Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 11 April 2022
Sec. Schizophrenia
This article is part of the Research Topic Neuroimaging in Early Intervention in Psychiatry View all 10 articles

Thalamic Shape Abnormalities Differentially Relate to Cognitive Performance in Early-Onset and Adult-Onset Schizophrenia

\r\nDerin Cobia,*Derin Cobia1,2*Chaz RichChaz Rich3Matthew J. SmithMatthew J. Smith4Pedro Engel GonzalezPedro Engel Gonzalez2Will CronenwettWill Cronenwett2John G. CsernanskyJohn G. Csernansky2Lei Wang,\r\nLei Wang2,5
  • 1Department of Psychology and Neuroscience Center, Brigham Young University, Provo, UT, United States
  • 2Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, IL, United States
  • 3Department of Psychology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, IN, United States
  • 4School of Social Work, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, United States
  • 5Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Health, The Ohio State University Wexner Medical Center, Columbus, OH, United States

Early-onset schizophrenia (EOS) shares many biological and clinical features with adult-onset schizophrenia (AOS), but may represent a unique subgroup with greater susceptibility for disease onset and worsened symptomatology and progression, which could potentially derive from exaggerated neurodevelopmental abnormalities. Neurobiological explanations of schizophrenia have emphasized the involvement of deep-brain structures, particularly alterations of the thalamus, which have been linked to core features of the disorder. The aim of this study was to compare thalamic shape abnormalities between EOS and AOS subjects and determine whether unique behavioral profiles related to these differences. It was hypothesized abnormal thalamic shape would be observed in anterior, mediodorsal and pulvinar regions in both schizophrenia groups relative to control subjects, but exacerbated in EOS. Magnetic resonance T1-weighted images were collected from adult individuals with EOS (n = 28), AOS (n = 33), and healthy control subjects (n = 60), as well as collection of clinical and cognitive measures. Large deformation high-dimensional brain mapping was used to obtain three-dimensional surfaces of the thalamus. General linear models were used to compare groups on surface shape features, and Pearson correlations were used to examine relationships between thalamic shape and behavioral measures. Results revealed both EOS and AOS groups demonstrated significant abnormal shape of anterior, lateral and pulvinar thalamic regions relative to CON (all p < 0.007). Relative to AOS, EOS exhibited exacerbated abnormalities in posterior lateral, mediodorsal and lateral geniculate thalamic regions (p = 0.003). Thalamic abnormalities related to worse episodic memory in EOS (p = 0.03) and worse working memory (p = 0.047) and executive functioning (p = 0003) in AOS. Overall, findings suggest thalamic abnormalities are a prominent feature in both early- and late-onset schizophrenia, but exaggerated in EOS and have different brain-behavior profiles for each. The persistence of these abnormalities in adult EOS patients suggests they may represent markers of disrupted neurodevelopment that uniquely relate to the clinical and cognitive aspects of the illness.

Introduction

The onset of psychosis during childhood or early adolescence provides a unique research opportunity to explore the etiology of schizophrenia since children and adolescents with early-onset schizophrenia may represent a more homogenous subgroup associated with severe developmental deficits and greater familial susceptibility for the disorder (1). Additionally, given abnormal neural development is thought to contribute to the modulation of schizophrenia, early-onset schizophrenia offers a window to study the well-established neurodevelopmental hypothesis for schizophrenia (2). The anatomical pattern and the timing of the illness is still unclear, hence studies of early-onset schizophrenia could offer further insight into the pathophysiological process of the disease to precisely differentiate between normal brain development and the disease-associated pathological development.

Previously, it was unknown whether early-onset schizophrenia was an earlier extension of adult-onset schizophrenia or if it represented an independent pathophysiological process (3). Examination of early-onset schizophrenia has provided evidence for the continuity between early- and adult-onset groups since both types broadly share many of the same physiological and psychopathological features (4, 5). However, early-onset schizophrenia has been consistently associated with more severe premorbid psychopathology and cognitive impairment (2), which could potentially derive from an exaggeration of the neurodevelopmental abnormalities usually present in schizophrenia. For example, patients with early onset typically present with more severe premorbid language, motor, and social delays than patients with later onset in adolescence (4). Moreover, a study exploring cognitive differences between first-episode adolescents and first-episode adults with schizophrenia found that early-onset patients performed poorly in language and working memory tasks, as well as exhibited greater motor performance deficits compared with adult-onset (6).

Recent theories about the neurobiology of schizophrenia have emphasized the involvement of deep-brain structures, particularly the thalamus (7, 8). Due to the central role the thalamus plays in the coordination of information flow and cognition (9), dysfunction of this region is often implicated in many of the cardinal symptoms of schizophrenia, such as disorganized thought and executive dysfunction among others (10). Developmentally, the thalamus plays a pivotal role in the genesis of the cerebral cortex, with thalamic input being critical for appropriate functional differentiation of the cortex and intercommunicating regions (11), which is consistent with the neurodevelopmental hypothesis of schizophrenia (2). Neuroimaging studies of early-onset schizophrenia have demonstrated similar, and sometimes more pronounced, patterns of structural brain abnormalities with respect to schizophrenia in general (1214). There is evidence thalamic volume is globally reduced in early-onset subjects (15, 16), with specific volume loss in mediodorsal and pulvinar regions (17).

Most studies to date have examined the features of early-onset schizophrenia in child and adolescent populations (18, 19), with few investigating them later in the course of the illness. Furthermore, there are no studies that have specifically investigated subtle morphological alterations of the thalamus available through shape analytic procedures in early-onset schizophrenia and compared against a matched adult-onset group. The aim of the current study was to utilize high-dimensional surface-mapping to characterize regional abnormalities of the thalamus in well-matched adult groups of early-onset and adult-onset schizophrenia, as well as matched control participants, to assess the persistence of theorized neurobiological exacerbations of altered neurodevelopment in early-onset schizophrenia. Based on previous work (20), it was hypothesized that abnormal shape would be observed in anterior, mediodorsal and pulvinar regions in the schizophrenia groups, but exaggerated in early-onset schizophrenia. Furthermore, it was hypothesized that early-onset-associated shape changes would demonstrate stronger relationships with cognition and psychopathology than those associated with adult-onset schizophrenia.

Materials and Methods

Sample

Participants included 28 individuals with early-onset schizophrenia (EOS), 33 individuals with adult-onset schizophrenia (AOS) and 60 healthy control (CON) participants all group-matched (using random selection) with respect to age, gender, and parental SES. Given schizophrenia is associated with progressive gray matter loss (21, 22), AOS and EOS subjects were also group-matched based on duration of illness. Complete recruitment methods have been described previously (23). The project was approved by the IRB at Washington University in St. Louis, and informed consent was obtained from each subject after a complete description of the study was given.

Clinical Measures

Diagnosis of schizophrenia was determined by the consensus of a research psychiatrist and trained research clinicians using the Structured Clinician Interview for DSM-IV Axis I Disorders [SCID, (24)]. The criteria for coding age of illness onset were adapted from other longitudinal studies of EOS (2) where early-onset was defined as illness onset before 18 years of age, and AOS as onset by 18 years of age or older. Schizophrenia participants were asked to identify the age at which their acute psychotic symptoms first took place, which was provided using self-report during the SCID, as well as cross-referenced with medical records and a research evaluation by a psychiatrist. Duration of illness was computed as years difference between age of illness onset and current age.

The SCID was also used to identify lifetime diagnosis of a substance-use disorder for alcohol, cannabis, cocaine, stimulants, hallucinogens, sedatives, and opioids. Antipsychotic medication for schizophrenia participants was assessed through self-report, with first- and second-generation antipsychotic (FGA and SGA) treatments quantitatively measured based on type, dosage amount, duration of use, and the calculation of chlorpromazine equivalents using published guidelines (25). Nicotine use was estimated using a semi-structured interview adapted from Sullivan et al. (26), and alcohol use via the Lifetime Alcohol Consumption Assessment Procedure (27).

Clinical and Cognitive Assessments

A battery of neuropsychological measures assessing key cognitive domains affected in schizophrenia was administered to all participants (28); raw scores were converted into standardized scores then selected measures were factored into three cognitive domains: working memory, episodic memory, and executive functioning. An index of crystallized intelligence was also derived to estimate the generalized cognitive deficit in psychosis. Some missing data was observed for cognitive variables, which included three CON, two EOS, and two AOS individuals, who were not included in the analyses. Three psychopathology clusters (positive, negative, and disorganized symptoms) were assessed and calculated using global ratings from the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (29) and the Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (29). A full description of the specific measures used is reported in previous work (23).

Image Acquisition

Details of the image acquisition, surface mapping and analysis of subjects can be found in previously published reports (20, 30). Briefly, magnetic resonance scans were collected with a standard head coil on a Siemens Magnetom 1.5T (Erlangen, Germany) scanner using a turbo-FLASH sequence (repetition time = 20 ms, echo time = 5.4 ms, flip angle = 30°, 180 slices, FOV = 256 mm, matrix = 356 × 256, time = 13.5 min) that acquired 1 mm3 isotropic whole-head images. Total brain volume was estimated using an atlas scaling factor (ASF), which is the reciprocal of the determinant of the alignment matrix to Talairach atlas space, and signifies the extent that the brain volume contracts or expands during alignment (31). No between-group differences were observed in the ASF (F2,117 = 1.7, p = 0.19) and thus, was not used as a covariate in statistical analyses.

Surface Mapping

Thalamic surfaces were generated using Large-Deformation High-Dimensional Brain Mapping (HDBM-LD) procedures (32), an atlas-based approach that utilizes diffeomorphic transformations which aligns a template image to a target (i.e., subject) image and allows independent matching of individual surface points to maintain unique morphological features of each subject (33, 34). Validity and reliability for mapping the thalamus were established in previous reports (20, 30). Prior to diffeomorphic transformations, anatomic landmarks were placed by expert raters who were blinded to the group status of the scan being landmarked, detailed landmarking procedures can be found in previous publications (32, 33).

Statistical Analyses

Demographic and clinical characteristics were calculated using chi-squared statistics and analysis of variance (ANOVA) models. Group differences in cognition and psychopathology were also evaluated using ANOVA models.

To examine thalamic volume, a repeated-measures ANOVA was used with hemisphere as the within-subjects effect and group as the between-subjects effect. For thalamic shape, deformation values along each surface were calculated as a contrast from the sample mean based on triangulated surface points for all subjects. Next, a principal components analysis (PCA) was used to reduce the high dimensionality of the surfaces, yielding an orthonormal set of eigenvectors that represented variation in the shape of the structures (33). The first 15 eigenvectors of the PCA accounted for more than 90% of total shape variance (across subjects and hemispheres) and used for subsequent statistical modeling. To evaluate thalamic shape differences across groups, a multivariate analysis of variance (MANOVA) model was utilized with shape variation (using all 15 eigenvectors scores averaged across hemispheres) as the dependent variable, and group status (EOS, AOS, and CON) as a fixed effect. If the overall MANOVA statistic was significant, follow-up MANOVA models were used to identify whether specific significant group contrasts existed (EOS vs. CON and AOS vs. CON). For the EOS vs. AOS contrast, a follow-up multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) model was used to account for the potentially confounding effects of illness duration, medication, and lifetime presence of a substance use disorder, which were included as covariates.

Visualization of group differences in thalamic shape deformation was accomplished by the construction of vertex-wise studentized-t contrast maps of the composite surfaces for each group. Shape displacements were calculated at each surface point as the difference between the means of the group vectors in magnitude and coded using a colored scale; final maps reflected corrected p-values using a familywise error rate approach based on random field theory with a vertex-wise threshold of p < 0.05 and a cluster-wise threshold of p < 0.01 (35). Inward and outward displacements, or deformations, of the surface were estimated as representations of localized volume loss or exaggeration at the neurobiological level (36).

To evaluate the relationship between thalamic shape and behavioral measures, a canonical score was first computed as a representation of composite shape based on all left-right averaged eigenvectors scores of the thalamus (37). Bivariate Pearson correlation coefficients were then calculated between the thalamic canonical shape score and measures of cognition (working memory, episodic memory, and executive functioning) and psychopathology (positive, negative, and disorganized symptoms) separately for EOS and AOS groups.

Sensitivity Power Analysis

Sample sizes for the groups were fixed as data was derived from an archival schizophrenia dataset (8, 38). Sensitivity power analyses were calculated for the proposed models above to determine the smallest possible effect that could be detected from the data considering sample size restrictions. Using G*Power (39), it was determined that at 80% power, with a type I error rate of 0.05, and a combined sample of 121, there was power to detect the following minimal Cohen’s f effect sizes (40): ANOVA models for cognition = 0.29, and psychopathology = 0.26; thalamic volume RM-ANOVA = 0.28 (group effect). For the thalamic surface shape models, minimum Critical F-values were identified using G*Power for the main group effect given their multivariate nature: Three-group MANOVA = 1.51; 2-group MANOVAs for EOS vs. CON = 1.81, EOS vs. AOS = 1.89, AOS vs. CON = 1.79. For the correlation analyses, at a type I error rate of 0.05, there was 80% power to detect a correlation as large as: r = ±0.37 in the EOS group (n = 28); and r = ±0.34 in the AOS group (n = 33). The sample appears adequately powered to address the proposed research hypotheses, with the ability to, at a minimum, detect moderate effects (41). Cohen’s f values were calculated using criteria from Cohen (40) and Lenhard and Lenhard (42).

Results

Demographic, Clinical, and Confounding Variables

Anti-psychotic medication treatment has known effects on brain structure (43), while nicotine has been associated with reduced gray matter density (44), and a history of substance-use disorder can also affect brain morphometry (45). Given these findings, potential group differences for these confounds were examined; demographic and clinical variables are summarized in Table 1. Groups differed with respect to nicotine use, and lifetime histories of substance use disorders for alcohol, cannabis, cocaine, opiates, and sedatives. These variables were subsequently examined as fixed effect covariates in EOS vs. AOS linear models, with an aggregate measure (any lifetime history of a substance use disorder = 1, no lifetime history = 0) used for substance use. The EOS and AOS subjects also differed on mean dose years of first-generation antipsychotic treatment using the chlorpromazine equivalent, which was also included as a fixed effect covariate.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of study sample.

Cognition and Psychopathology

Cognition was compared across all three groups using ANOVA models with group as a fixed factor. Results (see Table 1) revealed a significant main effect of group for crystallized intelligence (F2,113 = 13.7, p < 0.001, Cohen’s f = −2.6), working memory (F2,113 = 29.8, p < 0.001, Cohen’s f = 0.65), episodic memory (F2,113 = 50.5, p < 0.001, Cohen’s f = 0.77), and executive functioning (F2,113 = 32.6, p < 0.001, Cohen’s f = 0.62). For all four cognitive domains, CON scored significantly higher than EOS and AOS (all p-values < 0.001). Contrasts between EOS and AOS did not achieve statistical significance (all p-values > 0.10). Results from ANOVA models evaluating EOS and AOS group differences on positive, negative, and disorganized symptoms were all non-significant.

Thalamic Volume Analyses

For the volume of the thalamus, there was a significant main effect for hemisphere (F1,118 = 13.5, p < 0.001), but not for group (F2,118 = 2.93, p = 0.06, Cohen’s f = 0.173) or a group-by-hemisphere interaction (F2,118 = 0.83, p = 0.44).

Thalamic Shape Analyses

A MANOVA model of thalamic eigenvectors (averaged across hemispheres) revealed an overall significant main effect of group on shape metrics (F2,118 = 2.4, p < 0.001). Post hoc comparisons using two-group MANOVA designs found significant differences between EOS (F1,86 = 2.4, p = 0.007) and AOS (F1,91 = 2.6, p = 0.003) versus CON. In the EOS vs. AOS MANCOVA, there was a significant main effect for group (F15,40 = 2.44, p = 0.012), as well as duration of illness (F15,40 = 4.3, p < 0.001), but not first-generation antipsychotic use, cigarette usage, or lifetime presence of a substance use disorder on the model. Notably, all of the significant F-values surpassed the Critical F thresholds calculated from the sensitivity analyses above.

Visualization of RFT-corrected thalamic shape maps (Figure 1) revealed that EOS was characterized by prominent inward deformation, indicative of localized volume loss, in ventral lateral and lateral geniculate nuclei, as well as in anterior (right only) and pulvinar nuclei relative to CON (Figure 1A). For AOS, notable inward deformations were also observed in pulvinar nuclei in addition to left-sided anterior and lateral regions relative to CON (Figure 1B). Regarding the comparison between EOS and AOS, prominent inward deformations in EOS were observed in posterior ventral and left dorsal regions, as well as in the lateral geniculate nuclei (Figure 1C).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Thalamic surface shape displacement maps between: (A) Early-Onset Schizophrenia (EOS) patients and control (CON) participants; (B) Adult-Onset Schizophrenia (AOS) patients and CON participants; and (C) EOS and AOS patients. Cooler colors indicate significant regions of inward shape differences and warmer colors indicate significant regions of outward shape differences corrected for multiple comparisons using random field theory (RFT).

Correlation Analyses

Calculation of canonical scores for thalamic shape revealed increases in these values equated to greater shape abnormality (i.e., more disparate from the surface shape of the healthy comparison subjects). An outlier canonical score was observed in a single EOS participant (>3 SD above the mean), which was adjusted in to the 3 SD value using Winsorization procedures. In the EOS group, there was an inverse correlation between episodic memory scores and thalamic shape (r = −0.43, p = 0.03; Figure 2A), such that more abnormal thalamic surface shape related to poorer episodic memory performance. In the AOS group, a similar inverse correlation was observed between thalamic shape and working memory (r = −0.36, p = 0.047; Figure 2B) and executive functioning (r = −0.52, p = 0.003; Figure 2C). No other correlations between cognition or psychopathology and brain structure were significant (all p-values > 0.10).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Thalamic shape progressing from healthy control (CON) to schizophrenia (SCZ) correlated with (A) poorer episodic memory performance (r = –0.43, p = 0.03) in early-onset schizophrenia (EOS); (B) poorer working memory performance (r = –0.36, p = 0.047) and (C) poorer executive functioning (r = –0.52, p = 0.003) in adult-onset schizophrenia (AOS).

Discussion

Age of onset continues to represent an important factor for understanding and conceptualizing the pathology associated with the development of schizophrenia (46). This study sought to examine whether morphological differences of the thalamus, a highly implicated structure in the pathophysiology of psychosis, exist between early-onset and adult-onset schizophrenia in adulthood. Findings revealed a hemispheric difference in thalamic volume, but only modest differences between the schizophrenia and control groups, and no significant differences between EOS and AOS. However, shape analysis revealed significant thalamic abnormalities in EOS relative to CON in multiple anterior, posterior, and lateral regions; with similar patterns observed in AOS relative to CON. Examination of differences between the psychosis groups revealed exaggerated localized volume loss in EOS relative to AOS in ventral posterior and medial regions. Multivariate eigenvector models were also highly significant and support the observed vertex-wise group differences. Finally, unique relationships between shape and cognition were noted in the psychosis groups, with EOS demonstrating increased episodic memory impairment, and AOS worse working memory and executive functions, as thalamic shape became increasingly abnormal. Overall, these findings reveal the exaggerated effects of early-onset psychosis in adulthood on a brain structure critical to the pathophysiology of schizophrenia.

Early-onset schizophrenia is described as a condition with greater developmental and premorbid departures relative to AOS, likely due to a stronger genetic component to their presentation (2). This exacerbated presentation often leads to poorer clinical and cognitive outcomes (47), which has prompted considerations for focused treatments (48). Furthermore, unique brain abnormalities observed in EOS are generally more neurobiologically severe that in AOS (4). Summarized by Brent et al. (49), the most consistent findings include cortical abnormalities of frontal, temporal, and parietal regions, in addition to reduced global cerebral and cerebellar volumes. Furthermore, there is strong support for reduced thalamic volumes in EOS (5052), with some evidence for progressive loss over time (17). While previous work has examined the gross anatomical volumetrics of the thalamus in EOS, the regional specificity of these abnormalities has yet to be characterized. This is especially relevant given the diverse connectivity matrix and unique nuclei specialization contained within the organization of the thalamus (9). Results from the current study revealed distinct patterns of abnormal shape, representative of localized volume loss, in EOS participants relative to AOS and healthy-matched individuals. Specifically, EOS was noted to have widespread inward deformations in ventral lateral regions, lateral geniculate nuclei, and in anterior and pulvinar nuclei relative to CON. The AOS group demonstrated a similar pattern relative to CON, but with more diffuse changes in lateral aspects and relative sparing in right regions. When the schizophrenia groups were compared directly against each other, it was observed EOS showed significant abnormal inward deformation in posterior ventral and dorsal regions, and in the lateral geniculate nucleus. The results suggest a pattern of abnormal thalamic shape in EOS that is similar to, but exacerbated, relative to AOS, which strongly implicates and supports a continuum model for the neurobiology of schizophrenia (53). Our finding is consistent with other neuroimaging work that found cortical gray matter loss in EOS is exaggerated, but mimics that in AOS (54). Furthermore, changes in global gray matter brain volume also support a neurodevelopmental continuum in psychosis as noted in a study examining these features in the offspring of probands with schizophrenia (55). Overall, neuroimaging markers, especially those involved in the pathophysiology of schizophrenia such as the thalamus, appear to be a robust approach for supporting a dimensional model of disease onset in psychosis-spectrum disorders (56).

Our pattern of findings within these onset types are broadly consistent with previous work on thalamic morphology from our group using different derivations of the sample, and include primarily alterations in anterior and posterior extremes in chronic cases (20), and a similar presentation in siblings (30). However, research using different methodology and sample compositions also provide consistent support for our results. For example, a study conducted by Janssen et al. (17) on thalamic volumes in an adolescent sample of male-only early-onset psychosis patients revealed regional volume loss in anterior mediodorsal and pulvinar areas in the right thalamus using a surface-based approach. In addition, using a combined voxel-based morphometry and novel thalamic nuclei segmentation procedure, Huang et al. (57) identified smaller pulvinar volumes in a large sample of youths with psychotic spectrum disorders. And in another recent report, Zhang et al. (58) observed abnormal functional connectivity (both hyper-and hypoconnectivity relative to healthy individuals) in the thalamocortical circuits of an EOS sample that included lateral and mediodorsal nuclei. While there is agreement between the above studies, the absence of anterior and ventral lateral abnormalities which was observed in our work is noted. This discrepancy could reflect the mean age difference in the samples used; as previously noted, our sample consisted of adult-aged EOS subjects while others were of early-onset adolescents. Thus, our findings may reflect an exaggerated pattern of abnormality that occurs as EOS ages into adulthood, which meaningfully informs an anticipated trajectory of development for these individuals, particularly in reference to AOS counterparts.

Cognitive dysfunction is a known feature in EOS (59) and has a general profile similar to that observed in AOS (60). We found that across various cognitive domains both EOS and AOS were significantly impaired relative to the healthy control group in crystallized IQ, working memory, episodic memory, and executive functioning, but did not significantly differ from each other in these domains. This is consistent with the known level of impairment observed in previous work on EOS where aspects of working memory, episodic memory and executive functioning are generally impaired to the same degree at AOS (60). The only exception to this literature is we found no difference between groups in crystallized IQ where other studies have found this domain to be more impaired in EOS (60). Again, it is important to note our comparisons were conducted on adult-aged patients, regardless of onset status, the EOS cognitive profiles we examined were ∼20 years post-onset. Work examining the longitudinal course of cognition in EOS as they transition into adulthood observes a broad attenuation of cognitive development relative to peers with no further decline after that (59, 6163). Thus, it appears cognitive trajectories of EOS mimic that of AOS into adulthood.

Studies of neuroimaging markers for cognitive impairment in AOS are relatively plentiful (6467), while much fewer have been conducted in EOS (68, 69). The behavioral substrates of affected thalamic nuclei we observed from the unique shape deformation patterns in EOS and AOS suggests these brain features may partially explain their observed cognitive impairment. In particular, higher-order aspects of cognitive control are known to involve the mediodorsal thalamus (7072), while attentional and memory processes involve the pulvinar (73), and the anterior thalamus is implicated in episodic memory as part of Papez’ circuit (74). Clues regarding the relevance of thalamic involvement in cognition are well-detailed in experimental studies of animal mechanisms. For example, interrogations of mediodorsal nuclei in mice has revealed that excitation of this region is critical for sustaining task-related activity of the prefrontal cortex (75), and can also assist in modulating decision-making abnormalities via separate pathways between these areas (76). Linking disruption of thalamocortical circuitry with behavioral dysfunction in animal models such as these has yielded new insights into the pathophysiology of schizophrenia (77), and reinforced many findings from human imaging studies such as those described here. As such, abnormalities of the thalamus are increasingly considered putative biomarkers of schizophrenia given their endophenotypic potential with cognitive functioning and predictive ability for functional outcome and disease burden (78). While both EOS and AOS groups in our study demonstrated significant cognitive-thalamic relationships, the nature of these relationships differed. For EOS, it appears thalamic shape abnormalities in combined bilateral pulvinar and ventrolateral, with right anterior regions, strongly related to poorer performance in episodic memory. For AOS it was also bilateral pulvinar, but also left anterior and dorsolateral regions that related to poorer working memory and executive functioning. Given cognitive dysfunction in all these domains is a common feature for both groups, it is interesting to observe that a potential substrate for the impairment was not common. The development of brain features in EOS clearly differs from that of AOS, both in pattern and timing (18, 7981). How these features relate to the maturation of cognitive functioning across childhood, adolescence and eventually adulthood is unclear and not well-studied. Our findings provide some insight into this process insomuch that, at least in adulthood, the possible underlying mechanisms of equivalently observed dysfunction are ultimately separate. This highlights the persistent conversation of heterogeneity in schizophrenia and its relevance to diagnosis, progression, treatment, and outcome (8285).

One primary limitation to our study was the relatively small sample sizes of each group. To address this we conducted a sensitivity analysis for each model, which revealed sufficient power to detect even small-to-moderate effects in the aforementioned analyses. Given many of the Cohen’s f effect sizes, critical F, and r values for the shape MANOVA models and correlations were moderate-to-large, we believe the findings were not likely spurious or underpowered.

Conclusion

Our research findings suggest that abnormalities of the thalamus are a prominent feature in both early- and late-onset schizophrenia. Using shape analyses we determined that the region pattern of these abnormalities was relatively similar between the early and late onset groups, occurring primarily in pulvinar, anterior and lateral regions. However, early-onset subjects demonstrated exaggerated abnormalities in ventral, left dorsomedial and lateral geniculate regions relative to adult-onset. Interestingly, abnormal thalamic shape features differentially related to cognition in each group – episodic memory for early-onset and working memory and executive functioning for adult-onset. These differences may be potentially useful as markers to understanding the developmental effects of schizophrenia onset on the neurobiology and cognitive functioning of this condition.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets presented in this study can be found in online repositories. The names of the repository/repositories and accession number(s) can be found below: http://schiz connect.org.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the IRB at Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO, United States. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DC and MS were responsible for study design, data analysis and interpretation, and drafting of the manuscript. CR and PE were responsible for data analysis and manuscript drafting. WC was responsible for manuscript drafting. JC and LW were responsible for study design and data analysis and interpretation. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by funding from the National Institutes of Health (R01 MH056584 and P50 MH071616 to JC; R01 MH084803, U01 MH097435, and R01 EB020062 to LW; and NINDS T32 NS047987 to DC) and National Science Foundation (NSF SP0037646 and NSF BCS 1734853 to LW). Funding sources had no further role in study design; collection, analysis, and interpretation of data; writing of the report; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Conflict of Interest

JC is a consultant for Indivior Pharmaceuticals. DC is a consultant for Sage Pharmaceuticals.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We acknowledge research staff at the Northwestern University Schizophrenia Research Group (NU-SRG) and Conte Center for the Neuroscience of Mental Disorders at Washington University School of Medicine for clinical and neuropsychological assessments, and database management. We also acknowledge the Northwestern University Neuroimaging and Applied Computational Anatomy Laboratory (NIACAL) for image processing.

References

1. Douaud G, Mackay C, Andersson J, James S, Quested D, Ray M, et al. Schizophrenia delays and alters maturation of the brain in adolescence. Brain. (2009) 132:2437–448. doi: 10.1093/brain/awp126

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Rapoport JL, Addington AM, Frangou S, Psych MRC. The neurodevelopmental model of schizophrenia: update 2005. Mol Psychiatr. (2005) 10:434–49. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001642

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Watkins JM, Asarnow RF, Tanguay PE. Symptom development in childhood onset schizophrenia. J Child Psychol Psyc. (1988) 29:865–78. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1988.tb00759.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Nicolson R, Lenane M, Hamburger SD, Fernandez T, Bedwell J, Rapoport JL. Lessons from childhood-onset schizophrenia. Brain Res Rev. (2000) 31:147–56. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00032-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Jacobsen LK, Rapoport JL. Research update: childhood-onset schizophrenia: implications of clinical and neurobiological research. J Child Psychol Psyc. (1998) 39:101–13. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00305

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. White T, Ho B-C, Ward J, O’Leary D, Andreasen NC. Neuropsychological performance in first-episode adolescents with schizophrenia: a comparison with first-episode adults and adolescent control subjects. Biol Psychiat. (2006) 60:463–71. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Andrews J, Wang L, Csernansky JG, Gado MH, Barch DM. Abnormalities of thalamic activation and cognition in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. (2006) 163:469. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.163.3.463

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Cobia DJ, Smith MJ, Salinas I, Ng C, Gado M, Csernansky JG, et al. Progressive deterioration of thalamic nuclei relates to cortical network decline in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2017) 180:27.

Google Scholar

9. Halassa MM, Sherman SM. Thalamocortical circuit motifs: a general framework. Neuron. (2019) 103:762–70. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Byne W, Hazlett EA, Buchsbaum MS, Kemether E. The thalamus and schizophrenia: current status of research. Acta Neuropathol. (2009) 117:368. doi: 10.1007/s00401-008-0404-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Deck M, Lokmane L, Chauvet S, Mailhes C, Keita M, Niquille M, et al. Pathfinding of corticothalamic axons relies on a rendezvous with thalamic projections. Neuron. (2013) 77:484. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.11.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Greenstein DK, Lerch J, Shaw P, Clasen L, Giedd J, Gochman P, et al. Childhood onset schizophrenia: cortical brain abnormalities as young adults. J Child Psychol Psyc. (2006) 47:1012. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01658.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Giedd JN, Jeffries NO, Blumenthal J, Castellanos FX, Vaituzis AC, Fernandez T, et al. Childhood-onset schizophrenia: progressive brain changes during adolescence. Biol Psychiat. (1999) 46:892–8. doi: 10.1016/s0006-3223(99)00072-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Johnson SLM, Wang L, Alpert KI, Greenstein D, Clasen L, Lalonde F, et al. Hippocampal shape abnormalities of patients with childhood-onset schizophrenia and their unaffected siblings. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2013) 52:527.e–36.e. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Sowell ER, Levitt J, Thompson PM, Holmes CJ, Blanton RE, Kornsand DS, et al. Brain abnormalities in early-onset schizophrenia spectrum disorder observed with statistical parametric mapping of structural magnetic resonance images. Am J Psychiat. (2000) 157:1475–84. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.9.1475

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Sowell ER, Toga AW, Asarnow R. Brain abnormalities observed in childhood-onset schizophrenia: a review of the structural magnetic resonance imaging literature. Ment Retard Dev D R. (2000) 6:180–5. doi: 10.1002/1098-2779(2000)6:3<180::AID-MRDD5>3.0.CO;2-I

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Janssen J, Alemán-Goméz Y, Reig S, Schnack HG, Parellada M, Graell M, et al. Regional specificity of thalamic volume deficits in male adolescents with early-onset psychosis. Brit J Psychiat. (2012) 200:30–6. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.111.093732

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Gurholt TP, Lonning V, Nerland S, Jørgensen KN, Haukvik UK, Alloza C, et al. Intracranial and subcortical volumes in adolescents with early-onset psychosis: a multisite mega-analysis from the ENIGMA consortium. Hum Brain Mapp. (2020) 43:373–84. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25212

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Zhou H, Shi L, Shen Y, Fang Y, He Y, Li H, et al. Altered topographical organization of grey matter structural network in early-onset schizophrenia. Psychiat Res Neuroimaging. (2021) 316:111344. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2021.111344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Csernansky JG, Schindler MK, Splinter NR, Wang L, Gado M, Selemon LD, et al. Abnormalities of thalamic volume and shape in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. (2004) 161:896–902. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.5.896

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Cobia DJ, Smith MJ, Wang L, Csernansky JG. Longitudinal progression of frontal and temporal lobe changes in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2012) 139:6. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Haren NEM, van Schnack HG, Cahn W, Heuvel MP, van den Lepage C, Collins L, et al. Changes in cortical thickness during the course of illness in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat. (2011) 68:880. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.88

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Delawalla Z, Barch DM, Eastep JLF, Thomason ES, Hanewinkel MJ, Thompson PA, et al. Factors mediating cognitive deficits and psychopathology among siblings of individuals with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bull. (2006) 32:537. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbj082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Spitzer M, Robert L, Gibbon M, Williams J. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Non-patient Edition (SCID-I/NP). New York, NY: New York State Psychiatric Institute (2002).

Google Scholar

25. Andreasen NC, Pressler M, Nopoulos P, Miller D, Ho B-C. Antipsychotic dose equivalents and dose-years: a standardized method for comparing exposure to different drugs. Biol Psychiat. (2010) 67:262. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.08.040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Sullivan EV, Rosenbloom MJ, Lim KO, Pfefferbaum A. Longitudinal changes in cognition, gait, and balance in abstinent and relapsed alcoholic men: relationships to changes in brain structure. Neuropsychology. (2000) 14:188. doi: 10.1037//0894-4105.14.2.178

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Skinner HA. Development and Validation of a Lifetime Alcohol Consumption Assessment Procedure. Toronto, ON: Addiction Research Foundation Substudy (1982).

Google Scholar

28. Nuechterlein KH, Barch DM, Gold JM, Goldberg TE, Green MF, Heaton RK. Identification of separable cognitive factors in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res. (2004) 72:39. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Andreasen NC. Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms. Iowa City, IA: University of Iowa (1984).

Google Scholar

30. Harms MP, Wang L, Mamah D, Barch DM, Thompson PA, Csernansky JG. Thalamic shape abnormalities in individuals with schizophrenia and their nonpsychotic siblings. J Neurosci. (2007) 27:13842. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2571-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Buckner RL, Head D, Parker J, Fotenos AF, Marcus D, Morris JC, et al. A unified approach for morphometric and functional data analysis in young, old, and demented adults using automated atlas-based head size normalization: reliability and validation against manual measurement of total intracranial volume. Neuroimage. (2004) 23:738. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Csernansky JG, Wang L, Joshi SC, Ratnanather JT, Miller MI. Computational anatomy and neuropsychiatric disease: probabilistic assessment of variation and statistical inference of group difference, hemispheric asymmetry, and time-dependent change. Neuroimage. (2004) 23:S5668. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Beg M, Miller M, Trouvé A, Younes L. Computing large deformation metric mappings via geodesic flows of diffeomorphisms. Int J Comput Vis. (2005) 61:139–57. doi: 10.1023/b:visi.0000043755.93987.aa

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Miller MI, Trouvé A, Younes L. On the metrics of euler-lagrange equations of computational anatomy. Annu Rev Biomed Eng. (2002) 4:375–405. doi: 10.1146/annurev.bioeng.4.092101.125733

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Flandin G, Friston KJ. Topological Inference. In: Toga W editor. Methods and Modeling. Waltham: Academic Press (2015). p. 495–500. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-397025-1.00322-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Hanko V, Apple AC, Alpert KI, Warren KN, Schneider JA, Arfanakis K, et al. In vivo hippocampal subfield shape related to TDP-43, amyloid beta, and tau pathologies. Neurobiol Aging. (2019) 74:181. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2018.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Wang L, Mamah D, Harms MP, Karnik M, Price JL, Gado MH, et al. Progressive deformation of deep brain nuclei and hippocampal-amygdala formation in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiat. (2008) 64:1060–8. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.08.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Womer FY, Wang L, Alpert KI, Smith MJ, Csernansky JG, Barch DM, et al. Basal ganglia and thalamic morphology in schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Psychiat Res Neuroimaging. (2014) 223:83. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang A-G, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences. Amsterdam: Elsevier Science (2013).

Google Scholar

41. Selya AS, Rose JS, Dierker LC, Hedeker D, Mermelstein RJ. A practical guide to calculating cohen’s F2, a measure of local effect size, from PROC MIXED. Front Psychol. (2012) 3:111. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00111

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Lenhard W, Lenhard A. Calculation of Effect Sizes. (2016). Available online at: https://www.psychometrica.de/effect_size.html (accessed July 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

43. Voineskos AN, Mulsant BH, Dickie EW, Neufeld NH, Rothschild AJ, Whyte EM, et al. Effects of antipsychotic medication on brain structure in patients with major depressive disorder and psychotic features. JAMA Psychiat. (2020) 77:36. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Swan GE, Lessov-Schlaggar CN. The effects of tobacco smoke and nicotine on cognition and the brain. Neuropsychol Rev. (2007) 17:273. doi: 10.1007/s11065-007-9035-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Smith MJ, Cobia DJ, Reilly JL, Gilman JM, Roberts AG, Alpert KI, et al. Cannabis-related episodic memory deficits and hippocampal morphological differences in healthy individuals and schizophrenia subjects. Hippocampus. (2015) 25:1042–51. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22427

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. DeLisi LE. The significance of age of onset for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bull. (1992) 18:209–15. doi: 10.1093/schbul/18.2.209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Schimmelmann BG, Conus P, Cotton S, McGorry PD, Lambert M. Pre-treatment, baseline, and outcome differences between early-onset and adult-onset psychosis in an epidemiological cohort of 636 first-episode patients. Schizophr Res. (2007) 95:1–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2007.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Giannitelli M, Consoli A, Raffin M, Jardri R, Levinson DF, Cohen D, et al. An overview of medical risk factors for childhood psychosis: implications for research and treatment. Schizophr Res. (2018) 192:39–49. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2017.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Brent BK, Thermenos HW, Keshavan MS, Seidman LJ. Gray matter alterations in schizophrenia high-risk youth and early-onset schizophrenia a review of structural MRI findings. Child Adol Psych Cl. (2013) 22:689–714. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2013.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. James AC, James S, Smith DM, Javaloyes A. Cerebellar, prefrontal cortex, and thalamic volumes over two time points in adolescent-onset schizophrenia. Am J Psychiat. (2004) 161:1029. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.161.6.1023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Frazier JA, Giedd JN, Hamburger SD, Albus KE, Kaysen D, Vaituzis AC, et al. Brain anatomic magnetic resonance imaging in childhood-onset schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat. (1996) 53:617. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830070065010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Rapoport JL, Giedd J, Kumra S, Jacobsen L, Smith A, Lee P, et al. Childhood-onset schizophrenia: progressive ventricular change during adolescence. Arch Gen Psychiat. (1997) 54:897–903. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830220013002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. DeRosse P, Karlsgodt KH. Examining the psychosis continuum. Curr Behav Neurosci Rep. (2015) 2:80–9. doi: 10.1007/s40473-015-0040-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Gogtay N. Cortical brain development in schizophrenia: insights from neuroimaging studies in childhood-onset schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bull. (2008) 34:36. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Sugranyes G, de la Serna E, Borras R, Sanchez-Gistau V, Pariente JC, Romero S, et al. Clinical, cognitive, and neuroimaging evidence of a neurodevelopmental continuum in offspring of probands with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Schizophrenia Bull. (2017) 43:1208–19. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbx002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Driver DI, Gogtay N, Rapoport JL. Childhood onset schizophrenia and early onset schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Child Adol Psych. (2013) 22:539–55. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2013.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Huang AS, Rogers BP, Sheffield JM, Jalbrzikowski ME, Anticevic A, Blackford JU, et al. Thalamic nuclei volumes in psychotic disorders and in youths with psychosis spectrum symptoms. Am J Psychiat. (2020) 177:1159–67. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.19101099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Zhang M, Palaniyappan L, Deng M, Zhang W, Pan Y, Fan Z, et al. Abnormal thalamocortical circuit in adolescents with early-onset schizophrenia. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiat. (2020) 60:903. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2020.07.903

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Frangou S. Neurocognition in early-onset schizophrenia. Child Adol Psych. (2013) 22:715–26. doi: 10.1016/j.chc.2013.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Harvey PD, Isner EC. Cognition, social cognition, and functional capacity in early-onset schizophrenia. Child Adol Psych Cl. (2020) 29:171–82.

Google Scholar

61. Frangou S, Hadjulis M, Vourdas A. The maudsley early onset schizophrenia study: cognitive function over a 4-year follow-up period. Schizophrenia Bull. (2008) 34:52–9. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbm124

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Juuhl-Langseth M, Holmén A, Thormodsen R, Øie M, Rund BR. Relative stability of neurocognitive deficits in early onset schizophrenia spectrum patients. Schizophr Res. (2014) 156:241–7. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.04.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Øie MG, Sundet K, Haug E, Zeiner P, Klungsøyr O, Rund BR. Cognitive performance in early-onset schizophrenia and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a 25-year follow-up study. Front Psychol. (2021) 11:606365. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.606365

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Glahn DC, Ragland JD, Abramoff A, Barrett J, Laird AR, Bearden CE, et al. Beyond hypofrontality: a quantitative meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of working memory in schizophrenia. Hum Brain Mapp. (2005) 25:69. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20138

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Minzenberg MJ, Laird AR, Thelen S, Carter CS, Glahn DC. Meta-analysis of 41 functional neuroimaging studies of executive function in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiat. (2009) 66:822. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.91

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Goghari VM III, Macdonald AW, Sponheim SR. Relationship between prefrontal gray matter volumes and working memory performance in schizophrenia: a family study. Schizophr Res. (2014) 153:121. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.01.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Ehrlich S, Brauns S, Yendiki A, Ho B-C, Calhoun V, Schulz SC, et al. Associations of cortical thickness and cognition in patients with schizophrenia and healthy controls. Schizophrenia Bull. (2012) 38:1062.

Google Scholar

68. Tamnes CK, Agartz I. White matter microstructure in early-onset schizophrenia: a systematic review of diffusion tensor imaging studies. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiat. (2016) 55:269–79. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2016.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Arsalidou M, Yaple Z, Jurcik T, Ushakov V. Cognitive brain signatures of youth with early onset and relatives with schizophrenia: evidence from FMRI meta-analyses. Schizophrenia Bull. (2020) 46:857–68. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbz130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Ferguson B, Delevich K. Mediodorsal thalamus and prefrontal cortex: specialized partners in cognitive control. J Neurosci. (2020) 40:5515–7. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0820-20.2020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Halassa MM, Kastner S. Thalamic functions in distributed cognitive control. Nat Neurosci. (2017) 20:1679. doi: 10.1038/s41593-017-0020-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Phillips JM, Fish LR, Kambi NA, Redinbaugh MJ, Mohanta S, Kecskemeti SR, et al. Topographic organization of connections between prefrontal cortex and mediodorsal thalamus: evidence for a general principle of indirect thalamic pathways between directly connected cortical areas. Neuroimage. (2019) 189:846. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.01.078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Guedj C, Vuilleumier P. Functional connectivity fingerprints of the human pulvinar: decoding its role in cognition. Neuroimage. (2020) 221:117162. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2020.117162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Nishio Y, Hashimoto M, Ishii K, Ito D, Mugikura S, Takahashi S, et al. Multiple thalamo-cortical disconnections in anterior thalamic infarction: implications for thalamic mechanisms of memory and language. Neuropsychologia. (2014) 53:273. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.11.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Schmitt LI, Wimmer RD, Nakajima M, Happ M, Mofakham S, Halassa MM. Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains attentional control. Nature. (2017) 545:219–23. doi: 10.1038/nature22073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Mukherjee A, Lam NH, Wimmer RD, Halassa MM. Thalamic circuits for independent control of prefrontal signal and noise. Nature. (2021) 600:100–4. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-04056-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Schmitt LI, Halassa MM. Interrogating the mouse thalamus to correct human neurodevelopmental disorders. Mol Psychiatr. (2017) 22:183–91. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Steullet P. Thalamus-related anomalies as candidate mechanism-based biomarkers for psychosis. Schizophr Res. (2020) 226:147–57. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2019.05.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Kyriakopoulos M, Frangou S. Pathophysiology of early onset schizophrenia. Int Rev Psychiatr. (2009) 19:315–24. doi: 10.1080/09540260701486258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Toga AW, Thompson PM, Sowell ER. Mapping brain maturation. Trends Neurosci. (2006) 29:148–59. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2006.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Chung Y, Allswede D, Addington J, Bearden CE, Cadenhead K, Cornblatt B, et al. Cortical abnormalities in youth at clinical high-risk for psychosis: findings from the NAPLS2 cohort. Neuroimage Clin. (2019) 23:101862. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2019.101862

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Wolfers T, Rokicki J, Alnæs D, Berthet P, Agartz I, Kia SM, et al. Replicating extensive brain structural heterogeneity in individuals with schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Hum Brain Mapp. (2021) 42:2546–55. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Mizuno Y, McCutcheon RA, Brugger SP, Howes OD. Heterogeneity and efficacy of antipsychotic treatment for schizophrenia with or without treatment resistance: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychopharmacol. (2019) 45:622–31. doi: 10.1038/s41386-019-0577-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Fusar-Poli P, Cappucciati M, Borgwardt S, Woods SW, Addington J, Nelson B, et al. Heterogeneity of psychosis risk within individuals at clinical high risk: a meta-analytical stratification. JAMA Psychiat. (2016) 73:120. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2015.2324

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Collado-Torres L, Burke EE, Peterson A, Shin J, Straub RE, Rajpurohit A, et al. Regional heterogeneity in gene expression, regulation, and coherence in the frontal cortex and hippocampus across development and schizophrenia. Neuron. (2019) 103:203216.e8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: psychosis, development, MRI, surface-mapping, neuroimaging

Citation: Cobia D, Rich C, Smith MJ, Engel Gonzalez P, Cronenwett W, Csernansky JG and Wang L (2022) Thalamic Shape Abnormalities Differentially Relate to Cognitive Performance in Early-Onset and Adult-Onset Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 13:803234. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.803234

Received: 27 October 2021; Accepted: 02 March 2022;
Published: 11 April 2022.

Edited by:

Jihane Homman-Ludiye, Monash University, Australia

Reviewed by:

Michael M. Halassa, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, United States
Wen Qin, Tianjin Medical University General Hospital, China

Copyright © 2022 Cobia, Rich, Smith, Engel Gonzalez, Cronenwett, Csernansky and Wang. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Derin Cobia, derin_cobia@byu.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.