Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychiatry, 16 November 2021
Sec. Autism
This article is part of the Research Topic Autism and Schizophrenia: Neurodevelopmental, Transdiagnostic, Dimensional Constructs View all 5 articles

A Unifying Theory for Autism: The Pathogenetic Triad as a Theoretical Framework

  • 1Gillberg Neuropsychiatry Centre, Department of Psychiatry and Neurochemistry, Institute of Neuroscience and Physiology, Sahlgrenska Academy, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden
  • 2Department of Radiology, Sahlgrenska University Hospital, Gothenburg, Sweden
  • 3MedTech West, Gothenburg, Sweden

This paper presents a unifying theory for autism by applying the framework of a pathogenetic triad to the scientific literature. It proposes a deconstruction of autism into three contributing features (an autistic personality dimension, cognitive compensation, and neuropathological risk factors), and delineates how they interact to cause a maladaptive behavioral phenotype that may require a clinical diagnosis. The autistic personality represents a common core condition, which induces a set of behavioral issues when pronounced. These issues are compensated for by cognitive mechanisms, allowing the individual to remain adaptive and functional. Risk factors, both exogenous and endogenous ones, show pathophysiological convergence through their negative effects on neurodevelopment. This secondarily affects cognitive compensation, which disinhibits a maladaptive behavioral phenotype. The triad is operationalized and methods for quantification are presented. With respect to the breadth of findings in the literature that it can incorporate, it is the most comprehensive model yet for autism. Its main implications are that (1) it presents the broader autism phenotype as a non-pathological core personality domain, which is shared across the population and uncoupled from associated features such as low cognitive ability and immune dysfunction, (2) it proposes that common genetic variants underly the personality domain, and that rare variants act as risk factors through negative effects on neurodevelopment, (3) it outlines a common pathophysiological mechanism, through inhibition of neurodevelopment and cognitive dysfunction, by which a wide range of endogenous and exogenous risk factors lead to autism, and (4) it suggests that contributing risk factors, and findings of immune and autonomic dysfunction are clinically ascertained rather than part of the core autism construct.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (hereafter referred to as autism) is a collective term for neurodevelopmental disorders characterized by impaired social communication, and a rigid and repetitive behavior (1). It is under strong genetic influence (2, 3) and occurs in roughly 1–2% of the general population (4). Its reported incidence has steadily risen in the last decades owing to increased awareness, as well as changes in diagnostic criteria (5, 6).

The autism literature has been particularly fruitful in recent decades; genetic studies have made substantial headway toward identifying contributing variants (3, 7); countless etiological risk factors have been identified (8); endophenotype studies have identified potential biomarkers (9), eventually paving the way for the realization that the underlying features of autism (10), like that of other neuropsychiatric disorders (11, 12), are dimensional rather than categorical. Despite great progress, there are still some outstanding questions meriting intense discussions regarding their theoretical implications and practical relevance.

One of the main scientific and clinical issues is that of heterogeneity (13); behaviorally and biologically, there is no universal risk factor or biomarker that accounts for all autism cases, leading to problems with academic reproducibility and clinical generalizability. It has been argued that the concept of autism lacks validity (14), and that a singular definition should be abandoned (15).

Besides conceptual ambiguity, the lack of a singular explanatory framework that can incorporate findings from across the literature certainly contributes to the heterogeneity. Imagine the heterogeneity of cancer—being caused by risk factors ranging from lack of physical exercise and smoking, to tanning and viral infections, occurring in a wide range of different tissues—before identifying genetic dysregulation of cellular division as its central mechanism. Although the rates and patterns of co-occurring neuropsychiatric conditions (16), sex bias (17, 18), and multitude of risk factors (8) have been well-characterized, the absence of a framework results in a lack of empirical and predictive explanations for the observed patterns.

Several, expectedly overlapping, frameworks have been proposed (see Table 1 for a non-exhaustive list) accounting for various aspects of its pathogenesis. However, none have encompassed all aspects of findings in the autism literature, and many have failed to provide concrete, biologically grounded mechanisms for how these aspects contribute to autism across development and its biological hierarchy. In general, they have been descriptive and lacked operationalization, prohibiting modeling and testing. Even though a unifying theory for autism is lacking, most of the proposed models have significant merit in their own domains, which would complement any top-down unifying theory.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. A selection of models for autism, their explanatory potentials, and relations to the scientific literature.

The aim of this paper is to apply the framework of a pathogenetic triad (19) on autism, and exemplify how it relates to the existing literature on autism. The framework consists of three factors (an autistic personality, cognitive compensation, and neuropathological burden) that interact to cause an observable behavioral phenotype which may be identified as maladaptive, sometimes fulfilling criteria for an autism diagnosis. The factors are operationalized, and mechanisms of pathogenesis are outlined. Ultimately, the goal is to synthesize a vast transdisciplinary literature, and account for as many aspects of known findings in autism research as possible. In many respects, it can be complemented by findings and explanations in previously proposed models. A far-reaching goal such as this cannot be completely incorporated into a single framework or paper, and future studies will be needed to specify different aspects of the framework. However, it will hopefully illustrate how a relatively simple and concrete model can account for several findings in the autism literature, as well as propose approaches for solving existing conundrums. Implications and postulates of the framework, and methods for testing and falsification are presented at the end of the paper.

Brief Outline of Pathogenetic Triad

Fundamentally, the framework proposes three separate and interacting factors:

1) A common autism core, conceptualized as an Autistic Personality dimension (AP), although not intrinsically pathological, constitutes an obligate “first hit” for the development of an autistic-like behavior. It is a construct operationalized and represented at different biological levels by a specific genetic architecture, governing the development of a neurobiological and cognitive endophenotype, itself determining the behavioral phenotype.

2) Cognitive compensation, conceptualized as Cognitive Capacity (CC), constituting a relative “second hit.” It represents the general ability of the individual to respond to, and learn from environmental cues and demands. It is a construct operationalized as an index of the individual's executive functioning (EF) and intelligence.

3) Risk factors, conceptualized as the Neuropathological Burden (NB), constituting an optional “third hit.” It is conceived as the exposure burden to a diverse array of somatic and psychological insults that converge on a common pathway of neurodevelopmental disruption, secondarily inhibiting CC.

Figure 1 illustrates the pathogenetic triad, and shows how the constituent parts interact to cause an observable maladaptive behavioral phenotype requiring a diagnosis of autism. Briefly, the intensity of the AP depends on the genetic load from common variants and correlates positively with the quantity and intensity of expressed autistic-like traits (ALT), as well as the risk of diagnosis. The AP presents with issues (and strengths) that are qualitatively similar to the diagnostic criteria of autism. These issues are partly compensated for by the individual's CC. For example, high verbal intelligence, theory of mind, and empathizing ability all ameliorate issues with social communication. Similarly, well-developed non-social EFs, such as working memory and inhibitory control, allow individuals to deal with sensory disturbances and digressions from their repetitive and restricted behaviors while maintaining their thought processes, making them appear less “autistic” and more “functional.” Endogenous and exogenous risk factors act as neuropathological insults and disrupt the normal processes of brain and cognitive development. In so doing, they lower the protection offered by the CC, disinhibiting the maladaptive behaviors associated with the AP, sometimes to the point that a diagnosis can be made. There is a disproportionate inhibitory effect on higher cognitive functions (HCF), which themselves are particularly important for compensatory ability. These insults can be alleviated (as a defense mechanism), or caused (autoimmunity and inflammatory reactions), by the individual's immune system, and modulated by the autonomic system; the resulting aggregate effect of this interaction is termed the NB. The heterogeneity in autism, and overlap between different neurodevelopmental disorders, stem from the CC/NB-complex; different individual profiles of CC and neuropathological insults are associated with different neurobiological endophenotypes and behavioral phenotypes within the autism population.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The dynamics of the pathogenetic triad. Presentation of the pathogenetic triad outlining of the interactions between the operationalized factors. Red boxes indicate contributing mechanisms, having positive correlations with severity of clinical phenotype and probability of diagnosis, and blue boxes represent protective mechanisms. Full and dashed arrows indicate contributory and inhibitory mechanisms, respectively. The double arrow illustrates a bidirectional interaction with both contributory and inhibitory mechanisms. Transparent arrows indicate possible mechanisms for which research, with regard to autism, is lacking. There are also gene-environment interactions through other cognitive functions (than cognitive capacity), such as perception and personality, that bidirectionally affect the neuropathological burden, but these have been purposedly left out for model simplicity.

A Common Autism Core

The Broader Autism Phenotype

The social, cognitive, and personality characteristics that define autism are also present in neurotypical individuals to a lesser degree without warranting a diagnosis. These characteristics are called ALTs and their presence is referred to as the broader autism phenotype [BAP; see review by (52); also (5355)]. Autism and ALTs are part of the same common distribution, with the diagnosis merely occurring at the extreme end of that distribution (56, 57). As such, the more ALTs an individual has, the higher the probability of having an autism diagnosis (58).

The importance of the BAP as a concept has gained increasing traction in recent times, and its presence in relatives of individuals with autism has received particular attention (5963). There is a high heritability, not only for autism, but also for the BAP (64, 65). Studies have shown that relatives of individuals with autism have more such traits than the general population (66, 67), that the degree of genetic relatedness for relatives of diagnosed individuals predicts the risk of them being diagnosed with autism (68), and the degree of BAP in parents predicts some aspects of disease severity in their diagnosed children (62, 66, 67, 69). Multiplex families (more than one child diagnosed with autism) have more ALTs than simplex families (one diagnosed child), which again have more such traits than families without autism diagnoses (60, 61, 6971).

Given its similarity and overlap with autism, it may offer a window into its core. Assuming it is a phenotype that is shared across the diagnostic threshold (57), one can imagine the BAP to be the defining feature of that which is inherently autistic; its absence will not warrant a diagnosis of autism, no matter the severity of that individual's cognitive impairments or additional risk factors. In other words, it represents the behavioral expression of a core autistic condition (see Box 1 for proposal regarding nomenclature and nosography).

Box 1. The broader autism phenotype—nomenclature and nosography.

The existence of a personality dimension which is not intrinsically pathological implicates a normalization of the underlying autistic behavioral pattern. This not only explains the presence of strengths in the autistic phenotype, but is more in line with Occam's razor; postulating a continuous dimensional construct, rather than attempting to explain the presence of the BAP as a categorical extended phenotype in relatives of diagnosed individuals, within a plethora of studies pointing to its polygenic background and normal distribution across the population.

Regarding the nomenclature, rather than recycling the term “BAP-positive” as a binary identifier along a continuous spectrum of ALTs, the term autistic personality is introduced to denote the presence of a pronounced BAP. It represents the behavioral expression of an underlying neurobiological endophenotype, which is associated with an increased risk of diagnosis. This is similar to the nosography applied for schizophrenia spectrum disorders (49):

• The autistic personality dimension and psychoticism represent underlying personality dimensions

• Pronounced personality types are identified as an autistic personality and schizotypy

• When these types are maladaptive and give rise to mental health issues, they are denoted autistic and schizotypal personality disorder

• When diagnostic criteria are fulfilled, one receives a diagnosis of either autism or schizophrenia spectrum disorder

Even though individuals with an AP may experience mental health issues, a diagnosis of autism isn't necessarily justified. They will nevertheless be at increased risk of secondary mental health issues such as social phobia, anxiety, depression, and avoidant and restrictive food intake. Long-term outcomes from psychiatric services can be improved if an autistic personality is identified as the cause, such as sensory issues being the reason for food restriction rather than applying a diagnosis of anorexia nervosa. This is particularly true for females with normal or supranormal cognitive ability, who often receive multiple diagnoses and may receive a late autism diagnosis, or altogether fail to fulfill the complete diagnostic criteria for a diagnosis (50, 51).

The BAP as a Personality Inventory

The pervasiveness of BAP expression across behaviors and the population (72), its normal pattern of distribution (65, 73), its high heritability and polygenic background (74, 75), its stability over time (56, 75, 76), and its potentially beneficial effects from an evolutionary standpoint (72, 7779) are all hallmarks of a personality inventory [already proposed by Asperger in 1944 (80)].

It is likely that families with a higher genetic burden are right-shifted (increased) on the normal distribution of these traits, leading both to a higher BAP in the non-diagnosed relatives and to a greater familial risk of exhibiting the clinical phenotype which warrants an autism diagnosis [implying a higher incidence in those families; see Figure 1 in (10)].

The Biology of the BAP

A polygenic nature and population-wide distributions are typical for complex traits (8183) and common disorders (84). Evolutionary adaptive change is mainly determined by changes in common variants (81, 83, 85), without significant contributions from rare variants (81, 86). The genetic architecture of autism, an arguably complex disease which is associated with strengths, suggests additive effects from both common and rare variants (8789), where common variants predict risk across the spectrum of disabilities (88, 90), and rare variants are associated with cognitive dysfunction in both clinical and non-clinical populations. For this reason, and reasons that will become evident in sections Cognitive Compensation Protects Against Phenotypic Maladaptation and Risk Factors Affect Neurodevelopment, common variants are postulated to underly the development of a common core autistic neurobiological endophenotype (see section Operationalization of the Autistic Personality), while rare variants will be discussed in section Risk Factors Affect Neurodevelopment as risk factors that influence cognitive development (with at most partial contribution to the core autism endophenotype, in situations where pathophysiological mechanisms overlap).

Neurobiological studies have identified myriad potential biomarkers for autism. Some of them have been identified also in undiagnosed relatives of probands, usually with intermediate phenotypes between probands and neurotypical controls [see reviews by (9, 91)], as expected for a phenotype with a continuous distribution; altered connectivity patterns, excitation/inhibition disbalance, neurophysiological alterations, behavioral and neuropsychological differences, as well as extracranial somatic biomarkers (which are less likely to contribute to the core condition due to lack of specificity; see section Risk Factors Affect Neurodevelopment). Future studies are needed to elucidate which of these endophenotypes may be part of the postulated core autism condition, and which are clinically ascertained endophenotypes resulting from risk factors (potentially offering a route for biological stratification). One telling sign is the indication that common variants converge on synaptic function (7, 92, 93), which may indicate that dysconnectivity and alteration of excitation/inhibition, secondary to synaptic dysfunction, are key biological pathways toward development of the BAP.

Androgens are associated with male behavioral and endophenotypes (94, 95). The increased rates of ALTs (73) and autism (96) among males prompted the conceptualization of the extreme male brain theory of autism (26). Increased testosterone may contribute to differences in phenotypic expression [see reviews by (94, 95); also (97, 98); although (99)]. However, it remains to be seen whether the main mechanism of androgens is to directly increase autism risk by increasing ALTs, or if they contribute indirectly through accentuation of male vulnerability through negative effects on social cognition [(97); see section Heritability and Clinical Implications of Low Cognitive Ability] or by modulating biological sensitivity to somatic insults (see section Sex Difference in Biological Vulnerability).

Operationalization of the Autistic Personality

It is suggested that there exists a common autism core, an autistic personality domain termed the BAP, which is shared across individuals, irrespective of diagnostic status. A pronounced BAP is operationalized as the presence of an AP type. Underlying the AP is a biological hierarchy stemming from common variants. These variants give rise to an intermediate neurobiological endophenotype that ultimately results in a complex trait that we identify as the BAP. The strength of each level along the hierarchy will be positively associated with the BAP and the probability of receiving an autism diagnosis.

The AP is an obligate “first hit” toward receiving an autism diagnosis. A mild personality de facto implies a diagnosis of autism shouldn't be made. A pronounced personality may lead to quirks, strengths, and mental health issues, and when the compensatory abilities are low, a diagnosis of autism. Although far from conclusive, genetic and neurobiological studies hint at a core endophenotype that stems from alterations in synaptic function.

The AP can be quantified at each step of the biological hierarchy: common variants may be used to calculate polygenic scores (which should be estimated using ALTs rather than diagnostic status); neurobiological endophenotypes can be identified using brain imaging and neurophysiological methods (which is hampered by biological heterogeneity and difficulty of development of stable biomarkers due to small sample sizes); behavioral questionnaires [such as the Autism Spectrum Quotient (72), or the Social Responsiveness Scale (100)] can estimate the BAP through ALTs. With improved methods and decreased costs, future modeling may allow for the creation of a weighted index from measurements across the hierarchy, further improving clinical utility.

Although individuals with an AP share the phenotypic issues with individuals having a diagnosis, some will not warrant a clinical diagnosis. The reasons some of these individuals develop a maladaptive behavioral phenotype, to the point of requiring a diagnosis, are outlined in sections Cognitive Compensation Protects Against Phenotypic Maladaptation and Risk Factors Affect Neurodevelopment.

Cognitive Compensation Protects Against Phenotypic Maladaptation

A frequent finding in the literature is the presence of low cognitive ability (intelligence, EF) compared to the neurotypical population [see reviews by (101, 102); also (103105)]. This is further exemplified by the high rate of clinical co-occurrence of intellectual disability (ID) and autism; 45–75% of patients with autism have co-occurring ID, and 20–40% of patients with ID are found to have autism (4, 103, 106).

Cognitive ability correlates negatively with the number of ALTs being expressed (101, 107), the severity of autism (108), psychotherapy response (109, 110), good outcome (111115), compensatory ability (116, 117), aspects of social cognition (118), as well as risk of diagnosis (101, 119) and comorbidities (120), illustrating a significant modulatory function of the observed phenotype.

Within the domain of cognitive functions, there is a disproportionate disruption of HCFs, and their impairment seem to be the norm rather than the exception (29, 101, 102, 104, 105, 108, 118, 121126). Methodological difficulties with operationalization and testing of HCFs exist (105, 122), which likely explains some discrepancies within this field. However, given the protective and enabling effect of HCFs on autism (116, 117, 127129), it is not surprising that they are often reported to be low in the clinical population.

This pattern of findings may also explain the presence of clumsiness in autism (130), where fine motor coordination is more affected than gross motor coordination (29, 131). Clumsiness and coordination disorder correlate with low cognitive ability (132, 133), also in neurotypical controls and other neurodevelopmental disorders (134), and controlling for cognitive ability removes that association (135). It is possible that suboptimal coordination of neural ensembles in the frontal lobes and across higher cognitive areas gives rise to higher cognitive dysfunction, and that suboptimal neural coordination in the motor systems may give rise to fine motor dysfunction, which, if more pronounced, leads to gross motor dysfunction. Potential reasons why HCFs and fine motor control are disproportionately affected is outlined in section Risk Factors Affect Neurodevelopment.

The presence of an AP induces its own set of behavioral difficulties, such as misinterpretation of social cues and sensory disturbances, that both create issues of their own, and lead to an increased cognitive load. The HCFs enable an adaptive response to such difficulties, as well as to environmental demands [see (129); also (136) for outline of social information-processing mechanisms, and (137) for iterative reprocessing model]. That is, for an adaptive response to a complex environmental demand, such as flexibility in response to a malleable social environment, as opposed to reacting to a sound, the HCFs are instrumental. The compensation likely occurs through both volitional [e.g., IQ and EF facilitating conscious learning of social communication, or through camouflaging; see (129)] and non-volitional cognitive mechanisms [e.g., greater EF allowing for simultaneous processing of sensory stimuli and internal thought processes, or through neural remodeling; (138)].

Clinical Ascertainment of Low Cognitive Ability

The lower the cognitive ability in general, and the HCFs in particular, the lower the ability will be to adequately respond to the environment and to learn from social interactions (139). This will cause the autistic behavioral phenotype to become, not only more pronounced and easily identified, but also more maladaptive. This increases the probability of being diagnosed, and decreases adaptive ability and long-term prognosis.

The disproportionate disruption of the HCFs can be viewed as particularly unfortunate. However, the flip side is that it can be viewed as telling of the underlying pathogenesis of the clinical phenotype; its presence is expected when viewed through the lens of an ascertainment bias (40). Individuals with an AP that also have a low cognitive ability (relative to their AP) will be more likely to seek psychiatric services and receive a diagnosis.

Heritability and Clinical Implications of Low Cognitive Ability

Cognitive ability is highly heritable (140142) with polygenic inheritance [(142, 143); which, as for the BAP, shows convergence on synaptic function, potentially explaining the relationship between polygenic burden for autism and high intelligence (78)]. Its heritability explains why parents and siblings of children with lower functioning autism also have low cognitive ability on average (144147), but not parents of the subgroup of high-functioning autism (148). Despite the strong clinical connection to low cognitive ability, the BAP and intelligence have been shown to be genetically distinct (149, 150) [for clinical correlate see (111)].

This leads to two conclusions: (1) rather than the AP and low cognitive ability categorically co-occurring (being biologically coupled), a likely explanation is that the distribution of cognitive ability in the clinical population is left-shifted toward lower abilities, and (2) the presence of cognitive dysfunction represents a risk factor, a “second hit,” for developing and expressing a maladaptive behavioral phenotype that we identify as autism because of lower compensation. The idea of reversing the chain of causation is not new (40, 41, 54, 151). A possible causal effect of IQ on the development of schizophrenia (152) has been identified; given similarities in risk factors and cognitive dysfunction across the disorders, the causality may hold true for autism as well.

It is thus possible that the BAP and cognitive ability are distinct phenotypes, and that the presence of a clinical ascertainment bias accounts for the frequent finding of low cognitive ability in autism. Supporting this are numerous studies indicating loss of an association with autism following control of IQ as a confounding variable [co-occurrence of epilepsy (153), coordination problems (135), cognitive abilities in presence of rare genetic variants (154)].

Operationalization of Cognitive Capacity

CC is conceptualized as the general mechanisms that allow the individual to compensate for, and overcome issues that arise from the presence of an AP. A decreased CC causes the underlying autistic phenotype to become maladaptive, sometimes to the point of requiring a diagnosis. The main components include intelligence and EFs [similar to (29, 129)]. CC can be quantified through the use of various neuropsychological tests.

This framework makes no assumptions about the relative importance of particular subdomains of cognitive ability, but merely acknowledges that (1) some have a greater protective effect regarding the autistic phenotype than others [such as HCF compared with lower cognitive functions, or social cognitive ability and verbal intelligence being better predictors of adaptive ability and optimal outcome than non-social cognitive ability and inhibitory control (129, 155)], and (2) that individual subdomains likely have different effects on different aspects of the phenotype (such as social cognitive ability and verbal IQ having greater effects on Theory of Mind deficits than non-social cognitive ability or non-verbal IQ). It is the task of future neuropsychological studies to elucidate the relative importance of specific cognitive subdomains. One can imagine that a cognitive index with different weighting for each subdomain (which may differ between sexes, see Superior Social Cognition in Females) can be used to improve disorder modeling and diagnostic resolution when employing the pathogenetic triad.

It should be noted that this conceptualization relies heavily on compensation by way of cognitive abilities in order to emphasize the connection to findings of low IQ and EF, and effects of risk factors. However, the concept of compensation in autism is both wider than outlined in this paper, and its presence is not unequivocally positive [see review by (129)].

Superior Social Cognition in Females

One can identify certain cognitive subdomains that are intrinsically non-social– spatial intelligence, working memory, inhibitory and cognitive control etc.—as well as subdomains that more specifically aid processing of social information and increase social ability—verbal intelligence, interpretation of emotion and biological motion, social cue interpretation, cooperativeness, social motivation etc. Even though the phenotypic separateness of social and non-social intelligence has long been debated (156, 157), they can be construed as distinct concepts (158160). The purpose of deconstructing cognitive ability into social and non-social domains lies in presenting a mechanism by which CC may give rise to a skewed sex ratio in autism [similar to that of (27)].

Studies investigating typically developing individuals have indicated that females have, what can be considered, a superior social cognitive ability. Some of the sex differences, in favor of females, include superior recognition of non-verbal social and emotional cues, such as faster and more accurate recognition of facial expressions and bodily emotions, more sharing, turn taking and cooperative behavior, being more prosocial, sympathetic and empathetic, and having a higher social motivation [see reviews by (161163)]. Given the seeming distinctness of BAP and cognitive ability (149, 150), as well as social and non-social cognitive domains (158, 159, 164), it is likely that these sex differences hold true, not only for the NT population, but also for those with autism (163).

The presented sex differences are far from universal, but it is clear that a sex discrepancy within the domain of social cognition exists. On the one hand, females on average show greater social competence. On the other, they are more likely to seek exposure to social interactions, either due to greater inherent social motivation or a lower rate of unsatisfactory and deterring attempts from such interactions, further increasing that competence. Over the course of a lifetime, the difference in social learning and adaptive ability may lead to different life trajectories and subsequent differences in observable clinical phenotype [at least in the subgroup with higher cognitive ability, which has the highest sex-ratio bias (165, 166)].

The distribution of compensatory ability of females may thus be right-shifted (increased), leading to a negative clinical ascertainment bias. Considering a certain base rate of identification (psychiatrists having a set internalized threshold for applying an autism diagnosis), the presence of a protective compensatory mechanism predicts an increased disease burden in other domains. In other words, improved social cognition and adaptive functioning may underly the female protective effect (167) and explain why diagnosed females have lower average cognitive ability than males (168, 169), even when controlling for their lower average BAP (170), or a higher disease burden (107, 166).

In the presence of an underlying autistic phenotype, the cognitive compensation may or may not be complete (129, 155) and can lead to psychiatric problems (171) either due to inadequately compensated social difficulties and a discrepancy between social competence and motivation for social interactions (which may be particularly strong for females), or as a consequence of delayed or undiagnosed autism in females (50, 129, 172). These issues illustrate the need for improved detection of autism in females. If there is a discrepancy in social cognitive ability between the sexes, and it underlies the female protective effect, its inclusion in disease modeling (through sex-specific weighting of social subdomains for cognitive ability) may improve diagnostic resolution for females when applying the pathogenetic triad for identification purposes.

Relationship Between the Autistic Personality and Cognitive Capacity

Given that CC and the AP are biologically uncoupled, and that their interaction determines the behavioral phenotype, one can construct a two-dimensional space, which is presented in Figure 2. Since the severity and risk of diagnosis are negatively associated with CC, the phenotypic space is transversed by a diagnostic threshold (DT) with a positive slope (whose true shape is likely slightly parabolic, but is illustrated straight for simplicity). Individuals above the threshold do not have a maladaptive behavioral phenotype and do not require a diagnosis. Individuals below the threshold have a CC that is low relative to their AP, meaning they are not able to compensate for their ALTs. This leads to a maladaptive phenotype requiring a clinical diagnosis. The DT is not well-demarcated due to the difficulty in assigning a categorical threshold to a dimensional construct. The distance to the DT, illustrated by the shading, indicates how maladaptive and severe the observed phenotype is [functional levels according to DSM-5 (1)]. It will also relate to the stability of the diagnosis over time. Figure 3 presents an example of three individuals (X, Y, and Z) with differing degrees of CC and AP, and how that affects their observed behavioral phenotypes.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Relationship between cognitive capacity and the autistic personality dimension. This figure illustrates why a higher cognitive capacity, and thus ability to compensate for various behavioral issues, decreases the probability that the intensity of the autistic personality is behaviorally maladaptive. The diagnostic threshold separates the clinical phenotypes into those that are clinically neurotypical and those that fulfill diagnostic criteria for autism. It is a fuzzy demarcation due to the difficulty in determining diagnostic status in borderline cases. The shading below the threshold corresponds to the severity of the clinical phenotype, the probability of having attracted clinical attention and received a diagnosis, and the stability of the diagnosis over time. The true diagnostic threshold is not necessarily straight, but is likely somewhat parabolic and slightly levels off with increasing cognitive capacity.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Examples of individuals with varying degrees of cognitive capacity and autistic personality. Presentation of three individuals and their observed phenotypes. Individual X has a high cognitive capacity, which fully compensates for the presence of a pronounced autistic personality. Although X may be perceived as socially odd, no diagnosis is warranted since X is above the diagnostic threshold. Individual Y has an equally pronounced autistic personality. However, the cognitive capacity is much lower, and doesn't allow for adequate compensation of its autistic features. Y fulfills diagnostic criteria, and likely has a moderate to severe autism (long distance to diagnostic threshold). Individual Z also has a low cognitive capacity, which is unable to compensate adequately. However, Z has a milder autistic personality than Y, leading to a milder autistic phenotype (smaller distance to the diagnostic threshold). Finally, X' represents individual X following a neurological insult that negatively affects cognitive capacity. One can imagine X being conceived by healthy parents with a genetic architecture that supports high cognitive capacity and a pronounced autistic personality, but having experienced a perinatal complication or an insult during childhood. The insult lowers the cognitive capacity enough to impair compensation, leading to a diagnosis of autism.

The figure offers empirical explanations for some findings in the literature. First, it has been found that individuals with higher cognitive ability are more likely to achieve optimal outcome, and lose their diagnosis (no longer fulfill criteria) during future neuropsychiatric evaluations (113115). This is illustrated by the lower average distance to the DT in groups with higher CC. One can appreciate that within the range of CC for X', the average distance to the DT is much lower than within the range of CC for Y and Z.

Second, it illustrates the counterintuitive finding that diagnosed individuals with a higher IQ have greater deficits in the social domain than those with low IQ (151, 173). This is because the average AP is more pronounced the higher the CC, due to the positive slope of the DT.

Third, although cognitive abilities are temporally stable (in the absence of neurological insults, see section Risk Factors Affect Neurodevelopment), due to changes in stress and cognitive demands throughout life (such as moving to a higher educational level or getting a new job), individuals will experience variable degrees of compensatory ability and thus slightly jitter along the y-axis (similar to movement from X to X' in Figure 3, which is detailed in section Acquired and Atypical Autism, although less pronounced). This exemplifies why individuals with a milder phenotype are more likely to lose their diagnosis. It also illustrates why there is lower diagnostic stability under the autism spectrum disorder umbrella, than there is for autism as a whole [see review by (174); also (175)]; the probability of moving across a single DT is lower than moving across multiple hypothetical thresholds under the DT.

Risk Factors Affect Neurodevelopment

Within the field of autism research, studies of its causes have increased significantly in the last decades (176). Many different risk factors have been identified [see reviews by (8, 177179)] and everything from heavy metals and air pollution (8, 178, 180), vitamin deficiency (181), and medications (182, 183), to infections (8, 178, 184), immune disturbances (47, 178, 184186), pregnancy-related stress (187, 188), and pre- (189, 190) perinatal complications (189, 191) have been noted to increase the risk for autism, with no single cause being specific for the disorder or accounting for a majority of cases. The heterogeneity of autism and its associated risk factors has hampered the development of a unitary pathophysiological pathway. However, since autism is a neurological condition, it is logical that the etiologies show physiological convergence through their effects on brain development.

Neural Insults Undermine Brain Development

Brain development is a complex and delicate process. Perturbations resulting from biological insults inadvertently shift resources from growth and development, to defense and repair (192), possibly limiting cognitive development in a dose-response manner. There are many examples of the negative effects on cognitive development by risk factors, and similar effects have been found not only for autism (section Risk Factors Affect Neurodevelopment), but also in neurotypical individuals [see Table 2 in (19)]. For example, air pollution (196, 197), vitamin deficiency (198), medications (199), infection (200, 201), immune disturbances (200, 202, 203), pre- (204, 205), and postnatal stress (206208), and pre- and perinatal complications (209212) are just some examples of insults that have negative effects on the development of cognitive abilities also in neurotypical individuals.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Insult characteristics and their effects on the neuropathological burden.

Rather than identifying only very clear insults with large effects [such as valproate exposure during pregnancy, or the presence of genetic syndromes and copy number variants (CNV)], one can imagine that neural insults are normally distributed in the strength of their neurodevelopmental inhibition and thus in their effect on cognitive development. It is near impossible to quantify the detrimental effect of a single mild infection on the individual level, due to miniscule effects on future attainment of cognitive ability. However, one can conceptualize a neural exposome (213, 214) that takes into account all such exposures (stress, toxin exposure, infections etc.), which across an individual's development affect brain development and attained cognitive ability. The psychopathology-factor (34) likely correlates with the extent of the conceptualized exposome, and illustrates that it is shared across neurodevelopmental disorders.

The HCFs are disproportionately affected by insults (215). A likely explanation for the discrepant effect is that HCFs and fine motor coordination rely on the coordination between diffuse brain areas and require larger and more complex neural networks (142, 215, 216). This offers many more potential vantage points for disruptions to cause impairment and affect output. Lower cognitive functions and gross motor coordination, on the other hand, rely on localized brain areas and simpler neural networks, limiting the effects of detrimental insults and explaining the faster and improved recovery for these cognitive functions, as well as the possibility of neural remodeling recovering the function. Insults are associated with impaired motor coordination (217, 218), and motor dysfunction relates to cognitive ability also in neurotypical controls and ADHD [Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder; (217, 219)], supporting the notion that motor dysfunction may not be a core feature of autism, but a marker of cognitive dysfunction.

Recovery following neurological insults in childhood is less complete than previously thought (193). This may be due to disproportionate effects on HCFs (215, 220) which are difficult to measure reliably and their importance differs across developmental stages (220). It has long been thought that the plasticity of young brains allows for reorganization and catch-up development following insults, a notion that newer studies with longer follow-up and improved methodologies seem to contradict (193, 221). Although it is established that strong insults are associated with low cognitive ability, the difficulty of identifying subtler disruptions of HCFs, and that their importance increases throughout childhood and adolescence, likely explains why milder insults have not received the attention they deserve.

Genetic Contributions to Risk

As mentioned previously, the genetic architecture of autism implicates both common and rare variants (88, 222) [for presentation of genetic models see (2)]. Rare variants may be inherited or occur de novo, and their rate is increased in autism (223, 224). The most common (225) and well-studied types of rare variants in autism are the CNVs (43, 225227). Rare variants are associated with increased cognitive dysfunction (88, 154, 224, 228231), disorder severity (224), and rate of comorbidities (88, 166) in individuals with autism. They are also associated with cognitive dysfunction in neurotypical individuals (154, 228, 230). The association between an increased rate of rare variants and autism disappears when controlling for IQ (154), indicating that they may increase autism risk through negative effects on cognitive ability. The prevalence of rare de novo variants in the neurotypical population is lower than in autism (43, 229), despite a stochastic distribution across the population (232, 233), further supporting their role in inducing a clinical ascertainment bias.

Several genetic syndromes show increased rates of autism [see review by (234)]. The increased co-occurrence of autism and genetic syndromes may be largely explained by negative effects on CC (234). In other words, the degree of cognitive dysfunction predicts the rate of autism. Which individuals with genetic syndromes ultimately develop autism may depend on the presence of an underlying AP (231). Social dysfunction (235) and a greater polygenic burden for autism (236) in parents predicts the risk of autism in offspring with genetic syndromes, suggesting additive heritability between the AP and NB/CC-complex (sum total of interaction between NB and CC). Additive heritability also predicts higher rates of de novo variants in simplex families (43, 229).

Also supporting the notion of genetic syndromes as additive risk factors is that those with autism and genetic syndromes have less social impairment than those with only autism (237, 238) despite a greater disease burden and likely lower adaptive functioning [similarly counterintuitive as for higher IQ (151)]. This may be explained by the presence of a genetic syndrome as an additional risk factor [same pattern also found for co-occurring schizophrenia (239)], which lowers either the diagnostic threshold (upward movement of threshold on y-axis in Figure 2) or the CC (position closer to the bottom), making the autistic phenotype maladaptive at a lower intensity (milder average AP as a group).

Further suggesting that rare variants cause autism through a modulating variable, rather than directly as a core component, is that they are unspecific and shared among the neuropsychiatric disorders (21, 224, 226, 228, 240243), which is not the case for common variants (240). In other words, the genetic architecture is such that common variants increase the probability of autism across severities and risk factors (236), while rare variants and genetic syndromes cause disinhibition of maladaptive phenotypes, such as autism, by negatively affecting CC.

Although it is expected that common variants are mutually exclusive between disorders, it should be noted that some studies find slight positive polygenic correlations between the disorders (88, 244). A slight overlap is expected due to ascertainment at the disorder-level, rather than trait-level. This leads to erroneous identification of shared common variants that contribute to the NB/CC-complex (inherited common variants for immune and autonomic dysfunction), rather than the AP dimension.

Epigenetic mechanisms, such as methylation (245) and parental imprinting (246), have also been implicated in the development of autism (2, 93, 226). Some epigenetic markers are related to effects of risk factors (237) through gene-environment interactions. There is also some clustering of both rare variants (247, 248) and epigenetic effects (93, 246) on neuronal signaling and synaptic functioning, possibly contributing directly to the development of the AP. Otherwise, the specific epigenetic machinery in relation to autism development is still largely unknown, making it difficult to specify epigenetic mechanisms within an explanatory framework at this time.

Immune Function as a Modulator of Exogenous Insults and an Endogenous Risk Factor

Many studies have identified immune dysfunction in autism (48, 186, 249251). For example, differences in human leukocyte antigens (185), such as specific variants (186, 252) and homozygosity (253), are linked to increased risk of autism.

Although central nervous system infections can act as direct risk factors [see review by (254)], indirect effects through gestational maternal immune activation may be of greater significance (178, 255). Indicating a possible causative mechanism by the inflammatory response is that, although gestational fever is associated with increased risk, it can be attenuated by antipyretics (256). The inflammatory response includes production of a range of inflammatory proteins, such as autoantibodies and cytokines.

Autoantibodies are frequently identified (186, 249, 257, 258), illustrating the connection to autoimmunity (250, 257, 259) and pointing to the importance of both systemic and neuroinflammation in autism (251, 260, 261). Differences in the levels of cytokines have been found (178, 255), which may represent both variations in immune activation, and exposure to environmental agents. Cytokines are particularly interesting for their ability to affect aspects of brain function and development (178, 262), such as synaptic function and HPA-axis activation (263).

Similar to the wide range of identified risk factors, the diversity of specific immune alterations that are implicated in the development of autism means that it is unlikely that a singular alteration or mechanism can be identified. Rather, it may be the case that (1) immune dysfunction is an indirect causative agent through mediation between insults and disability, making individuals with immune alterations less able to defend against the deleterious effects of various insults and that (2) immune activation itself acts as an indirect unspecific insult. Supporting this is that both mechanisms may result in cognitive dysfunction (203, 264, 265), and have been identified in other neuropsychiatric disorders (266). The idea that neuropathological insults and immune dysfunctions are causative factors in the development of an autistic phenotype has been proposed previously (21, 32, 178).

Autonomic Function as Modulator of Insults

The autonomic system represents the interplay between the external and internal milieus. Environmental and cognitive demands are relayed to the internal physiology for proper allocation of resources, supporting either acute survival (a fight-or-flight stress state) or long-term survival (a rest-and-digest state of growth and repair). Adaptable, fast, and flexible coordination of the sympathetic and parasympathetic branches of the autonomic system (267) is necessary for timely allocation of resources. Any dyscoordination within this process (such as a sustained sympathetic arousal long after termination of a brief stressful situation or infection) leads to suboptimal allocation, and thus impaired growth and development.

Autonomic dysregulation is a frequent finding in psychiatric disorders in general [see review by (268); also (269271)], and autism in particular [see reviews by (45, 272); also (273) for hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis]. A flexible and adaptive autonomic response system can be indexed by a high heart rate variability (HRV) which is negatively related to inflammatory markers (268, 274, 275), HPA axis activation (267, 268, 274), mood disorders (268, 269), and risk of autism (45, 46, 267, 272), and positively related to social cognition (268, 272, 276), fluid intelligence (277), and EFs (272, 278).

Many disease states are associated with a decreased HRV; it may be that autonomic dysfunction in clinical populations is due to an ascertainment bias, representing a causative risk factor through its effect on brain and cognitive development. This is supported by higher functioning individuals with autism not having a disrupted HPA axis, while lower-functioning individuals do (273), and that prenatal HRV is more predictive of later social functioning than birth weight, medical comorbidities, or socio-economic status (279). These findings could signify that it is autonomic function, mediating the effect between insults and immunity, that is the determinant of adult morbidity and adaptive functioning, with potential utility in estimating the effect of the exposome.

Sex Difference in Biological Vulnerability

Sex differences with regard to the AP and CC have been outlined in sections The Biology of the BAP and Heritability and Clinical Implications of Low Cognitive Ability. There is also a potential sex difference within the NB. As proposed in the female protective effect theory, they have a greater genetic burden, illustrated by increased rates of, and more penetrant, risk factors and rare variants (227, 242, 247). This finding may be explained by the presence of risk-protective factors compared with males, such as milder BAP or higher social cognitive ability.

Males have an increased biological vulnerability compared to females [see reviews by (179, 280); also (193, 281)], which is exemplified by greater pre- and postnatal mortality (282, 283), and inhibitory effect by stress on development of the frontal lobe and EF (208). Studies investigating the sex ratio at birth have found that there are less males being born following maternal exposure toxins during pregnancy [although often at higher concentrations than in the general population, see review by (284)], as well as after floods (285), warfare (286, 287), earthquakes (288), and death within the family (289), which all point to an increased vulnerability for males in utero. Proposed explanations include a slower maturation and thus higher vulnerability for male fetuses and toddlers, a higher maternal immune reactivity to the presence of a Y-chromosome (280), and inhibitory, or at least modulatory, effects on immunity by sex hormones and glucocorticoids (193, 290292). Bearing in mind that the sexes have differential responses to exogenous risk factors (293), it is possible that males, as a group, are statistically associated with a higher vulnerability for a greater range of exogenous insults. If males are more biologically vulnerable, it follows that they have a higher average NB for each insult. This implies that females have a higher threshold for accumulating risk factors, with lower disinhibitory effects on cognitive compensation and thus lower rates of maladaptive behaviors and autism diagnoses.

Operationalization of the Neuropathological Burden

Since autism is a condition affecting the brain, it is logical that the vast range of etiologies show convergence through a common neuropathological mechanism. The NB is conceptualized as the sum total effect of various insults and the effect they have on brain development. Endogenous or exogenous risk factors that negatively affect normal brain development are likely to negatively influence cognitive attainment, impacting the ability to compensate for various cognitive difficulties, including those arising from an AP. To give an analogy, the interaction between the NB and CC acts like a funnel, where a wide range of etiologies come together and cause neurodevelopmental inhibition, which disinhibits a maladaptive phenotype.

Similar causative mechanisms have been proposed previously [variable insult model: (32); developmental brain dysfunction: (21)]. Although this framework obviously cannot outline the effects of specific insults on risk increase, it emphasizes their additive effects, and the importance of considering also the vast number of low-intensity insults which individuals are subjected to across development (such as stress, minor infections, physical injuries) rather than just the most deleterious ones (such as genetic syndromes, or intrauterine exposure to valproate and rubella).

An individual's immune function and autonomic regulation determine the extent to which insults affect brain development and the neuropsychological profile. Sometimes they alleviate the insult, as when the immune system fends off an infection; sometimes it is the main cause of damage, as when an activated immune system leads to secondary neuroinflammation.

Whether insults cause an altered immunity, or immune dysfunction increases the effects of insults remains to be completely elucidated; one may expect a combination of both. Either way, similar to that of cognitive ability, it is likely that there is a clinical ascertainment bias, wherein individuals with immune and/or autonomic dysfunction are at greater risk of disrupted development, leading to lower CC and increased vulnerability for autism. Finally, HRV may serve as an indirect indicator of the NB. Future studies will elucidate what role HRV plays in relation to autism development, whether it modulates adaptive ability in the face of an insulting agent, is merely a quantification of previous insults, or both.

Insult Characterization as a Means of Stratification

Insults that individually have low effects and are cumulatively continuously distributed (such as lifetime stress, and immune activation by minor infections or injuries) are more likely to be shared across the population. Conversely, those that have greater effects and a stochastic distribution (such as genetic syndromes, autoimmune diseases, and major stress or injury) are less likely to be shared. Part of the missing heritability may be explained by failure to account for risk-modifying insults, in particular those that are shared and difficult to measure. However, highly penetrant risk factors are more easily identified, allowing them to be individually characterized. Characterization of insults as outlined in Table 2 may be used for biological stratification and increase homogeneity. This extends a previously presented “bottom-up methodology” [(294); where they found increased homogeneity following stratification by insults] and incorporates it into a global framework.

Epilepsy as an Example of a Neuropathological Burden

Epilepsy is a frequent co-occurring condition in autism (4, 295). Although epilepsy is also a heterogeneous disorder, there are indications that the genetic architecture implicates common genetic variants (296). This is telling of an underlying continuous distribution in the population and predicts the existence of subclinical epileptic activity without seizures. Similar to the BAP and ALTs, this may suggest the existence of a broader epileptic endophenotype, where spontaneous discharges have an underlying normal distribution pattern, with the physiological mechanism being differences in the threshold for neuronal activation.

It is unlikely to be a categorical disorder where individuals with epilepsy differ from those with epileptic activity, who also differ from healthy individuals. Rather, there is a continuum of the extent of spontaneous neuronal activity in the brain, where some individuals have a low enough threshold for the epileptic activity to propagate and cause a seizure. A decrease in the threshold for neuronal activation decreases the signal-to-noise ratio, which effectively decreases the absolute amount of information that can be processed in each neural network, which would induce greater relative issues in the large and complex neural networks underlying the HCFs.

Epilepy has been found to occur in as much as 46% of patients with autism (4), compared to about 0.5% of the neurotypical population (297). Subclinical epileptic activity occurs in a larger proportion of both those with [30–76%; (295, 298, 299)], and without autism [1–4%; (300, 301)]. Following the reasoning outlined above, epileptic activity, and epilepsy even more so, are associated with lower cognitive ability (302, 303), with significantly greater impairment of the HCFs (302), resulting in lower social functioning, and greater severity of autism (153, 304). Thus, similar to the proposed reversal of causation for cognitive ability in autism, the decreased threshold in neural activation might be a (at least partial) cause for the lower cognitive ability and HCFs in that population.

Given the frequent clinical co-occurrence, the obvious effect on neural processing, and the similarity of findings regarding CC, epilepsy is presented as an example of a neuropathological insult causing an increased NB: it is a clearly defined neuropathological entity with different locations, extent of activity, and levels of severity (epilepsy with seizure activity confers a greater burden than subclinical epileptic activity without seizures) that can be postulated to negatively impact bottom-up neural processing, with down-stream disproportionate effects on top-down cognitive ability and HCFs.

The likely association between the conditions is such that the presence of epileptic activity represents a NB which disinhibits the behavioral phenotype of autism, both by negatively affecting cognitive ability, and by inducing its own set of cognitive and sensory disturbances. In support of this conclusion, when controlling for IQ, epilepsy has no effect on the severity of autism and ALTs (153) which may indicate that they co-occur clinically due to an interaction with cognitive ability. This lends further credence to the idea of CC and the NB being drivers for the expression of an autistic phenotype requiring a diagnosis, and their co-occurrence being due to a clinical ascertainment bias.

It should be noted that the increased neural activity with epileptic activity likely influences patterns of synaptic budding and pruning. This may overlap with the pathophysiologic mechanism of the AP, potentially explaining the disproportionate association with autism, possibly out of proportion to its negative effect on CC. Future studies will have to investigate this association within the framework of a pathogenetic triad more closely.

Implications of the Framework

Clinical Ascertainment Bias

The high heterogeneity of clinical cohorts suggests that some facets are unlikely to be part of the core autistic condition. This framework suggests that the presence of risk factors, due to their effects on CC, increases propensity to seek mental health services, leading to a self-selection of the clinical sample, and thus a bias in sampling. Conceptually, autism may be a single condition with one common pathway, or many conditions with many pathways with phenotypic convergence that is homogenous enough to be identified as autistic. It has previously been argued against the existence of a singular core autistic condition that is common across the spectrum (14, 15), which is reflected in the widening of the diagnostic criteria and employment of the umbrella term “autism spectrum disorder.” An alternative hypothesis, which is argued in this framework, is that there is a singular core autistic endophenotype where risk factors, such as low cognitive ability and immune dysfunction, co-occur clinically due to an ascertainment bias. Such a situation predicts both the heterogeneity and overlap of risk factors between different disorders [shared psychopathology (34)]. The presence of a clinical ascertainment bias is central to the framework, and it has important ramifications regarding the interpretation of research findings and study design [the importance of which has been noted previously (40, 305)].

Although it is precisely the clinical population, with its overlapping risk factors, that is of interest for clinical psychiatric research, for academic inquiry, it may be more fruitful to acknowledge and identify a core autistic condition (assuming it exists), which is presented in this framework as the AP (see Figure 4). For example, when studying risk factors, one wishes to include a control group that differs from the experimental group only with regard to its risk factors; individuals with autism should be compared with a control group that consists of undiagnosed individuals with a pronounced AP (autism vs. pronounced BAP). When studying the development of the core autistic condition, one should include undiagnosed individuals (without risk factors) who differ only with regard to the AP (mild vs. pronounced BAP).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The autistic personality type in relation to neurotypical individuals and those with autism. Illustration how the two clinically neurotypical groups above the diagnostic threshold compare with diagnosed individuals. It shows that individuals with an autistic personality differ from prototypically neurotypical individuals with regard to their position on the autistic personality dimension. It also shows that the difference between having a diagnosis, or not, is contingent on differences in the NB/CC-complex (the result of the interaction between the neuropathological burden and its effect on cognitive capacity).

Deconstructing autism into a core condition and associated risk factors may improve study power and predictive ability at the individual level. Population-based samples can limit the ascertainment bias. In studies where such sampling is not feasible, inclusion of undiagnosed individuals with a pronounced AP, for example as a third group within a case-control design, may mitigate the bias and allow for stronger conclusions to be drawn regarding which aspect of autism is affected.

Stratification

The biological, phenotypic, and clinical heterogeneity of autism presents a great obstacle to its scientific inquiry, having spurred interest in various forms of stratification. This is a difficult task, as it is yet unclear which phenotypes can be stratified and how, and whether one achieves the best results using neuroanatomical, cognitive, etiological, or other endophenotypes. The promises of stratification include greater homogeneity and power in scientific investigations, as well as personalized and tailored pharmacologic, and psychotherapeutic interventions. Not only that, differences in cognitive phenotype lead to different clinical outcomes (111), which necessitates stratification for optimal identification of individuals at high risk of poor outcome, and guidance of choice of treatment depending on who will benefit from it.

This framework argues that the heterogeneity almost exclusively stems from differences in functions and interactions within the NB/CC-complex; each patient has an individual neuropsychological profile (cognitive heterogeneity), and set of insults [biological heterogeneity, further increasing cognitive heterogeneity (154)], that give rise to a large phenotypic heterogeneity. Compounding these with normal variations in personality (outside the AP, such as extroversion/introversion), one can appreciate why no two individuals with autism are alike. A similar explanation has been proposed previously (32).

Unfortunately, most etiologies underlying the NB cannot be identified or quantified; those that can, may be used for stratification through a bottom-up approach for characterization of identifiable discrete insults (Table 2) (19, 294); those that cannot, may be continuously quantified using HRV as a tentative measure of the NB (though, this needs to be specifically addressed and tested in longitudinal studies). Finally, rather than stratifying cohorts according to known insults (which is both difficult and greatly limits sample sizes), employment of the study designs outlined above may be a more feasible approach toward achieving sample homogeneity.

Heritability and Familial Clustering

All three factors of the triad are heritable (74, 141, 306), with at least partly polygenic inheritance patterns (74, 143, 307, 308) predicting continuous distributions in the population. This illustrates the importance of considering parent phenotypes for risk stratification (231). For example, one can compare the parents' triad mean scores for individual risk stratification, since some individuals will have a starting point closer to a clinical diagnosis than others, irrespective of the presence of risk factors (230). The heritability implies that both ALTs, and idiosyncrasies of cognitive, immune, and autonomic function cluster in families, and in particular in multiplex families. It predicts that multiplex families have higher rates of ALTs than simplex families (60, 61, 70, 71), and that multiplex families have increased rates of rare inherited variants, while simplex families have higher rates of rare de novo variants in probands (43, 229). The heritability of the triad not only explains patterns of familial clustering of autism and its associated risk factors, but also the rate of comorbid neurodevelopmental disorders.

Relationship to Comorbidities

Individuals with a dysfunctional NB/CC-complex (inherited or acquired) will have greater difficulty compensating for the issues arising from the AP. Considering the symptoms of other neuropsychiatric disorders as also being part of dimensional personality types—such as schizotypal traits for schizophrenia spectrum disorders—it follows that an impaired compensatory ability will increase the probability (decrease threshold) that those traits become maladaptive. The lower the threshold, the greater the probability that any, and therefore several, of the phenotypes are pronounced enough to be clinically identified and warrant a diagnosis [as illustrated in Figure 2 in (231), though not only for ID]. This is exemplified by samples with more rare variants (88) and lower cognitive ability having higher rates of psychiatric symptoms and diagnoses, and that ID is the greatest risk factor for the presence of comorbidities (102, 120).

The clinical utility of this is that one can imagine the characterization of an individual's personality profile (pattern of intensities of each disorder-specific broader phenotype) determining which phenotypes are likely to become maladaptive, and that the degree of NB/CC-complex dysfunction determines the probability that each phenotype is maladaptive, and thus the number of diagnoses. Unfortunately, one can expect that the quantification of the effect that the NB/CC-complex has on each disorder is complicated by the fact that the weightings on different insults and cognitive subdomains may be somewhat disorder specific (Table 2), which will require specification through further studies.

Although the case was made above for splitting with regard to academic inquiry, the presence of shared NB/CC-complex dysfunction in clinical populations, due to an ascertainment bias, speaks in favor of lumping in a clinical context. Individuals with lower CC are expected to have greater difficulty across phenotypes, and are thus more likely to require help from psychiatric services. Prognosis and long-term outcome are often highly dependent on cognitive and adaptive ability (111, 115, 309) rather than on the strength of the underlying phenotype. Not fulfilling complete criteria for a single disorder does not rule out the possibility of subthreshold problems from several domains, which may still be associated with significant disability (21, 231, 310). There is likely a common underlying psychopathology that needs to be taken into account when assessing neuropsychiatric patients (21, 34); given its importance for disorder severity and long-term prognosis, its characterization should perhaps be given higher priority. The AP may be particularly illustrative, since individuals without NB/CC-complex dysfunction may lead fulfilling lives and have successful careers despite having pronounced ALTs (72, 311).

Sex Difference

The male-biased sex-ratio has steadily fallen due to improved detection and awareness of autism in females. However, potential sex differences due to differences in sex hormones exist in all three factors of the triad: females have a lower BAP, higher social cognitive ability, and they may be less biologically vulnerable to neuropathological insults. These differences predict that the true clinical sex-ratio is at least slightly male-biased, and that a clinical 1:1 ratio probably indicates overdiagnosis of females with autism.

Proposed theories for the sex difference include the extreme male brain theory [which proposes that higher testosterone and a neurobiologically male brain confer a greater risk for autism; (26, 312)], the female protective effect [which proposes genetic, or other features that lower their disorder severity, or raise the threshold for diagnosis; (167)], and a difference in behavioral phenotype between the sexes (313).

Neuroendocrinological differences, primarily due to lower testosterone, give rise to a milder BAP and higher social cognitive ability. This combination may lead to the emergence of different compensatory strategies and cause the clinical appearance of females to present with a different phenotype. Given a compensatory mechanism (such as a lower BAP, a higher social cognitive ability, or a lower biological vulnerability for insults), a higher disease burden is required for the same observed phenotypic expression and rate of diagnostic detection. In other words, the extreme male brain theory and difference in behavioral phenotype can explain the occurrence of the increased disease burden proposed within the female protective effect theory (at least within the clinical population). All three theories have supporting evidence, indicate that they all, to some extent, contribute to the sex differences. The presented framework instead attempts to offer one common conceptualization incorporating all three theories.

Loss of Diagnosis

In longitudinal follow-up studies, some individuals fail to fulfill the full diagnostic criteria, and thus lose their diagnosis. This is not to say that they are less “autistic” since the BAP is highly stable across time, even in individuals with a diagnosis (56). However, they can appear to be less maladaptive, thus not requiring a formal diagnosis. Studies indicate that this “loss of diagnosis” occurs more often in individuals with higher cognitive ability (114, 314), which is empirically predicted by the patient population having a lower average distance to the diagnostic threshold among those with higher cognitive ability (due to the positive slope of the threshold; see Figure 2). With regard to how dynamic changes in the psychosocial environment may lead to differing demands, affecting adaptive and maladaptive phenotypes, the pathogenetic triad can be supplemented by explanations in the dynamic model (23).

Acquired and Atypical Autism

The proposed framework opens the possibility for an acquired phenotype even beyond childhood; a drop in CC relative to the AP may induce a maladaptive phenotype warranting a diagnosis (X → X' in Figure 3). There have been reports of acquired autism following early childhood (315317). However, parental BAP or premorbid BAP in the proband were not reported. They may have already had increased liability through a more pronounced BAP. Future studies on acquired autism should include this aspect.

Even in the absence of an AP, it could be possible that impairment of specific functions or areas, such as those subserving social cognition or Theory of Mind (318), can induce a social communication deficit that appears autistic. This will likely not be associated with other patterns that are specific for autism, outlined in this framework, and may underly the presentation of atypical autism or pervasive developmental disorder-not otherwise specified. Transgenerational studies investigating these subgroups' endophenotypes and BAP may shed light on this possibility.

Strengths and Savant Syndrome

The BAP is associated with a set of strengths (72, 77, 78, 311), and the common variants underlying autism are associated with high cognitive ability and educational attainment (79, 88, 89, 319). These aspects may predict, at its extreme, an increased prevalence of savant syndrome in autism (320).

However, due to the frequent association with cognitive dysfunction in clinical autism, this requires an explanation. There are two possible explanations for these patterns: either autism represents a single endophenotype associated with an inverted U-curve with respect to cognitive ability, as predicted in (20), or the strength-associated phenotype (AP) is uncoupled from the clinical phenotype (autism), as predicted by the pathogenetic triad.

It has been found that patients with severe ID have higher polygenic scores for educational attainment than those with mild/moderate ID (89). Having in mind that this is a single study, this is more in line with the former explanation, and could represent a minor invalidation of the pathogenetic triad. The triad can accommodate this finding if the common variants underlying the AP induce differential susceptibility [decrease resilience and increase plasticity; (321, 322)], leading to increased attainment in enriched environment and lack of risk factors. One should remember that studies identifying an association between polygenetic burden and cognitive ability have primarily used samples from first-world countries. Future studies will have to investigate the possibility of the AP inducing differential susceptibility.

Discussion

This paper has attempted to outline the pathogenetic triad and illustrate how it relates to the autism literature. The discussion will outline the validity and limitations of the framework and provide examples of testable postulates and hypotheses.

Framework Validity

All models are wrong, but some are useful”—George Box

It is a fallacy to think one can model a complex and multifactorial disorder, such as autism, using a single reductionist model. One needs to identify an optimal tradeoff between simplicity and specificity, and the validity of the model ultimately depends on the balance of this tradeoff. This framework is no different; specificity has been traded for explanatory power with regard to global pathogenetic mechanisms. The lack of specification means there will always be individual studies that oppose the outlined mechanisms. This does not necessarily undermine the framework, and future systematic reviews focusing on each proposed mechanism and interaction will be instrumental in supporting its validity.

Clinical neuropsychiatric evaluations for autism involve the identification of autistic symptoms and behaviors (AP), neuropsychological testing (CC) and identification of severe and explicit insults, such as perinatal complications, diseases or genetic syndromes (NB). This general methodology implies a collective awareness of (at least) a three-factor model underlying autism, with similar factors as those presented in the framework, which supports the face validity of the proposed framework.

Within the autism literature, there are a range of findings that need to be incorporated into a model for autism before it can be considered complete: biological and phenotypic heterogeneity, differences in phenotype and prevalence between sexes, the presence of cognitive strengths in both autism and the BAP, and several cases of acquired autism beyond the usual time of onset. Besides these findings, a complete model must also account for findings regarding its genetic architecture, biological and cognitive endophenotypes, the behavioral phenotype, and clinical correlates such as prevalence rates and co-occurring disorders. As can be seen in Table 1, of the existing explanatory models, this framework is the one that pushes content validity the furthest in terms of the number and range of domains of autism research findings that it attempts to incorporate.

Criterion and discriminant validity implies that the framework, when applied practically, achieves a high diagnostic resolution for the studied phenomenon. The validity of the pathogenetic triad has been examined in a pilot study (323), with classification performed against the participants' diagnostic status based on gold standard neuropsychiatric evaluations and clinical diagnoses. The approach for operationalization outlined in this paper was used for quantification and classification: the autistic personality dimension as the AQ score (72), cognitive compensation as the working memory IQ subscale of the WAIS (324), and the neuropathological burden as the cardiac vagal index (a measure of HRV using electrocardiography). By collapsing the three-dimensional data space onto a one-dimensional axis using linear transformations one was able to achieve a high diagnostic accuracy with an area under the receiver operating characteristic of 96.3% [95% CI (0.913–1.000)]. The method in the pilot study exemplifies how the framework can be practically used, and illustrates its potential for yielding a high diagnostic resolution. The diagnostic odds ratio was 85.5 for the case-control classification which, unfortunately, included only normal IQ individuals matched for total IQ (due to convenience sample); a testament to its high discriminatory power. Since the ratio and AUC both depend on disease severity, one can expect even higher accuracy following inclusion of individuals with lower IQ, and particularly when tested on a sample that has not been IQ-matched. Divergent validity was not investigated, as no other NDDs were sampled. However, it is assumed that there is divergent validity due to the specificity of the first factor; substituting for a schizotypal personality type, for example, should by definition yield an unrelated classification (inversely to the degree of overlap between the quantitative measures used to estimate the core conditions, such as the AP and schizotypy). Convergent validity implies that constructs that are theoretically supposed to overlap, in fact overlap, such as shared etiologies between different neurodevelopmental disorders. The framework has convergent validity insofar as the NB/CC-complex dysfunction is postulated to be shared among disorders.

The divergent and convergent validities together make up, and support the construct validity of the framework. Its construct validity is further supported by the many findings in the literature that fit with the presented mechanisms, many of which (although non-exhaustively) have been outlined throughout the paper, implying that the inferences made from the model reflect the intended construct. Some of the predictions and testable postulates of the framework (Table 3) already have provisional support, while others are yet to be validated.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Testable postulates of the pathogenetic triad.

Limitations

There are several potential limitations that need to be addressed before this framework can be considered valid. The most important is that the conclusions are supported by a wide, transdisciplinary, and for obvious reasons, non-systematic literature search. The method of identification of relevant literature may have been affected by subjectivity and confirmation bias, and important references have certainly been omitted. This mandates that each aspect of the framework be subjected to individual systematic reviews.

The framework is presented as a top-down model that outlines global mechanisms for pathogenesis. As such, its direct clinical applicability is limited by the lack of specification of the operationalized variables. It merely acknowledges the likely importance of certain subdomains, which can guide development of weighted indices for each factor. Future studies applying the pathogenetic triad, such as classification studies, will indicate which weightings are superior. This will be guided by achieved classification accuracies and potentially illustrate underlying physiological and phenotypic mechanisms.

Although CC is presented as the holy grail of compensation and diagnostic prediction, it is not expected to be the sole determinant of long-term functioning and outcome. First, it emphasizes the relative contributions of different subdomains; some individuals with a very high IQ may still have a diagnosis of autism, likely because of the presence of a much more pronounced AP and/or deficits in some EFs which do not enable adequate compensation. Second, the behavioral phenotype is affected also by psychosocial factors, such as additional mental health issues, personality differences, and sociodemographics. For example, individuals with social phobia and high IQ may have worse outcomes than those without phobia and low IQ. Although left out for model simplicity, psychosocial factors may have to be included as a moderating variable for completeness (in which case they likely moderate, and are moderated by CC in Figure 1).

One can argue against the validity of grouping cognitive abilities into a common category. However, although another conceptualization may prove superior, this approach accommodates the range and pattern of risk factors in the clinical population, and provides a unifying mechanism by which the range of risk factors can induce autism. At least through the prism of an underlying singular core autistic condition. It is also possible that there is no such core condition, and that the autisms are truly as heterogeneous as they seem.

The proposed separation of rare and common variants is based on their co-occurrence with and without cognitive dysfunction, which also suggests differing contributory mechanisms. However, this view of the genetic architecture may be optimistic due to the complexity, and still relatively unknown inner workings of the genetic and epigenetic landscapes (325), requiring further study. In clinical populations, there is clustering of rare variants in parts of the genome associated with development of the brain and cognitive ability (154). Rare variants may occur in locations that overlap with the physiological mechanisms underlying the development of the AP, implying that they may give rise to the AP without affecting the CC. Oftentimes the locations are mutually exclusive, but sometimes they overlap, subserving the development of both the AP and CC; this pattern can be identified and empirically tested by comparing cognitive ability and the AP across genetic markers using large sample sizes.

Many conditions are associated with autism (e.g., ADHD, immune and autonomic dysfunction, developmental coordination disorder) and it is not always clear if they are merely clinically coupled, or inextricable parts of the autistic condition due to genetic and biological coupling. For example, it has been suggested that autism and ADHD are part of a single continuum with a common origin (326), and that autism is an autoimmune disease (47). These conclusions could be explained by a lack of control of extraneous variables, such as a shared dysfunction of the NB/CC-complex due to a clinical ascertainment bias, giving rise to a common endophenotype. Findings supporting a common origin invalidate the presented framework. However, some associations are due to methodological constraints, such as failure to account for extraneous variables. Before such studies can be said to undermine the validity of the framework, their conclusions and methodologies need to be revisited in light of the presented framework.

The biological hierarchy for the BAP, through the effects of common variants, is considered homogeneous for model simplicity. Whether the BAP consists of a single entity on a spectrum (10, 65, 327), or several disparate but, more often than not, co-occurring factors (164, 328) remains an open question. It may be the case that autism does not have a singular susceptibility core, but a combination of endophenotypes that co-occur more often than expected by chance (such as separate susceptibilities for social communication and rigidity/repetitive behaviors). If that is the case, the validity of the framework is not undermined. However, the first factor (AP) would require revision, increasing the complexity of the framework. In the proposed conceptualization, for quantification purposes and clinical testing, one can consider the BAP to be a single entity, since the absolute magnitude of such traits confers a risk factor; if the traits stem from separate factors, the presence of two rather than one such factor, or the presence of more intense such traits, confers a higher total aggregate risk. If that is the case, one can imagine some factors to be more “detrimental” than others, in terms of probability of requiring a diagnosis (such as social > rigidity/repetitiveness), but to outline their relative contributions is outside the scope of this paper.

Postulates and Testable Hypotheses

Postulates of the framework are presented in Table 3 as a means of generating hypotheses for future studies, and as a means of enabling falsifiability of the framework. Failure to replicate the following postulates, if not explainable through inadequate study design, may be considered as invalidating the framework.

Conclusion

A unifying framework for the development of autism is presented through the prism of a pathogenetic triad. The major aims of the framework have been to incorporate a wide range of transdisciplinary research findings, and to increase the practical utility of autism models through operationalization. The theory assumes a top-down approach by focusing on outlining global mechanisms of pathogenesis. There is limited specification at each of the factors and bottom-up studies are needed to delineate the specifics of each factor and their interactions. Despite this, it can still serve to inform reasoning regarding future research, interpretation of findings, and development of classification methods. In its simplest form, ALTs and polygenic risk scores can be collated with data on cognitive abilities (IQ, EF) and insult burden (rare genetic variant burden, insult characterization, HRV, immune/autonomic dysfunction polygenic risk scores) to allow for modeling and testing of its predictive ability on diagnostic status. Biologically grounded classification models may prove instrumental in improving the detection of autism in females and individuals with supranormal cognitive ability, for which the clinical interview has low sensitivity.

Although several aspects of the framework have been individually outlined previously, this is the first attempt at conceptualizing a unifying theory that ties together both previously proposed mechanisms, and the wide range of findings in the autism literature. The following are the main contributions of the framework:

- It is the first attempt to incorporate several patterns of findings in the autism literature into a single model: sex differences, strengths associated with the BAP, biological and clinical heterogeneity, and the possibility of acquired autism after early childhood.

- It presents a convergent pathophysiological mechanism, through inhibition of neurodevelopment and cognitive ability, as an explanation for the wide range of identified risk factors.

- It presents the BAP as the common core autistic condition, representing a personality dimension which is decoupled from the cognitive dysfunction of the clinical phenotype, and not pathological in itself.

- It proposes that low cognitive ability, and immune and autonomic dysfunction are not part of the core, but act as independent risk factors that induce a clinical ascertainment bias. This has implications for its scientific inquiry in general, and for explaining the heterogeneity and clinical overlap among disorders in particular.

- It emphasizes the importance of considering the cumulative effects of low intensity insults on brain development, compensatory ability, and risk of diagnosis.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Author Contributions

The author confirms being the sole contributor of this work and has approved it for publication.

Funding

The author was supported by Fredrik O Ingrid Thurings stiftelse (2018-00419; 2020-00581), the Gothenburg Society of Medicine and Kristina Stenborgs Stiftelse (GLS-960453), Stiftelsen Professor Bror Gadelius Minnesfond, Stiftelsen Systrarna Greta Johansson och Brita Anderssons minnesfond, and FoUU Department of Radiology, Sahlgrenska University Hospital. Other than financial support, there was no involvement in the paper.

Conflict of Interest

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Abbreviations

ADHD, Attention-Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder; AP, Autistic Personality; BAP, Broader Autism Phenotype; CC, Cognitive Capacity; DSM, Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of mental disorders; DT, Diagnostic Threshold; EF, Executive Function; HCF, Higher Cognitive Function; ID, Intellectual Dysfunction; IQ, Intelligence Quotient; NB, Neuropathological Burden.

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: Author (2013).

Google Scholar

2. Wisniowiecka-Kowalnik B, Nowakowska BA. Genetics and epigenetics of autism spectrum disorder-current evidence in the field. J Appl Genet. (2019) 60:37–47. doi: 10.1007/s13353-018-00480-w

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Thapar A, Rutter M. Genetic advances in autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2021) 51:4232–332. doi: 10.1007/s10803-020-04685-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Lai MC, Lombardo MV, Baron-Cohen S. Autism. Lancet. (2014) 383:896–910. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61539-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Lundstrom S, Reichenberg A, Anckarsater H, Lichtenstein P, Gillberg C. Autism phenotype versus registered diagnosis in Swedish children: prevalence trends over 10 years in general population samples. BMJ. (2015) 350:h1961. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h1961

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Arvidsson O, Gillberg C, Lichtenstein P, Lundstrom S. Secular changes in the symptom level of clinically diagnosed autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2018) 59:744–51. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12864

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bourgeron T. From the genetic architecture to synaptic plasticity in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2015) 16:551–63. doi: 10.1038/nrn3992

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Hertz-Picciotto I, Schmidt RJ, Krakowiak P. Understanding environmental contributions to autism: causal concepts and the state of science. Autism Res. (2018) 11:554–86. doi: 10.1002/aur.1938

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Frye RE, Vassall S, Kaur G, Lewis C, Karim M, Rossignol D. Emerging biomarkers in autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. Ann Transl Med. (2019) 7:792. doi: 10.21037/atm.2019.11.53

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Constantino JN. The quantitative nature of autistic social impairment. Pediatr Res. (2011) 69:55R–62R. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212ec6e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Haslam N, Holland E, Kuppens P. Categories versus dimensions in personality and psychopathology: a quantitative review of taxometric research. Psychol Med. (2012) 42:903–20. doi: 10.1017/S0033291711001966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Craddock N. Owen MJ. The Kraepelinian dichotomy - going, going but still not gone. Br J Psychiatry. (2010) 196:92–5. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.109.073429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Mottron L, Bzdok D. Autism spectrum heterogeneity: fact or artifact? Mol Psychiatry. (2020) 25:3178–85. doi: 10.1038/s41380-020-0748-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Waterhouse L, London E, Gillberg C. ASD validity. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. (2016) 3:302–29. doi: 10.1007/s40489-016-0085-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Happe F, Ronald A, Plomin R. Time to give up on a single explanation for autism. Nat Neurosci. (2006) 9:1218–20. doi: 10.1038/nn1770

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Casanova MF, Frye RE, Gillberg C, Casanova EL. Editorial: comorbidity and autism spectrum disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2020) 11:617395. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.617395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Mandy W, Lai MC. Towards sex- and gender-informed autism research. Autism. (2017) 21:643–5. doi: 10.1177/1362361317706904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Werling DM, Geschwind DH. Sex differences in autism spectrum disorders. Curr Opin Neurol. (2013) 26:146–53. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32835ee548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Sarovic D. A framework for neurodevelopmental disorders: operationalization of a pathogenetic triad. PsyArXiv [Preprint] (2019). doi: 10.31234/osf.io/mbeqh

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Markram K, Markram H. The intense world theory - a unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Front Hum Neurosci. (2010) 4:224. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2010.00224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Moreno-De-Luca A, Myers SM, Challman TD, Moreno-De-Luca D, Evans DW, Ledbetter DH. Developmental brain dysfunction: revival and expansion of old concepts based on new genetic evidence. Lancet Neurol. (2013) 12:406–14. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(13)70011-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Inui T, Kumagaya S, Myowa-Yamakoshi M. Neurodevelopmental hypothesis about the etiology of autism spectrum disorders. Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11:354. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00354

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Aggernaes B. Suggestion of a dynamic model of the development of neurodevelopmental disorders and the phenomenon of autism. AIMS Molecular Science. (2020) 7:122–82. doi: 10.3934/molsci.2020008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hu VW. From genes to environment: using integrative genomics to build a “systems-level” understanding of autism spectrum disorders. Child Dev. (2013) 84:89–103. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01759.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Hosman CMH, van Doesum KTM, Van Santvoort F. Prevention of emotional problems and psychiatric risks in children of parents with a mental illness in the Netherlands: I. The scientific basis to a comprehensive approach. Aust e-J Adv Mental Health. (2009) 8:250–63. doi: 10.5172/jamh.8.3.250

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Baron-Cohen S. The extreme male brain theory of autism. Trends Cogn Sci. (2002) 6:248–54. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01904-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Baron-Cohen S. Autism: the empathizing-systemizing (E-S) theory. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2009) 1156:68–80. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04467.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Rubenstein JL, Merzenich MM. Model of autism: increased ratio of excitation/inhibition in key neural systems. Genes Brain Behav. (2003) 2:255–67. doi: 10.1034/j.1601-183X.2003.00037.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Minshew NJ, Goldstein G, Siegel DJ. Neuropsychologic functioning in autism: profile of a complex information processing disorder. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. (1997) 3:303–16. doi: 10.1017/S1355617797003032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Geschwind DH, Levitt P. Autism spectrum disorders: developmental disconnection syndromes. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2007) 17:103–11. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2007.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Just MA, Keller TA, Malave VL, Kana RK, Varma S. Autism as a neural systems disorder: a theory of frontal-posterior underconnectivity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2012) 36:1292–313. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Goyal DK, Miyan JA. Neuro-immune abnormalities in autism and their relationship with the environment: a variable insult model for autism. Front Endocrinol (Lausanne). (2014) 5:29. doi: 10.3389/fendo.2014.00029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Frith U. Autism: Explaining the Enigma. Oxford: Basil Blackwell (1989).

Google Scholar

34. Caspi A, Houts RM, Belsky DW, Goldman-Mellor SJ, Harrington H, Israel S, et al. The p factor: one general psychopathology factor in the structure of psychiatric disorders? Clin Psychol Sci. (2014) 2:119–37. doi: 10.1177/2167702613497473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Baron-Cohen S. Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. Boston: MIT Press/Bradford Books (1995).

Google Scholar

36. Dadalko OI, Travers BG. Evidence for Brainstem contributions to autism spectrum disorders. Front Integr Neurosci. (2018) 12:47. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2018.00047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Rodier PM. Converging evidence for brain stem injury in autism. Dev Psychopathol. (2002) 14:537–57. doi: 10.1017/S0954579402003085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Baron-Cohen S, Ring HA, Bullmore ET, Wheelwright S, Ashwin C, Williams SC. The amygdala theory of autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2000) 24:355–64. doi: 10.1016/S0149-7634(00)00011-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Mottron L, Dawson M, Soulieres I, Hubert B, Burack J. Enhanced perceptual functioning in autism: an update, and eight principles of autistic perception. J Autism Dev Disord. (2006) 36:27–43. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-0040-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Skuse DH. Rethinking the nature of genetic vulnerability to autistic spectrum disorders. Trends Genet. (2007) 23:387–95. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2007.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Johnson MH. Executive function and developmental disorders: the flip side of the coin. Trends Cogn Sci. (2012) 16:454–7. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Courchesne E. Abnormal early brain development in autism. Mol Psychiatry. (2002) 7(Suppl. 2):S21–3. doi: 10.1038/sj.mp.4001169

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Sebat J, Lakshmi B, Malhotra D, Troge J, Lese-Martin C, Walsh T, et al. Strong association of de novo copy number mutations with autism. Science. (2007) 316:445–9. doi: 10.1126/science.1138659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. De Felice A, Ricceri L, Venerosi A, Chiarotti F, Calamandrei G. Multifactorial origin of neurodevelopmental disorders: approaches to understanding complex etiologies. Toxics. (2015) 3:89–129. doi: 10.3390/toxics3010089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Benevides TW, Lane SJ. A review of cardiac autonomic measures: considerations for examination of physiological response in children with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:560–75. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1971-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Bujnakova I, Ondrejka I, Mestanik M, Visnovcova Z, Mestanikova A, Hrtanek I, et al. Autism spectrum disorder is associated with autonomic underarousal. Physiol Res. (2016) 65(Suppl. 5):S673–S82. doi: 10.33549/physiolres.933528

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Ashwood P, Van de Water J. Is autism an autoimmune disease? Autoimmun Rev. (2004) 3:557–62. doi: 10.1016/j.autrev.2004.07.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ashwood P, Van de Water J. A review of autism and the immune response. Clin Dev Immunol. (2004) 11:165–74. doi: 10.1080/10446670410001722096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Nelson MT, Seal ML, Pantelis C, Phillips LJ. Evidence of a dimensional relationship between schizotypy and schizophrenia: a systematic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2013) 37:317–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Geurts HM, Jansen MD. A retrospective chart study: the pathway to a diagnosis for adults referred for ASD assessment. Autism. (2012) 16:299–305. doi: 10.1177/1362361311421775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bargiela S, Steward R, Mandy W. The experiences of late-diagnosed women with autism spectrum conditions: an investigation of the female autism phenotype. J Autism Dev Disord. (2016) 46:3281–94. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2872-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Landry O, Chouinard PA. Why we should study the broader autism phenotype in typically developing populations. J Cogn Dev. (2016) 17:584–95. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2016.1200046

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Bailey A, Parr J. Implications of the broader phenotype for concepts of autism. Novartis Found Symp. (2003) 251:26–35; discussion 6–47, 109–11, 281–97. doi: 10.1002/0470869380.ch3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Folstein S, Rutter M. Infantile autism: a genetic study of 21 twin pairs. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1977) 18:297–321. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1977.tb00443.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ruparelia K, Manji K, Abubakar A, Newton CR. Investigating the evidence of behavioral, cognitive, and psychiatric endophenotypes in autism: a systematic review. Autism Res Treat. (2017) 2017:6346912. doi: 10.1155/2017/6346912

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Robinson EB, Munir K, Munafo MR, Hughes M, McCormick MC, Koenen KC. Stability of autistic traits in the general population: further evidence for a continuum of impairment. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2011) 50:376–84. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lundstrom S, Chang Z, Rastam M, Gillberg C, Larsson H, Anckarsater H, et al. Autism spectrum disorders and autistic like traits: similar etiology in the extreme end and the normal variation. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2012) 69:46–52. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Woodbury-Smith MR, Robinson J, Wheelwright S, Baron-Cohen S. Screening adults for Asperger Syndrome using the AQ: a preliminary study of its diagnostic validity in clinical practice. J Autism Dev Disord. (2005) 35:331–5. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-3300-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Pisula E, Ziegart-Sadowska K. Broader autism phenotype in siblings of children with ASD–a review. Int J Mol Sci. (2015) 16:13217–58. doi: 10.3390/ijms160613217

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Schwichtenberg AJ, Young GS, Sigman M, Hutman T, Ozonoff S. Can family affectedness inform infant sibling outcomes of autism spectrum disorders? J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2010) 51:1021–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02267.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Gerdts JA, Bernier R, Dawson G, Estes A. The broader autism phenotype in simplex and multiplex families. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:1597–605. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1706-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Maxwell CR, Parish-Morris J, Hsin O, Bush JC, Schultz RT. The broad autism phenotype predicts child functioning in autism spectrum disorders. J Neurodev Disord. (2013) 5:25. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-5-25

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Grove R, Baillie A, Allison C, Baron-Cohen S, Hoekstra RA. The latent structure of cognitive and emotional empathy in individuals with autism, first-degree relatives and typical individuals. Mol Autism. (2014) 5:42. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-5-42

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Bolton P, Macdonald H, Pickles A, Rios P, Goode S, Crowson M, et al. A case-control family history study of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1994) 35:877–900. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1994.tb02300.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Autistic traits in the general population: a twin study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2003) 60:524–30. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.5.524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Sasson NJ, Lam KS, Parlier M, Daniels JL, Piven J. Autism and the broad autism phenotype: familial patterns and intergenerational transmission. J Neurodev Disord. (2013) 5:11. doi: 10.1186/1866-1955-5-11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Shi LJ, Ou JJ, Gong JB, Wang SH, Zhou YY, Zhu FR, et al. Broad autism phenotype features of Chinese parents with autistic children and their associations with severity of social impairment in probands. BMC Psychiatry. (2015) 15:168. doi: 10.1186/s12888-015-0568-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Sandin S, Lichtenstein P, Kuja-Halkola R, Larsson H, Hultman CM, Reichenberg A. The familial risk of autism. JAMA. (2014) 311:1770–7. doi: 10.1001/jama.2014.4144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Constantino JN, Todd RD. Intergenerational transmission of subthreshold autistic traits in the general population. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:655–60. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Losh M, Childress D, Lam K, Piven J. Defining key features of the broad autism phenotype: a comparison across parents of multiple- and single-incidence autism families. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. (2008) 147B:424–33. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Virkud YV, Todd RD, Abbacchi AM, Zhang Y, Constantino JN. Familial aggregation of quantitative autistic traits in multiplex versus simplex autism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. (2009) 150B:328–34. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.30810

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (AQ): evidence from Asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. (2001) 31:5–17. doi: 10.1023/a:1005653411471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Ruzich E, Allison C, Smith P, Watson P, Auyeung B, Ring H, et al. Measuring autistic traits in the general population: a systematic review of the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) in a nonclinical population sample of 6,900 typical adult males and females. Mol Autism. (2015) 6:2. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-6-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hoekstra RA, Bartels M, Verweij CJ, Boomsma DI. Heritability of autistic traits in the general population. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2007) 161:372–7. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.4.372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Holmboe K, Rijsdijk FV, Hallett V, Happe F, Plomin R, Ronald A. Strong genetic influences on the stability of autistic traits in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2014) 53:221–30. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2013.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Whitehouse AJ, Hickey M, Ronald A. Are autistic traits in the general population stable across development? PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e23029. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0023029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin E, Ashwin C, Tavassoli T, Chakrabarti B. Talent in autism: hyper-systemizing, hyperattention to detail and sensory hypersensitivity. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2009) 364:1377–83. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Crespi BJ. Autism as a disorder of high intelligence. Front Neurosci. (2016) 10:300. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00300

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Clarke TK, Lupton MK, Fernandez-Pujals AM, Starr J, Davies G, Cox S, et al. Common polygenic risk for autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is associated with cognitive ability in the general population. Mol Psychiatry. (2016) 21:419–25. doi: 10.1038/mp.2015.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Asperger H. “Autistischen Psychopaten” in Kindsalter. Archiv für Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten. (1944) 117:76–136. doi: 10.1007/BF01837709

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Iacono WG, Vaidyanathan U, Vrieze SI, Malone SM. Knowns and unknowns for psychophysiological endophenotypes: integration and response to commentaries. Psychophysiology. (2014) 51:1339–47. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12358

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Quintana-Murci L. Understanding rare and common diseases in the context of human evolution. Genome Biol. (2016) 17:225. doi: 10.1186/s13059-016-1093-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Boyle EA, Li YI, Pritchard JK. An expanded view of complex traits: from polygenic to omnigenic. Cell. (2017) 169:1177–86. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2017.05.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Schork NJ, Murray SS, Frazer KA, Topol EJ. Common vs. rare allele hypotheses for complex diseases. Curr Opin Genet Dev. (2009) 19:212–9. doi: 10.1016/j.gde.2009.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Pritchard JK, Pickrell JK, Coop G. The genetics of human adaptation: hard sweeps, soft sweeps, and polygenic adaptation. Curr Biol. (2010) 20:R208–15. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2009.11.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Pritchard JK. Are rare variants responsible for susceptibility to complex diseases? Am J Hum Genet. (2001) 69:124–37. doi: 10.1086/321272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Gaugler T, Klei L, Sanders SJ, Bodea CA, Goldberg AP, Lee AB, et al. Most genetic risk for autism resides with common variation. Nat Genet. (2014) 46:881–5. doi: 10.1038/ng.3039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Weiner DJ, Wigdor EM, Ripke S, Walters RK, Kosmicki JA, Grove J, et al. Polygenic transmission disequilibrium confirms that common and rare variation act additively to create risk for autism spectrum disorders. Nat Genet. (2017) 49:978–85. doi: 10.1038/ng.3863

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Niemi MEK, Martin HC, Rice DL, Gallone G, Gordon S, Kelemen M, et al. Common genetic variants contribute to risk of rare severe neurodevelopmental disorders. Nature. (2018) 562:268–71. doi: 10.1038/s41586-018-0566-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Bralten J, van Hulzen KJ, Martens MB, Galesloot TE, Arias Vasquez A, Kiemeney LA, et al. Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits share genetics and biology. Mol Psychiatry. (2018) 23:1205–12. doi: 10.1038/mp.2017.98

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Billeci L, Calderoni S, Conti E, Gesi C, Carmassi C, Dell'Osso L, et al. The broad autism (endo)phenotype: neurostructural and neurofunctional correlates in parents of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Front Neurosci. (2016) 10:346. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2016.00346

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Huguet G, Ey E, Bourgeron T. The genetic landscapes of autism spectrum disorders. Annu Rev Genomics Hum Genet. (2013) 14:191–213. doi: 10.1146/annurev-genom-091212-153431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Bourgeron T. Genetics and epigenetics of autism spectrum disorders. In: Sassone-Corsi P, Christen Y, editors. Epigenetics, Brain and Behavior. Berlin: Springer (2012).

Google Scholar

94. Knickmeyer RC, Baron-Cohen S. Fetal testosterone and sex differences in typical social development and in autism. J Child Neurol. (2006) 21:825–45. doi: 10.1177/08830738060210101601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Gore AC, Martien KM, Gagnidze K, Pfaff D. Implications of prenatal steroid perturbations for neurodevelopment, behavior, and autism. Endocr Rev. (2014) 35:961–91. doi: 10.1210/er.2013-1122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Werling DM, Geschwind DH. Understanding sex bias in autism spectrum disorder. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2013) 110:4868–9. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1301602110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Knickmeyer R, Baron-Cohen S, Fane BA, Wheelwright S, Mathews GA, Conway GS, et al. Androgens and autistic traits: a study of individuals with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. Horm Behav. (2006) 50:148–53. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2006.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Auyeung B, Baron-Cohen S, Ashwin E, Knickmeyer R, Taylor K, Hackett G. Fetal testosterone and autistic traits. Br J Psychol. (2009) 100:1–22. doi: 10.1348/000712608X311731

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Jamnadass ES, Keelan JA, Hollier LP, Hickey M, Maybery MT, Whitehouse AJ. The perinatal androgen to estrogen ratio and autistic-like traits in the general population: a longitudinal pregnancy cohort study. J Neurodev Disord. (2015) 7:17. doi: 10.1186/s11689-015-9114-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Constantino JN, Gruber CP. Social Responsiveness Scale: Manual. Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services (2005).

Google Scholar

101. Vig S, Jedrysek E. Autistic features in young children with significant cognitive impairment: autism or mental retardation? J Autism Dev Disord. (1999) 29:235–48. doi: 10.1023/A:1023084106559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Matson JL, Shoemaker M. Intellectual disability and its relationship to autism spectrum disorders. Res Dev Disabil. (2009) 30:1107–14. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2009.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. La Malfa G, Lassi S, Bertelli M, Salvini R, Placidi GF. Autism and intellectual disability: a study of prevalence on a sample of the Italian population. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2004) 48:262–7. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2003.00567.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Wang Y, Zhang YB, Liu LL, Cui JF, Wang J, Shum DH, et al. A meta-analysis of working memory impairments in autism spectrum disorders. Neuropsychol Rev. (2017) 27:46–61. doi: 10.1007/s11065-016-9336-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Russo N, Flanagan T, Iarocci G, Berringer D, Zelazo PD, Burack JA. Deconstructing executive deficits among persons with autism: implications for cognitive neuroscience. Brain Cogn. (2007) 65:77–86. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2006.04.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Saemundsen E, Juliusson H, Hjaltested S, Gunnarsdottir T, Halldorsdottir T, Hreidarsson S, et al. Prevalence of autism in an urban population of adults with severe intellectual disabilities–a preliminary study. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2010) 54:727–35. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2010.01300.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Wing L. Language, social, and cognitive impairments in autism and severe mental retardation. J Autism Dev Disord. (1981) 11:31–44. doi: 10.1007/BF01531339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Kercood S, Grskovic JA, Banda D, Begeske J. Working memory and autism: a review of literature. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2014) 8:1316–32. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2014.06.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Sallows GO, Graupner TD. Intensive behavioral treatment for children with autism: four-year outcome and predictors. Am J Ment Retard. (2005) 110:417–38. doi: 10.1352/0895-8017(2005)110[417:IBTFCW]2.0.CO;2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Eapen V, Crncec R, Walter A. Exploring links between genotypes, phenotypes, and clinical predictors of response to early intensive behavioral intervention in autism spectrum disorder. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:567. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Fein D, Stevens M, Dunn M, Waterhouse L, Allen D, Rapin I, et al. Subtypes of pervasive developmental disorder: clinical characteristics. Child Neuropsychol. (1999) 5:1–23. doi: 10.1076/chin.5.1.1.7075

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Troyb E, Rosenthal M, Eigsti IM, Kelley E, Tyson K, Orinstein A, et al. Executive functioning in individuals with a history of ASDs who have achieved optimal outcomes. Child Neuropsychol. (2014) 20:378–97. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2013.799644

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Helles A, Gillberg CI, Gillberg C, Billstedt E. Asperger syndrome in males over two decades: stability and predictors of diagnosis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2015) 56:711–8. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12334

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Helt M, Kelley E, Kinsbourne M, Pandey J, Boorstein H, Herbert M, et al. Can children with autism recover? If so, how? Neuropsychol Rev. (2008) 18:339–66. doi: 10.1007/s11065-008-9075-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M. Adult outcome for children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2004) 45:212–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Pugliese CE, Anthony L, Strang JF, Dudley K, Wallace GL, Kenworthy L. Increasing adaptive behavior skill deficits from childhood to adolescence in autism spectrum disorder: role of executive function. J Autism Dev Disord. (2015) 45:1579–87. doi: 10.1007/s10803-014-2309-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Pugliese CE, Anthony LG, Strang JF, Dudley K, Wallace GL, Naiman DQ, et al. Longitudinal examination of adaptive behavior in autism spectrum disorders: influence of executive function. J Autism Dev Disord. (2016) 46:467–77. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2584-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Leung RC, Vogan VM, Powell TL, Anagnostou E, Taylor MJ. The role of executive functions in social impairment in autism spectrum disorder. Child Neuropsychol. (2016) 22:336–44. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2015.1005066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Klusek J, Martin GE, Losh M. Consistency between research and clinical diagnoses of autism among boys and girls with fragile X syndrome. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2014) 58:940–52. doi: 10.1111/jir.12121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Bartak L, Rutter M. Differences between mentally retarded and normally intelligent autistic children. J Autism Child Schizophr. (1976) 6:109–20. doi: 10.1007/BF01538054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Hughes C, Russell J, Robbins TW. Evidence for executive dysfunction in autism. Neuropsychologia. (1994) 32:477–92. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(94)90092-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Hill EL. Executive dysfunction in autism. Trends Cogn Sci. (2004) 8:26–32. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2003.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Hill EL. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Developmental Review. (2004) 24:189–233. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. O'Hearn K, Asato M, Ordaz S, Luna B. Neurodevelopment and executive function in autism. Dev Psychopathol. (2008) 20:1103–32. doi: 10.1017/S0954579408000527

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Craig F, Margari F, Legrottaglie AR, Palumbi R, de Giambattista C, Margari L, et al. review of executive function deficits in autism spectrum disorder and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2016) 12:1191–202. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S104620

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Robinson S, Goddard L, Dritschel B, Wisley M, Howlin P. Executive functions in children with autism spectrum disorders. Brain Cogn. (2009) 71:362–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2009.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Gilotty L, Kenworthy L, Sirian L, Black DO, Wagner AE. Adaptive skills and executive function in autism spectrum disorders. Child Neuropsychol. (2002) 8:241–8. doi: 10.1076/chin.8.4.241.13504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Sun J, Buys N. Early executive function deficit in preterm children and its association with neurodevelopmental disorders in childhood: a literature review. Int J Adolesc Med Health. (2012) 24:291–9. doi: 10.1515/ijamh.2012.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Livingston LA, Happe F. Conceptualising compensation in neurodevelopmental disorders: reflections from autism spectrum disorder. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 80:729–42. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.06.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Fournier KA, Hass CJ, Naik SK, Lodha N, Cauraugh JH. Motor coordination in autism spectrum disorders: a synthesis and meta-analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. (2010) 40:1227–40. doi: 10.1007/s10803-010-0981-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. MacDonald M, Lord C, Ulrich DA. Motor skills and calibrated autism severity in young children with autism spectrum disorder. Adapt Phys Activ Q. (2014) 31:95–105. doi: 10.1123/apaq.2013-0068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

132. Hilton CL, Zhang Y, Whilte MR, Klohr CL, Constantino J. Motor impairment in sibling pairs concordant and discordant for autism spectrum disorders. Autism. (2012) 16:430–41. doi: 10.1177/1362361311423018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Choi B, Leech KA, Tager-Flusberg H, Nelson CA. Development of fine motor skills is associated with expressive language outcomes in infants at high and low risk for autism spectrum disorder. J Neurodev Disord. (2018) 10:14. doi: 10.1186/s11689-018-9231-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

134. Smits-Engelsman B, Hill EL. The relationship between motor coordination and intelligence across the IQ range. Pediatrics. (2012) 130:e950–6. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-3712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

135. Ghaziuddin M, Butler E. Clumsiness in autism and Asperger syndrome: a further report. J Intellect Disabil Res. (1998) 42:43–8. doi: 10.1046/j.1365-2788.1998.00065.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

136. Crick NR, Dodge KA. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children's social adjustment. Psychol Bull. (1994) 115:74–101. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

137. Cunningham WA, Zelazo PD. Attitudes and evaluations: a social cognitive neuroscience perspective. Trends Cogn Sci. (2007) 11:97–104. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

138. Kaiser MD, Hudac CM, Shultz S, Lee SM, Cheung C, Berken AM, et al. Neural signatures of autism. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2010) 107:21223–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010412107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

139. Nelson EE, Guyer AE. The development of the ventral prefrontal cortex and social flexibility. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2011) 1:233–45. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

140. Plomin R, Deary IJ. Genetics and intelligence differences: five special findings. Mol Psychiatry. (2015) 20:98–108. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

141. Deary IJ, Johnson W, Houlihan LM. Genetic foundations of human intelligence. Hum Genet. (2009) 126:215–32. doi: 10.1007/s00439-009-0655-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

142. Friedman NP, Miyake A. Unity and diversity of executive functions: individual differences as a window on cognitive structure. Cortex. (2017) 86:186–204. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.04.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

143. Cirulli ET, Kasperaviciute D, Attix DK, Need AC, Ge D, Gibson G, et al. Common genetic variation and performance on standardized cognitive tests. Eur J Hum Genet. (2010) 18:815–20. doi: 10.1038/ejhg.2010.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

144. August JG, Stewart MA, Tsai L. The incidence of cognitive disabilities in the siblings of autistic children. Brit J Psychiat. (1981) 138:416–22. doi: 10.1192/bjp.138.5.416

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

145. Hughes C, Leboyer M, Bouvard M. Executive function in parents of children with autism. Psychol Med. (1997) 27:209–20. doi: 10.1017/S0033291796004308

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

146. Hughes C, Plumet MH, Leboyer M. Towards a cognitive phenotype for autism: increased prevalence of executive dysfunction and superior spatial span amongst siblings of children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1999) 40:705–18. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00487

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

147. Wong D, Maybery M, Bishop DV, Maley A, Hallmayer J. Profiles of executive function in parents and siblings of individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Genes Brain Behav. (2006) 5:561–76. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2005.00199.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

148. McLean RL, Johnson Harrison A, Zimak E, Joseph RM, Morrow EM. Executive function in probands with autism with average IQ and their unaffected first-degree relatives. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2014) 53:1001–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2014.05.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

149. Hoekstra RA, Happe F, Baron-Cohen S, Ronald A. Association between extreme autistic traits and intellectual disability: insights from a general population twin study. Br J Psychiatry. (2009) 195:531–6. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.060889

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

150. Hoekstra RA, Happe F, Baron-Cohen S, Ronald A. Limited genetic covariance between autistic traits and intelligence: findings from a longitudinal twin study. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. (2010) 153B:994–1007. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.31066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

151. Rommelse N, Langerak I, van der Meer J, de Bruijn Y, Staal W, Oerlemans A, et al. Intelligence may moderate the cognitive profile of patients with ASD. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0138698. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0138698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

152. Kendler KS, Ohlsson H, Sundquist J, Sundquist K. IQ and schizophrenia in a Swedish national sample: their causal relationship and the interaction of IQ with genetic risk. Am J Psychiatry. (2015) 172:259–65. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2014.14040516

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

153. Viscidi EW, Johnson AL, Spence SJ, Buka SL, Morrow EM, Triche EW. The association between epilepsy and autism symptoms and maladaptive behaviors in children with autism spectrum disorder. Autism. (2014) 18:996–1006. doi: 10.1177/1362361313508027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

154. Stefansson H, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Steinberg S, Magnusdottir B, Morgen K, Arnarsdottir S, et al. CNVs conferring risk of autism or schizophrenia affect cognition in controls. Nature. (2014) 505:361–6. doi: 10.1038/nature12818

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

155. Fein D, Barton M, Eigsti IM, Kelley E, Naigles L, Schultz RT, et al. Optimal outcome in individuals with a history of autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2013) 54:195–205. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

156. Riggio RE, Messamer J, Throckmorton B. Social and academic intelligence: conceptually distinct but overlapping constructs. Person Individ Diff. (1991) 12:695–702. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(91)90225-Z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

157. Lee J, Wong CT, Day JD, Maxwell SE, Thorpe P. Social and academic intelligences: a multitrait-multimethod study of their crystallized and fluid characteristics. Person Individ Diff. (2000) 29:539–53. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(99)00213-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

158. Barnes ML, Sternberg RJ. Social intelligence and decoding nonverbal cues. Intelligence. (1989) 13:263–87. doi: 10.1016/0160-2896(89)90022-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

159. Silvera DH, Martinussen M, Dahl TI. The Tromso social intelligence scale, a self report measure of social intelligence. Scand J Psychol. (2001) 42:313–9. doi: 10.1111/1467-9450.00242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

160. Weis S, Süss H. Reviving the search for social intelligence - a multitrait-multimethod study of its structure and construct validity. Person Individ Diff. (2007) 42:3–14. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2006.04.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

161. Christov-Moore L, Simpson EA, Coude G, Grigaityte K, Iacoboni M, Ferrari PF. Empathy: gender effects in brain and behavior. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2014) 46:604–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

162. Kret ME, De Gelder B. A review on sex differences in processing emotional signals. Neuropsychologia. (2012) 50:1211–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.12.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

163. Hull L, Mandy W, Petrides KV. Behavioral and cognitive sex/gender differences in autism spectrum condition and typically developing maes and females. Autism. (2017) 21:706–27. doi: 10.1177/1362361316669087

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

164. Warrier V, Toro R, Won H, Leblond CS, Cliquet F, Delorme R, et al. Social and non-social autism symptoms and trait domains are genetically dissociable. Commun Biol. (2019) 2:328. doi: 10.1038/s42003-019-0558-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

165. Gillberg C, Cederlund M, Lamberg K, Zeijlon L. Brief report: “the autism epidemic”. The registered prevalence of autism in a Swedish urban area. J Autism Dev Disord. (2006) 36:429–35. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0081-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

166. Polyak A, Rosenfeld JA, Girirajan S. An assessment of sex bias in neurodevelopmental disorders. Genome Med. (2015) 7:94. doi: 10.1186/s13073-015-0216-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

167. Robinson EB, Lichtenstein P, Anckarsater H, Happe F, Ronald A. Examining and interpreting the female protective effect against autistic behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2013) 110:5258–62. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1211070110

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

168. Rivet TT, Matson JL. Review of gender differences in core symptomatology in autism spectrum disorders. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2011) 5:957–76. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

169. Van Wijngaarden-Cremers PJ, van Eeten E, Groen WB, Van Deurzen PA, Oosterling IJ, Van der Gaag RJ. Gender and age differences in the core triad of impairments in autism spectrum disorders: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Autism Dev Disord. (2014) 44:627–35. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1913-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

170. Dworzynski K, Ronald A, Bolton P, Happe F. How different are girls and boys above and below the diagnostic threshold for autism spectrum disorders? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2012) 51:788–97. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2012.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

171. Belcher H, Stagg SD, Ford R editors. The hidden disorder: undiagnosed autism spectrum disorder in women. In: International Society for Autism Research International Meeting. Baltimore, MD (2016).

172. Begeer S, Mandell D, Wijnker-Holmes B, Venderbosch S, Rem D, Stekelenburg F, et al. Sex differences in the timing of identification among children and adults with autism spectrum disorders. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:1151–6. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1656-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

173. Hall SS, Lightbody AA, Hirt M, Rezvani A, Reiss AL. Autism in fragile X syndrome: a category mistake? J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2010) 49:921–33. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2010.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

174. Woolfenden S, Sarkozy V, Ridley G, Williams KA. systematic review of the diagnostic stability of autism spectrum disorder. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2012) 6:345–54. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2011.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

175. Kocovska E, Billstedt E, Ellefsen A, Kampmann H, Gillberg IC, Biskupsto R, et al. Autism in the Faroe Islands: diagnostic stability from childhood to early adult life. Sci World J. (2013) 2013:592371. doi: 10.1155/2013/592371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

176. Rossignol DA, Frye RE. A review of research trends in physiological abnormalities in autism spectrum disorders: immune dysregulation, inflammation, oxidative stress, mitochondrial dysfunction and environmental toxicant exposures. Mol Psychiatry. (2012) 17:389–401. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.165

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

177. Bolte S, Girdler S, Marschik PB. The contribution of environmental exposure to the etiology of autism spectrum disorder. Cell Mol Life Sci. (2019) 76:1275–97. doi: 10.1007/s00018-018-2988-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

178. Goines PE, Ashwood P. Cytokine dysregulation in autism spectrum disorders (ASD): possible role of the environment. Neurotoxicol Teratol. (2013) 36:67–81. doi: 10.1016/j.ntt.2012.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

179. Worsham W, Dalton S, Bilder DA. The prenatal hormone milieu in autism spectrum disorder. Front Psychiatry. (2021) 12:655438. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.655438

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

180. Volk HE, Lurmann F, Penfold B, Hertz-Picciotto I, McConnell R. Traffic-related air pollution, particulate matter, and autism. JAMA Psychiatry. (2013) 70:71–7. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2013.266

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

181. Fernell E, Bejerot S, Westerlund J, Miniscalco C, Simila H, Eyles D, et al. Autism spectrum disorder and low vitamin D at birth: a sibling control study. Mol Autism. (2015) 6:3. doi: 10.1186/2040-2392-6-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

182. Stromland K, Nordin V, Miller M, Akerstrom B, Gillberg C. Autism in thalidomide embryopathy: a population study. Dev Med Child Neurol. (1994) 36:351–6. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1994.tb11856.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

183. Man KK, Tong HH, Wong LY, Chan EW, Simonoff E, Wong IC. Exposure to selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors during pregnancy and risk of autism spectrum disorder in children: a systematic review and meta-analysis of observational studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2015) 49:82–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.11.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

184. Newschaffer CJ, Croen LA, Daniels J, Giarelli E, Grether JK, Levy SE, et al. The epidemiology of autism spectrum disorders. Annu Rev Public Health. (2007) 28:235–58. doi: 10.1146/annurev.publhealth.28.021406.144007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

185. Torres AR, Westover JB, Rosenspire AJ. HLA immune function genes in autism. Autism Res Treat. (2012) 2012:959073. doi: 10.1155/2012/959073

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

186. Gesundheit B, Rosenzweig JP, Naor D, Lerer B, Zachor DA, Prochazka V, et al. Immunological and autoimmune considerations of autism spectrum disorders. J Autoimmun. (2013) 44:1–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jaut.2013.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

187. Class QA, Abel KM, Khashan AS, Rickert ME, Dalman C, Larsson H, et al. Offspring psychopathology following preconception, prenatal and postnatal maternal bereavement stress. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:71–84. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000780

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

188. Kinney DK, Munir KM, Crowley DJ, Miller AM. Prenatal stress and risk for autism. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2008) 32:1519–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

189. Kolevzon A, Gross R, Reichenberg A. Prenatal and perinatal risk factors for autism: a review and integration of findings. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2007) 161:326–33. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.4.326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

190. Gardener H, Spiegelman D, Buka SL. Prenatal risk factors for autism: comprehensive meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. (2009) 195:7–14. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.108.051672

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

191. Glasson EJ, Bower C, Petterson B, de Klerk N, Chaney G, Hallmayer JF. Perinatal factors and the development of autism: a population study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2004) 61:618–27. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.61.6.618

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

192. Dawes P, Cruickshanks KJ, Moore DR, Fortnum H, Edmonson-Jones M, McCormack A, et al. The effect of prenatal and childhood development on hearing, vision and cognition in adulthood. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0136590. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136590

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

193. Anderson V, Spencer-Smith M, Wood A. Do children really recover better? Neurobehavioral plasticity after early brain insult. Brain. (2011) 134:2197–221. doi: 10.1093/brain/awr103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

194. Anderson V, Spencer-Smith M, Coleman L, Anderson P, Williams J, Greenham M, et al. Children's executive functions: are they poorer after very early brain insult. Neuropsychologia. (2010) 48:2041–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.03.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

195. Anderson V, Spencer-Smith M, Leventer R, Coleman L, Anderson P, Williams J, et al. Childhood brain insult: can age at insult help us predict outcome? Brain. (2009) 132:45–56. doi: 10.1093/brain/awn293

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

196. Piper BJ, Corbett SM. Executive function profile in the offspring of women that smoked during pregnancy. Nicotine Tob Res. (2012) 14:191–9. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr181

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

197. Harris MH, Gold DR, Rifas-Shiman SL, Melly SJ, Zanobetti A, Coull BA, et al. Prenatal and childhood traffic-related pollution exposure and childhood cognition in the project viva cohort (Massachusetts, USA). Environ Health Perspect. (2015) 123:1072–8. doi: 10.1289/ehp.1408803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

198. Venkatramanan S, Armata IE, Strupp BJ, Finkelstein JL. Vitamin B-12 and cognition in children. Adv Nutr. (2016) 7:879–88. doi: 10.3945/an.115.012021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

199. Liew Z, Bach CC, Asarnow RF, Ritz B, Olsen J. Paracetamol use during pregnancy and attention and executive function in offspring at age 5 years. Int J Epidemiol. (2016) 45:2009–17. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyw296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

200. Cordeiro CN, Tsimis M, Burd I. Infections and brain development. Obstet Gynecol Surv. (2015) 70:644–55. doi: 10.1097/OGX.0000000000000236

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

201. Kaur J, Singhi P, Singhi S, Malhi P, Saini AG. Neurodevelopmental and behavioral outcomes in children with sepsis-associated encephalopathy admitted to pediatric intensive care unit: a prospective case control study. J Child Neurol. (2016) 31:683–90. doi: 10.1177/0883073815610431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

202. Barichello T, Sayana P, Giridharan VV, Arumanayagam AS, Narendran B, Della Giustina A, et al. Long-term cognitive outcomes after sepsis: a translational systematic review. Mol Neurobiol. (2019) 56:186–251. doi: 10.1007/s12035-018-1048-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

203. Ross G, Sammaritano L, Nass R, Lockshin M. Effects of mothers' autoimmune disease during pregnancy on learning disabilities and hand preference in their children. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2003) 157:397–402. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.157.4.397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

204. Virk J, Obel C, Li J, Olsen J. In-utero exposure to bereavement and offspring IQ: a Danish national cohort study. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e88477. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088477

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

205. Buss C, Davis EP, Hobel CJ, Sandman CA. Maternal pregnancy-specific anxiety is associated with child executive function at 6-9 years age. Stress. (2011) 14:665–76. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2011.623250

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

206. Kavanaugh BC, Dupont-Frechette JA, Jerskey BA, Holler KA. Neurocognitive deficits in children and adolescents following maltreatment: neurodevelopmental consequences and neuropsychological implications of traumatic stress. Appl Neuropsychol Child. (2017) 6:64–78. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2015.1079712

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

207. Ansell EB, Rando K, Tuit K, Guarnaccia J, Sinha R. Cumulative adversity and smaller gray matter volume in medial prefrontal, anterior cingulate, and insula regions. Biol Psychiatry. (2012) 72:57–64. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.11.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

208. McEwen BS, Morrison JH. The brain on stress: vulnerability and plasticity of the prefrontal cortex over the life course. Neuron. (2013) 79:16–29. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.06.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

209. Allotey J, Zamora J, Cheong-See F, Kalidindi M, Arroyo-Manzano D, Asztalos E, et al. Cognitive, motor, behavioural and academic performances of children born preterm: a meta-analysis and systematic review involving 64 061 children. BJOG. (2018) 125:16–25. doi: 10.1111/1471-0528.14832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

210. Figueiro-Filho EA, Mak LE, Reynolds JN, Stroman PW, Smith GN, Forkert ND, et al. Neurological function in children born to preeclamptic and hypertensive mothers - a systematic review. Pregnancy Hypertens. (2017) 10:1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.preghy.2017.07.144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

211. Seidman LJ, Buka SL, Goldstein JM, Horton NJ, Rieder RO, Tsuang MT. The relationship of prenatal and perinatal complications to cognitive functioning at age 7 in the New England Cohorts of the National Collaborative Perinatal Project. Schizophr Bull. (2000) 26:309–21. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a033455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

212. Adane AA, Mishra GD, Tooth LR. Diabetes in pregnancy and childhood cognitive development: a systematic review. Pediatrics. (2016) 137:e20154234. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-4234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

213. Heffernan AL, Hare DJ. Tracing environmental exposure from neurodevelopment to neurodegeneration. Trends Neurosci. (2018) 41:496–501. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2018.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

214. Renz H, Holt PG, Inouye M, Logan AC, Prescott SL, Sly PD. An exposome perspective: Early-life events and immune development in a changing world. J Allergy Clin Immunol. (2017) 140:24–40. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2017.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

215. Kesler SR, Ogg R, Reddick WE, Phillips N, Scoggins M, Glass JO, et al. Brain network connectivity and executive function in long-term survivors of childhood acute lymphoblastic leukemia. Brain Connect. (2018) 8:333–42. doi: 10.1089/brain.2017.0574

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

216. Paz-Alonso PM, Bunge SA, Ghetti S. Emergence of Higher Cognitive Functions: Reorganization of Large-Scale Brain Networks During Childhood and Adolescence. Oxford Handbooks (2014). p. 28

Google Scholar

217. Bortagarai FM, Moraes AB, Pichini FDS, Souza APR. Risk factors for fine and gross motor development in preterm and term infants. Codas. (2021) 33:e20200254. doi: 10.1590/2317-1782/20202020254

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

218. Ghassabian A, Sundaram R, Wylie A, Bell E, Bello SC, Yeung E. Maternal medical conditions during pregnancy and gross motor development up to age 24 months in the upstate KIDS study. Dev Med Child Neurol. (2016) 58:728–34. doi: 10.1111/dmcn.12947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

219. Klupp S, Mohring W, Lemola S, Grob A. Relations between fine motor skills and intelligence in typically developing children and children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Res Dev Disabil. (2021) 110:103855. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2021.103855

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

220. Corcoles-Parada M, Gimenez-Mateo R, Serrano-Del-Pueblo V, Lopez L, Perez-Hernandez E, Mansilla F, et al. Born too early and too small: higher order cognitive function and brain at risk at ages 8-16. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:1942. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

221. Westmacott R, MacGregor D, Askalan R, deVeber G. Late emergence of cognitive deficits after unilateral neonatal stroke. Stroke. (2009) 40:2012–9. doi: 10.1161/STROKEAHA.108.533976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

222. Iakoucheva LM, Muotri AR, Sebat J. Getting to the cores of autism. Cell. (2019) 178:1287–98. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.07.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

223. Ronemus M, Iossifov I, Levy D, Wigler M. The role of de novo mutations in the genetics of autism spectrum disorders. Nat Rev Genet. (2014) 15:133–41. doi: 10.1038/nrg3585

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

224. Girirajan S, Rosenfeld JA, Coe BP, Parikh S, Friedman N, Goldstein A, et al. Phenotypic heterogeneity of genomic disorders and rare copy-number variants. N Engl J Med. (2012) 367:1321–31. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1200395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

225. Cook EH Jr, Scherer SW. Copy-number variations associated with neuropsychiatric conditions. Nature. (2008) 455:919–23. doi: 10.1038/nature07458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

226. Rylaarsdam L, Guemez-Gamboa A. Genetic causes and modifiers of autism spectrum disorder. Front Cell Neurosci. (2019) 13:385. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2019.00385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

227. Levy D, Ronemus M, Yamrom B, Lee YH, Leotta A, Kendall J, et al. Rare de novo and transmitted copy-number variation in autistic spectrum disorders. Neuron. (2011) 70:886–97. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

228. Iossifov I, O'Roak BJ, Sanders SJ, Ronemus M, Krumm N, Levy D, et al. The contribution of de novo coding mutations to autism spectrum disorder. Nature. (2014) 515:216–21. doi: 10.1038/nature13908

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

229. Marshall CR, Noor A, Vincent JB, Lionel AC, Feuk L, Skaug J, et al. Structural variation of chromosomes in autism spectrum disorder. Am J Hum Genet. (2008) 82:477–88. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.12.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

230. Moreno-De-Luca A, Evans DW, Boomer KB, Hanson E, Bernier R, Goin-Kochel RP, et al. The role of parental cognitive, behavioral, and motor profiles in clinical variability in individuals with chromosome 16p112 deletions. JAMA Psychiatry. (2015) 72:119–26. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2014.2147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

231. Finucane B, Challman TD, Martin CL, Ledbetter DH. Shift happens: family background influences clinical variability in genetic neurodevelopmental disorders. Genet Med. (2016) 18:302–4. doi: 10.1038/gim.2015.92

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

232. Nozawa M, Kawahara Y, Nei M. Genomic drift and copy number variation of sensory receptor genes in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2007) 104:20421–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0709956104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

233. Jakobsson M, Scholz SW, Scheet P, Gibbs JR, VanLiere JM, Fung HC, et al. Genotype, haplotype and copy-number variation in worldwide human populations. Nature. (2008) 451:998–1003. doi: 10.1038/nature06742

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

234. Moss J, Howlin P. Autism spectrum disorders in genetic syndromes: implications for diagnosis, intervention and understanding the wider autism spectrum disorder population. J Intellect Disabil Res. (2009) 53:852–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2009.01197.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

235. Lowenthal R, Paula CS, Schwartzman JS, Brunoni D, Mercadante MT. Prevalence of pervasive developmental disorder in Down's syndrome. J Autism Dev Disord. (2007) 37:1394–5. doi: 10.1007/s10803-007-0374-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

236. Davies RW, Fiksinski AM, Breetvelt EJ, Williams NM, Hooper SR, Monfeuga T, et al. Using common genetic variation to examine phenotypic expression and risk prediction in 22q112 deletion syndrome. Nat Med. (2020) 26:1912–8. doi: 10.1038/s41591-020-1103-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

237. Hall L, Kelley E. The contribution of epigenetics to understanding genetic factors in autism. Autism. (2014) 18:872–81. doi: 10.1177/1362361313503501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

238. Moss J, Richards C, Nelson L, Oliver C. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder symptomatology and related behavioural characteristics in individuals with Down syndrome. Autism. (2013) 17:390–404. doi: 10.1177/1362361312442790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

239. Abu-Akel AM, Wood SJ, Hansen PC, Apperly IA. Perspective-taking abilities in the balance between autism tendencies and psychosis proneness. Proc Biol Sci. (2015) 282:20150563. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2015.0563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

240. Vorstman JA, Anney RJ, Derks EM, Gallagher L, Gill M, de Jonge MV, et al. No evidence that common genetic risk variation is shared between schizophrenia and autism. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. (2013) 162B:55–60. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

241. McCarthy SE, Gillis J, Kramer M, Lihm J, Yoon S, Berstein Y, et al. De novo mutations in schizophrenia implicate chromatin remodeling and support a genetic overlap with autism and intellectual disability. Mol Psychiatry. (2014) 19:652–8. doi: 10.1038/mp.2014.29

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

242. Jacquemont S, Coe BP, Hersch M, Duyzend MH, Krumm N, Bergmann S, et al. A higher mutational burden in females supports a “female protective model” in neurodevelopmental disorders. Am J Hum Genet. (2014) 94:415–25. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

243. Van Den Bossche MJ, Johnstone M, Strazisar M, Pickard BS, Goossens D, Lenaerts AS, et al. Rare copy number variants in neuropsychiatric disorders: specific phenotype or not? Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. (2012) 159B:812–22. doi: 10.1002/ajmg.b.32088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

244. Bulik-Sullivan B, Finucane HK, Anttila V, Gusev A, Day FR, Loh PR, et al. An atlas of genetic correlations across human diseases and traits. Nat Genet. (2015) 47:1236–41. doi: 10.1038/ng.3406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

245. Tremblay MW, Jiang YHDNA. Methylation and susceptibility to autism spectrum disorder. Annu Rev Med. (2019) 70:151–66. doi: 10.1146/annurev-med-120417-091431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

246. Li J, Lin X, Wang M, Hu Y, Xue K, Gu S, et al. Potential role of genomic imprinted genes and brain developmental related genes in autism. BMC Med Genomics. (2020) 13:54. doi: 10.1186/s12920-020-0693-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

247. Pinto D, Delaby E, Merico D, Barbosa M, Merikangas A, Klei L, et al. Convergence of genes and cellular pathways dysregulated in autism spectrum disorders. Am J Hum Genet. (2014) 94:677–94. doi: 10.1016/j.ajhg.2014.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

248. Satterstrom FK, Kosmicki JA, Wang J, Breen MS, De Rubeis S, An JY, et al. Large-scale exome sequencing study implicates both developmental and functional changes in the neurobiology of autism. Cell. (2020) 180:568–84.e23. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2019.12.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

249. Mead J, Ashwood P. Evidence supporting an altered immune response in ASD. Immunol Lett. (2015) 163:49–55. doi: 10.1016/j.imlet.2014.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

250. Hughes HK, Mills Ko E, Rose D, Ashwood P. Immune dysfunction and autoimmunity as pathological mechanisms in autism spectrum disorders. Front Cell Neurosci. (2018) 12:405. doi: 10.3389/fncel.2018.00405

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

251. Jiang NM, Cowan M, Moonah SN, Petri WA Jr. The impact of systemic inflammation on neurodevelopment. Trends Mol Med. (2018) 24:794–804. doi: 10.1016/j.molmed.2018.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

252. Needleman LA, McAllister AK. The major histocompatibility complex and autism spectrum disorder. Dev Neurobiol. (2012) 72:1288–301. doi: 10.1002/dneu.22046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

253. Stubbs EG, Ritvo ER, Mason-Brothers A. Autism and shared parental HLA antigens. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry. (1985) 24:182–5. doi: 10.1016/S0002-7138(09)60445-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

254. Libbey JE, Sweeten TL, McMahon WM, Fujinami RS. Autistic disorder and viral infections. J Neurovirol. (2005) 11:1–10. doi: 10.1080/13550280590900553

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

255. Meltzer A, Van de Water J. The role of the immune system in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2017) 42:284–98. doi: 10.1038/npp.2016.158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

256. Zerbo O, Iosif AM, Walker C, Ozonoff S, Hansen RL, Hertz-Picciotto I. Is maternal influenza or fever during pregnancy associated with autism or developmental delays? Results from the CHARGE (childhood autism risks from genetics and environment) study. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:25–33. doi: 10.1007/s10803-012-1540-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

257. Chen SW, Zhong XS, Jiang LN, Zheng XY, Xiong YQ, Ma SJ, et al. Maternal autoimmune diseases and the risk of autism spectrum disorders in offspring: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Behav Brain Res. (2016) 296:61–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2015.08.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

258. Edmiston E, Ashwood P, Van de Water J. Autoimmunity, autoantibodies, and autism spectrum disorder. Biol Psychiatry. (2017) 81:383–90. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2016.08.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

259. Croen LA, Qian Y, Ashwood P, Daniels JL, Fallin D, Schendel D, et al. Family history of immune conditions and autism spectrum and developmental disorders: findings from the study to explore early development. Autism Res. (2019) 12:123–35. doi: 10.1002/aur.1979

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

260. Pardo CA, Vargas DL, Zimmerman AW. Immunity, neuroglia and neuroinflammation in autism. Int Rev Psychiatry. (2005) 17:485–95. doi: 10.1080/02646830500381930

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

261. Bjorklund G, Saad K, Chirumbolo S, Kern JK, Geier DA, Geier MR, et al. Immune dysfunction and neuroinflammation in autism spectrum disorder. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars). (2016) 76:257–68. doi: 10.21307/ane-2017-025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

262. McAfoose J, Baune BT. Evidence for a cytokine model of cognitive function. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2009) 33:355–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

263. Turnbull AV, Rivier C. Regulation of the HPA axis by cytokines. Brain Behav Immun. (1995) 9:253–75. doi: 10.1006/brbi.1995.1026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

264. Jones KL, Croen LA, Yoshida CK, Heuer L, Hansen R, Zerbo O, et al. Autism with intellectual disability is associated with increased levels of maternal cytokines and chemokines during gestation. Mol Psychiatry. (2017) 22:273–9. doi: 10.1038/mp.2016.77

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

265. Piras IS, Haapanen L, Napolioni V, Sacco R, Van de Water J, Persico AM. Anti-brain antibodies are associated with more severe cognitive and behavioral profiles in Italian children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Brain Behav Immun. (2014) 38:91–9. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.12.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

266. Estes ML, McAllister AK. Maternal immune activation: implications for neuropsychiatric disorders. Science. (2016) 353:772–7. doi: 10.1126/science.aag3194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

267. Porges SW. The polyvagal theory: phylogenetic contributions to social behavior. Physiol Behav. (2003) 79:503–13. doi: 10.1016/S0031-9384(03)00156-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

268. Kemp AH, Quintana DS. The relationship between mental and physical health: insights from the study of heart rate variability. Int J Psychophysiol. (2013) 89:288–96. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

269. Henry BL, Minassian A, Paulus MP, Geyer MA, Perry W. Heart rate variability in bipolar mania and schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res. (2010) 44:168–76. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

270. Montaquila JM, Trachik BJ, Bedwell JS. Heart rate variability and vagal tone in schizophrenia: a review. J Psychiatr Res. (2015) 69:57–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.07.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

271. Sgoifo A, Carnevali L, Alfonso Mde L, Amore M. Autonomic dysfunction and heart rate variability in depression. Stress. (2015) 18:343–52. doi: 10.3109/10253890.2015.1045868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

272. Klusek J, Roberts JE, Losh M. Cardiac autonomic regulation in autism and Fragile X syndrome: a review. Psychol Bull. (2015) 141:141–75. doi: 10.1037/a0038237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

273. Taylor JL, Corbett BA. A review of rhythm and responsiveness of cortisol in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2014) 49:207–28. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2014.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

274. Thayer JF, Sternberg E. Beyond heart rate variability: vagal regulation of allostatic systems. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2006) 1088:361–72. doi: 10.1196/annals.1366.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

275. Tonhajzerova I, Mokra D, Visnovcova Z. Vagal function indexed by respiratory sinus arrhythmia and cholinergic anti-inflammatory pathway. Respir Physiol Neurobiol. (2013) 187:78–81. doi: 10.1016/j.resp.2013.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

276. Quintana DS, Guastella AJ, Outhred T, Hickie IB, Kemp AH. Heart rate variability is associated with emotion recognition: direct evidence for a relationship between the autonomic nervous system and social cognition. Int J Psychophysiol. (2012) 86:168–72. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2012.08.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

277. Gao Y, Borlam D, Zhang W. The association between heart rate reactivity and fluid intelligence in children. Biol Psychol. (2015) 107:69–75. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2015.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

278. Holzman JB, Bridgett DJ. Heart rate variability indices as bio-markers of top-down self-regulatory mechanisms: a meta-analytic review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 74:233–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.12.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

279. Doussard-Roosevelt JA, McClenny BD, Porges SW. Neonatal cardiac vagal tone and school-age developmental outcome in very low birth weight infants. Dev Psychobiol. (2001) 38:56–66. doi: 10.1002/1098-2302(2001)38:1<56::AID-DEV5>3.0.CO;2-K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

280. DiPietro JA, Voegtline KM. The gestational foundation of sex differences in development and vulnerability. Neuroscience. (2017) 342:4–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.07.068

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

281. McCarthy MM. Sex differences in the developing brain as a source of inherent risk. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. (2016) 18:361–72. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2016.18.4/mmccarthy

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

282. Mizuno R. The male/female ratio of fetal deaths and births in Japan. Lancet. (2000) 356:738–9. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(00)02637-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

283. Balsara SL, Faerber JA, Spinner NB, Feudtner C. Pediatric mortality in males versus females in the United States, 1999-2008. Pediatrics. (2013) 132:631–8. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-0339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

284. Terrell ML, Hartnett KP, Marcus M. Can environmental or occupational hazards alter the sex ratio at birth? A systematic review. Emerg Health Threats J. (2011) 4:7109. doi: 10.3402/ehtj.v4i0.7109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

285. Lyster WR. Altered sex ratio after the London smog of 1952 and the Brisbane flood of 1965. J Obstet Gynaecol Br Commonw. (1974) 81:626–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-0528.1974.tb00529.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

286. Catalano R, Bruckner T, Marks AR, Eskenazi B. Exogenous shocks to the human sex ratio: the case of September 11, 2001 in New York City. Hum Reprod. (2006) 21:3127–31. doi: 10.1093/humrep/del283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

287. Grech V. The effect of warfare on the secular trends in sex ratios at birth in Israel, Egypt, and Kuwait over the past 60 years. Libyan J Med. (2014) 9:23448. doi: 10.3402/ljm.v9.23448

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

288. Fukuda M, Fukuda K, Shimizu T, Moller H. Decline in sex ratio at birth after Kobe earthquake. Hum Reprod. (1998) 13:2321–2. doi: 10.1093/humrep/13.8.2321

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

289. Hansen D, Moller H, Olsen J. Severe periconceptional life events and the sex ratio in offspring: follow up study based on five national registers. BMJ. (1999) 319:548–9. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7209.548

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

290. Roberts ML, Buchanan KL, Evans MR. Testing the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis: a review of the evidence. Anim Behav. (2004) 68:227–39. doi: 10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

291. Schaafsma SM, Pfaff DW. Etiologies underlying sex differences in Autism Spectrum Disorders. Front Neuroendocrinol. (2014) 35:255–71. doi: 10.1016/j.yfrne.2014.03.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

292. Nowak J, Pawlowski B, Borkowska B, Augustyniak D, Drulis-Kawa Z. No evidence for the immunocompetence handicap hypothesis in male humans. Sci Rep. (2018) 8:7392. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25694-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

293. Migliore L, Nicoli V, Stoccoro A. Gender specific differences in disease susceptibility: the role of epigenetics. Biomedicines. (2021) 9:52. doi: 10.3390/biomedicines9060652

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

294. Unwin LM, Maybery MT, Wray JA, Whitehouse AJ. A “bottom-up” approach to aetiological research in autism spectrum disorders. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:606. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00606

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

295. Spence SJ, Schneider MT. The role of epilepsy and epileptiform EEGs in autism spectrum disorders. Pediatr Res. (2009) 65:599–606. doi: 10.1203/PDR.0b013e31819e7168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

296. Leu C, Stevelink R, Smith AW, Goleva SB, Kanai M, Ferguson L, et al. Polygenic burden in focal and generalized epilepsies. Brain. (2019) 142:3473–81. doi: 10.1093/brain/awz292

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

297. Fiest KM, Sauro KM, Wiebe S, Patten SB, Kwon CS, Dykeman J, et al. Prevalence and incidence of epilepsy: a systematic review and meta-analysis of international studies. Neurology. (2017) 88:296–303. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0000000000003509

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

298. Chez MG, Chang M, Krasne V, Coughlan C, Kominsky M, Schwartz A. Frequency of epileptiform EEG abnormalities in a sequential screening of autistic patients with no known clinical epilepsy from 1996 to 2005. Epilepsy Behav. (2006) 8:267–71. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2005.11.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

299. Kim HL, Donnelly JH, Tournay AE, Book TM, Filipek P. Absence of seizures despite high prevalence of epileptiform EEG abnormalities in children with autism monitored in a tertiary care center. Epilepsia. (2006) 47:394–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1167.2006.00434.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

300. Cavazzuti GB, Cappella L, Nalin A. Longitudinal study of epileptiform EEG patterns in normal children. Epilepsia. (1980) 21:43–55. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1980.tb04043.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

301. Capdevila OS, Dayyat E, Kheirandish-Gozal L, Gozal D. Prevalence of epileptiform activity in healthy children during sleep. Sleep Med. (2008) 9:303–9. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2007.03.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

302. Aldenkamp AP. Effect of seizures and epileptiform discharges on cognitive function. Epilepsia. (1997) 38(Suppl. 1):S52–5. doi: 10.1111/j.1528-1157.1997.tb04520.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

303. Aldenkamp AP, Gutter T, Beun AM. The effect of seizure activity and paroxysmal electroencephalographic discharges on cognition. Acta Neurologica Scandinavia. (1992) 86:111–21. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.1992.tb04479.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

304. Ko C, Kim N, Kim E, Song DH, Cheon KA. The effect of epilepsy on autistic symptom severity assessed by the social responsiveness scale in children with autism spectrum disorder. Behav Brain Funct. (2016) 12:20. doi: 10.1186/s12993-016-0105-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

305. Constantino JN. Early behavioral indices of inherited liability to autism. Pediatr Res. (2019) 85:127–33. doi: 10.1038/s41390-018-0217-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

306. Roederer M, Quaye L, Mangino M, Beddall MH, Mahnke Y, Chattopadhyay P, et al. The genetic architecture of the human immune system: a bioresource for autoimmunity and disease pathogenesis. Cell. (2015) 161:387–403. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2015.02.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

307. Daub JT, Hofer T, Cutivet E, Dupanloup I, Quintana-Murci L, Robinson-Rechavi M, et al. Evidence for polygenic adaptation to pathogens in the human genome. Mol Biol Evol. (2013) 30:1544–58. doi: 10.1093/molbev/mst080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

308. Grimbacher B, Warnatz K, Yong PFK, Korganow AS, Peter HH. The crossroads of autoimmunity and immunodeficiency: lessons from polygenic traits and monogenic defects. J Allergy Clin Immunol. (2016) 137:3–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jaci.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

309. Billstedt E, Gillberg IC, Gillberg C. Autism after adolescence: population-based 13- to 22-year follow-up study of 120 individuals with autism diagnosed in childhood. J Autism Dev Disord. (2005) 35:351–60. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-3302-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

310. Gillberg C. The ESSENCE in child psychiatry: early symptomatic syndromes eliciting neurodevelopmental clinical examinations. Res Dev Disabil. (2010) 31:1543–51. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2010.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

311. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Burtenshaw A, Hobson E. Mathematical talent is linked to autism. Hum Nat. (2007) 18:125–31. doi: 10.1007/s12110-007-9014-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

312. Baron-Cohen S, Knickmeyer RC, Belmonte MK. Sex differences in the brain: implications for explaining autism. Science. (2005) 310:819–23. doi: 10.1126/science.1115455

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

313. Kopp S, Gillberg C. The autism spectrum screening questionnaire (ASSQ)-revised extended version (ASSQ-REV): an instrument for better capturing the autism phenotype in girls? A preliminary study involving 191 clinical cases and community controls. Res Dev Disabil. (2011) 32:2875–88. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2011.05.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

314. Anderson DK, Liang JW, Lord C. Predicting young adult outcome among more and less cognitively able individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2014) 55:485–94. doi: 10.1111/jcpp.12178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

315. Gillberg IC. Autistic syndrome with onset at age 31 years: herpes encephalitis as a possible model for childhood autism. Dev Med Child Neurol. (1991) 33:920–4. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1991.tb14804.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

316. Ghaziuddin M, Al-Khouri I, Ghaziuddin N. Autistic symptoms following herpes encephalitis. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2002) 11:142–6. doi: 10.1007/s00787-002-0271-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

317. Riva D, Giorgi C. The cerebellum contributes to higher functions during development: evidence from a series of children surgically treated for posterior fossa tumours. Brain. (2000) 123:1051–61. doi: 10.1093/brain/123.5.1051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

318. Absher JR, Cloutier J, editors. Neuroimaging Personality, Social Cognition, and Character. San Diego, CA: Academic Press (2016).

Google Scholar

319. Grove J, Ripke S, Als TD, Mattheisen M, Walters RK, Won H, et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat Genet. (2019) 51:431–44.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

320. Treffert DA. The savant syndrome: an extraordinary condition. A synopsis: past, present, future. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2009) 364:1351–7. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0326

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

321. Homberg JR, Jagiellowicz J. A neural model of vulnerability and resilience to stress-related disorders linked to differential susceptibility. Mol Psychiatry. (2021). doi: 10.1038/s41380-021-01047-8. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

322. Belsky J, Pluess M. Beyond risk, resilience, and dysregulation: phenotypic plasticity and human development. Dev Psychopathol. (2013) 25:1243–61. doi: 10.1017/S095457941300059X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

323. Sarovic D, Schneiderman JF, Hadjikhani N, Riaz B, Orekhova EV, Lundstrom S editors. The clinical feasibility of deconstructing autism into a pathogenetic triad: classification results from a case-control cohort. In: International Society for Autism Research Annual Meet, Montreal, QC (2019).

324. Wechsler D. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (2008).

Google Scholar

325. McCarthy MI, Abecasis GR, Cardon LR, Goldstein DB, Little J, Ioannidis JP, et al. Genome-wide association studies for complex traits: consensus, uncertainty and challenges. Nat Rev Genet. (2008) 9:356–69. doi: 10.1038/nrg2344

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

326. Kern JK, Geier DA, Sykes LK, Geier MR, Deth RC. Are ASD and ADHD a continuum? A comparison of pathophysiological similarities between the disorders. J Atten Disord. (2015) 19:805–27. doi: 10.1177/1087054712459886

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

327. Constantino JN, Gruber CP, Davis S, Hayes S, Passanante N, Przybeck T. The factor structure of autistic traits. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2004) 45:719–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00266.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

328. van Lang ND, Boomsma A, Sytema S, de Bildt AA, Kraijer DW, Ketelaars C, et al. Structural equation analysis of a hypothesised symptom model in the autism spectrum. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2006) 47:37–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01434.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: endophenotype, framework, genetic architecture, autism model, autism (ASD), exposome, etiology, neurodevelopmental disorder

Citation: Sarovic D (2021) A Unifying Theory for Autism: The Pathogenetic Triad as a Theoretical Framework. Front. Psychiatry 12:767075. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.767075

Received: 30 August 2021; Accepted: 27 October 2021;
Published: 16 November 2021.

Edited by:

Bodil Aggernæs, University of Copenhagen, Denmark

Reviewed by:

Owen Murray Rennert, Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD), United States
Antonella Gagliano, University of Cagliari, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Sarovic. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Darko Sarovic, ZGFya28uc2Fyb3ZpYyYjeDAwMDQwO2d1LnNl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.