Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 08 March 2022
Sec. Aging Psychiatry

Interactive Effects of Agitation and Cognitive Impairment on Odor Identification in Patients With Late-Life Depression

\nSi Zhang&#x;Si Zhang1Ben Chen&#x;Ben Chen1Xiaomei Zhong&#x;Xiaomei Zhong1Min ZhangMin Zhang1Qiang WangQiang Wang1Zhangying WuZhangying Wu1Le HouLe Hou2Huarong ZhouHuarong Zhou2Xinru ChenXinru Chen2Meiling LiuMeiling Liu1Mingfeng YangMingfeng Yang1Gaohong LinGaohong Lin1Thomas HummelThomas Hummel3Yuping Ning,,
Yuping Ning1,4,5*
  • 1Department of Geriatric Psychiatry, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University, Memory Clinic, Guangzhou, China
  • 2Department of Neurology, The Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University, Guangzhou, China
  • 3Department of Otorhinolaryngology, Technische Universität Dresden, Smell and Taste Clinic, Dresden, Germany
  • 4The First School of Clinical Medicine, Southern Medical University, Guangzhou, China
  • 5Guangdong Engineering Technology Research Center for Translational Medicine of Mental Disorders, Guangzhou, China

Background: Late-life depression (LLD) is a risk factor for cognitive decline in older adults, and odor identification (OI) deficits are an early indicator of cognitive decline with LLD. However, neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPSs) are common in LLD and are associated with OI deficits. In subjects with LLD, when OI deficits forecast cognitive decline, whether and how NPS affects the relationship between OI and cognition still must be further explored.

Objective: To comprehensively explore the potential effects of various NPSs on the relationship between OI and cognition in participants with LLD.

Methods: There were 167 patients with LLD and 105 normal elderly (NE) participants. The odor identification test (Sniffin' Sticks), cognitive function assessments (global cognition, memory, executive function, attention, language, visual space), and an NPS assessment (the neuropsychiatric inventory questionnaire) were performed on the subjects. In patients with LLD, the relationship among OI, cognition and NPSs was examined using correlation analysis and moderation analysis.

Results: In patients with LLD, OI was positively correlated with cognition (global cognition, memory, executive function, attention, language) and negatively associated with NPSs (agitation and aberrant motor behavior). In NE group, OI was correlated with executive function. Moderation analysis showed that there was an interactive effect of agitation and cognitive impairment (language deficit or attention deficit) on OI in patients with LLD.

Conclusion: The coexistence of agitation and language or attention deficit was associated with worse OI in subjects with LLD. Agitation should be considered since OI predicts cognitive decline in patients with LLD.

Introduction

The current prevalence of late-life depression (LLD) is 20% among older adults in China (1). Subjects with LLD have a variety of cognitive deficits, such as deficits memory function, executive function and visuospatial function (2). Previous studies have shown that subjects with LLD were more likely than normal older adults to develop major neurocognitive disorders (3), and a follow-up study suggested that an increased risk of all-cause mortality was affiliated with cognitive impairment and neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPSs) with LLD (4). Therefore, to intervene in a timely manner and enhance quality of life, screening for subjects with LLD who are at high risk for developing dementia in the early stage is essential.

Odor identification (OI) has been regarded as an early marker of preclinical major neurocognitive disorders (5, 6). A review of studies has shown that many brain regions in the olfactory circuit play an important role in cognitive processes, and that damage to these brain regions could lead to simultaneous impairment of cognition and olfactory function (7). The predictive power of OI deficits for developing dementia has been proved in normal elderly individuals (8) and individuals with subjective cognitive decline (SCD) (9), mild cognitive impairment (MCI) (10). Interestingly, our previous studies suggested that OI deficits might also act as a promising predictor of the development of major neurocognitive disorder in subjects with LLD, who with impaired OI not only exhibited poorer cognitive function, but also had more functional and structural impairments in brain regions (11, 12).

However, there is growing evidence showing that olfactory impairment is also associated with other NPSs, which are very common in elderly individuals and might increase the risk of major neurocognitive disorders (1315). A previous systematic review suggested that 35 to 85% of MCI subjects worldwide have NPSs (16), and the incidence of NPSs is as high as 90% at different stages of AD (17). Moreover, it has been reported that various types of NPSs are associated with olfactory dysfunction, such as agitation, apathy, anxiety and loss of appetite (18, 19). Our previous study suggested that, for elderly individuals, depression moderates the relationship between cognitive impairment and OI (11). In addition, OI dysfunction occurs in many neuropsychiatric diseases, including frontotemporal dementia (20), schizophrenia (21), bipolar disorders (22), autism spectrum disorder (23). Furthermore, behavioral and psychological symptoms in AD, such as agitation, anxiety, and irritability (24), would change upon exposure to fragrance (25).

Overall, there are many joint processing stages among OI dysfunction, cognitive impairment and NPS, and it remains unclear whether and how other NPSs might influence the relationship between OI and cognitive impairment with LLD. Therefore, the aim of this study was to thoroughly explore the potential effects of various NPSs on the relationship between OI and cognition in subjects with LLD. The present results clarify the confounding effects of which NPSs should be considered when applying OI dysfunction to predict the decline of cognitive function with LLD, and to provide a more comprehensive and novel method to understand how OI, cognition and NPS interact with each other.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

This study included 167 patients with LLD who came from the Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University and 105 normal elderly (NE) adults from the community center. All of the subjects signed informed consent forms after learning about the research content. This study was approved by the ethics committee of the Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University. The inclusion criteria for the participants with LLD were as follows: (1) age ≥ 55 years old; (2) meeting the criteria for major depressive disorder in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV); and (3) diagnosed by a clinical psychiatrist. NE participants had normal cognitive function and no history of depression. The exclusion criteria for LLD and NE were as follows: (1) meeting any other diagnostic criteria from the DSM-IV; (2) having hypothyroidism, vitamin B12 deficiency, folic acid deficiency, or syphilis infection; (3) having a history of drug or alcohol abuse; (4) having a neurological disease, such as stroke, white matter injuries, Parkinson's disease, brain tumor, and hydrocephalus; (5) a history of traumatic brain injury; and (6) any condition significantly influencing olfaction, such as sinusitis, nasal polyps, intranasal tumors, congenital or acquired anosmia. All of the subjects were diagnosed by two neuropsychiatrists, one neuropsychologist, and one psychiatrist.

Neuropsychological Assessments

The neuropsychological assessments included the following: (1) global cognition: Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE); (2) memory: Auditory Verbal Learning Test (AVLT) (Sum of N1, N2, N3, N4 and N5 scores); (3) executive function: Trail Making Test (TMT) A and B; (4) attention: Digital Span Test (DST); (5) language ability: animal Verbal Fluency Test (VFT); and (6) visual space skill: Clock Drawing Test (CDT). Because the TMT-A and TMT-B are tested under a time limit, less time shows better capability, and assessing this capability uses the inverse of the scores. NPSs were evaluated using the neuropsychiatric inventory questionnaire (NPI). The total score of the NPI is the sum of the scores for 12 items: delusions, hallucinations, agitation, dysphoria, anxiety, euphoria, apathy, disinhibition, irritability, aberrant motor behavior, nighttime behavioral disturbances, and appetite. The score for each symptom is the product of frequency and severity. Depressive symptoms were assessed by the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale-17 (HAMD-17).

Olfactory Assessments

The Sniffin' Sticks Screen 16 test evaluates OI (26), and 16 pens had different odors. Subjects smelled one pen, they smelled the next pen after 20 s, and they smelled each pen ~3–4 s. After smelling a pen, the subjects were given four options to define the odor they had just smelled. To avoid the effects of eating on olfactory function, subjects were forbidden to eat any food (including water) for 30 min. The subjects completed an investigation of whether there was a history of impairment of olfactory function. One the day of the neuropsychological assessments, the subjects were given the OI test.

Statistical Analyses

SPSS software, version 25.0 (IBM, Chicago, Illinois, USA) was used for all analyses, and statistical significance was defined as p < 0.05. Continuous variables are represented as the means ± standard deviations, and categorical variables are represented as percentages and numbers. One-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was used to compare demographic and clinical variables between the LLD and NE groups for the continuous variables. The chi-square (χ2) test was used to compare demographic and clinical variables between the LLD and NE groups for the categorical variables. Partial correlations were used to analyze the associations among OI, cognitive scores and NPI scores, adjusted for sex, and years of education.

To detect the interaction effect, PROCESS software (27) was used to analyze regressions. The model entered predictor variables (each item's cognitive score) and the interaction term (each item's NPI score) at the same time. OI served as an outcome variable, and age was a control variable in the model. Regression equations were constructed to perform simple slope analysis. The relationship between the predictor variable and the outcome variable was explored at high (+1 SD) and low (−1 SD) levels of the interacting variable. Confidence intervals determined the significance of the results explored by PROCESS software. Zero was not in the significant interval.

Results

Demographic Data

The demographic data of the NE and LLD groups are listed in Table 1. There were no significant differences in age, smoking history, hypertension history, or diabetes history between the NE and LLD groups (p > 0.05). The NE group had a significantly lower percentage of women, more years of education, higher OI scores and cognitive scores and lower NPI scores than the LLD group (p < 0.05) (see Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data, clinical information, cognitive and olfactory function.

Correlation Analyses

OI was associated (all p values < 0.05) with all cognitive scores in all subjects, including MMSE (r = 0.31), AVLT N1-N5 total (r = 0.32), 1/TMTA-Time (r = 0.25), 1/TMTB-Time (r = 0.25), VFT (r = 0.21), CDT (r = 0.14), and DST (r = 0.18). Furthermore, OI was correlated with NPI scores, including agitation (r = −0.18), dysphoria (r = −0.14), anxiety (r = −0.12), apathy (r = −0.14), and aberrant motor behavior (r = −0.16). In the NE group, OI was correlated with 1/TMTA-Time (r = 0.23) and 1/TMTB-Time (r = 0.21). In the LLD group, OI was correlated with MMSE (r = 0.37), AVLT N1-N5 total (r = 0.35), 1/TMTA-Time (r = 0.18), 1/TMTB-Time (r = 0.20), VFT (r = 0.29), and DST (r = 0.22); OI was associated with NPI scores, including agitation (r = −0.20) and aberrant motor behavior (r = −0.17) (see Table 2; Figure 1).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Correlations between OI and cognition, OI and NPI.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Correlations among OI, cognitive scores and NPI scores. In the NE group, OI was correlated with 1/TMTA (r = 0.231, p = 0.020) and 1/TMTB (r = 0.212, p = 0.034). In the LLD group, OI was correlated with MMSE (r = 0.369, p < 0.001), AVLT N1-N5 total (r = 0.351, p < 0.001), 1/TMTA (r = 0.184, p = 0.025), 1/TMTB (r = 0.195, p = 0.019), VFT (r = 0.288, p < 0.001), and DST (r = 0.220, p = 0.007); OI was correlated with NPI scores, including agitation (r = −0.200, p = 0.010) and aberrant motor behavior (r = −0.165, p = 0.035). LLD, late-life depression; NE, normal elderly; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; AVLT, Auditory Verbal Learning; TMT, Trail-Making Test; VFT, Verbal Fluency Test; DST, Digital Span Test.

Testing the Hypothesized Interaction

Considering the significant associations between OI and two NPI symptoms (agitation and aberrant motor behavior) in LLD, we further analyzed the interactive effects of these two NPI items and cognitive impairment on OI. In the NE group, OI was not related to NPI symptoms, so the interaction analysis could not be performed.

In the LLD group, when VFT served as a predictor variable, OI was significantly correlated with age (B = −0.091, 95% CI [−0.150, −0.032], p < 0.05), VFT (B = 0.177, 95% CI [0.069, 0.285], p < 0.05), and agitation (B = −0.210, 95% CI [−0.386, −0.035], p < 0.05). Additionally, there was an interaction between VFT and agitation (B = 0.054, 95% CI [0.012, 0.096], p < 0.05) on OI (R2 = 0.197; p < 0.05) (see Table 3). Follow-up simple slope analysis revealed that the positive association between VFT and OI became significantly stronger as agitation increased from low (−1 SD; B = 0.108, 95% CI [−0.014, 0.229], p > 0.05) to high (+1 SD; B = 0.305, 95% CI [0.159, 0.450], p < 0.05) values (see Figure 2).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Multiple linear regression analysis of the potential impact factors on OI with LLD.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The moderating effect of agitation and VFT on OI with LLD. (A) The interactive model among agitation, VFT and OI. There is an interactive effect of VFT*Agitation (β = 0.054, p = 0.012) on OI. (B) When agitation increased from low (−1 SD; B = 0.108) to high (+1 SD; B = 0.305) values, the positive association between VFT and OI became significantly stronger by follow-up simple slope analysis. LLD, late-life depression; VFT, Verbal Fluency Test; OI, odor identification.

In the LLD group, when DST served as a predictor variable, OI was significantly correlated with age (B = −0.092, 95% CI [−0.149, −0.034], p < 0.05), DST (B = 0.242, 95% CI [0.067, 0.417], p < 0.05), and agitation (B = −0.193, 95% CI [−0.374, −0.011], p < 0.05). Additionally, there was an interaction between DST and agitation (B = 0.085, 95% CI [0.010, 0.160], p < 0.05) on OI (R2 = 0.168; p < 0.05) (see Table 3). Follow-up simple slopes analysis revealed that the positive association between DST and OI became significantly stronger as agitation increased from low (−1 SD; B = 0.134, 95% CI [−0.059, 0.327], p > 0.05) to high (+1 SD; B = 0.440, 95% CI [0.183, 0.696], p < 0.05) values (see Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The interactive effect of agitation and DST on OI with LLD. (A) The interactive model among agitation, DST and OI. There is an interactive effect of DST*Agitation (β = 0.085, p = 0.027) on OI. (B) When agitation increased from low (−1 SD; B = 0.134) to high (+1 SD; B = 0.440) values, the positive association between DST and OI became significantly stronger by follow-up simple slopes analysis. DST, Digital Span Test; OI, odor identification; LLD, late-life depression.

When other cognitive scores (MMSE, AVLT N1-N5, 1/TMTA-Time, 1/TMTB-Time) were used as predictor variables in the LLD group, there were no significant correlations between cognitive scores and agitation, and the interaction terms between cognitive scores and each of agitation symptoms were not associated with OI, including MMSE*agitation (p = 0.113), AVLT N1-N5*agitation (p = 0.584), 1/TMTA-Time *agitation (p = 0.222), 1/TMTB-Time (p = 0.789) and 1/TMTB-Time *agitation (p = 0.006). Furthermore, no significant interactive effect was found in aberrant motor behavior regarding the associations between OI deficits and cognitive impairment.

Discussion

The present study comprehensively analyzed the interactive effects of NPSs and cognitive impairment on OI in patients with LLD and mainly found three results. First, in different cognitive domains, global cognition, memory, executive function, language, and attention performance were positively associated with OI scores in patients with LLD. Second, OI scores were negatively correlated with NPI scores, including agitation and aberrant motor behavior in patients with LLD. Third, the complication by agitation of cognitive impairment (language or attention deficit) would lead to worse OI, suggesting a relationship between OI and cognitive function changes as agitation increases or decreases in patients with LLD.

Consistent with our previous research in elderly individuals (11, 12, 28), there is a positive correlation between OI and cognitive performance. OI deficits could serve as a marker of cognitive impairment (8, 10) because the olfactory pathway and cognitive circuit share many brain areas (7). Murphy provided a hypothesis about how olfactory deficits interact with cognitive impairment: if a patient with olfactory dysfunction wants to remember or identify an odor, the olfactory and brain areas with cognitive function will be hyperactive. In one's life, neurodegeneration leads to structural abnormalities of the brain with cognitive impairment and accelerates the transition to AD (29). For patients with LLD, our previous study suggested that OI deficits could serve as a promising predictor of developing major neurocognitive disorder, and neuroimaging showed that OI deficits and cognitive function had additive effects on the super synchronization of the hippocampus or fusiform gyrus (12). However, whether NPSs, which are very common in patients with LLD, might influence the relationship between OI deficits and cognitive impairment remains unknown.

The current study suggested that OI scores were negatively correlated with NPSs in patients with LLD, including agitation, apathy and aberrant motor behavior. In addition, agitation and cognitive impairment (language deficit or attention deficit) showed an interactive effect on OI in NPSs, consistent with previous research that olfactory dysfunction, cognitive impairment and agitation are strongly associated with each other. Regarding to the relationships between agitation and OI, previous studies have indicated that numerous brain regions related to agitation also participate in the olfactory circuit, such as the hippocampus, amygdala, insula, orbitofrontal cortex and habenula nucleus (30, 31). Most studies found that the application of olfactory stimulation using essential oils, such as lavender or lemon, could improve the symptoms of agitation (3234). With respect to the relationships between agitation and cognitive function, previous studies have shown that language, attention and other aspects of cognitive function are improved when agitation symptoms are alleviated (35, 36). Additionally, agitation was associated with structural and functional abnormalities of specific areas of the gray and white matter in subjects with cognitive impairment (37). The structure of the olfactory network and limbic system in brain regions could shed light on the relationship among agitation, OI and cognitive function and could explained their deficits in conjunction with different neuropsychiatric diseases. A large number of these regions have been regarded as providing the neural basis for neuropsychiatric symptoms, olfactory processing and cognitive processing, including the amygdala, insula, hippocampus, orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate cortex. Thus, the present results are an essential supplement to our previous study showing that there is a moderating effect between depression and cognitive impairment (global cognition, memory and language) on OI (28). This finding suggests that we should comprehensively evaluate agitation and exclude its confounding effects when applying OI to predict cognitive decline (language and attention) in patients with LLD.

Apart from agitation, the present study also suggested that aberrant motor behavior had a significant effect on OI. In line with a previous study showing that OI dysfunction was connected to frontotemporal dementia with changes in behavior and personality (38), our results suggested that OI was associated with aberrant motor behavior in patients with LLD. The mechanism remains unclear and must be further explored. Overall, although aberrant motor behavior was associated with OI in patients with LLD, it did not exhibit an interactive effect, such as with agitation. Therefore, it is not necessary to exclude the possible confounding effects of aberrant motor behavior when OI predicts cognitive decline in patients with LLD.

In this study, years of education was not set as a moderator because most of the studies supported that education was not a factor affecting OI. According to a review study by James, the relationship between education level and olfactory function is currently debated and must be further explored (39). Some studies have shown that there is no relationship between OI and years of education (40). But the study has shown that economic status and educational level were significantly related to OI, reflecting that the degree of exposure to foreign substances leads to differences in familiarity with smells or their names (41). Simplifying testing or using a more world-wide approach to testing is the solution to this problem (42). Overall, the relationship between OI and education level needs to be explored comprehensively in future study.

There are several limitations to our study. First, the current study is a cross-sectional study; whether alterations in NPS may affect OI performance and whether NPS have a mixed effect when using OI predict cognitive decline in patients with LLD needs to be further demonstrated in follow-up studies. Second, the current study only analyzed behavior, but the underlying mechanism of the mediating effect of NPS on cognition and OI is still unclear. We look forwards to future neuroimaging studies to find the mechanism to better understand their relationship. Third, the current study did not rule out the effects of drugs that patients with LLD took many different antidepressants. Fourth, although patients with Parkinson's disease was excluded, depression and olfactory dysfunction can be early symptoms of Parkinson's disease without motor symptoms, which could have been a potential confounding factor in the present study.

Overall, the coexistence of agitation and language or attention deficit was associated with worse OI in patients with LLD, and research exploring the relationship between OI and cognitive function should consider as an analysis of agitation and regulate its mixed effects. The present results provide a multilevel and comprehensive understanding of how NPS, cognition and OI influence each other, and they could be helpful for the realistic use of OI in clinical practice.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committees of the Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

All authors contributed to the writing and revision of the manuscript. All authors have read and approved of the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by a grant from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos. 81701341, 82101508, and 82171533), Guangzhou Municipal Psychiatric Diseases Clinical Transformation Laboratory (No: 201805010009), Key Laboratory for Innovation Platform Plan, the Science and Technology Program of Guangzhou, China, Science and Technology Plan Project of Guangdong Province (No. 2019B030316001), and National Key Research and Development Program of China (No. 2016YFC0906300).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank Weiru Zhang, Yinling Liu and Hengyue Wang, Cong Ouyang for assisting in collecting the data. We thank the Department of Geriatric Psychiatry and the Department of Neurology of the Affiliated Brain Hospital of Guangzhou Medical University for its support.

References

1. Tang T, Jiang J, Tang X. Prevalence of depressive symptoms among older adults in mainland China: A systematic review and meta-analysis. J Affect Disord. (2021) 293:379–90. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2021.06.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Reinlieb M, Ercoli LM, Siddarth P, St Cyr N, Lavretsky H. The patterns of cognitive and functional impairment in amnestic and non-amnestic mild cognitive impairment in geriatric depression. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2014) 22:1487–95. doi: 10.1016/j.jagp.2013.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Kaup AR, Byers AL, Falvey C, Simonsick EM, Satterfield S, Ayonayon HN, et al. Trajectories of depressive symptoms in older adults and risk of dementia. JAMA Psychiatry. (2016) 73:525–31. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.0004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Cai W, Mueller C, Shetty H, Perera G, Stewart R. Predictors of mortality in people with late-life depression: a retrospective cohort study. J Affect Disord. (2020) 266:695–701. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Laske C, Sohrabi HR, Frost SM, López-de-Ipiña K, Garrard P, Buscema M, et al. Innovative diagnostic tools for early detection of Alzheimer's disease. Alzheimer's Dement. (2015) 11:561–78. doi: 10.1016/j.jalz.2014.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lafaille-Magnan M-E, Poirier J, Etienne P, Tremblay-Mercier J, Frenette J, Rosa-Neto P, et al. Odor identification as a biomarker of preclinical AD in older adults at risk. Neurology. (2017) 89:327–35. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000004159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Marine N, Boriana A. Olfactory markers of depression and Alzheimer's disease. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 45:262–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Growdon ME, Schultz AP, Dagley AS, Amariglio RE, Hedden T, Rentz DM, et al. Odor identification and Alzheimer disease biomarkers in clinically normal elderly. Neurology. (2015) 84:2153–60. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000001614

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Wang Q, Chen B, Zhong X, Zhou H, Zhang M, Mai N, et al. Olfactory Dysfunction Is Already Present with Subjective Cognitive Decline and Deepens with Disease Severity in the Alzheimer's Disease Spectrum. J Alzheimers Dis. (2021) 79:585–95. doi: 10.3233/JAD-201168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Roberts RO, Christianson TJH, Kremers WK, Mielke MM, Machulda MM, Vassilaki M, et al. Association between olfactory dysfunction and amnestic mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer disease dementia. JAMA Neurol. (2016) 73:93-101. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2015.2952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Chen B, Zhong X, Mai N, Peng Q, Wu Z, Ouyang C, et al. Cognitive impairment and structural abnormalities in late life depression with olfactory identification impairment: an Alzheimer's disease-like pattern. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol. (2018) 21:640–8. doi: 10.1093/ijnp/pyy016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Chen B, Zhong X, Zhang M, Mai N, Wu Z, Chen X, et al. The additive effect of late-life depression and olfactory dysfunction on the risk of dementia was mediated by hypersynchronization of the hippocampus/fusiform gyrus. Transl Psychiatry. (2021) 11:172. doi: 10.1038/s41398-021-01291-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Pink AAJI, Geda YE, Roberts RO, Christianson TJ, Pankratz VS, Sochor O, et al. From prevalent MCI to incident dementia as predicted by baseline neuropsychiatric symptoms: the mayo clinic study of aging. J Neurol Sci. (2013) 333(Suppl.1):e345–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2013.07.1273

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Tascone LDS, Bottino CMdC. Neurobiology of neuropsychiatric symptoms in Alzheimer's disease: a critical review with a focus on neuroimaging. Dement Neuropsychol. (2013) 7:236–43. doi: 10.1590/S1980-57642013DN70300002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Apostolova LG, Di LJ, Duffy EL, Brook J, Elashoff D, Tseng C-H, et al. Risk factors for behavioral abnormalities in mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer's disease. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. (2014) 37:315–26. doi: 10.1159/000351009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Monastero R, Mangialasche F, Camarda C, Ercolani S, Camarda R. A systematic review of neuropsychiatric symptoms in mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimer's Dis. (2009) 18:11–30. doi: 10.3233/JAD-2009-1120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Tanaka H, Hashimoto M, Fukuhara R, Ishikawa T, Yatabe Y, Kaneda K, et al. Relationship between dementia severity and behavioural and psychological symptoms in early-onset alzheimer's disease. Psychogeriatrics. (2015) 15:242–7. doi: 10.1111/psyg.12108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Miwa T, Furukawa M, Tsukatani T, Costanzo RM, DiNardo LJ, Reiter ER. Impact of olfactory impairment on quality of life and disability. Arch Otolaryngol Head Neck Surg. (2001) 127:497–503. doi: 10.1001/archotol.127.5.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Seligman SC, Kamath V, Giovannetti T, Arnold SE, Moberg PJ. Olfaction and apathy in Alzheimer's disease, mild cognitive impairment, and healthy older adults. Aging Ment Health. (2013) 17:564–70. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2013.768208

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Kamath V, Chaney G-AS, DeRight J, Onyike CU. A meta-analysis of neuropsychological, social cognitive, and olfactory functioning in the behavioral and language variants of frontotemporal dementia. Psychol Med. (2019) 49:2669–80. doi: 10.1017/S0033291718003604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Li Z-T, Li S-B, Wen J-F, Zhang X-Y, Hummel T, Zou L-Q. Early-onset schizophrenia showed similar but more severe olfactory identification impairment than adult-onset schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:626. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Li S-B, Li Z-T, Lyu Z-H, Zhang X-Y, Zou L-Q. Odour identification impairment is a trait but not a disease-specific marker for bipolar disorders: comparisons of bipolar disorder with different episodes, major depressive disorder and schizophrenia. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2021) 4867421998774. doi: 10.1177/0004867421998774

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Sweigert JR, St John T, Begay KK, Davis GE, Munson J, Shankland E, et al. Characterizing olfactory function in children with autism spectrum disorder and children with sensory processing dysfunction. Brain Sci. (2020) 10:362. doi: 10.3390/brainsci10060362

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Takahashi Y, Shindo S, Kanbayashi T, Takeshima M, Imanishi A, Mishima K. Examination of the influence of cedar fragrance on cognitive function and behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia in Alzheimer type dementia. Neuropsychopharmacol Rep. (2020) 40:10–5. doi: 10.1002/npr2.12096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Press-Sandler O, Freud T, Volkov I, Peleg R, Press Y. Aromatherapy for the treatment of patients with behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia: a descriptive analysis of RCTs. J Alternat Complement Med (New York, NY). (2016) 22:422–8. doi: 10.1089/acm.2015.0186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Hummel T, Sekinger B, Wolf SR, Pauli E, Kobal G. 'Sniffin' sticks': olfactory performance assessed by the combined testing of odor identification, odor discrimination and olfactory threshold. Chem Senses. (1997) 22:39–52. doi: 10.1093/chemse/22.1.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Hayes A. Introduction to mediation, moderation, and conditional process analysis. J Educ Measure. (2013) 51:335–7. doi: 10.1111/jedm.12050

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Chen B, Zhong X, Mai N, Peng Q, Zhang M, Chen X, et al. Interactive effect of depression and cognitive impairment on olfactory identification in elderly people. J Alzheimer's Dis. (2018) 66:1645–55. doi: 10.3233/JAD-180760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Murphy C. Olfactory and other sensory impairments in Alzheimer disease. Nat Rev Neurol. (2019) 15:11–24. doi: 10.1038/s41582-018-0097-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Behrman S, Chouliaras L, Ebmeier KP. Considering the senses in the diagnosis and management of dementia. Maturitas. (2014) 77:305–10. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2014.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Saito N, Yamano E, Ishii A, Tanaka M, Nakamura J, Watanabe Y. Involvement of the olfactory system in the induction of anti-fatigue effects by odorants. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0195263. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0195263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Snow LA, Hovanec L, Brandt J. A controlled trial of aromatherapy for agitation in nursing home patients with dementia. J Altern Complement Med (New York, NY). (2004) 10:431–7. doi: 10.1089/1075553041323696

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. van der Ploeg ES, Eppingstall B, O'Connor DW. The study protocol of a blinded randomised-controlled cross-over trial of lavender oil as a treatment of behavioural symptoms in dementia. BMC Geriatr. (2010) 10:49. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-10-49

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Sakamoto Y, Ebihara S, Ebihara T, Tomita N, Toba K, Freeman S, et al. Fall prevention using olfactory stimulation with lavender odor in elderly nursing home residents: a randomized controlled trial. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2012) 60:1005–11. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2012.03977.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. van der Ploeg ES, Eppingstall B, Camp CJ, Runci SJ, Taffe J, O'Connor DW. A randomized crossover trial to study the effect of personalized, one-to-one interaction using Montessori-based activities on agitation, affect, and engagement in nursing home residents with Dementia. Int Psychogeriatr. (2013) 25:565–75. doi: 10.1017/S1041610212002128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Balzotti A, Filograsso M, Altamura C, Fairfield B, Bellomo A, Daddato F, et al. Comparison of the efficacy of gesture-verbal treatment and doll therapy for managing neuropsychiatric symptoms in older patients with dementia. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2019) 34:1308–15. doi: 10.1002/gps.4961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Makovac E, Serra L, Spanò B, Giulietti G, Torso M, Cercignani M, et al. Different patterns of correlation between grey and white matter integrity account for behavioral and psychological symptoms in Alzheimer's disease. J Alzheimer's Dis. (2016) 50:591–604. doi: 10.3233/JAD-150612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Carnemolla SE, Hsieh JW, Sipione R, Landis BN, Kumfor F, Piguet O, et al. Olfactory dysfunction in frontotemporal dementia and psychiatric disorders: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2020) 118:588–611. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. James J, Tsvik AM, Chung SY, Usseglio J, Gudis DA, Overdevest JB. Association between social determinants of health and olfactory function: a scoping review. Int Forum Allergy Rhinol. (2021) 11:1472–93. doi: 10.1002/alr.22822

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Szajer J, Murphy C. Education level predicts retrospective metamemory accuracy in healthy aging and Alzheimer's disease. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2013) 35:971–82. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2013.844771

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Fornazieri MA, Doty RL, Bezerra TFP, de Rezende Pinna F, Costa FO, Voegels RL, et al. Relationship of socioeconomic status to olfactory function. Physiol Behav. (2019) 198:84–9. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2018.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Cha H, Kim S, Son Y. Associations between cognitive function, depression, and olfactory function in elderly people with dementia in Korea. Front Aging Neurosci. (2022) 13:799897. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2021.799897

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: late-life depression, neuropsychiatric symptoms, agitation, odor identification, cognition, Alzheimer's disease

Citation: Zhang S, Chen B, Zhong X, Zhang M, Wang Q, Wu Z, Hou L, Zhou H, Chen X, Liu M, Yang M, Lin G, Hummel T and Ning Y (2022) Interactive Effects of Agitation and Cognitive Impairment on Odor Identification in Patients With Late-Life Depression. Front. Psychiatry 13:839012. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.839012

Received: 19 December 2021; Accepted: 10 February 2022;
Published: 08 March 2022.

Edited by:

Gianfranco Spalletta, Santa Lucia Foundation (IRCCS), Italy

Reviewed by:

Sanjeev Kumar, University of Toronto, Canada
Nilton Custodio, Peruvian Institute of Neurosciences (IPN), Peru

Copyright © 2022 Zhang, Chen, Zhong, Zhang, Wang, Wu, Hou, Zhou, Chen, Liu, Yang, Lin, Hummel and Ning. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yuping Ning, ningjeny@126.com

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.