097/CHI.0b013e3181984f30

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Rich BA, Schmajuk M, Perez-Edgar KE, Fox NA, Pine DS, Leibenluft E. Different psychophysiological and behavioral responses elicited by frustration in pediatric bipolar disorder and severe mood dysregulation. Am J Psychiatry. (2007) 164:309–17. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.2.309

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Dunn W. The impact of sensory processing abilities on the daily lives of young children and their families: a conceptual model. Infants Young Child. (1997) 9:23–35. doi: 10.1097/00001163-199704000-00005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Rogers SJ, Hepburn S, Wehner E. Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2003) 33:631–42. doi: 10.1023/b:jadd.0000006000.38991.a7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Adleman NE, Kayser R, Dickstein D, Blair RJR, Pine D, Leibenluft E. Neural correlates of reversal learning in severe mood dysregulation and pediatric bipolar disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2011) 50:1173–85.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Perlman SB, Jones BM, Wakschlag LS, Axelson D, Birmaher B, Phillips ML. Neural substrates of child irritability in typically developing and psychiatric populations. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2015) 14:71–80. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2015.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Amsel A. The role of frustrative nonreward in noncontinuous reward situations. Psychol Bull. (1958) 55:102–19. doi: 10.1037/h0043125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Pawliczek CM, Derntl B, Kellermann T, Gur RC, Schneider F, Habel U. Anger under control: neural correlates of frustration as a function of trait aggression. PLoS One. (2013) 8:e78503. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. O’Doherty J, Dayan P, Schultz J, Deichmann R, Friston KJ, Dolan RJ. Dissociable roles of ventral and dorsal striatum in instrumental conditioning. Science. (2004) 304:452–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1094285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Nakamura K, Hikosaka O. Role of dopamine in the primate caudate nucleus in reward modulation of saccades. J Neurosci. (2006) 26:5360–9. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4853-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Wallace MT, Meredith MA, Stein BE. Converging influences from visual, auditory, and somatosensory cortices onto output neurons of the superior colliculus. J Neurophysiol. (1993) 69:1797–809. doi: 10.1152/jn.1993.69.6.1797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Tanaka E, Inui K, Kida T, Miyazaki T, Takeshima Y, Kakigi R. A transition from unimodal to multimodal activations in four sensory modalities in humans: an electrophysiological study. BMC Neurosci. (2008) 9:116. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Diaconescu AO, Alain C, McIntosh AR. The co-occurrence of multisensory facilitation and cross-modal conflict in the human brain. J Neurophysiol. (2011) 106:2896–909. doi: 10.1152/jn.00303.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Barata PC, Holtzman S, Cunningham S, O’Connor BP, Stewart DE. Building a definition of irritability from academic definitions and lay descriptions. Emot Rev. (2016) 8:164–72. doi: 10.1177/1754073915576228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Holtzman S, O’Connor BP, Barata PC, Stewart DE. The brief irritability test (BITe): a measure of irritability for use among men and women. Assessment. (2015) 22:101–15. doi: 10.1177/1073191114533814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Toohey MJ, DiGiuseppe R. Defining and measuring irritability: construct clarification and differentiation. Clin Psychol Rev. (2017) 53:93–108. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: chronic irritability, mood dysregulation, sensory processing difficulties, sensory over-responsivity, short sensory profile, children behavior checklist (CBCL)

Citation: Harima Y, Miyawaki D, Goto A, Hirai K, Sakamoto S, Hama H, Kadono S, Nishiura S and Inoue K (2022) Associations Between Chronic Irritability and Sensory Processing Difficulties in Children and Adolescents. Front. Psychiatry 13:860278. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.860278

Received: 22 January 2022; Accepted: 05 April 2022;
Published: 28 April 2022.

Edited by:

Roberto Canitano, Siena University Hospital, Italy

Reviewed by:

David Houghton, University of Texas Medical Branch at Galveston, United States
Tjhin Wiguna, University of Indonesia, Indonesia
Lucres Nauta-Jansen, Amsterdam University Medical Center, Netherlands

Copyright © 2022 Harima, Miyawaki, Goto, Hirai, Sakamoto, Hama, Kadono, Nishiura and Inoue. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dai Miyawaki, miyawakidai@omu.ac.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.