Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 05 April 2022
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

Altered Neural Reactivity in Adolescents With Nonsuicidal Self-Injury During Exposure to Self-Injury Related Cues: Electrophysiological Evidence From a Two-Choice Oddball Paradigm

\nDong-Dong Zhou
Dong-Dong Zhou1*Lin ZhaoLin Zhao2Ling-Li MaLing-Li Ma2Jin-Hui HuJin-Hui Hu1Ran ChenRan Chen1Zheng-Hao JiangZheng-Hao Jiang1Xiao-Qing HeXiao-Qing He1Xin-Yu PengXin-Yu Peng2Xin-Yi LiuXin-Yi Liu1Xiao LiXiao Li2Wan-Jun ChenWan-Jun Chen2Wo WangWo Wang1Li Kuang,
Li Kuang1,2*
  • 1Mental Health Center, University-Town Hospital of Chongqing Medical University, Chongqing, China
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, The First Affiliated Hospital of Chongqing Medical University, Chongqing, China

Background: Nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) may be a type of addiction, that is characterized by cue reactivity. We aimed to explore the behavioral performance and neural reactivity during exposure to self-injury cues in adolescents with NSSI and major depressive disorder (MDD).

Methods: Eighteen MDD patients, 18 MDD patients with NSSI, and 19 healthy controls (HC) were recruited to perform a two-choice oddball paradigm. All subjects were 12–18 years old. Neutral cues and self-injury related cues separately served as deviant stimuli. Difference waves in N2 and P3 (N2d and P3d) were derived from deviant waves minus standard waves. Accuracy cost and reaction time (RT) cost were used as behavioral indexes, while the N2d and P3d were used as electrophysiological indexes; the N2d reflects early conflict detection, and the P3d reflects the process of response inhibition.

Results: No significant main effects of group or cue or an effect of their interaction were observed on accuracy cost and P3d latency. For RT cost, N2d amplitude, and N2d latency, there was a significant main effect of cue. For P3d amplitude, there was a significant main effect of cue and a significant group × cue interaction. In the NSSI group, the P3d amplitude with self-injury cues was significantly larger than that with neutral cues. However, there was no such effect in the MDD and HC groups.

Conclusions: Adolescents with NSSI showed altered neural reactivity during exposure to self-injury cue. Further studies with larger sample sizes are needed to confirm our results.

Introduction

Nonsuicidal self-injury (NSSI) refers to deliberate self-injuring behaviors that lack suicidal intent. These behaviors often occur in adolescents. It was reported that the lifetime incidence of NSSI is ~17–60% (1). Notably, ~41.6% of adolescents with NSSI were diagnosed with major depressive disorder (MDD) (2). NSSI is also an independent risk factor for future suicide attempts and completed suicide (3). Thus, the negative consequences of NSSI cannot be ignored, although the behavior is generally not fatal.

Typically, people have the ability to inhibit impulsive or inappropriate behavior, which is called response inhibition or behavioral inhibitory control (BIC). The capacity to inhibit responses is important for better adaptation (4, 5). However, adolescents with NSSI repeatedly engage in self-injury behaviors, which suggests a lack of inhibitory control. A previous study demonstrated that executive function was abnormal in subjects with self-injury (6). The functional connectivity between the right orbitofrontal cortex and anterior cingulate gyrus is reduced in patients with self-injury (7). Based on a regional homogeneity (ReHo) analysis, the self-injury group had significantly lower ReHo values in brain areas such as the left fusiform gyrus, inferior frontal gyrus, left hippocampus, and bilateral parahippocampal gyrus than the healthy control group (6). These neuroimaging studies indicated abnormal brain activity in areas involved in response inhibition in subjects with self-injury.

Event-related potential (ERP) is a useful tool for investigating changes in brain activity. The psychological paradigms investigating response inhibition traditionally include go/nogo, stop-signal, and two-choice oddball paradigms (8). In the two-choice oddball paradigm, two types of stimuli are presented: standard stimuli (frequently presented) and deviant stimuli (infrequently presented). Subjects needed to differentially respond to the standard and deviant stimuli as quickly as possible. When deviant stimuli are presented, subjects need to inhibit their more frequent responses to standard stimuli and make the correct responses to deviant stimuli. In the go/nogo paradigm, only accuracy can serve as a behavioral index. There is no reaction time (RT) index for deviant stimuli because subjects do not respond to nogo stimuli (9). In contrast, the stop-signal paradigm can provide RT as a behavioral index but cannot provide accuracy (10). Compared with those two paradigms, the two-choice oddball paradigm provides both accuracy and RT for deviant stimuli, which provides a more comprehensive evaluation of the behavior index (8). Alternatively, the antisaccade task and emotional stop-signal task are also able to provide accuracy and response time (1113).

The ERP components associated with response inhibition include N200 and P300, also referred to as N2 and P3. Generally, N2 is a negative wave that occurs ~200–300 ms after stimulus presentation. N2 represents early conflict detection and monitoring sourced from the anterior cingulate cortex (14). P3 is a positive wave that occurs ~300 ms after stimulus presentation. P3 represents the process of response inhibition control, sourced from the anterior cingulate cortex and lateral orbital frontal cortex (OFC) (9, 15).

Drug addiction is characterized by a recurring desire to continue taking a drug despite the resulting negative consequences. Behavioral addiction is similar to drug addiction but in different domains, such as food addiction and internet addiction (16). Similarly, NSSI may be regarded as a type of behavioral addiction (17). The emotional state experienced when patients with NSSI do not injure themselves is similar to the withdrawal state experienced by individuals who use drugs (18). Washburn et al. (19) pointed out that patients repeatedly engaging in NSSI have a strong desire for self-harming behavior. Moreover, self-harming behavior can result in the release of endogenous opioid peptides, which can ease the pain caused by the self-harm and result in pleasure and satisfaction (20). Similar to patients with NSSI, ERP, and neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that neural activity involved in response inhibition is altered in individuals with drug use disorders (21). Therefore, NSSI and addiction may have similar neurobiological underpinnings.

Drug addiction is characterized by cue reactivity (22), which has been demonstrated at both behavioral and neurobiological levels using different methodologies including ERP (23). The physiological and neurochemical changes during cue exposure may contribute to initiation and maintenance of drug use (23). An ERP study showed that the amplitude of the N2 difference wave (N2d) in heroin abstainers was significantly smaller than that in controls when exposed to drug-related cues. For heroin abstainers, the amplitude of the N2d wave with drug-related cue exposure was smaller than the amplitude with neutral cue presentation (24). These may suggest the early conflict detection is impaired during drug-related cue exposure. Additionally, the accuracy on nogo trials was decreased in smokers (25), and smoking-related cues may interfere with normal performance (26). This cue reactivity is not only exhibited in drug addiction but also in behavioral addiction, such as gambling, gaming, and buying disorders (27). Patients with pathological gambling had a higher accuracy on nogo trials during exposure to gambling related cues (28). Thus, behavioral performance and neural reactivity patterns may also be altered in adolescents with NSSI during exposure to self-injury related cues. In a previous study using a stop-signal task, self-injuring patients made more errors to negative emotional pictures, but made fewer errors to self-injury pictures (29). Another study suggested that impaired emotional response inhibition to self-injury related stimuli may be a risk factor for self-injury urges (30), thereby suggesting that cue reactivity may be a predictor for future self-injury behaviors.

Therefore, we hypothesized that adolescents with NSSI would have altered neural reactivity patterns and behavioral performance during exposure to self-injury cues. In this study, we aimed to explore the behavioral performance and neural reactivity during exposure to self-injury cues using the two-choice oddball paradigm in adolescents with NSSI.

Methods

Participants

Adolescents aged 12–18 years old, who had a current diagnosis of MDD, engaged in NSSI or not, and were previously untreated, were recruited from the outpatient department of University-Town Hospital of Chongqing Medical University and The First Affiliated Hospital of Chongqing Medical University. NSSI was determined by the criteria proposed in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5) (31): in the most recent year, self-harm behaviors occurred at least five times. There was no restriction on gender. All subjects were right-handed, had normal or corrected normal vision, and had normal hearing. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) diagnosis of schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, mental retardation, obsessive-compulsive disorder, substance use disorder, or another primary diagnosis other than MDD; (b) previous suicide attempt; (c) previous head trauma or epilepsy; (d) pregnancy; and (e) chronic or severe physical conditions, such as thyroid disease, autoimmune disease, or heart and lung disease. Healthy controls (HC) were age-matched adolescents recruited via advertisement. They were also right-handed, had normal or corrected normal vision, and had normal hearing. They were not diagnosed with MDD and were not currently or previously injuring themselves. Other exclusion criteria were similar to those applied to the patients.

Measures and Procedure

All subjects were interviewed using the MINI-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I. KID 5.0) (32). Two well-trained psychiatrists evaluated them using the following scales: (a) Hamilton Depression Scale (HAMD) (33); (b) Barratt Impulsiveness Scale 11th version (BIS-11) (34); and (c) Ottawa self-injury inventory (OSI) (35).

Subjects were seated in front of a computer screen at a distance of approximately 60 cm. E-Prime 3.0 was used to present stimuli. A two-choice oddball paradigm was used in our study. The paradigm is presented in Figure 1 and described as follows. Visual stimuli included frequent standard stimuli (75%) and infrequent deviant stimuli (25%). The paradigm included 2 conditions. One condition utilized neutral pictures as deviant stimuli, and the other condition utilized self-injury related pictures (cutting-related pictures) as deviant stimuli. In both conditions, a neutral cup was chosen as a standard stimulus. Each condition consisted of 200 trials, with a total of 400 trials included. In each trial, a fixation cross was initially presented for a randomly chosen duration between 500 and 1,000 ms, followed by a 300-ms blank screen. Then, a picture was presented for 1,000 ms or prematurely terminated by a response. Finally, a blank screen was presented for 1,000 ms. All pictures were chosen from free online resources and were retouched to the same size. The deviant stimuli were randomly chosen from 10 neutral pictures (neutral cues) and 10 self-injury related pictures (self-injury cues). The participants were required to press buttons based on the presented picture as quickly and accurately as possible. The subjects were asked to press the “1” key when a standard stimulus was presented and press the “2” key when a deviant stimulus was presented. The subjects needed to achieve at least 80% accuracy in practice before the formal trials were initiated.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Schematic illustration of the experimental procedure and example stimuli. Each trial presented a single deviant or standard stimulus. Subjects pressed the “1” or “2” keys to respond to standard or deviant stimuli, respectively.

Acquisition and Preprocessing

A Neuroscan Quick cap with 64 scalp sites was used to record electroencephalography (EEG) data in the Curry 8 system. The impedance of each electrode was kept below 5 kΩ. The reference electrode between Cz and Cpz was chosen as an online reference. The sampling rate was 1,000 Hz, and the bandpass was 0.05–100 Hz. The accuracy and RT were recorded for each response. Offline analyses were conducted using the EEGLAB toolbox in MATLAB 2013b (36). We resampled all EEG data at 500 Hz. The offline bandpass filter was set at 0.1–30 Hz. All EEG data were re-referenced to the average of the M1 and M2 electrodes. EEG data were epoched to include 200 ms of the pre-stimulus and 1,000 ms of the post-stimulus periods. Epochs with an incorrect response were removed. Epochs with large artifacts were rejected, and bad channels were interpolated using the spherical method. Then, we ran independent component analysis (ICA) to remove artifact components, which mainly included blinking, horizontal eye movements, and some muscle artifacts (37). For each condition, epochs were overlapped and averaged separately. The 200-ms pre-stimulus baseline was subtracted by the post-stimulus wave to correct the baseline.

Statistical Analysis

Three groups (HC, MDD, and MDD+NSSI) were included in our study. Regarding the behavioral indexes, accuracy cost was derived from the standard condition minus deviant condition, and RT cost was derived from the deviant condition minus standard condition. Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) was separately conducted for accuracy cost and RT cost. Cue served as the within-subject factor, and group served as the between-subject factor. Regarding the electrophysiological indexes, difference waves were derived from deviant waves minus standard waves. The peak latency and mean amplitude of N2d (200–300 ms) and P3d (350–550 ms) were extracted from the difference waves. N2 is typically largest in anterior areas, and P3 is typically largest in parietal or posterior areas (3840). As in previous studies (41, 42), FCz was chosen as the electrode of interest for N2, and Cz was chosen as the electrode of interest for P3. Repeated measures ANOVA was performed for the mean amplitude and peak latency of N2d and P3d. The between-subject factor was the group (three levels: HC, MDD, and MDD+NSSI). The within-subject factor was the cue type (two levels: neutral cue and self-injury related cue). Greenhouse-Geisser correction was used when the sphericity assumption was violated. The significance level was set at p < 0.05. Post hoc comparisons were Bonferroni-adjusted. All the above analyses were performed using Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS) version 25. We used the ERP reliability analysis (ERA) toolbox to obtain reliability estimates with a threshold of 0.70 (43, 44).

Post hoc Analyses

Previous studies have suggested that impulsivity is associated with response inhibition (45). N2 represents early conflict detection and P3 represents the process of response inhibition. Thus, post hoc correlation analyses were performed between the BIS-11 total score and several variables (self-injury cue: P3d amplitude, P3d latency, N2d amplitude, N2d latency, RT cost, and accuracy cost; neutral cue: P3d amplitude, P3d latency, N2d amplitude, N2d latency, RT cost, and accuracy cost) for the three groups. Bonferroni corrections were used to correct for multiple comparisons.

Results

General Characteristics

We initially recruited 19 healthy controls, 19 MDD patients without NSSI, and 19 MDD patients with NSSI. One MDD patient and one NSSI patient were excluded from the analysis because of too many artifacts during EEG acquisition. The general characteristics of the three groups are presented in Table 1. Age and sex ratio were not significantly different across groups. However, BIS-11 total scores were significantly different among the groups [F(2, 52) = 24.61, p < 0.001]. The BIS-11 total score in the HC group (33.72 ± 11.86) was significantly lower than that in the MDD (56.57 ± 13.59) and MDD+NSSI groups (63.05 ± 14.71) (Bonferroni-adjusted p < 0.001 for both). No significant BIS-11 total score difference was observed between the MDD and MDD+NSSI groups. No significant differences in the HAMD scores were found between the MDD and MDD+NSSI groups. The most frequent self-injury type was cutting (100%). A few adolescents also tried pinching, biting, knocking, or burning themselves. The reliability estimates are presented in Table 2. All reliability estimates were acceptable but NSSI group had a lower dependability for N2 latency in self-injury related deviant stimuli.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant characteristics of the HC, MDD, and MDD+NSSI groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Reliability estimates for electrophysiological and behavioral indices.

Behavioral Indexes

A mixed ANOVA (group × cue) showed no significant main effect or interaction effect on accuracy cost. A mixed ANOVA (group × cue) for RT cost showed a significant main effect of cue [F(1, 52) = 12.64, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.20] but no significant interaction effect [F(2, 52) = 0.56, p = 0.58, ηp2 = 0.02] or main effect of group [F(2, 52) = 1.38, p = 0.26, ηp2 = 0.05].

Electrophysiological Indexes

Regarding N2d amplitudes, a mixed ANOVA (group × cue) showed a significant main effect of cue [F(1, 52) = 46.37, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.47]. The main effect of group and the interaction effect were not significant. Regarding N2d latencies, a mixed ANOVA (group × cue) showed a significant main effect of cue [F(1, 52) = 5.61, p = 0.022, ηp2 = 0.10]. The main effect of group and the interaction effect were not significant.

Regarding P3d amplitudes, a mixed ANOVA (group × cue) showed a significant main effect of cue [F(1, 52) = 9.46, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.15] and a significant group × cue interaction [F(2, 52) = 5.15, p = 0.009, ηp2 = 0.17]. The main effect of group was not significant. A simple effect analysis indicated that with self-injury cues, the P3d amplitude in the NSSI group was larger than that in the HC group (Bonferroni-adjusted p = 0.067). In the NSSI group, the P3d amplitude with self-injury cues was significantly larger than that with neutral cues (Bonferroni-adjusted p < 0.001). However, there was no such effect in the MDD and HC groups. Regarding P3d latencies, a mixed ANOVA (group × cue) showed no significant main effects or interaction effect. Grand mean ERPs were presented in Figure 2 and topographic maps of the N2d and P3d were presented in Figure 3.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Average of difference ERPs for neutral cue condition and self-injury cue condition in HC group, MDD group, and NSSI group. ERP, event-related potential; HC, healthy control; MDD, major depressive disorder; NSSI, nonsuicidal self-injury.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Grand-average topographic maps of the N2d (200–300 ms) and P3d (350–550 ms).

Correlation Analyses

For the three groups, correlation analyses were performed between the BIS-11 total score and several variables (self-injury cue: P3d amplitude, P3d latency, N2d amplitude, N2d latency, RT cost, and accuracy cost; neutral cue: P3d amplitude, P3d latency, N2d amplitude, N2d latency, RT cost, and accuracy cost). However, no p-value was significant after Bonferroni correction.

Discussion

This study utilized a two-choice oddball paradigm to investigate the behavioral performance and neural reactivity during exposure to self-injury related cues in adolescents with NSSI. Three groups (NSSI+MDD, MDD, and HC) were compared. A simple effect analysis showed that the P3d amplitude with self-injury cues was significantly larger than that with neutral cues in the NSSI group (Bonferroni-adjusted p < 0.001). However, there was no such effect in the MDD and HC groups. These results suggest that the neural reactivity was altered during exposure to self-injury cues in adolescents with NSSI.

Regarding the behavioral indexes, no significant difference between groups was observed for accuracy cost. This result is consistent with previous studies and suggests little-to-no association between NSSI and behavioral performance (47, 48). For RT cost, the ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of cue, indicating that the RT cost with the self-injury cue was larger than that with the neutral cue regardless of group. Consistent with this finding, in a previous study, both the NSSI and control groups showed worse behavioral performance in response to self-injury related pictures than that in response to neutral pictures (29). Surprisingly, the NSSI group in that study showed significantly fewer errors during exposure to self-injury pictures, which was explained by an enhanced response inhibition to self-injury cues (29). Moreover, subjects with a different behavioral addiction, pathological gambling, also showed an enhanced response inhibition during exposure to gambling related cues (28). However, these effects were not observed in our study, which possibly due to differences in recruited participants or paradigms. Only adolescents were recruited in our study, while only adults with NSSI were recruited in a previous study (29). Further studies are needed to confirm this effect.

Regarding the electrophysiological indexes, only the main effect of cue was significant for N2d amplitudes and N2d latency. The N2d represents conflict detection and monitoring (8). The significant main effect of cue may suggest general detection of a larger conflict during exposure to self-injury cues regardless of group. The P3d represents the process of response inhibition. The P3d amplitude in the NSSI group was larger than that in the HC group with self-injury related cues, which suggested that adolescents with NSSI needed more neural resources to successfully complete the inhibition. This result was consistent with some studies. Alcohol-related cues induced larger P3 amplitudes in heavy drinkers than in light drinkers (49). Consistently, a heroin addiction group showed a larger P3d amplitude than controls (50). The internet addiction disorder (IAD) group also showed a larger P3 amplitude than controls (51). However, some studies have reported that a lower P3 amplitude reflects a deficient capability of inhibition (52). Smaller P3 amplitudes were observed in heavy drinkers than in light drinkers (46). Smokers had a reduced P3 amplitude compared with non-smokers (42). Additionally, P3 amplitude was negatively associated with nogo errors, suggesting that subjects with reduced P3 amplitude had more errors (42). These studies seem to be inconsistent with our results. Through compensatory mechanisms, binge drinkers may increase nogo P3 amplitudes and produce a greater activation in prefrontal areas, to achieve a performance level comparable to controls (53). In our study, the NSSI group may be compensating to increase their P3d amplitude to achieve a similar accuracy as the HC group. These results also suggested that the inhibitory capacity was impaired in adolescents with NSSI.

In the NSSI group, the P3d amplitude with self-injury related cues was significantly larger than that with neutral cues. No such effect was observed in the MDD or HC groups. Thus, similar to individuals with addiction disorders, adolescents with NSSI may have an altered neural reactivity when exposed to self-injury cues. Addicted subjects showed altered brain activation to drug-related cues, involving the salience, executive, and reward networks (54). Compared with the controls, subjects with NSSI in a previous study showed enhanced activity in the inferior frontal cortex (IFC) and OFC when exposed to self-injury cues (55). This neuroimaging result is consistent with our results; both indicate that adolescents with NSSI may process self-injury cues differently. Drug-related cues are more salient to addicted subjects than neutral cues (56, 57). Likewise, self-injury cues may be more salient to subjects with NSSI, resulting in more neural resources being needed to inhibit inappropriate responses when exposed to self-injury cues.

We should note that both P3 amplitude reductions and increases have been inconsistently reported in previous studies (42, 46, 4951, 57, 58). It appears to be very confusing that both P3 reductions and increases can indicate deficiency in response inhibition. In our opinion, behavioral and electrophysiological indexes should be used simultaneously to evaluate response inhibition. Here, we propose two concepts: subthreshold and suprathreshold deficiency in response inhibition. Suprathreshold deficiency represents behavioral performance that was significantly worse than performance in controls (i.e., accuracy was lower and RT was longer), while subthreshold deficiency represents behavioral performance that was similar to performance in controls. Usually, reduced P3 amplitudes can be observed in instances of suprathreshold deficiency (42, 58). Increased P3 amplitudes may accompany subthreshold deficiency, suggesting that subjects use more neural resources to achieve a similar behavioral performance (51). Suprathreshold deficiency accompanied by a larger P3 amplitude may indicate a worse inhibitory capacity, such as cases where subjects have worse performance although more neural resources are used (49, 50).

This study had some limitations. First, although we did not find an overall sex difference among the groups, there were more female participants in the MDD+NSSI group (83.3%). This may be because females are more likely to engage in NSSI (1). The NSSI group was younger than the other groups, although this difference was not significant. Thus, studies with larger sample sizes and groups that are more balanced in terms of sex and age are required in the future. Our results should be treated with caution. Second, all subjects with NSSI cut themselves, and only cutting-related pictures were used as self-injury cues. Our results thus may not be generalizable to subjects with other forms of self-injury. Third, this was a cross-sectional study; thus, it remains unclear whether altered neural reactivity during exposure to self-injury cues predicts future self-injury or suicidal behavior. The deficits in inhibitory control with self-injury cues may be a risk factor for self-injury urges (30), suggesting that cue reactivity may be a predictor for NSSI in the future. Further longitudinal studies are needed. Fourth, all adolescents with NSSI in our study were diagnosed with MDD; thus, our results may not apply to adolescents without MDD or individuals with other diagnoses.

Conclusions

Our study provided electrophysiological evidence that neural reactivity in adolescents with NSSI was altered during exposure to self-injury cues. Specifically, the P3d amplitude was larger during exposure to self-injury cues. Studies with larger sample sizes are needed to confirm our results.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the Ethics Committee of the University-Town Hospital of Chongqing Medical University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

D-DZ, RC, and LK were involved in the study concept and design. LZ, L-LM, J-HH, Z-HJ, X-QH, X-YP, X-YL, XL, W-JC, and WW were involved in data preparation and interpretation. D-DZ, LZ, L-LM, X-QH, and J-HH were involved in the statistical analysis. D-DZ, RC, and Z-HJ wrote the manuscript. D-DZ and LK were involved in study supervision and edited the manuscript. All authors had full access to all data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Funding

This work was supported by a grant from the Science and Technology Bureau of Chongqing (cstc2020jcyj-msxmX0222) and a grant from the National Natural Science Foundation of China (81971286).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. Brown RC, Plener PL. Non-suicidal self-injury in adolescence. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2017) 19:20. doi: 10.1007/s11920-017-0767-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Nock MK, Joiner TE Jr., Gordon KH, Lloyd-Richardson E, Prinstein MJ. Non-suicidal self-injury among adolescents: diagnostic correlates and relation to suicide attempts. Psychiatry Res. (2006) 144:65–72. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Hamza CA, Stewart SL, Willoughby T. Examining the link between nonsuicidal self-injury and suicidal behavior: a review of the literature and an integrated model. Clin Psychol Rev. (2012) 32:482–95. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2012.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Goldstein M, Brendel G, Tuescher O, Pan H, Epstein J, Beutel M, et al. Neural substrates of the interaction of emotional stimulus processing and motor inhibitory control: an emotional linguistic go/no-go fMRI study. Neuroimage. (2007) 36:1026–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.01.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Li CS, Huang C, Constable RT, Sinha R. Imaging response inhibition in a stop-signal task: neural correlates independent of signal monitoring and post-response processing. J Neurosci. (2006) 26:186–92. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3741-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Cao J, Chen JM, Kuang L, Ai M, Fang WD, Gan Y, et al. Abnormal regional homogeneity in young adult suicide attempters with no diagnosable psychiatric disorder: a resting state functional magnetic imaging study. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 231:95–102. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Osuch E, Ford K, Wrath A, Bartha R, Neufeld R. Functional MRI of pain application in youth who engaged in repetitive non-suicidal self-injury vs. psychiatric controls. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 223:104–12. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Yuan JJ, Xu MM, Yang JM, Li H. The application of the two-choice oddball paradigm to the research of behavioral inhibitory control (in Chinese). Sci Sin Vitae. (2017) 47:1065–73. doi: 10.1360/N052017-00125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Albert J, López-Martín S, Carretié L. Emotional context modulates response inhibition: neural and behavioral data. Neuroimage. (2010) 49:914–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.08.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Rubia K, Smith AB, Brammer MJ, Toone B, Taylor E. Abnormal brain activation during inhibition and error detection in medication-naive adolescents with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. (2005) 162:1067–75. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.6.1067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Allen KJD, Hooley JM. Negative Emotional Action Termination (NEAT): support for a cognitive mechanism underlying negative urgency in nonsuicidal self-injury. Behav Ther. (2019) 50:924–37. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2019.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Allen KJD, Johnson SL, Burke TA, Sammon MM, Wu C, Kramer MA, et al. Validation of an emotional stop-signal task to probe individual differences in emotional response inhibition: Relationships with positive and negative urgency. Brain Neurosci Adv. (2021) 5:1–11. doi: 10.1177/23982128211058269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Pearlstein JG, Johnson SL, Modavi K, Peckham AD, Carver CS. Neurocognitive mechanisms of emotion-related impulsivity: the role of arousal. Psychophysiology. (2019) 56:e13293. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Bekker EM, Kenemans JL, Verbaten MN. Source analysis of the N2 in a cued Go/NoGo task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. (2005) 22:221–31. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2004.08.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bokura H, Yamaguchi S, Kobayashi S. Electrophysiological correlates for response inhibition in a Go/NoGo task. Clin Neurophysiol. (2001) 112:2224–32. doi: 10.1016/S1388-2457(01)00691-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Zou Z, Wang H, d'Oleire Uquillas F, Wang X, Ding J, Chen H. Definition of substance and non-substance addiction. Adv Exp Med Biol. (2017) 1010:21–41. doi: 10.1007/978-981-10-5562-1_2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Blasco-Fontecilla H, Fernández-Fernández R, Colino L, Fajardo L, Perteguer-Barrio R, de Leon J. The addictive model of self-harming (non-suicidal and suicidal) behavior. Front Psychiatry. (2016) 7:8. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Faye P. Addictive characteristics of the behavior of self-mutilation. J Psychosoc Nurs Ment Health Serv. (1995) 33:36–9. doi: 10.3928/0279-3695-19950601-08

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Washburn JJ, Juzwin KR, Styer DM, Aldridge D. Measuring the urge to self-injure: preliminary data from a clinical sample. Psychiatry Res. (2010) 178:540–4. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Bresin K, Gordon KH. Endogenous opioids and nonsuicidal self-injury: a mechanism of affect regulation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2013) 37:374–83. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Luijten M, Machielsen MW, Veltman DJ, Hester R, de Haan L, Franken IH. Systematic review of ERP and fMRI studies investigating inhibitory control and error processing in people with substance dependence and behavioural addictions. J Psychiatry Neurosci. (2014) 39:149–69. doi: 10.1503/jpn.130052

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Drummond DC. What does cue-reactivity have to offer clinical research? Addiction. (2000) 95(Suppl. 2):S129–44. doi: 10.1046/j.1360-0443.95.8s2.2.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Rose AK, Field M, Franken IHA, Munafò M. Chapter 43 - cue reactivity. In: Miller PM, editor. Principles of Addiction. San Diego, CA: Academic Press (2013). p. 413–23.

Google Scholar

24. Su B, Wang S, Sumich A, Li S, Yang L, Cai Y, et al. Reduction in N2 amplitude in response to deviant drug-related stimuli during a two-choice oddball task in long-term heroin abstainers. Psychopharmacology. (2017) 234:3195–205. doi: 10.1007/s00213-017-4707-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Luijten M, Littel M, Franken IH. Deficits in inhibitory control in smokers during a Go/NoGo task: an investigation using event-related brain potentials. PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e18898. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Zhao H, Turel O, Brevers D, Bechara A, He Q. Smoking cues impair monitoring but not stopping during response inhibition in abstinent male smokers. Behav Brain Res. (2020) 386:112605. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2020.112605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Starcke K, Antons S, Trotzke P, Brand M. Cue-reactivity in behavioral addictions: a meta-analysis and methodological considerations. J Behav Addict. (2018) 7:227–38. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.39

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. van Holst RJ, van Holstein M, van den Brink W, Veltman DJ, Goudriaan AE. Response inhibition during cue reactivity in problem gamblers: an fMRI study. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e30909. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0030909

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Allen KJ, Hooley JM. Inhibitory control in people who self-injure: evidence for impairment and enhancement. Psychiatry Res. (2015) 225:631–7. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.11.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Burke TA, Allen KJD, Carpenter RW, Siegel DM, Kautz MM, Liu RT, et al. Emotional response inhibition to self-harm stimuli interacts with momentary negative affect to predict nonsuicidal self-injury urges. Behav Res Ther. (2021) 142:103865. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2021.103865

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Association. AP. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013).

Google Scholar

32. Liu YX, Liu J, Wang YF. Reliability and validity of Chinese version of the mini international neuropsychiatric interview for children and adolescents (child version). Chinese Ment Health J. (2010) 25:8–13. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2011.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1960) 23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Yao S, Yang H, Zhu X, Auerbach RP, Abela JR, Pulleyblank RW, et al. An examination of the psychometric properties of the Chinese version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale, 11th version in a sample of Chinese adolescents. Percept Mot Skills. (2007) 104:1169–82. doi: 10.2466/pms.104.4.1169-1182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Martin J, Cloutier PF, Levesque C, Bureau JF, Lafontaine MF, Nixon MK. Psychometric properties of the functions and addictive features scales of the Ottawa Self-Injury Inventory: a preliminary investigation using a university sample. Psychol Assess. (2013) 25:1013–8. doi: 10.1037/a0032575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Delorme A, Makeig S. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J Neurosci Methods. (2004) 134:9–21. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2003.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Chaumon M, Bishop DV, Busch NA. A practical guide to the selection of independent components of the electroencephalogram for artifact correction. J Neurosci Methods. (2015) 250:47–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2015.02.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Folstein JR, Van Petten C. Influence of cognitive control and mismatch on the N2 component of the ERP: a review. Psychophysiology. (2008) 45:152–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00602.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Polich J. Updating P300: an integrative theory of P3a and P3b. Clin Neurophysiol. (2007) 118:2128–48. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Kok A, Ramautar JR, De Ruiter MB, Band GP, Ridderinkhof KR. ERP components associated with successful and unsuccessful stopping in a stop-signal task. Psychophysiology. (2004) 41:9–20. doi: 10.1046/j.1469-8986.2003.00127.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Stock AK, Schulz T, Lenhardt M, Blaszkewicz M, Beste C. High-dose alcohol intoxication differentially modulates cognitive subprocesses involved in response inhibition. Addict Biol. (2016) 21:136–45. doi: 10.1111/adb.12170

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Yin J, Yuan K, Feng D, Cheng J, Li Y, Cai C, et al. Inhibition control impairments in adolescent smokers: electrophysiological evidence from a Go/NoGo study. Brain Imaging Behav. (2016) 10:497–505. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9418-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Clayson PE, Miller GA. Psychometric considerations in the measurement of event-related brain potentials: guidelines for measurement and reporting. Int J Psychophysiol. (2017) 111:57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Clayson PE, Miller GA. ERP Reliability Analysis (ERA) Toolbox: an open-source toolbox for analyzing the reliability of event-related brain potentials. Int J Psychophysiol. (2017) 111:68–79. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.10.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Aichert DS, Wöstmann NM, Costa A, Macare C, Wenig JR, Möller HJ, et al. Associations between trait impulsivity and prepotent response inhibition. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2012) 34:1016–32. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2012.706261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Oddy BW, Barry RJ. The relationship of N2 and P3 to inhibitory processing of social drinkers in a Go/NoGo task. Int J Psychophysiol. (2009) 72:323–30. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Liu RT, Trout ZM, Hernandez EM, Cheek SM, Gerlus N. A behavioral and cognitive neuroscience perspective on impulsivity, suicide, and non-suicidal self-injury: meta-analysis and recommendations for future research. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 83:440–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Hamza CA, Willoughby T, Heffer T. Impulsivity and nonsuicidal self-injury: a review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. (2015) 38:13–24. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.02.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Kreusch F, Quertemont E, Vilenne A, Hansenne M. Alcohol abuse and ERP components in Go/No-go tasks using alcohol-related stimuli: impact of alcohol avoidance. Int J Psychophysiol. (2014) 94:92–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Zheng ZL, Wang PF, Su DQ, Guo WJ, Sun N, Ma YK, et al. Differences in brain reactivity in relation to different types of drug-associated cues and disinhibition among heroin addicts: an ERP study. Acta Psychol Sin. (2020) 52:317–28. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2020.00317

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Dong G, Zhou H, Zhao X. Impulse inhibition in people with Internet addiction disorder: electrophysiological evidence from a Go/NoGo study. Neurosci Lett. (2010) 485:138–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2010.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Brennan GM, Baskin-Sommers AR. Brain-behavior relationships in externalizing: P3 amplitude reduction reflects deficient inhibitory control. Behav Brain Res. (2018) 337:70–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.09.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. López-Caneda E, Cadaveira F, Crego A, Gómez-Suárez A, Corral M, Parada M, et al. Hyperactivation of right inferior frontal cortex in young binge drinkers during response inhibition: a follow-up study. Addiction. (2012) 107:1796–808. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03908.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Zilverstand A, Huang AS, Alia-Klein N, Goldstein RZ. Neuroimaging impaired response inhibition and salience attribution in human drug addiction: a systematic review. Neuron. (2018) 98:886–903. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.03.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Plener PL, Bubalo N, Fladung AK, Ludolph AG, Lulé D. Prone to excitement: adolescent females with Non-suicidal self-injury (NSSI) show altered cortical pattern to emotional and NSS-related material. Psychiatry Res. (2012) 203:146–52. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.12.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Wang GB, Zhang XL, Zhao LY, Sun LL, Wu P, Lu L, et al. Drug-related cues exacerbate decision making and increase craving in heroin addicts at different abstinence times. Psychopharmacology. (2012) 221:701–8. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2617-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Bartholow BD, Lust SA, Tragesser SL. Specificity of P3 event-related potential reactivity to alcohol cues in individuals low in alcohol sensitivity. Psychol Addict Behav. (2010) 24:220–8. doi: 10.1037/a0017705

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Wang J, Dai B. Event-related potentials in a two-choice oddball task of impaired behavioral inhibitory control among males with tendencies towards cybersex addiction. J Behav Addict. (2020) 9:785–96. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: nonsuicidal self-injury, event-related potentials, N2, P3, response inhibition, adolescent

Citation: Zhou D-D, Zhao L, Ma L-L, Hu J-H, Chen R, Jiang Z-H, He X-Q, Peng X-Y, Liu X-Y, Li X, Chen W-J, Wang W and Kuang L (2022) Altered Neural Reactivity in Adolescents With Nonsuicidal Self-Injury During Exposure to Self-Injury Related Cues: Electrophysiological Evidence From a Two-Choice Oddball Paradigm. Front. Psychiatry 13:827480. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.827480

Received: 02 December 2021; Accepted: 17 March 2022;
Published: 05 April 2022.

Edited by:

Konrad Bresin, University of Louisville, United States

Reviewed by:

J. D. Allen, Oberlin College, United States
Mariya Cherkasova, West Virginia University, United States

Copyright © 2022 Zhou, Zhao, Ma, Hu, Chen, Jiang, He, Peng, Liu, Li, Chen, Wang and Kuang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Dong-Dong Zhou, zhoudongdong@cqmu.edu.cn; Li Kuang, kuangli0308@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.