Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 25 November 2021
Sec. Computational Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Gearing up for Phase II: Front-Line Issues in MRI Marker Development for Psychiatric Disorders View all 6 articles

Modeling Heterogeneous Brain Dynamics of Depression and Melancholia Using Energy Landscape Analysis

  • 1Division of Information Science, Graduate School of Science and Technology, Nara Institute of Science and Technology, Ikoma, Japan
  • 2Department of Computer Science, College of Engineering, University of the Philippines Diliman, Quezon City, Philippines
  • 3Center for Brain, Mind and KANSEI Research Sciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan
  • 4Department of Neurology, Faculty of Medicine, Shimane University, Izumo, Japan
  • 5School of Medicine, Fujita Health University, Toyoake, Japan
  • 6Department of Psychiatry and Neurosciences, Hiroshima University, Hiroshima, Japan

Our current understanding of melancholic depression is shaped by its position in the depression spectrum. The lack of consensus on how it should be treated—whether as a subtype of depression, or as a distinct disorder altogethe—interferes with the recovery of suffering patients. In this study, we analyzed brain state energy landscape models of melancholic depression, in contrast to healthy and non-melancholic energy landscapes. Our analyses showed significant group differences on basin energy, basin frequency, and transition dynamics in several functional brain networks such as basal ganglia, dorsal default mode, and left executive control networks. Furthermore, we found evidences suggesting the connection between energy landscape characteristics (basin characteristics) and depressive symptom scores (BDI-II and SHAPS). These results indicate that melancholic depression is distinguishable from its non-melancholic counterpart, not only in terms of depression severity, but also in brain dynamics.

1. Introduction

Depression and melancholia are synonymous terms in common speech, but have different technical meanings in psychiatry (1). While depression is associated with “deepened or prolonged sadness in everyday life,” melancholia is predominantly marked by “loss of pleasure or interest” (or anhedonia), along with many other depressive symptoms (1, 2). In the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, third edition (DSM-III), depression and melancholia were clumped together into a single disorder: the major depressive disorder (MDD) (3). However, this has been rectified in DSM-IV, albeit not entirely, by recognizing melancholia as a specifier for depression (4). The recognition has attracted research on melancholic depression as a distinct subtype of depression, which has been neglected before in clinical assessment and treatment (2, 5, 6).

Since then, there has been significant thrust on classifying melancholic depression. Because of the overlapping symptoms between depression and melancholia, researchers have been focusing on distinguishing the two. This distinction is important for both diagnosis and treatment, since melancholic patients (i.e., MDD with melancholic features) may have different responses to treatment (2). For example, they respond better to tricyclic antidepressants (TCAS) than to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) (7). On the contrary, they do not respond well with psychotherapy and placebo, as compared to non-melancholic patients (8). Thus clearly, correct diagnosis of melancholic depression is the first step toward the right treatment.

In the recent years, computational and statistical models have been used to differentiate melancholic from non-melancholic depression (9). The complexity and design of these models range from fundamental statistical analysis (1013) to machine learning models (1416). In most of these studies, they analyzed neuroimaging data to identify the brain activities involved in melancholic depression (1116). In particular, they extracted functional connections from resting state functional MRI (rs-fMRI) data (1316). Functional connectivity analysis (FCA) provides insights on which brain regions are temporally correlated, and is useful for studying brain networks and neural circuit dynamics (14). However, FCA-based models assume that pairwise interactions between brain regions are independent of each other, and thus, may disregard information about higher-order interactions (17).

Our research further explores the brain dynamics of melancholic depression. Several neuropsychiatric disorders are rooted in cognitive dysfunctions caused by disruptions in the dynamics of functional brain networks (18, 19). Evidences from previous FCA studies show that functional disconnections are prevalent in melancholic depression (13, 14). Here, we use energy landscape analysis (ELA) as an alternative to FCA. Embedded in ELA is the pairwise maximum entropy model (P-MEM), which estimates both individual activities and pairwise interactions of brain regions (17). Compared to FCA, P-MEM has been shown to be more physiologically informative since it infers global activity patterns instead of independent pairwise interactions. Furthermore, ELA accurately models the structure of multistable brain networks by reflecting both anatomical and functional connections in rs-fMRI data (17).

In ELA, the brain is characterized as a dynamic system that switches to multiple stable states (20). Each state is defined by its energy, which is inversely proportional to its probability of occurrence; thus, states with lower energy have higher probability. And since the states will vary in energy level, the energy landscape of these states can be portrayed by “peaks” (unstable) and “basins” (stable states), such that the brain has the tendency to “roll” down the basins. It would be interesting to see if this imagery of the brain being mired in a basin translates to the depressive feeling of being in a stuck state (21).

This study examines the energy landscapes of melancholic depression. Here, we show that melancholic depression exhibits a distinct energy landscape, in which its switching dynamics will be different from that of non-melancholic and healthy brain networks. By building an energy landscape model for melancholic depression, we further delineate the boundary between melancholia and depression; not necessarily to set them apart, but rather to clear the obfuscations that currently entangle them.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Participants

This study is a secondary analysis of rs-fMRI data of healthy and depressed volunteers recruited starting in 2012 from four medical institutions in Hiroshima, Japan (15). Prior to the administration of any experimental procedure, written informed consent was obtained from all participants. The experiments were carried out in accordance with relevant guidelines and regulations, and all our experimental protocols were approved by the Ethics Committee of Hiroshima University. The mental condition of the volunteers were evaluated using Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I) according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed. (DSM-IV) criteria (22). Initial screening resulted to 281 healthy participants with no history of any mental or neurological disorder, and 281 diagnosed with major depressive disorder (MDD).

Additional screening of participants was performed to emphasize the boundary between groups in the data set. First, the participants were screened based on their Beck Depression Inventory-II scores (BDI-II; healthy ≤ 13; depressed ≥ 20) (23). Second, participants were removed if they have incomplete data during interview or excessive head movements during fMRI recording. Third, healthy female participants were randomly sampled to match the number of healthy males. Last, the depressed participants were categorized into the melancholic group if they exhibited melancholic features (M.I.N.I, DSM-IV criteria) (22). Otherwise, they are categorized into the non-melancholic group.

In the end, a total of 142 healthy (71 F / 71 M) and 120 depressed (60 F / 60 M) participants were screened and selected for analysis. Within the depressed group, there were 89 melancholic (44 F / 45 M) and 31 non-melancholic (16 F / 15 M) participants. Details on their demographic data are summarized in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of healthy and depressed participants.

2.2. Data Acquisition and Preprocessing

We recorded resting-state functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (rs-fMRI) data of the participants using gradient echo planar imaging (EPI) sequences. The imaging device and parameters differ depending on the recording site (15). During recording, participants were instructed to focus on a cross mark displayed on a monitor, and to avoid thinking of anything or falling asleep. The fMRI recording lasted for 5–10 min, depending on the recording site (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Imaging protocols for different fMRI recording sites in Hiroshima.

Data preprocessing for the rs-fMRI data was performed using SPM8 (Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, UK) on Matlab (Mathworks inc., USA). The preprocessing procedure included slice-timing correction, mean image realignment, normalization and resampling through segmentation of structural image aligned with the mean functional image, and smoothing with an isotropic 6 mm full-width half-maximum Gaussian kernel (24). The potential confounding effects (i.e., the temporal fluctuations of the white matter, cerebrospinal fluid, and the global signal, as well as six head motion parameters) were linearly regressed out from the fMRI time series to remove the physiological noise and motion artifacts (25, 26). These components were determined by the T1 images, which were simultaneously recorded with the rs-fMRI data. Finally, head motion artifacts were scrubbed from the functional images based on the relative changes (i.e., translational displacements along X, Y, and Z axes, and rotational displacements of pitch, yaw, and roll) between the image frames through time, with a frame-wise displacement (FD) threshold = 0.5 mm (27).

2.3. Functional Brain Networks

When selecting the regions of interest (ROIs) for analysis, we considered functional brain networks that were associated with depression. Time series data were extracted from 74 ROIs according to the 90-ROI Shirer Brain Atlas (28). Some ROIs were excluded due to either unreliable images during recording (e.g., in cerebellum area) (29), or insufficient number of ROIs of corresponding functional network (e.g., primary and higher visual networks). When extracting the time series data, the global mean and confounding effects of both cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) and white matter were linearly regressed out as part of the preprocessing step. A total of 12 distinct functional brain networks were analyzed in this study. For the brevity of this paper, we highlighted most of the results for three main networks: basal ganglia network (BGN), dorsal default mode network (DDMN), and left executive control network (LECN). These networks have been associated with melancholic depression (14, 30), and melancholic symptoms such as anhedonia (31) and rumination (32). The list of anatomical ROIs for all of the analyzed networks networks are listed in Table 3 and Supplementary Table 1.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Functional brain networks associated with melancholic depression symptoms.

2.4. Pairwise Maximum Entropy Model

To compare the brain dynamics of melancholic and non-melancholic participants, we implemented the Energy Landscape Analysis (ELA) method which utilizes the Pairwise Maximum Entropy Model (P-MEM) (Figure 1) (36). In this study, ELA was conducted separately for each of the 12 distinct functional networks (see Supplementary Table 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Methodology for energy landscape analysis. (a) Selection of ROI based on functional brain networks associated with depression. (b1) Extraction of fMRI BOLD signals from each ROI. (b2) All signals from subjects within group (healthy, melancholic, non-melancholic) are combined. (c) Binarization of each signal using the mean BOLD value for each ROI. (d) Estimation of the state energy based on the emprical state frequency/probability. (e) Energy landscape analysis, which includes analyses of state energy levels, basin state landscape, and state transition dynamics. Note that as the energy of a state increases, its probability of occurrence decreases.

In ELA, we began by combining the rs-fMRI data of all participants in a group (healthy, non-melancholic, melancholic) to create group-level dynamics model. In each group, the concatenated time series data were extracted from the corresponding ROIs of the chosen functional network (Figures 1A,B). Then, the time series data were converted to binarized signals, using the average signal value as the threshold (Figure 1C). At any time point, binarized signals were either 1 (active) or 0 (inactive). The threshold value was computed for each ROI across the combined data of the group participants.

Binarizing the signals allowed us to encode the time signals into brain state sequences, wherein a brain state was defined by the activity pattern, i.e., active and inactive ROIs, at a given time (Figures 1C,D). Given brain network with n ROIs, there are 2n possible states. For each brain state σ, we computed the empirical probability p(σ),

p(σ)=nσT    (1)

where nσ is the number of occurrences that the state σ appeared in the time series, and T is the total length of the time series (Figure 1D).

From the brain state probabilities, we defined the state energy using Boltzmann distribution (37),

P(σ|h,J)=exp[-E(σ|h,J)]σ[-E(σ|h,J)]    (2)

where the sum in denominator is taken over all 2n possible states (each state is denoted by σ′). In P-MEM, the state energy E(σ) in Equation 2 was restricted in the quadratic form, and expressed as:

E(σ|h,J)=-i=1nhiσi-12i=1nj=1jinJi,jσiσj    (3)

with h=[hi]i=1n and J=[Ji,j]i,j=1n corresponding to the individual ROI activity and the pairwise ROI interaction, respectively. As implied by these equations, more active ROIs correspond to higher h and J, which leads to lower (more negative) energy (Equation 3), and higher occurrence probability (Equation 2) (36).

We then built the P-MEM from the model parameters h & J by maximizing their likelihood, as defined by

(h,J)=arg maxh,JL(h,J)    (4)
L(h,J)=t=1TP(σ(t)|h,J)    (5)

for the entire time series of length T.

2.5. Energy Landscapes Analysis

With the P-MEMs for healthy, non-melancholic and melancholic groups, we analyzed the energy landscapes and transition dynamics of heterogeneous depressed brain networks (Figure 1E). The ELA features analyzed in this study are summarized in Supplementary Table 2.

To start with, basin state dendrograms were constructed by finding the basin states and their clusters. Basin states (or basins) are states with the lowest energy relative to their neighboring states. States are said to be neighbors if they differ in only one active/inactive region. If neighboring states have lower energy barriers relative to other states, they become part of the closest basin cluster (38). Basins are the core of energy landscapes since these states are presumed to be the most stable. Djikstra's algorithm was performed to search for the basins, and construct the leaves (basins) and branches (energy barriers) of the dendrogram. In Figure 1E: Energy Barriers, the energy barrier between states a and b are highlighted (red: ab; blue: ba). Here, ab has higher barrier, and thus has lower probability of occurring (38).

Energy landscapes were then constructed to depict the energy level and cluster size of the basins. In Figure 1E: Energy Landscape Analysis, a 3D schematic diagram depicts the basins in an arbitrary state space. Here, basin clusters are plotted as concentric circles, where the basin is at the center, and neighboring states are on circles with radius equivalent to the their distance to the basin (i.e., a state that differs in one region with the basin has distance of 1, state the differs in two regions has distance of 2, etc.). Thus, each circle represented the states that were equidistant to the basin. Then for each circle (including the center), its depth was equivalent to the energy of the state with lowest energy (Equation 3) in that circle.

Since energy landscapes modeled only the group-level brain dynamics, we opted to analyze the brain dynamics of individual participants by computing the occurrence frequency of basins f(A) on individual fMRI time series. The occurrence frequencies of all basins (major and minor) are then summed for each group and for each network.

We also analyzed the transition dynamics on the energy landscapes. For each network, we selected two major basins as the basins with the lowest energy, A1 and A2; with P1 as clustered states for A1, P2 for A2. Then we computed the following participant-level transition rates,

TR(AA)=n(A1A2)+n(A2A1)T    (6)
TR(PP)=n(P1P2)+n(P2P1)T    (7)

where n(UV) is the number of times the participant's time series entered state U and arrived at state V; TR(AA′) is the major transition rate for major basins; TR(PP′) is the peripheral transition rate for clusters of major basins. Moreover, we computed the staying rates of the states,

SR(AA)=n(A1A1)+n(A2A2)T    (8)
SR(PP)=n(P1P1)+n(P2P2)T    (9)

where this time we counted the number of times the participant's brain activity stayed on the major basins (SR(AA′)), or the periphery (SR(PP′)). The transition rates were used to define the Traveling Score, which measured the amount of times the brain successfully traveled from one major basin to another (Equation 10, Figure 5A). Similarly, the staying rates were used to define the Lingering Score, which measured the amount of times the brain lingered on a basin or along its periphery (Equation 11, Figure 5A),

Traveling Score=TR(AA)TR(PP)    (10)
Linger Score=SR(AA)+SR(PP)    (11)

Note that the traveling score (Equation 10) is also known as efficiency score, which is the index of ease of transitions (38).

2.6. Statistical Group Differences

One-way Analysis of Variance (ANOVA) was performed to find significant group differences (healthy vs. non-melancholic vs. melancholic) in participants' age, IQ, BDI-II and SHAPS (Snaith-Hamilton Pleasure Scale) scores. Bonferroni correction was applied to compensate for multiple group comparisons. Similarly, Chi-squared test was performed for group differences in sex. Group differences are presumed to be statistically significant at p < 0.05 for both types of statistical analyses. One-way ANOVA with Bonferroni correction was also applied to test for statistical significance of results when comparing individual- and group-level differences in energy landscape characteristics such as basin frequency and transition rates. These results were also verified by Kruskal-Wallis test to cope with non-normally distributed data as well.

2.7. Depressive Symptoms Correlation

Finally, we investigated the correlation between depressive symptoms and basin characteristics. Two criteria were used for analyzing depressive symptoms: BDI-II for depression diagnosis (23), and SHAPS for assessing anhedonia (39). Multivariate linear regression model were fitted on the data, with p < 0.05 deemed as statistically significant relationship between the variables.

3. Results

3.1. Depressive Symptom Severity

We found significant differences between healthy, non-melancholic, and melancholic groups in terms of BDI-II and SHAPS score (Table 1). For both criteria, the melancholic group had higher average scores (p < 0.05, Healthyvs. Melancholic; p < 0.001, Nonmelancholicvs. Melancholic), followed by non-melancholic group (p < 0.001, Hvs. N). The results were consistent with many previous reports (4042).

In terms of other demographical factors, we found no significant group differences in sex distribution, age, and IQ levels of the participants. However, there was significant difference between the number of melancholic and non-melancholic participants recruited on four different sites (Table 1).

3.2. Basins and Energy Barriers

The basin dendrograms of each group for all network revealed differences in major basins and energy levels. The dendrograms for Basal Ganglia Network (BGN), Dorsal DMN (DDMN), and Left ECN (LECN) are shown in Figure 2. Here, the major basins (purple) were deeper (i.e., had relatively lower energy) than the minor basins (green).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Major and minor basin states. Major basin states (purple) and minor basin states (green) for Healthy, Non-Melancholic, and Melancholic groups on BGN, DDMN, and LECN. For each state, colored boxes correspond to activated regions, while white boxes are inactive. Energies of the deepest major basins for each group are also indicated (see Supplementary Table 3 for summary of all major basin energies).

In both BGN and LECN, the non-melancholic and melancholic groups had deeper major basins than the healthy group. Remarkably, a different pattern can be observed in the DDMN, where the non-melancholic group had shallower major basins than the healthy and melancholic groups.

Furthermore, some major basins were unique to specific groups. For example, non-melancholic group had a unique major basin in DDMN, where the left and right angular gyri (LAG, RAG), posterior cingulate cortex (PCC), and precuneus regions were activated. Similarly on LECN, the non-melancholic group also had a unique major basin, where left inferior frontal gyrus (LIFG), orbitofrontal gyrus (OFG), left inferior temporal gyrus (LITG), middle temporal gyrus (MTG), and left thalamus regions were activated. Complete results for basin energies are reported in Supplementary Table 3.

Lastly, the major basin pairs had antisynchronized activations (Figure 2). In BGN, the Left and Right Inferior Frontal Gyrus (LIFG, RIFG) were antisynchronized with the Left and Right Thalamus, Caudate, Putamen and Pons. In DDMN, the Left and Right Angular Gyrus (LAG, RAG), Right Superior Frontal Gyrus (RSFG), Posterior Cingulate Cortex (PCC) and Precuneus were synchronized with each other. A similar pattern can be observed in LECN, where the Left Inferior Temporal Gyrus (LITG), Middle Temporal Gyrus (MTG) and Left Thalamus were synchronized.

3.3. Basin Size and Energy Landscapes

Energy landscapes are then constructed based on the basin information. In Figure 3, different energy landscapes can be observed from each group in DDMN. The melancholic group has the most number of basins (n = 6), followed by healthy group (n = 5), then non-melancholic group (n = 4). Because of this shortage of basins, the non-melancholic has relatively larger major basins (sA = 98.6%), compared to healthy (sA = 92.0%) and melancholic groups (sA = 89.4%). Note that, as mentioned previously, the non-melancholic group has a unique major basin (A2). Also, there is a hill on basin cluster B2 of the non-melancholic group, which may be inferred as highly unstable cluster of states. This hill state cluster is absent in the other groups. We compared the basin sizes (number of states clustered to the basin) of each group for each network but found only minimal differences (e.g., p = 0.08, N vs. M, DDMN). Complete results for basin sizes are reported in Supplementary Figure 1 and Supplementary Table 3.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Energy landscapes of healthy, non-melancholic and melancholic groups. Schematic diagram of energy landscapes for Healthy, Non-melancholic, and Melancholic groups on DDMN. States are clustered to the their nearest basin state (Section 2.5). States A1 (S279) and A2 (S232) are the common major basins among the groups, while B1 (S340) and B2 (S419) are the common minor basins (Supplementary Table 3).

3.4. Basin Frequency

Significant group differences were found on the basin frequencies across all selected networks. The most notable results are summarized in Figure 4. In some networks, there was a natural decreasing trend from healthy to melancholic group (e.g., p < 0.005; H vs. M, N vs. M; Auditory Network). In other networks, an opposite, increasing trend appeared (e.g., p < 0.005; H vs. N, H vs. M, N vs. M; Anterior Salience Network, DDMN). And for the rest, the non-melancholic group had either the lowest (e.g., p < 0.005; H vs. N, H vs. M, N vs. M; BGN, LECN) or highest frequency (e.g., p < 0.005; H vs. N, H vs. M, N vs. M; Posterior Salience Network). Similar results for the other functional networks are summarized in Supplementary Table 4.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Basin states frequency. Occurrence frequency of basins for Healthy, Non-melancholic, and Melancholic groups on various brain networks. (***) p < 0.005; between-group, pairwise comparison of basin frequencies using one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni correction.

3.5. Brain State Transition Dynamics

Depression heterogeneity was also evident when comparing the transition dynamics in some networks. As shown in Figure 5B, there were significant differences between non-melancholic and melancholic groups in traveling scores (p < 0.05; N vs. M; DDMN), and lingering scores (p < 0.01; N vs. M; DDMN, LECN) of individual participants. There was also significant group differences in the lingering scores between healthy and non-melancholic (p < 0.01; H vs. N; DDMN), and healthy and melancholic groups (p < 0.01; H vs. M; DDMN, LECN). Furthermore, there is significant increase in lingering scores of melancholic group in LECN (Lingering = 0.61 ± 0.06) as compared to non-melancholic (Lingering = 0.55 ± 0.06; p < 0.005) and healthy groups (Lingering = 0.53 ± 0.06; p < 0.005). Results for other functional networks are summarized in Supplementary Table 5.

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Brain state transition dynamics. (A) Schematic diagram illustrating the transition dynamics for traveling score (basin-to-basin; Equation 10) and lingering score (within-basin or within-peripheral; Equation 11). (B) Traveling and lingering scores for Healthy, Non-melancholic, and Melancholic groups on BGN, DDMN, and LECN. (*)p < 0.05, (**)p < 0.01, and (***)p < 0.005; between-group, pairwise comparison of traveling/lingering scores using one-way ANOVA with Bonferroni correction.

4. Discussion

In this study, we characterized the brain dynamics of melancholic depression by analyzing the energy landscape models constructed from temporal rs-fMRI signals. The distinguishing energy landscape features of melancholic depression included major basin energy barriers, basin frequency, and state transition dynamics. Statistical results were consistent across our analyses on 12 functional brain networks, which indicates the robustness and discriminative power of our model. Moreover, these results agree with existing studies on melancholic features, such as depression severity (33, 40, 43), anhedonia (33, 44), and rumination (45, 46).

4.1. Confirmation of Depression Heterogeneity

Our first goal was to show heterogeneity in our depressed participants. Depression heterogeneity has plagued research advancement on the diagnosis and treatment of the disorder (4749). Considerable efforts has been made toward drawing boundaries within the currently accepted definition of major depressive disorder (47, 48), suggesting that investigations on individual depressive symptoms and their interactions may lead to new discoveries (49).

Melancholia is recognized as a specifier for MDD, characterized by symptoms that overlap with MDD such as anhedonia, excessive guilt, psychomotor disturbance, cognitive impairment, weight loss, and worse mood in the morning (2, 50). Although melancholic depression is not anymore considered as a depression subtype by DSM-5 (which instead uses the technical term depression with melancholic features) (50, 51), its clinical characteristics distinguishes it from non-melancholic and atypical depression (43). Our analysis results confirm such distinctions, as observed from significant differences between melancholic and non-melancholic groups in BDI-II and anhedonia scores (Table 1); basin frequencies on 12 networks (Supplementary Table 4); and transition dynamics scores in DDMN, LECN, PSN, RECN, VDMN, and VSPN (Supplementary Table 5).

4.2. Melancholia and Depression Severity

Next, we sought meaningful relationship between energy landscape signatures and depression severity. Melancholic depression is associated with greater symptom severity in comparison to depression with non-melancholic features (40, 43). This association was evident in our participant-level analysis of BDI-II scores, where the melancholic group scored significantly higher than the other groups (Table 1).

Readers may wonder if energy landscape features can be a proxy to evaluate some symptoms associated with melancholic features. To check this possibility, we performed regression analysis on basin frequencies and depression severity score (BDI-II). But despite having clear boundaries among the groups, no significant correlation were found in any of the networks (Supplementary Figure 2A).

The order of increasing severity from healthy, to non-melancholic, to melancholic depression may seem intuitive. However, some studies argue against the severity-based categorization of melancholic depression (52, 53), and do not identify melancholia as a “more severe” form of depression (52). This is evident in Supplementary Figure 2A, where the BDI-II scores for non-melancholic and melancholic groups coincide. In fact, we can even argue that the basin frequency of our energy landscape model were better than the depressive scores in discriminating melancholic depression (Figure 4).

4.3. Melancholia and Anhedonia

Following depression severity, we then tested if the correlation with basin frequency would also hold true for anhedonia. Anhedonia is one of the overlapping symptoms for both melancholia and depression (2, 42). This effect was observed from the statistical analysis of the subject demographics, where melancholic group has significantly higher SHAPS score for anhedonia (Table 1).

Similar to BDI-II scores, there were no significant correlation between SHAPS scores and basin frequencies (Supplementary Figure 2B). However, a closer inspection of results in the DDMN reveals a plausible connection between these features. Studies report that the decrease in functional connectivity of dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) with posterior cingulate cortex / precuneus (PCC/PCUN) is related to depression severity and anhedonia (33, 44).

Looking at the major basins found by our model in DDMN (Figure 2), the major basin pairs for all groups have antisynchronized activations of mPFC and PCUN (S279:{mPFC = 1, PCC/PCUN = 0}, H, N, M; S232:{mPFC = 0, PCC/PCUN = 1}, H, M; S168:{mPFC = 1, PCC/PCUN = 0}, N). This decoupling between mPFC and PCC/PCUN may be attributed to impaired reward anticipation, which is a key feature of anhedonia (44). Although current research has not yet established the direct connection between dynamic functional connectivity and energy landscape model (38), our results provide insight to the potential connection between these models.

4.4. Melancholia and Ruminative State

Lastly, we investigated the possible presence of ruminative states in the energy landscapes. Rumination is the tendency to dwell on the same, usually negative, thoughts for prolonged periods of time. Currently, rumination is not recognized a diagnostic feature for melancholic depression. However, since past studies have noted the significant correlation of rumination to both melancholy (45) and depression (54), we explored the energy landscape characteristics to provide more evidences on this correlations. Ruminative thinking is marked by increased connectivity in DMN (55), and decreased activity in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (left dlPFC; equivalent to LECN) (46). In terms of our energy landscape model, we define dynamic activity as being able to transition from one basin to another. This is in contrast to static activity, which tends to stay at the same basin. We derived two measures for brain activity: traveling score (Equation 10) for dynamic activity, and lingering score (Equation 11) for static activity. Thus, high traveling score and low lingering score would imply a more active network.

The significant increase of lingering scores of the melancholic group in LECN (Figure 5) could imply that the melancholic group has greater tendency to be “stuck” in a basin, or within its cluster. This may be related to the decreased activity in left dlPFC found in rumination (46). On the contrary, in the dorsal part of DMN (DDMN), the lingering scores of melancholic group (Lingering = 0.55 ± 0.07) are significantly lower than healthy (Lingering = 0.58 ± 0.06; p < 0.01) and non-melancholic groups (Lingering = 0.63 ± 0.05; p < 0.005). Although this may support previous studies showing increased connectivity in DMN during ruminative thinking, analyzing the lingering scores in ventral part of DMN (VDMN) leads to contradicting results (55). Nevertheless, the traveling scores in DDMN are significantly higher in melancholic group (Traveling = 0.10 ± 0.09) than in non-melancholic group (Traveling = 0.05 ± 0.06; p < 0.05). For LECN and VDMN, there were no significant differences in traveling scores between groups.

4.5. Limitations and Future Directions

Unlike typical functional connectivity (FC) models that are based on correlation between two regions (i.e., using Pearson's correlation coefficient), the ELA model does not assume that pairwise interactions of regions are independent from each other (17). This allows our model to more accurately capture the global activity patterns that may possibly be overlooked by FC-based models (17). Some FC-based models try to work around this limitation by using a different FC metric (such as precision matrix), or by introducing a sliding window in the analysis (also known as dynamic FCA) (56).

Hidden Markov Model (HMM) is another common brain dynamics model for resting-state fMRI. In comparison to ELA which utilizes pairwise MEM, HMM is more complex, and may be more expressive (57). Thus, in the future, our ELA results can serve as a baseline for deeper analysis on brain dynamics of melancholic depression using models such as HMM.

Furthermore, our present study have other limitations. ELA assumes a memoryless process, such that predicting the next state only depends on the current state, and not the past states (38). This saves us memory space and processing time (58), but this only works on systems at equilibrium, which typically requires huge amount of data (59). To address this problem, first, we decided to use resting-state fMRI data since these are expected as equilibrium states (60). Then, we combined the fMRI time signals of all participants in a group, to essentially produce “long” temporal data. Since there are substantially more healthy participants, it is possible that the model for the healthy group has converged closer to equilibrium than the two depressed subgroups.

Although concatenation of individual fMRI data should only have minimal effect to our analysis due to the memorylessness aspect of ELA (61), it should be noted that combining participants fMRI data could still potentially introduce bias due to BOLD signal differences across participants. We addressed this by using individual subjects' mean fMRI BOLD signals as threshold for binarizing the signals. However, it is recommended to further investigate the implications of data concatenation, and the ways to mitigate the intensity differences in individual fMRI.

Our selection of ROIs relies on the parcellation method [Shirer Atlas, (28)] and availability of data. As such, we removed some ROIs due to unreliable or insufficient data. Inclusion of these ROIs might affect the results. Choosing a different parcellation method might also produce different results. Thus, testing the robustness and consistency of our model on complete sets of ROIs or with different parcellation methods would be a reasonable step in the future.

It is often pointed out that the site difference could be a potential confounding factor for the observed BOLD signals (62). In fact, when we applied two-way ANOVA considering two factors of participant groups and site ID to basin frequencies, all main effects and their interaction were statistically significant (Supplementary Table 4). While this still supports that basin frequencies across different networks could differentiate participants suffering from melancholic and non-melancholic MDD in a population level, its reliability as a biomarker for the personalized medicine should never be overestimated. Thus, the potential site bias is a limitation in the present study and we should address the issue in our future work.

Even though we hypothesized a priori that the energy landscape of melancholic depression will be different from non-melancholic due to depression heterogeneity, our analyses and interpretations of the results were done post-hoc. For statistical analyses, we applied Bonferroni correction to compensate for multiple comparison tests that would increase the risk of Type 1 error (63).

Our discussion on the relation between melancholia and rumination is more suggestive than conclusive since we lack rumination quantifiers to analyze with our models. In the future, it would be crucial to record ruminative tendencies of participants using standardized measures such as Ruminative Response Scale (RRS) (64).

Finally, we would like to emphasize that melancholia, in itself, is also heterogeneous (50). It is characterized by multiple symptoms, and thus its severity and symptoms may vary from one person to another. Current biomarker models focus on the distinguishing features of melancholic depression on a group level (9, 10, 14). ELA allows us to study not only the group-wide brain dynamics of melancholic depression, but also the individual nuances in brain states and functional region interactions. In the future, it would be beneficial to study more of these individual differences so we can directly confront the issue of heterogeneity.

4.6. Conclusion

Melancholic depression is a debilitating disorder that robs a person the pleasure and excitement from activities they used to enjoy. In this study, we developed an energy landscape model to better understand the brain dynamics involved in melancholic depression. Relative to healthy and non-melancholic groups, the melancholic group showed significant differences on basin energy, basin frequency, and transition dynamics in several functional networks. Moreover, possible connections were traced between major basins and depressive symptom scores such as depression severity and anhedonia. Taken together, these results suggest that melancholic depression is a distinct disorder, and thus should be diagnosed and treated with utmost precision and care.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of Hiroshima University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was not obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

PR: methodology, software, investigation, writing—original draft, and writing—review and editing. MT: resources and writing—review and editing. TN: conceptualization, supervision, and writing—review and editing. NI, AF, and GO: resources. YO and SY: supervision and funding acquisition. KI: supervision and writing—review and editing. JY: conceptualization, methodology, investigation, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing, and funding. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Strategic Research Program for Brain Sciences (Integrated Research on Depression, Dementia and Development Disorders) from the Japan Agency for Medical Research and Development, AMED, grant nos. JP20dm0107093 and 20dm0107096. This work is also partially supported by JST-Mirai Program grant no. JPMJMI20D6, Japan; and JSPS KAKENHI grant nos. JP21H00949, JP21K07521, JP20H00625, and JP20K06874.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Drs. Mitsuo Kawato and Okito Yamashita (ATR Brain Information Communication Research Laboratory Group, Japan) for their valuable comments. PR is also grateful to DOST ERDT-SEI for the Foreign PhD Faculty Development Program scholarship.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.780997/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Ohmae S. The difference between depression and melancholia: two distinct conditions that were combined into a single category in DSM-III. Seishin Shinkeigaku Zasshi. (2012) 114:886–905.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

2. Parker G, Fink M, Shorter E, Taylor MA, Akiskal H, Berrios G, et al. Issues for DSM-5: whither melancholia? The case for its classification as a distinct mood disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2010) 167:745–7. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101525

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-3. American Psychiatric Association (1980).

Google Scholar

4. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-4. American Psychiatric Association (2000).

Google Scholar

5. Gaynes BN, Warden D, Trivedi MH, Wisniewski SR, Fava M, Rush AJ. What did STAR*D teach us? Results from a large-scale, practical, clinical trial for patients with depression. Psychiatric Services. (2009) 60:1439–45. doi: 10.1176/ps.2009.60.11.1439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Crismon ML, Trivedi M, Pigott TA, Rush AJ, Hirschfeld RM, Kahn DA, et al. The texas medication algorithm project. J Clin Psychiatry. (1999) 60:142–56. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v60n0302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Perry P. Pharmacotherapy for major depression with melancholic features: relative efficacy of tricyclic versus selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor antidepressants. J Affect Disord. (1996) 39:1–6. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(96)00014-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Brown WA. Treatment response in melancholia. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2007) 115:125–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.00970.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Day CV, Williams LM. Finding a biosignature for melancholic depression. Expert Rev Neurother. (2012) 12:835–47. doi: 10.1586/ern.12.72

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Day CV, Gatt JM, Etkin A, DeBattista C, Schatzberg AF, Williams LM. Cognitive and emotional biomarkers of melancholic depression: an iSPOT-D report. J Affect Disord. (2015) 176:141–50. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.01.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Guo CC, Nguyen VT, Hyett MP, Parker GB, Breakspear MJ. Out-of-sync: disrupted neural activity in emotional circuitry during film viewing in melancholic depression. Sci Rep. (2015) 5:11605. doi: 10.1038/srep11605

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Weinberg A, Shankman SA. Blunted reward processing in remitted melancholic depression. Clin Psychol Sci. (2016) 5:14–25. doi: 10.1177/2167702616633158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Workman CI, Lythe KE, McKie S, Moll J, Gethin JA, Deakin JF, et al. Subgenual cingulate amygdala functional disconnection and vulnerability to melancholic depression. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2016) 41:2082–90. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Ichikawa N, Lisi G, Yahata N, Okada G, Takamura M, Hashimoto Ri, et al. Primary functional brain connections associated with melancholic major depressive disorder and modulation by antidepressants. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:3542. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-73436-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Nakano T, Takamura M, Ichikawa N, Okada G, Okamoto Y, Yamada M, et al. Enhancing multi-center generalization of machine learning-based depression diagnosis from resting-state fMRI. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:400. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Yan M, He Y, Cui X, Liu F, Li H, Huang R, et al. Disrupted regional homogeneity in melancholic and non-melancholic major depressive disorder at rest. Frontiers in Psychiatry. (2021) 12:618805. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.618805

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Watanabe T, Hirose S, Wada H, Imai Y, Machida T, Shirouzu I, et al. A pairwise maximum entropy model accurately describes resting-state human brain networks. Nat Commun. (2013) 4:1370. doi: 10.1038/ncomms2388

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Uhlhaas P, Singer W. Neuronal dynamics and neuropsychiatric disorders: toward a translational paradigm for dysfunctional large-scale networks. Neuron. (2012) 75:963–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kopell N, Gritton H, Whittington M, Kramer M. Beyond the connectome: the dynome. Neuron. (2014) 83:1319–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Kang J, Pae C, Park HJ. Energy landscape analysis of the subcortical brain network unravels system properties beneath resting state dynamics. Neuroimage. (2017) 149:153–64. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Holtzheimer PE, Mayberg HS. Stuck in a rut: rethinking depression and its treatment. Trends Neurosci. (2011) 34:1–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2010.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Otsubo T, Tanaka K, Koda R, Shinoda J, Sano N, Tanaka S, et al. Reliability and validity of Japanese version of the Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview. Psychiatry Clin. Neurosci. (2005) 59:517–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1819.2005.01408.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Beck AT, Brown GK, Steer RA. BDI-II, Beck Depression Inventory: Manual. Harcourt Brace (1996).

24. Yahata N, Morimoto J, Hashimoto R, Lisi G, Shibata K, Kawakubo Y, et al. A small number of abnormal brain connections predicts adult autism spectrum disorder. Nat Commun. (2016) 7:11254. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

25. Power JD, Mitra A, Laumann TO, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Methods to detect, characterize, and remove motion artifact in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. (2014) 84:320–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.08.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

26. Power JD, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Recent progress and outstanding issues in motion correction in resting state fMRI. Neuroimage. (2015) 105:536–51. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.10.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Power JD, Barnes KA, Snyder AZ, Schlaggar BL, Petersen SE. Spurious but systematic correlations in functional connectivity MRI networks arise from subject motion. Neuroimage. (2012) 59:2142–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Shirer WR, Ryali S, Rykhlevskaia E, Menon V, Greicius MD. Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cerebral Cortex. (2011) 22:158–65. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Tokuda T, Yoshimoto J, Shimizu Y, Okada G, Takamura M, Okamoto Y, et al. Identification of depression subtypes and relevant brain regions using a data-driven approach. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:14082. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-32521-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

30. Cui X, Guo W, Wang Y, Yang Tx, Yang Xh, Wang Y, et al. Aberrant default mode network homogeneity in patients with first-episode treatment-naive melancholic depression. Int J Psychophysiol. (2017) 112:46–51. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Pizzagalli DA, Holmes AJ, Dillon DG, Goetz EL, Birk JL, Bogdan R, et al. Reduced caudate and nucleus accumbens response to rewards in unmedicated individuals with major depressive disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2009) 166:702–10. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08081201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bernstein EE, Heeren A, McNally RJ. Unpacking rumination and Executive control: a network perspective. Clin Psychol Sci. (2017) 5:816–26. doi: 10.1177/2167702617702717

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Rzepa E, McCabe C. Anhedonia and depression severity dissociated by dmPFC resting-state functional connectivity in adolescents. J Psychopharmacol. (2018) 32:1067–74. doi: 10.1177/0269881118799935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Nejad AB, Fossati P, Lemogne C. Self-Referential processing, rumination, and cortical midline structures in major depression. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:666. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00666

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. LeWinn KZ, Strigo IA, Connolly CG, Ho TC, Tymofiyeva O, Sacchet MD, et al. An exploratory examination of reappraisal success in depressed adolescents: preliminary evidence of functional differences in cognitive control brain regions. J Affect Disord. (2018) 240:155–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.07.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Ezaki T, Watanabe T, Ohzeki M, Masuda N. Energy landscape analysis of neuroimaging data. Philos Trans R Soc A Math Phys Eng Sci. (2017) 375:20160287. doi: 10.1098/rsta.2016.0287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. McQuarrie D. Statistical Mechanics. New York, NY: Harper & Row (1973).

38. Ezaki T, Sakaki M, Watanabe T, Masuda N. Age-related changes in the ease of dynamical transitions in human brain activity. Hum Brain Mapp. (2018) 39:2673–88. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Snaith RP, Hamilton M, Morley S, Humayan A, Hargreaves D, Trigwell P. A scale for the assessment of hedonic tone the snaith hamilton pleasure scale. Br J Psychiatry. (1995) 167:99–103. doi: 10.1192/bjp.167.1.99

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Zimmerman M. Diagnostic criteria for melancholia: the comparative validity of DSM-III and DSM-III-R. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1989) 46:361. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1989.01810040067010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. McGrath PJ, Khan AY, Trivedi MH, Stewart JW, Morris DW, Wisniewski SR, et al. Response to a selective serotonin reuptake inhibitor (citalopram) in major depressive disorder with melancholic features. J Clin Psychiatry. (2008) 69:1847–55. doi: 10.4088/JCP.v69n1201

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Fletcher K, Parker G, Paterson A, Fava M, Iosifescu D, Pizzagalli DA. Anhedonia in melancholic and non-melancholic depressive disorders. J Affect Disord. (2015) 184:81–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.05.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Gili M, Roca M, Armengol S, Asensio D, Garcia-Campayo J, Parker G. Clinical patterns and treatment outcome in patients with melancholic, atypical and non-melancholic depressions. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e48200. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0048200

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Zhang B, Li S, Zhuo C, Li M, Safron A, Genz A, et al. Altered task-specific deactivation in the default mode network depends on valence in patients with major depressive disorder. J Affect Disord. (2017) 207:377–83. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2016.08.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Nelson JC, Mazure C. Ruminative thinking. J Affect Disord. (1985) 9:41–6. doi: 10.1016/0165-0327(85)90008-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Ferdek MA, van Rijn CM, Wyczesany M. Depressive rumination and the emotional control circuit: An EEG localization and effective connectivity study. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2016) 16:1099–113. doi: 10.3758/s13415-016-0456-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Østergaard SD, Jensen SO, Bech P. The heterogeneity of the depressive syndrome: when numbers get serious. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2011) 124:495–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01744.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Monroe SM, Anderson SF. Depression: the shroud of heterogeneity. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2015) 24:227–31. doi: 10.1177/0963721414568342

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Fried E. Moving forward: how depression heterogeneity hinders progress in treatment and research. Expert Rev Neurother. (2017) 17:423–5. doi: 10.1080/14737175.2017.1307737

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Fried EI, Coomans F, Lorenzo-Luaces L. The 341737 ways of qualifying for the melancholic specifier. Lancet Psychiatry. (2020) 7:479–80. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30169-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-5. American Psychiatric Association (2017).

Google Scholar

52. Parker G, Fletcher K, Barrett M, Synnott H, Breakspear M, Rees AM, et al. Inching toward Bethlehem: Mapping melancholia. J Affect Disord. (2010) 123:291–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Parker G. A case for reprising and redefining melancholia. Can J Psychiatry. (2013) 58:183–9. doi: 10.1177/070674371305800402

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Alderman BL, Olson RL, Bates ME, Selby EA, Buckman JF, Brush CJ, et al. Rumination in major depressive disorder is associated with impaired neural activation during conflict monitoring. Front Hum Neurosci. (2015) 9:269. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00269

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Berman MG, Peltier S, Nee DE, Kross E, Deldin PJ, Jonides J. Depression, rumination and the default network. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2010) 6:548–55. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsq080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Hutchison RM, Womelsdorf T, Allen EA, Bandettini PA, Calhoun VD, Corbetta M, et al. Dynamic functional connectivity: promise, issues, and interpretations. Neuroimage. (2013) 80:360–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Savin C, Tkačik G. Maximum entropy models as a tool for building precise neural controls. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2017) 46:120–6. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2017.08.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Painsky A, Rosset S, Feder M. Memoryless representation of Markov processes. In: 2013 IEEE International Symposium on Information Theory. Istanbul: IEEE (2013).

Google Scholar

59. Gu S, Cieslak M, Baird B, Muldoon SF, Grafton ST, Pasqualetti F, et al. The energy landscape of neurophysiological activity implicit in brain network structure. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:2507. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-20123-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Deco G, Jirsa VK. Ongoing cortical activity at rest: criticality, multistability, and ghost attractors. J Neurosci. (2012) 32:3366–75. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2523-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Watanabe T, Masuda N, Megumi F, Kanai R, Rees G. Energy landscape and dynamics of brain activity during human bistable perception. Nat Commun. (2014) 5:4765. doi: 10.1038/ncomms5765

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Dansereau C, Benhajali Y, Risterucci C, Pich EM, Orban P, Arnold D, et al. Statistical Power and prediction accuracy in multisite resting-state fmri connectivity. Neuroimage. (2017) 149:220–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.01.072

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Miller RG. Simultaneous Statistical Inference. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill (1966).

Google Scholar

64. Treynor W, Gonzalez R, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Rumination reconsidered: A psychometric analysis. Cognit Ther Res. (2003) 27:247–59. doi: 10.1023/A:1023910315561

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: depression, melancholia, energy landscape analysis, resting state fMRI, functional brain network

Citation: Regonia PR, Takamura M, Nakano T, Ichikawa N, Fermin A, Okada G, Okamoto Y, Yamawaki S, Ikeda K and Yoshimoto J (2021) Modeling Heterogeneous Brain Dynamics of Depression and Melancholia Using Energy Landscape Analysis. Front. Psychiatry 12:780997. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.780997

Received: 22 September 2021; Accepted: 01 November 2021;
Published: 25 November 2021.

Edited by:

Jong-Hwan Lee, Korea University, South Korea

Reviewed by:

Gregory Fonzo, The University of Texas at Austin, United States
Martin Fungisai Gerchen, University of Heidelberg, Germany

Copyright © 2021 Regonia, Takamura, Nakano, Ichikawa, Fermin, Okada, Okamoto, Yamawaki, Ikeda and Yoshimoto. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Junichiro Yoshimoto, juniti-y@is.naist.jp

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.