Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 26 August 2021
Sec. Addictive Disorders

Future Expectations and Internet Addiction Among Adolescents: The Roles of Intolerance of Uncertainty and Perceived Social Support

  • Faculty of Psychology, Time Psychology Research Center, Southwest University, Chongqing, China

Internet addiction is a common and challenging problem among adolescents. Previous studies have shown that future time orientation is an important protective factor against internet addiction. In this study, the mediating effect of intolerance of uncertainty and the moderating role of perceived social support were examined on the association between future expectations, regarded as the “prospective life course” perspective of future time orientation, and internet addiction among adolescents. A total of 1,006 Chinese adolescents (54% male and 46% female; Mage = 15.42 years, SD = 1.32) recruited from middle schools completed questionnaires. Results indicated that future expectations were significantly negatively associated with internet addiction, and the link was mediated by intolerance to uncertainty in adolescents. Further, the latent moderated structural equation showed that perceived social support moderated the association between future expectations and intolerance of uncertainty. The association was significant only for adolescents with a higher level of perceived social support. The findings of this study provide specific guidelines for how to prevent adolescent internet addiction.

Introduction

Internet surfing has become one of the most popular leisure activities among adolescents. More than 174 million Chinese adolescents aged 10–19 years are internet users, accounting for 19.3% of cyber citizens in China (1). The internet helps to expand adolescents' social networks and relieve their emotional distress. It may also lead to addiction, an excessive or compulsive internet usage characterized by a loss of personal control (2), which results in poor mental health or other maladaptive behaviors (3). Previous studies have shown that present-oriented factors (e.g., impulsiveness, sensory seeking, and obsessive passion) can lead to internet addiction (4). However, there are few studies on future-oriented and motivational factors (e.g., expectations, goals, aspiration). If individuals have low motivational factors, they would indulge in the present and find it hard to refuse pleasure-seeking in daily life (5). Consequently, the current study focused on the relationship between future expectations and internet addiction. Further, it examined the mediating effect of intolerance of uncertainty and the moderating role of perceived social support to provide suggestions for the prevention of and interventions for adolescent internet addiction.

Future Expectation and Internet Addiction

Thinking about the future is an essential part of human cognition (6). Future expectations are defined as beliefs or expectancies about the likelihood of a specific event occurring in the future (79). Future expectations are considered synonymous with the “prospective life course” perspective of future time orientation (10), which tends to be task-oriented (e.g., family, education, career) and has been shown to help regulate behavior and emotional well-being (11).

Individuals' cognitive representations of future events could have implications for present behaviors (12). Expectancy-value theory (13), a self-regulation theory in motivational psychology, posits that expectations are crucial. Further, it assumes that individuals who have positive expectations for a goal tend to adopt approach behaviors and continuously shorten the distance between the present and the goal to be achieved (14). In this process, expectations serve as powerful motivators of current decisions that push individuals away from immediate enjoyment. Specifically, expectations stimulate individuals to consider the future consequences of their current behavior to devote themselves less to immediate rewards (e.g., excessive internet usage). Previous studies have shown that future time perspective negatively predicted Internet and Facebook addiction (15, 16). Furthermore, a positive future time perspective was a significant predictor of internet gaming disorder symptoms, either immediately or 3 years later (17). A recent meta-analysis revealed that future time perspective was positively associated with health-related behaviors, as indicated by a negative association with substance use (18). Therefore, this study hypothesized that future expectations are negatively associated with internet addiction.

Although studies have shown that future expectations may reduce or affect adolescents' internet addiction, few studies have examined the possible mechanisms that might underpin the association between future expectations and internet addiction. Potential mediators and moderators of the relationship between future expectations and internet addiction that may intensify or weaken the association are still unknown. The study expanded on previous research by proposing a moderated mediation model (see Figure 1), which may clarify the mediation and moderation processes underlying the association between future expectations and internet addiction.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The proposed moderated mediation model.

Intolerance of Uncertainty as a Mediator

Intolerance of uncertainty (IU) is defined as a cognitive bias that affects how a person perceives, interprets, and responds to uncertain situations at the cognitive, emotional, and behavioral levels (19). Future expectations may reduce one's level of IU. Although uncertainty can feel intolerable, humans' subjective initiatives can help them take control of their lives. They can adopt more proactive approaches (e.g., cognitive reappraisal and pre-planning) to cope with the anxiety of uncertainty. This means that people's positive advantages or expectations can play a vital role in some uncertain situations. van den Bos et al., (20) proposed an integrated model of coping with personal uncertainty, which points out that humans' ultimate goal is to pursue self-realization and actively seeking goals relieves personal uncertainty. Self-uncertainty can be regarded as temporary confusion or doubt in the process of following one's goals. McGregor et al., (21) showed that writing about how personal goals promote core values and identifications may relieve anxious uncertainty and prevent subsequent defensive reactions to personal uncertainty. Equally, as a prospective thought, future expectations should guide individuals toward established goals when faced with uncertainty. Consequently, it can be assumed that future expectations have an essential impact on IU.

Simultaneously, IU is a cognitive bias that could increase the possibility of excessive internet use. Due to the uncertainty and anxiety caused by rapid social changes and brutal social competition, many individuals lacking social competitiveness tend to seek temporary peace and detachment through the Internet. Luhmann et al., (22) observed that IU was associated with reward-based decision-making by utilizing a simple laboratory gambling task and found that higher IU was associated with a tendency to select the immediately available, but less valuable and less probable, rewards. With an increasing degree of IU, the probability of choosing immediate safety reward behavior would increase correspondingly (23). It was inferred that participants with higher IU are more inclined to engage in present hedonic behaviors and ignore these behaviors' long-term consequences.

Given the associations among future expectations, IU, and internet addiction described above, IU may act as a mediator of the relationship between future expectations and internet addiction. This means that future expectations may reduce the IU level (i.e., facilitating tolerance of uncertainty), thus alleviating adolescents' internet addiction.

Perceived Social Support as a Moderator

Perceived social support (PSS) is defined as the perception of available resources/supportive social ties acquired via social interaction (24). It is usually regarded as a cumulative protective factor that considers the support of parents, teachers, and friends. According to the stress-buffering model (25), PSS can buffer individuals from the influence of stressful events or circumstances by providing appropriate coping responses. Individuals with high PSSs will experience greater feelings of being respected and understood in the face of uncertain situations. Previous studies have shown that one-on-one peer support intervention could facilitate uncertainty management and enhance psychosocial functioning in patients diagnosed with HIV (26). Conversely, individuals with low PSSs lack the necessary psychological resources to ineffectively deal with uncertain situations or face more maladaptive consequences (e.g., online game addiction, negative emotions).

Based on the above, it is proposed that PSS may serve as a moderator of the relationship between future expectations and internet addiction via IU. According to ecological systems theory, there is an interaction between individuals and the environment (27). Theoretically, it is feasible to analyze and verify the moderator role of PSS. Individual-context relations theory (28) also suggests that individual behaviors are formed and developed during the interaction between individuals and the environment surrounding them. Empirical studies have confirmed that the interaction between individual factors and PSS can affect individual mentalities. For example, PSS moderated the associations between fear of missing out and authentic adolescent self-presentation on social networking sites (29). The effects of cyber victimization on adolescent depression were moderated by PSS (30). These findings indicate that PSS may interact with individual factors (e.g., future expectations) to affect IU and internet addiction.

The Present Study

The study examined a model of the process by which future expectations predict internet addiction among adolescents. In particular, (a) to test whether IU mediates the relationship between future expectations and internet addiction, and (b) to test whether PSS would moderate the indirect associations between future expectations and internet addiction through IU. Taken together, this integrated moderated mediation model can address questions about both mediation (i.e., how do future expectations relate to internet addiction?) and moderation (i.e., when or for whom is the relation most or least potent?) in a single model.

Materials and Methods

Participants and Procedures

A total of 1,006 middle school students (54% male and 46% female) participated in this survey. The participants were recruited from eight middle schools in China. The mean age was 15.42 years (SD = 1.32, range = 11–18 years). The results showed that 806 participants had mobile phones and internet access (accounting for 80.1 %), and 804 participants had computers and internet access at home (accounting for 79.9%). The participants' average online time was 1.84 h per day (SD = 2.07 h), and the average number of years was 5.84 years since they first got internet access (SD = 2.72 years).

The Ethics in Human Research Committee of the corresponding author's university approved all materials and procedures. Students were invited to participate in the survey anonymously in classrooms. Informed consent was obtained from each participant and school. Well-trained postgraduate psychology students collected the survey data on paper. The authenticity, independence, and integral nature of all answers were emphasized to the participants. Students were informed that they could terminate their responses anytime they wanted. Data collection was performed within 30 min to complete all the questions in the questionnaires.

Measures

Future Expectations

Future expectations were assessed using the Future Expectations Scale for Adolescents (7), which has been translated into Chinese (31). The scale consists of 24 items and five dimensions, including work and education, marriage and family, social group, health, and children's futures. Sample items are “When I am an adult, I will achieve the level of education that I want” and “When I am an adult, I will have good health.” For each item, adolescents indicated the degree to which they agreed with the statement on a seven-point scale, ranging from 1 (I do not believe this at all) to 7 (I certainly believe this). The reliability coefficients for the total score (α = 0.92) and the subscales (α = 0.77-0.91) in the current study were satisfactory. Confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) showed that the scale fit the data quite well (χ2/df = 4.66, CFI = 0.93, TLI = 0.88, RMSEA = 0.07, 90% CI [0.07, 0.08], SRMR = 0.03).

Internet Addiction

Internet addiction was assessed using the Adolescent Internet Addiction Diagnosis Questionnaire (2), adapted for use in Chinese culture (30). The scale consists of 10 items (e.g., “I feel the need to use the internet with increasing amounts of time to achieve satisfaction”). Responses were made on a six-point scale, ranging from 1 (extremely disagree) to 6 (extremely agree). In this study, Cronbach's α coefficient (alpha) of the measure was 0.88. CFA showed that the scale fit the data quite well (χ2/df = 3.55, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.97, RMSEA = 0.051, 90% CI [0.038, 0.065], SRMR = 0.017).

Intolerance of Uncertainty

Intolerance of uncertainty was assessed with the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale-Chinese version (32, 33). The scale consists of 12 items and three dimensions involving prospective anxiety, prospective behavior, and inhibitory behavior. Sample items are “Uncertainty keeps me from living a full life” and “Unforeseen events upset me greatly.” For each item, adolescents indicated the degree to which they agreed with the statement on a five-point scale, ranging from 1 (not my characteristic at all) to 5 (entirely my characteristic). The IUS dimensions (α = 0.65, 0.67, 0.78, respectively) and total score (α = 0.80) demonstrated good internal consistency in this study. CFA showed that the scale fit the data quite well (χ2/df = 5.01, CFI = 0.95, TLI = 0.90, RMSEA = 0.064, 90% CI [0.055, 0.074], SRMR =0.028).

Perceived Social Support

Perceived social support was assessed using the PSS Scale (34) and revised for Chinese participants (35). The scale consists of 12 items and three support domains: family, friend, and significant others. An example item is “I have friends with whom I can share my joys and sorrows.” For each of the 12 items, adolescents indicated the degree to which they agreed with the statement on a seven-point scale, ranging from 1 (extremely disagree) to 7 (extremely agree). Cronbach's α for the measure (α = 0.92) and its three facets (α = 0.85, 0.86, and 0.84, respectively) in the present sample were satisfactory. CFA showed that the scale fit the data quite well (χ2/df = 6.71, CFI = 0.97, TLI = 0.94, RMSEA = 0.077, 90% CI [0.068, 0.087], SRMR = 0.023).

Data Analysis

The data were analyzed using Mplus 7.4 (36), which tested the hypothesized moderated mediation model with structural equation modeling (SEM). The bootstrap method based on 5,000 samples was used to obtain bias-corrected and accelerated 95% confidence intervals (CIs) (37). The 95% CI that does not include zero suggests a significant effect. The model fit was assessed using multiple fit indices, including ratio chi-square over degrees of freedom (χ2/df), comparative fit index (CFI), Tucker-Lewis index (TLI), root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), and the standard root mean square residual (SRMR). The SEM study suggested a good model fit was defined by the following criteria: CFI ≥ 0.90, TLI ≥ 0.90, RMSEA ≤ 0.08, and SRMR ≤ 0.08 (38).

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Analysis

The skewness of the variables was between −0.37-0.50, and kurtosis was between −0.09 and 0.89, indicating that the variables conformed to the typical distribution hypothesis (39). Descriptive statistics and Pearson correlation analyses were conducted, and the results are shown in Table 1. The correlation analyses found that most future expectations were significantly correlated with dimensions of IU, PSS, and factors of internet addiction (see the measurement model). The dimensions of IU were significantly correlated with the dimensions of PSS and aspects of internet addiction. Further, the dimensions of PSS were significantly associated with the factors of internet addiction. Gender and age were correlated with future expectations and internet addiction, and were included as covariates in subsequent analyses.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlations for all variables.

Testing for the Measurement Model

In the measurement model, future expectations, PSS, and IU were treated as latent variables. Meanwhile, given that the internet addiction questionnaire is a one-dimensional scale, the factorial algorithm's balanced method was adopted to package 10 items into three factors (40). Internet addiction was also treated as a latent variable. In this way, the measurement model with four latent variables and 14 observation variables was finally obtained. The measurement model parameters were tested and estimated using the maximum likelihood method of the covariance structure model. Missing data were handled using the full maximum-likelihood estimation procedure. Overall, the measurement model fit well with the data (χ2/df = 4.55, CFI = 0.94, TLI = 0.93, RMSEA = 0.064, 90% CI [0.057, 0.071], SRMR = 0.045). As shown in Table 2, the latent variables' standardized factor loadings were significant (p < 0.001). The correlation coefficient of each latent variable is shown in Table 3. Specifically, future expectations were negatively correlated with IU and internet addiction and positively correlated with PSS. IU was negatively correlated with PSS and positively correlated with internet addiction, while PSS was negatively correlated with internet addiction.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Measurement model: latent variable factor loadings.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Correlation coefficients of latent variables.

Testing for Mediation

All covariates were held constant during the analysis. First, the direct effect of future expectations on internet addiction was tested, and the results showed that the model fit the data well (χ2/df =3.36, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.048, 90%CI [0.036, 0.062], SRMR = 0.027). Future expectations significantly predicted internet addiction (γ = −0.23, 95% CI [−0.32, −0.14], p < 0.001), and explained 5.9% of the total variance in internet addiction. Next, IU was added as a mediator variable based on the original model. As shown in Figure 2, the results also showed that the model was a good fit with the data (χ2/df = 2.50, CFI = 0.99, TLI = 0.98, RMSEA = 0.039, 90% CI [0.028, 0.049], SRMR = 0.026. Future expectations significantly predicted IU (γ = −0.25, 95% CI [−0.33, −0.18], p < 0.001); IU significantly predicted internet addiction (γ = 0.34, 95% CI [0.25, 0.42], p < 0.001). Future expectations still significantly predicted internet addiction (γ = −0.14, 95% CI [−0.23, −0.06], p < 0.01), but the effect was weakened. Future expectations and IU explained 13.9% of the total variance in internet addiction. The results of the analyses suggested that IU had a significant mediating effect between future expectations and internet addiction. The mediating effect size was −0.09, 95% CI [−0.12, −0.06], p < 0.001, and the mediation effect accounted for 37.0% of the total effect.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. The mediating effect of intolerance of uncertainty in the association between future expectations and internet addiction. **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

Testing for Moderated Mediation

The latent moderated structural equation was applied to examine the moderating role of PSS (41). Two models were established in this study. In model 1, the main effects of PSS were added based on the mediation model of IU. The results showed that Model 1 fit well with the data (χ2/df = 2.98, CFI = 0.98, TLI = 0.97, RMSEA = 0.044, 90% CI [0.037, 0.052], SRMR = 0.037). The model explained 14.5% of the variance in internet addiction. As seen in Figure 3, in model 2, the latent interaction variable (future expectation × perceived social support) was added based on model 1. The researchers tried to assess whether model 2 was better than model 1. The model fit was assessed using the likelihood ratio to do this. In model 1 without the latent interaction variable (LogL [restricted model] = −17641.642) and in model 2 with the latent interaction variable (LogL [full model] = −17638.564), LR (df = 2) = 6.156, p < 0.05. These results indicate that model 2 trumped model 1. As shown in Figure 3, PSS significantly moderated the relationship between future expectations and IU (γ = −0.10, p < 0.05), but it did not moderate the association between future expectation and internet addiction (γ = −0.01, p > 0.05). Thus, PSS played a moderating role in the first half of the mediating effect.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. The moderating effect of perceived social support. The dotted line indicates that the coefficients are not significant. †p < 0.10, *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.

To further probe the interaction effect, a simple slope analysis was conducted. Participants were analyzed at two levels (low level, M—SD; high level, M + SD) according to the moderator's levels. Figure 4 shows that higher levels of future expectations (1 SD above the mean) were more strongly associated with IU when adolescents reported higher levels of PSS (1 SD above the mean) (β = −0.15, SE = 0.04, t = −3.61, p < 0.001). However, for adolescents with lower levels of PSS (1 SD below the mean), the effect of future expectations on IU was not significant (β = −0.04, SE = 0.04, t = −1.10, p > 0.05). That is, PSS moderated the indirect association between future expectations and internet addiction through IU.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. The moderating effect of perceived social support between future expectations and intolerance of uncertainty.

Discussion

Despite burgeoning evidence for the role of future time orientation in reducing internet addiction in adolescents, little is known about the mediating and moderating mechanisms underlying this relationship. To address this gap, this study established a complex theoretical model that explored both mediating (i.e., IU) and moderating (i.e., PSS) mechanisms in the association between future expectations and internet addiction among Chinese adolescents.

The Mediating Role of Intolerance of Uncertainty

This study showed that future expectations significantly and negatively predicted internet addiction. This supports our hypothesis and is consistent with previous studies on the negative association between future time perspective and internet addiction [e.g., (15, 16)]. It further suggests drawing attention to future thinking when preventing and intervening internet addiction in adolescents. The existing literature has pointed out that the future time perspective is a cognitive-motivational concept (42). Besides playing a cognitive role in human functioning, future time perspective has a powerful motivational function. The motivational function for future expectations, the “prospective life course” perspective of future time orientation, enables individuals to maintain their stated goals and remain undistracted so that dependence on the internet in terms of time and energy is significantly discounted. It is also in line with the expectancy value theory (13), which assumes that the expectations of success probability and relative value are the key determinants of behavioral choices. If adolescents believe that future events are more likely to happen (positive expectations), they would prefer to focus on actions that help them achieve expectations. Meanwhile, one crucial reason teenagers are addicted to the internet is that they cannot meet their daily needs, thereby turning to the online world for comfort—typically, online games, dating, and chatting (43). If there is constructive compensation in real life, teenagers will not seek satisfaction online. For example, when individuals have higher goals and expectations, they will know where to go forward and pursue what satisfies them so as to avoid overuse of the internet.

The findings showed that future expectations significantly and negatively predicted IU. In the face of environmental uncertainty, individuals who have intrinsic motivations and positive qualities would tolerate uncertainty to a certain extent and be more consistent with common goals. Vela et al., (44) found that hope and search for meaning can significantly and positively predict grit, characterized by persistence in goals in uncertain situations. Although there is uncertainty in the future, uncertainty also comes with many possibilities. These possibilities reflect the nature of humans' expectations and probably make people believe that positive future events will happen. How do we develop specific coping mechanisms in the face of numerous uncertainties? This study provides an impressive perspective, namely, cultivating the capability to think about the future. Believing that positive future events are more likely to happen brings confidence and direction to adolescents, reducing personal uncertainty. Therefore, it would be said that future expectations display a compensating effect on IU.

IU significantly positively predicted internet addiction. The occurrence of internet addiction is inseparable from individual cognitive factors. As a cognitive bias, IU usually leads to many adverse outcomes (e.g., negative emotions and impulsive behaviors). Previous studies have shown that IU is a risk factor for social phobia, obsessive-compulsive disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and symptoms of depression (45). This study's findings align with those of (46), revealing that community adults with high fear of the unknown (high IU) may experience cyberchondria and excessive use of the internet. These findings provide more evidence for the prevention and intervention of internet addiction. IU may be a trait variable and a state variable (47), so it is possible to provide useful guidance for individuals with high IU. For example, relying on families and teachers' cooperation, adolescents with high IU could change their thinking styles from “negative problem orientation” to “positive problem orientation.” They must accept that solving these problems requires more time and effort, and they believe that they can rely on their abilities to deal with issues rather than go online for hedonistic activities. From the perspective of clinical application, cognitive behavioral therapy provides cognitive reconstruction strategies for this maladaptive cognition to achieve a rational use of network functions (48).

Consistent with this hypothesis, the study indicates that IU mediated the relationship between future expectations and internet addiction. This suggests that apart from the direct effect of future expectations on internet addiction, IU may play an essential role in this relationship. Adolescence, a transition period from childhood to adulthood, is a critical period for individuals' development and usually involves various simulations and temptations in their environment. Although the internet does bring convenience to adolescents, its freshness, anonymity, and excitability may breed addiction. It appears that, when challenged with problems or behavioral concerns, serving as a protective factor (49), positive future expectations are conducive to making individuals tolerate some uncertainty (high tolerance of uncertainty) and focus on the task. This may reduce or even eliminate internet addiction behavior.

The Moderating Role of Perceived Social Support

This study showed that PSS only moderates the link between future expectations and IU (the first half of the mediating link). As expected, the relationship between future expectations and IU was significant for adolescents with a high PSS level and not substantial for adolescents with a low PSS level. These findings indicate that adolescents with a high PSS level may develop an adaptive cognitive style, such as tolerance of uncertainty if they have a high level of future expectations. However, adolescents with a low level of PSS in their real life are not likely to choose an adaptive cognitive style, even if they have a high level of future expectations. How can we reduce individual IU? As mentioned above, it is necessary to focus on cultivating individuals' abilities of future thinking and improving the degree of PSS. As the stress-buffering model (25) emphasizes, PSS may buffer individuals from the influence of adverse situations. Social support is inherently characterized by intimacy and assistance. Therefore, the moderating role of PSSs could be understood in terms of individuals using external resources to enhance the positive impact of future expectations on IU to reduce personal uncertainty.

However, the study did not find that PSS moderated the relationship between future expectations and internet addiction (the direct pathway). This result could be explained by the potent role that future expectations play in adolescent lives. We argue that expectations are fundamental and too pervasive human motivation, which profoundly impacts adolescent emotions, cognitions, and behaviors. Therefore, individuals with high future expectations should possess low-intensity internet addiction, regardless of whether they have a high or low PSS level. The findings also demonstrate that the influence of individuals' internal traits on their behaviors is gradually stable in the adolescent period. In this study, however, both future expectations and PSS had independent effects on internet addiction. Further, PSS was negatively associated with internet addiction, in line with prior research, revealing that cumulative risk is related to adolescent internet addiction (50). If adolescents' social support is insufficient or there are not enough perceived resources in their environment, they may seek satisfaction in other areas such as the virtual world.

Limitations and Future Directions

Despite the novelty of our data and its essential literary contribution, it is subject to some limitations. First, we employed a cross-sectional design that made it difficult to infer causality. Future researchers should use longitudinal designs to confirm causal hypotheses. Specifically, previous studies have obtained age differences in individuals' future orientation and delay discounting (51). Therefore, future research could explore the trajectory of future expectations and their longitudinal associations with internet addiction. Second, future expectations were mainly about positive future expectations and less about negative future expectations (or future fear, future anxiety). If both positive and negative future expectations were measured, the results would be more reliable and generalizable. Additionally, the measures were based on self-reports; future studies should try to collect data from multiple informants (e.g., parents' expectations and teachers' expectations) to strengthen the conclusions further. Third, although the Future Expectations Scale for Adolescents has been translated into Chinese, it is still derived from Western culture. Culture shapes the way individuals think about the past, present, and future time. Existing research has shown that Chinese teenagers pay more attention to their parents' happiness and health regarding hopes and fears for the future (52). However, this attentiveness has not been reflected in measurement tools. Fourth, additional research is needed to design to capture specific internet use (e.g., social media platforms, video games, shopping, surfing, etc) which would be timely and far more useful.

Implications

Despite these limitations, our findings may contribute to understanding internet addiction and its potential protective and risk factors. First, the present study supports early motivation theories that emphasize the crucial role of expectations in behavior regulation (13). Our findings support the notion that adolescents' future expectations decrease internet addiction risk, highlighting the need to implement future thinking cultivation programs. Second, our study provided additional evidence that IU may be a mechanism linking future expectations and internet addiction, suggesting that interventions that target Internet addicts should help them establish more reasonable belief systems that allow them to cope with the uncertain experience. Third, the study indicates that perceived social support may help protect adolescents from low IU levels associated with high levels of future expectations. This shows that one crucial component in intervention programs is enhancing students' social support networks (30) and assisting the youth in developing the necessary skills to access those networks.

Conclusion

The evidence from our study suggests that future expectations are protective factors for internet addiction in adolescents, and this effect is partially mediated by IU. Additionally, PSS plays a moderating role in the association between future expectations and IU in adolescents. These findings suggest that adolescents' future expectations, IU, and PSS may be key targets for prevention and intervention programs dealing with internet addiction. This study will help to inspire future researchers to better understand the phenomenon of internet addiction and advance the field of internet research.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

This study was approved by the ethics committee of Faculty of Psychology at Southwest university. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

GD and HL conceived and designed experiments. GD contributed to the data analysis and writing. HL revised it critically for important content. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities (SWU1909027), the Chongqing Humanities and Social Science Key Research Base Project (18SKB02), and the National Social Science Fund of China (14BSH080).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher's Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

References

1. China Internet Network Information Center. The 45th China Statistical Report on Internet Development. (2020). Available online at: http://www.cac.gov.cn/2020-04/27/c_1589535470378587.htm (accessed April 28, 2020).

2. Young KS. Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychol Behav. (1998) 1:237–44. doi: 10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237

CrossRef Full Text

3. Ciarrochi J, Parker P, Sahdra B, Marshall S, Jackson C, Gloster AT, et al. The development of compulsive Internet use and mental health: a four-year study of adolescence. Dev Psychol. (2016) 52:272–83. doi: 10.1037/dev0000070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Burnay J, Billieux J, Blairy S, Larøi F. Which psychological factors influence Internet addiction? Evidence through an integrative model. Comput Hum Behav. (2015) 43:28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2014.10.039

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Zimbardo P, Boyd J. The Time Paradox: The New Psychology of Time That Will Change Your Life. New York, NY: Free Press (2008).

Google Scholar

6. Suddendorf T, Moore. C. Introduction to special issue: the development of episodic foresight. Cogn Dev. (2011) 26:295–8. doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2011.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. McWhirter EH, McWhirter BT. Adolescent future expectations of work, education, family and community. Youth Soc. (2008) 40:182–202. doi: 10.1177/0044118X08314257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Oettingen G, Mayer D. The motivating function of thinking about the future: expectations versus fantasies. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2002) 83:1198–212. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.5.1198

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Wyman PA, Cowen EL, Work WC, Kerley JH. The role of children's future expectations in self-system functioning and adjustment to life stress: a prospective study of urban at-risk children. Dev Psychopathol. (1993) 5:649649–61. doi: 10.1017/S0954579400006210

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Sipsma HL, Ickovics JR, Lin H, Kershaw TS. Future expectations among adolescents: a latent class analysis. Am J Commun Psychol. (2012) 50:169–81. doi: 10.1007/s10464-011-9487-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Seginer R. Future orientation in times of threat and challenge: how resilient adolescents construct their future. Int J Behav Med. (2008) 32:272–82. doi: 10.1177/0165025408090970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Bandura A. Self-regulation of motivation and action through internal standards and goal systems. In Pervin LA, editor. Goal Concepts in Personality and Social Psychology. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (1989). p. 19–85.

13. Wigfield A, Eccles JS. Expectancy–value theory of achievement motivation. Contemp Educ Psychol. (2000) 25:68–81. doi: 10.1006/ceps.1999.1015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Scheier MF, Carver CS. Effects of optimism on psychological and physical well-being: theoretical overview and empirical update. Cogn Ther Res. (1992) 16:201–28. doi: 10.1007/BF01173489

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Przepiorka A, Blachnio A. Time perspective in Internet and Facebook addiction. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 60:13–8. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Przepiorka A, Blachnio A, Cudo A. The role of depression, personality, and future time perspective in Internet addiction in adolescents and emerging adults. Psychiatry Res. (2019) 272:340–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lukavská K. The immediate and long-term effects of time perspective on Internet gaming disorder. J Behav Addict. (2018) 7:44–51. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kooij DTAM, Kanfer R, Betts M, Rudolph CW. Future time perspective: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Appl Psychol. (2018) 103:867–93. doi: 10.1037/apl0000306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Dugas MJ, Schwartz A, Francis K. Brief report: Intolerance of uncertainty, worry, and depression. Cogn Ther Res. (2004) 28:835–42. doi: 10.1007/s10608-004-0669-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. van den Bos K, McGregor I, Martin LL. Security and uncertainty in contemporary delayed-return cultures: coping with the blockage of personal goals. In: Carroll PJ, Arkin RM, Wichman LDA, editors. Handbook of Personal Security. New York, NY: Psychology Press (2015).

Google Scholar

21. McGregor I, Zanna MP, Holmes JG, Spencer SJ. Compensatory conviction in the face of personal uncertainty: going to extremes and being oneself. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2001) 80:472–88. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.3.472

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Luhmann CC, Ishida K, Hajcak G. Intolerance of uncertainty and decisions about delayed, probabilistic rewards. Behav Ther. (2011) 42:378–86. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2010.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Carleton RN, Duranceau S, Shulman EP, Zerff M, Gonzales J, Mishra S. Self-reported intolerance of uncertainty and behavioural decisions. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2016) 51:58–65. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2015.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Barrera M. Distinctions between social support concepts, measures, and models. Am J Commun Psychol. (1986) 14:413–45. doi: 10.1007/BF00922627

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Cohen S, Wills TA. Stress, social support, and the buffering hypothesis. Psychol Bull. (1985) 98:310–57. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.98.2.310

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Brashers DE, Basinger ED, Rintamaki LS, Caughlin JP, Para M. Taking control: the efficacy and durability of a peer-led uncertainty management intervention for people recently diagnosed with HIV. Health Commun. (2016) 32:11–21. doi: 10.1080/10410236.2015.1089469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bronfenbrenner U. Ecological systems theory. In: Vasta R, editor. Six Theories of Child Development: Revised Formulations and Current Issues. Philadelphia, PA: Jessica Kingsley (1992). p. 187–248.

Google Scholar

28. Lerner RM, Lerner JV, Almerigi J, Theokas C. Dynamics of individual-context relations in human development: a developmental systems perspective. In: Thomas JC, Segal DL, Hersen M, Thomas JC, Segal DL, Hersen M, editors. Comprehensive Handbook of Personality and Psychopathology, Vol. 1: Personality and Everyday Functioning. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons Inc (2006). p. 23–43.

Google Scholar

29. Wang P, Xie X, Wang X, Wang X, Zhao F, Chu X, et al. The need to belong and adolescent authentic self-presentation on SNSs: a moderated mediation model involving FoMO and perceived social support. Pers Individ Differ. (2018) 128:133–8. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2018.02.035

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Li Y, Li D, Li X, Zhou Y, Sun W, Wang Y, et al. Cyber victimization and adolescent depression: the mediating role of psychological insecurity and the moderating role of perceived social support. Child Youth Serv Rev. (2018) 94:10–9. doi: 10.1016/j.childyouth.2018.09.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Du G, Lyu H. Relationship between adolescents' future expectation and Internet addiction: based on latent class analysis. Chin J School Health. (2018) 39:1835–7. doi: 10.16835/j.cnki.1000-9817.2018.12.020

CrossRef Full Text

32. Carleton RN, Norton MA, Asmundson GJ. Fearing the unknown: a short version of the intolerance of uncertainty scale. J Anxiety Disord. (2007) 21:105–17. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Wu L, Wang J, Qi X. Validity and reliability of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale-12 in middle school students. Chin Ment Health J. (2016) 30:700–5. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2016.09.012

CrossRef Full Text

34. Zimet GD, Dahlem NW, Zimet SG, Farley GK. The multidimensional scale of perceived social support. J Pers Assess. (1988) 52:30–41. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa5201_2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Wang X, Wang X, Ma H, . (Eds.). Rating Scales for Mental Health. 2nd ed. Beijing: Chinese Mental Health Journal Press (1999).

Google Scholar

36. Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus User's Guide. 7th ed. Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén (1998-2012).

37. Preacher KJ, Rucker DD, Hayes AF. Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivariate Behav Res. (2007) 42:185–227. doi: 10.1080/00273170701341316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Byrne BM. Structural Equation Modeling With Mplus: Basic Concepts, Applications, and Programming. 2nd ed. New York, NY: Routledge, Taylor and Francis Group (2012).

Google Scholar

39. Weston R, Gore PA. A brief guide to structural equation modeling. Couns Psychol. (2006) 34:719–51. doi: 10.1177/0011000006286345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Rogers WM, Schmitt N. Parameter recovery and model fit using multidimensional composites: a comparison of four empirical parceling algorithms. Multivariate Behav Res. (2004) 39:379–412. doi: 10.1207/S15327906MBR3903_1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Klein A, Moosbrugger H. Maximum likelihood estimation of latent interaction effects with the LMS method. Psychometrika. (2000) 65:457–74. doi: 10.1007/BF02296338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Lens W. Future time perspective: a cognitive motivational concept. In: Brown DR, Veroff J, editors. Frontiers of Motivational Psychology. New York, NY: Springer (1986). p. 173–90.

Google Scholar

43. Kardefelt-Winther D. A conceptual and methodological critique of Internet addiction research: towards a model of compensatory Internet use. Comput Hum Behav. (2014) 31:351–4. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.10.059

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Vela JC, Lu MTP, Lenz AS, Hinojosa K. Positive psychology and familial factors as predictors of Latina/o students' psychological grit. Hisp J Behav Sci. (2015) 37:287–303. doi: 10.1177/0739986315588917

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Mcevoy PM, Mahoney AE. To be sure, to be sure: intolerance of uncertainty mediates symptoms of various anxiety disorders and depression. Behav Ther. (2012) 43:533–45. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2011.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Bajcar B, Babiak J. Neuroticism and cyberchondria: the mediating role of intolerance of uncertainty and defensive pessimism. Pers Individ Differ. (2020) 162:110006. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Liu Y, Wei Z, Liu T. Intolerance of uncertainty: origin, development and research. Chin Ment Health J. (2014) 28:796–800. doi: 10.3969/j.issn.1000-6729.2014.10.014

CrossRef Full Text

48. Davis RA. A cognitive-behavioral model of pathological Internet use. Comput Hum Behav. (2001) 17:187–95. doi: 10.1016/S0747-5632(00)00041-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. O'Neal LTJ, Cotten SR. Promotive factors and psychosocial adjustment among urban youth. Child Youth Care Forum. (2016) 45:947–16. doi: 10.1007/s10566-016-9364-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Li D, Zhou Y, Zhao L, Wang Y, Sun W. Cumulative ecological risk and adolescent Internet addiction: the mediating role of basic psychological need satisfaction and positive outcome expectancy. Act Psychol Sinica. (2016) 48:1519–37. doi: 10.3724/SP.J.1041.2016.01519

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Steinberg L, Graham S, O'Brien L, Woolard J, Cauffman E, Banich M. Age differences in future orientation and delay discounting. Child Dev. (2009) 80:28–44. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2008.01244.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Zhang W, Chen L, Yu F, Wang S, Nurmi JE. Hopes and fears for the future among Chinese adolescents. J Res Adolescence. (2015) 25:622–9. doi: 10.1111/jora.12166

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: future expectations, intolerance of uncertainty, perceived social support, internet addiction, adolescents

Citation: Du G and Lyu H (2021) Future Expectations and Internet Addiction Among Adolescents: The Roles of Intolerance of Uncertainty and Perceived Social Support. Front. Psychiatry 12:727106. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.727106

Received: 18 June 2021; Accepted: 27 July 2021;
Published: 26 August 2021.

Edited by:

Marc N. Potenza, Yale University, United States

Reviewed by:

Ruben David Baler, National Institutes of Health (NIH), United States
Georgios Demetrios Kotzalidis, Sapienza University of Rome, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Du and Lyu. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Houchao Lyu, houchao928@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.