- 1Grupo de Investigación Emoción, Cognición y Conducta, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad Pontificia Bolivariana, Medellín, Colombia
- 2Grupo de Investigación Psicología y Neurociencias, Facultad de Psicología, Universidad de San Buenaventura, Medellín, Colombia
- 3Grupo de Investigación en Psiquiatría de Enlace (GIPE), Facultad de Medicina, Universidad Pontificia Bolivariana, Medellín, Colombia
- 4Grupo de Investigación Psique y Sociedad, Facultad de Ciencias de la Salud, Fundación Universitaria María Cano, Medellín, Colombia
Objective: The exposure to unfavorable environments during childhood negatively affects the development of the executive planning abilities in adult life. In countries with sociopolitical conflicts, children are exposed to traumatic events as a result of child abuse and sociopolitical violence. The purpose of this study was to analyze the effect of the exposure to both forms of adverse childhood experiences on the executive planning abilities in adults from the general population.
Method: The history of child abuse and sociopolitical violence during childhood was assessed, as well as the executive planning abilities, in 59 adults older than 49 without cognitive impairment or depressive disorder.
Results: Of the sample, 88.1% experienced at least one child abuse event and 47.5% was exposed to sociopolitical violence. Sexual abuse and physical abuse (child abuse) were associated with reduced performance in executive planning. Forced displacement and extortion (sociopolitical violence) had a mixed relationship with planning ability, improving some aspects, and worsening some others. Kidnapping was associated with increased capacity and control of the working memory and executive planning.
Conclusions: The traumatic events during childhood have differential effects on the executive planning skills in the adult life. The exposure to sexual and physical abuse negatively affects executive skills; on the other hand, sociopolitical violence has a mixed or positive impact. Specifically, kidnapping favors the executive planning processes, probably under an evolutionary adaptive mechanism.
Introduction
Executive planning abilities is important for the autonomy of adults. It is the ability to integrate, sequence, and develop intermediate steps to accomplish goals; it ensures the adaptation to environmental demands, and it favors the organization and direction of behavior, thus determining the functional capacity. Since no behavior is as productive as a planned one, the functionality during adult and old age depends on the integrity of these processes (1).
Executive planning abilities depend on the activation of the prefrontal cortex, responsible for resolution of new and complex situations. The development of these functions follows a gradual pattern that covers critical periods of brain maturation between the ages of 5 and 17 (2). The exposure to abuse or neglect during these ages produces constant modifications in structures and circuits of the nervous central system (CNS), which increases the risk of mental disorders in adults (3). Since the development of these functions follows a slow and progressive course during childhood, the time interval during which they are susceptible to damage due to environmental adversities is wide, with the consequence of compromising the executive planning abilities in subsequent stages (4).
The different forms of child abuse have a differential effect on particular brain regions and circuits. They have been related to, for example, reduced volume in areas of the anterior cingulum, precuneus, parahippocampal gyrus, and hippocampus, regions involved in short- and long-term memory, working memory, planning, sequencing, and monitoring of one's own behavior (5). Specifically, sexual abuse is linked to reduced performance in inhibitory control (6) and verbal memory (7); physical abuse is related to reduced performance in auditory attention and visual-motor integration, problem solving, abstraction, and planning (8); and emotional abuse is connected with perseverative errors (9).
In contrast, some studies have reported a positive effect of adverse early experiences on adult cognition. Ritchie et al. (10) observed that physical and sexual abuse is positively related to verbal fluency and memory. Barnes et al. (11) described that, in the African-American population compared with white population, early adversities, such as food restriction, had a protective effect against cognitive impairment (11). Subsequently Feeney et al. (12) reported similar results, where child sexual abuse history was associated with better overall cognition, memory, executive function, and processing speed. However, most of the research works agree on demonstrating the harmful role of adverse environments during childhood on the cognitive development in adult life (3).
Child abuse has had an exponential growth in the cohorts of children born in the early twentieth century (13). In Latin America, 30% of minors have experienced some form of abuse (14), and in Colombia, its prevalence has been estimated in 36% (15). This abuse involves physical and sexual aggressions of high traumatic intensity, which are recognized as experiences that cause severe stress (10).
Furthermore, in Colombia, there is another source of traumatic experiences during childhood: the extended war situation of varying intensity known as “la violencia” dating back to the 1950s. People who are older than 50 years today were potentially exposed to two forms of trauma (child abuse and extended sociopolitical violence) when they were children (16). A recent study on Colombian adults estimated the prevalence of child abuse above 80%, and the traumatic events associated with sociopolitical violence before the age of 18 reached 13.5% in the case of kidnapping, 17.3% in the cases of forced displacement and extortion, and 20.4% in the case of poverty caused by war (17).
It is not clear yet whether the combination of child abuse and sociopolitical violence during childhood is connected with the decline of cognition during the adult life. It is known that the combination of adverse experiences throughout life with depression or anxiety in adults increases the risk of cognitive impairment and dementia (4); however, it has not been sufficiently studied whether the severity of traumatic childhood events explains a deficient performance in the executive planning of adults.
The purpose of this study was to analyze the effect of the exposure to child abuse and sociopolitical violence during childhood on the executive planning abilities of adults older than 49, controlling for variables such as depressive symptoms, other neuropsychiatric symptoms, and the overall cognitive performance.
The hypothesis is that the severity of the trauma for being exposed to the combination of abuse and sociopolitical violence before the age of 18 causes disturbances in the development of the executive planning ability, associated with poor performance in the adult life.
Methods
This was a cross-sectional analytical study that analyzes the relationship between the severity of combined exposure to abuse and sociopolitical violence during childhood and executive planning performance, controlling for the effect of neuropsychiatric symptoms, and overall cognitive performance, in a group of adults of the general population from Medellín, Colombia.
Participants
The sample consisted of 59 men and women older than 49. This age limit was established to ensure that the youngest participant had been born before 1969, time when Colombia was already immersed in the sociopolitical violence dating back to 1948 approximately (16); thus, all the individuals included in the sample grew in Colombia during the period of expansion and consolidation of the violence. In addition, at that age, it is already possible to track changes in the cognitive function due to the effects of aging (18).
According to the curve of prediction for sample size (19), we needed a minimum of 55 participants for the multivariate linear models, with a maximum of 10 predictors within each model, to detect significance for R2 > 0.250.
Individuals who met the inclusion criteria were identified and, using the snowball technique, they helped locate others with similar characteristics. Most of them were enrolled during 2019 by the members of the Psychology and Neuroscience research group of the Faculty of Psychology of Universidad de San Buenaventura (Medellín), who explained the objectives of the study to potential participants and referred them to the researchers for assessment. This mechanism allowed obtaining a sample that included adults from the general population, older people who attended comprehensive care centers for the elderly and older adults attending nursing homes during the day or who lived there as their regular residence in Medellín (Table 1).
People with cognitive impairment (score < 24 in the Mini-Mental State Examination, MMSE), depressive disorders (score > 5 in the Yesavage Geriatric Depression Scale, GDS), history of alcohol or drug abuse within the 6 months prior to the assessment (clinical interview), and sensory deficits hindering a proper assessment were excluded.
The study was approved by the bioethics committee of Universidad de San Buenaventura, Medellín. All the participants gave their written informed consent after receiving full information.
Instruments and Measures
Sociodemographic and Predictor Variables
We collected sociodemographic data such as gender, age, and years of education in a standard form.
Despite having excluded several psychiatric disorders, the presence of neuropsychiatric symptoms was assessed to control for their interference in the relationship between traumatic childhood experiences and executive planning. To that end, the study used (i) the GDS (20), which, besides depressive disorder, identifies the presence of depressive symptoms in patients without the disorder; according to the exclusion criteria, individuals scoring between 0 and 5 were included in the study; and (ii) the Neuropsychiatric Inventory (NPI) (21), which establishes the presence of 12 symptoms: delusions, hallucinations, agitation/aggression, dysphoria/depression, anxiety, euphoria/elation, apathy/indifference, disinhibition, irritability/lability, aberrant motor, nighttime behavior, and appetite/eating.
We used the total score of the MMSE for assessing the overall cognitive performance (22). It comprises 20 questions related to domains such as spatial and temporal orientation, memory, attention, and language, where higher scores indicate better function, and the maximum score is 30. According to the exclusion criteria, only individuals scoring between ≥24 were included.
The early trauma was assessed using the Colombian version of the Early Trauma Inventory-Self Report (ETI-SRCol) (17). The Inventory is valid and reliable for self-report assessment of traumatic antecedents during childhood (23, 24), and the appropriate culturally adapted versions are amply utilized for measuring early trauma in adult population (25–29). The ETI-SRCol assesses 84 traumatic events (items). It is a self-administered questionnaire that measures, in adults, eight traumatic categories (subscales) before the age of 18. The ETI-SRCol has four subscales for abuse and neglect events: general trauma (31 items), physical abuse (9 items), emotional abuse (7 items), and sexual abuse (15 items); and four subscales for events attributable to sociopolitical violence: displacement (9 items), kidnapping (5 items), extortion (4 items), and poverty (4 items).
Each ETI-SRCol item is dichotomously classified as “existing” or “non-existent,” and the frequency of occurrence of each item is also registered. Therefore, The ETI-SRCol yields to the Childhood Trauma Severity Index (CTSI), a continuous measure that quantifies the total burden of the trauma within each one of the eight subscales of the ETI-SRCol during childhood.
To estimate a CTSI, the frequency of each adverse event is registered on a Likert scale from 1 (once a year) to 6 (more than once a day). The CTSI is calculated by multiplying each positive/existing item by the frequency of the event and then adding these values within each subscale, resulting in a measurement of the trauma severity (24).
In this study, the eight CTSIs represent the predictor variables, as they are a weighted measurement of the severity of the harm caused and are the ones used in multivariate analyses. The sum of all ETI-SRCol “existing” items (ETI-SRCol total score) is reported.
Outcome Measures
We used the Tower of London-Drexel University Version (TOL-DX) test to assess the outcome variable, executive planning (30). The TOL-DX examinees should reorganize a series of different colored beads from a start position to a goal position; they must move the beads in three pegs to get the positions indicated by the examiner, following two rules: (1) certain number of beads should remain in each peg and (2) only one bead can be moved at a time. The test consists of 10 items of increasing difficulty with a minimum and maximum acceptable number of moves.
The TOL-DX has five executive planning indicators: (a) Total correct score, which determines the working memory capacity and control; (b) Total moves score, as indicator of the executive planning quality; (c) Total initiation time score, as measurement of the inhibitory control and planning preparation processes; (d) Total execution time score, which indicates the speed or pace at which the plans are put into practice from the start of the execution to its completion or discontinuation; and (e) Total problem-solving time score, which shows the overall speed of executive planning and speed of problem solving.
A scaled score was calculated for each of the five TOL-DX measures with a mean of 10 and a standard deviation of 3, adjusted by age according to the Colombian normative data for the test (31).
Data Analysis
The demographic, clinical, and neurocognitive characteristics are described through frequencies (percentages) and mean ± standard deviation (SD).
To analyze the relationship of demographic variables (gender, age, and years of education) and clinical variables [neuropsychiatric symptoms (NPI), depressive symptoms (GDS), and overall cognitive performance (MMSE)] with scaled scores in the five TOL-DX executive planning scores, we used the Spearman rho correlation coefficient or the Mann–Whitney U-test, as appropriate, reporting, in the latter, the estimated size of the r effect.
The Spearman rho coefficient was also used to examine the correlation between the five executive planning scores of the TOL-DX and the CTSI of each subscale of traumatic childhood experiences (ETI-SRCol).
In order to study the relationship between the severity of traumatic childhood experiences and the variation in performance in the five scores of executive planning, controlling for the effect of demographic mediators, clinical mediators, or the overall cognitive performance, five multivariate hierarchical models of robust (bootstrapping) linear regression were adjusted.
Each model included as predictors, in the first step, the demographic, clinical, and/or overall cognitive performance covariates that were significantly associated with the executive planning scores in the bivariate analysis; in the second step, the four CTSIs of the abuse events; and, in the last step, the four CTSIs of the sociopolitical violence events. The eight CTSIs of each subscale were included, because it is feasible for an individual to be simultaneously exposed to different forms of abuse and violence. The scaled score of each executive planning score of the TOL-DX was the outcome or dependent variable in each model.
This hierarchical strategy was chosen to observe the effect on the variability of executive planning, by introducing first those variables that have been tested in other studies, then the abuse variables already studied, and lastly the traumatic events caused by the violence; thus, it is possible to control the additive effect of covariables and both forms of childhood trauma.
The simultaneous entry method was used for all the predictor variables in the models. The continuous variables were kept in their original form, and if any categorical variables were included, these were always dichotomous. Since the values of the continuous variables violated the assumptions of linear regression, wild bootstrap was applied with 2,000 samples, and 95% confidence intervals with bias correction (BCa) were derived for each beta coefficient in the final model.
The significance level of p < 0.05 was assumed, and statistical tests were two-tailed. Data were analyzed on SPSS-22.0.
Results
The diagram of the participants is shown in Figure 1.
Characteristics of the Sample and Frequency of Traumatic Childhood Experiences
The sample of 59 participants was composed mostly of men (57.6%), with average age of 69.08 ± 9.41 and a mean of 9 ± 5.6 years of education. Despite having excluded individuals with depressive disorder, mood symptoms such as sleep disturbances (27.1%), apathy/indifference (25.4%), dysphoria/depression (18.5%), and irritability/lability (15.3%) were found. Regarding performance in the five executive planning scores, the means of the scaled scores are within the Colombian normative range (M = 10 ± 3) for age, with the total execution time and total problem-solving time scoring the lowest (Table 1).
Regarding traumatic childhood experiences, the total score of the ETI-SRCol was 10.05 ± 6.76. The 94.4% of participants experienced at least one adverse childhood event and, of these, 88.1% were related to experiences of abuse and/or neglect. The 37.3% mentioned having been exposed to three or more experiences of physical abuse, while 44% were victims of any form of sexual abuse (Table 2).
Table 2. Frequency of traumatic events during childhood (ETI-SRCol items) grouped in eight categories (subscales) and severity of each subscale (Childhood Trauma Severity Index) in 59 adults from Medellín, Colombia.
As for the types of sociopolitical violence, 47.5% (n = 28) reported having been exposed at least once before the age of 18, the most frequent being in poverty as a consequence of violence (39.1%) (Table 2).
Bivariate Association of Demographic and Clinical Characteristics With Executive Planning Ability Scores on the TOL-DX
Women performed significantly better on TOL-DX total initiation time score (Mdn = 13; RI = 3) compared to men (Mdn = 10 RI = 5), U = 251, z = −2.68, p = 0.007, r = −0.34. Regarding overall cognitive performance, a direct and significant relationship was found between the MMSE score and the scores in total correct (rs = 0.317 p = 0.014) and total problem-solving time scores (rs = 0.279 p = 0.032) (Table 3).
Table 3. Spearman correlation coefficient (and significance levels) between potential mediators, traumatic childhood experiences, and TOL-DX outcome measures in 59 adults from Medellín, Colombia.
When assessing the influence of neuropsychiatric symptoms on the executive planning abilities, we found that people with agitation/aggression symptoms obtained lower total execution time score (Mdn = 6 RI = 5) compared to those who did not present these symptoms (Mdn = 10 RI = 4), U = 45, z = −2.47, p = 0.013, r = −0.32. The same symptomatic group also scored significantly lower on total problem-solving time score (Mdn = 6 RI = 5) compared to the group without agitation/aggression symptoms (Mdn = 10 RI = 4), U = 53, z = −2.25, p = 0.024, r = −0.33. Lastly, the presence of exaltation symptoms, i.e., euphoria/elation, was associated with lower performance in the total initiation time score (Mdn = 9 RI = 7) compared to the absence of these symptoms (Mdn = 12 RI = 4), U = 62.5, z = −1.98, p = 0.047, r = −0.24. No other significant association was found for neuropsychiatric symptoms and executive planning (non-significant comparisons not shown).
Multivariate Models of the Relationship of Childhood Trauma With the Executive Planning Scores
Table 4 shows the last steps of the five models of the relationship between the severity of childhood traumas and TOL-DX executive planning scores as dependent variables.
Table 4. Variables in the final multiple linear regression model for executive planning indices, with bias corrected and accelerated 95% confidence intervals (95% CI).
After controlling for significant covariates in the bivariate analysis, the CTSI indexes of the ETI-SRCol related to traumatic abuse events, i.e., sexual and physical abuse, were associated with lower executive planning performance. Specifically, sexual abuse was associated with poorer performance in total moves score and total execution time score, while physical abuse was linked, also inversely, to the total initiation time score.
Sociopolitical violence indexes showed a complex association pattern with executive planning. Displacement and extortion CTSIs were associated with lower scores in total moves and total initiation time, respectively. Conversely, these two same CTSIs, displacement and extortion, were associated with better performance in total initiation time score and total moves score, respectively. Kidnapping was linked to better performance in total correct score, total moves score, and total execution time score (Table 4).
Discussion
This study investigated the effects of traumatic childhood experiences on the executive planning abilities in adults without cognitive impairment or depression. Besides analyzing exposure to traumatic experiences related to child abuse, we also addressed the effect of traumatic events caused by sociopolitical violence in early neurodevelopmental stages. The results of this research are innovative because, to our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the severity, which considers perpetuation in time, of traumatic events related to prolonged sociopolitical violence of varying intensity in childhood, combined with the typical forms of child abuse and their effect on cognition in adulthood.
A significant part of the sample (88.1%) was exposed to severe abuse/neglect by people who had a role of responsibility, trust, or power over them. These results are consistent with data on the prevalence of traumatic childhood experiences in adults (10). Likewise, almost half of the participants (47.5%) expressed having been exposed, before the age of 18, to some kind of violence perpetrated by armed groups whose acts were justified by political, economic, or social reasons. These findings coincide with those of a study with a sample of Colombian adults (17). Moreover, the score mean of 10 in the ETI-SRCol also reflects the high exposure to abuse and violence during childhood.
In the multivariate models, as expected, symptoms of euphoria/elation and agitation/aggression affected the inhibitory control capacity, the executive planning speed, and the pace for problem-solving plan implementation (Table 4). To understand the above, it is necessary to consider that increased motor activity, aggression, and euphoria are part of a global hyperactivity manifestation typical of dysfunction of the orbitofrontal cortex, the anterior cingulate region, the insula, and the frontotemporal junction (32). We also found that the women in this study performed better than men in inhibitory control and planning preparation, something that has already been discussed in the literature (33). Finally, a positive relationship was found between overall cognitive performance and working memory capacity and control.
According to the multivariate models, traumatic childhood experiences affected the executive planning abilities. Severe abuse events, especially those related to neglect and physical and sexual abuse, had negative impact on the quality of executive processes. In addition, experiences related to violence had a complex pattern, since some types of trauma had a mixed (positive and negative) effect on various aspects of planning, and others had a beneficial impact on several processes.
Negative Effects of Child Abuse on the Planning Ability
Sexual abuse was associated with lower executive planning quality, thus affecting the speed or pace at which executive plans are implemented. Similarly, physical abuse negatively impacted the inhibitory response processes and the planning preparation.
Similar cognitive patterns have been observed in previous studies on severe forms of this type of abuse, where exposure to these stressors was linked to deficient cognitive performance (4, 34). Specifically, a history of repeated sexual abuse is associated with decreased working memory performance (35).
Analyzing the impact of the severity of traumatic events on cognition allows a better estimate of the repercussions of early adversity during the brain development stages and the long-term impact on cognitive function. Severe stress due to adverse childhood events is known to cause changes in the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis, resulting in high glucocorticoid levels that affect the hippocampal development, which in the long term result, for example, in verbal declarative memory deficits (36).
In this study, the perpetuation of early abuse negatively impacted the executive planning abilities. This association may also be explained by the allostatic load hypothesis (i.e., the adaptive capacity to maintain homeostasis in response to stressors), which reveals how stress responses promote abnormal neurodevelopment (37), generating a pattern of increased stress and adversity throughout life, the cumulative effects of which may contribute to poor executive performance.
Another possible explanation is that adverse events lead to lower cognitive performance throughout life, given that—as it can be observed in the sample—these children often have fewer years of education and socioeconomic opportunities (38), resulting in lower cognitive reserve. However, in this study, the years of education or poverty conditions in childhood were not directly related to executive planning abilities, coinciding with other studies that have found a slight effect of this variable, in contrast to the direct relationship of early traumatic experiences (38, 39).
It is worth noting that there is evidence contrary to the relationship established in this study between child sexual abuse and cognition in adults. The work by Ritchie et al. (10) and Feeney et al. (12) describe a positive effect on some aspects of the information processing, which suggests the need to continue delving into more ample explanatory models on the role of distal neurodevelopmental events on adult cognition.
Mixed Effects of Sociopolitical Violence Events on the Planning Ability
Two types of sociopolitical violence showed a mixed pattern of relationship with the planning ability in the multivariate models, forced displacement, and extortion.
Displacement was associated with lower executive planning quality and, at the same time, was related to better inhibitory control and planning preparation. Conversely, extortion implied better executive planning quality, but it was associated with poorer inhibitory response and planning preparation.
Studies with children displaced by violence in Colombia have established that forced migration at early ages affects cognitive and emotional development, triggering a feeling of insecurity (40). It is possible that the mechanisms of neurodevelopmental damage due to physical and sexual abuse are similar to those of displacement.
Extortion implies that children are directly or indirectly threatened and coerced over a prolonged period of time. Being exposed during childhood to permanent threat of harm in contexts with a high probability of abuse reduces the overall cognitive performance during adulthood (41).
Regarding the beneficial effects of extortion and displacement on cognition, two explanatory hypotheses, not mutually exclusive, are to be considered. In the first place, the implementation of adaptive mechanisms weakens the effect of the trauma over time (12). These mechanisms would increase neuronal plasticity in structures of the prefrontal cortex, also promoting the cognitive reserve. In the second place, as part of the emotional response, traumas activate the amygdala, which favors the consolidation of memory and the processing of threatening stimuli, with consequent cognitive training to better respond to environmental demands (42).
Positive Effects of Sociopolitical Violence Events on the Planning Ability
An interesting finding was the positive effect of kidnapping on the planning ability. This event, characterized by being considerably prolonged in the Colombian case, was associated with better capacity and control of the working memory, quality of the executive planning, and speed at which executive plans are implemented.
These findings suggest that this situation, known for causing prolonged stress and the need to constantly search for solutions, activates resilience mechanisms that influence individuals' ability to adapt and thrive after exposure to trauma, leading to reduced vulnerability to cognitive impairment, probably thanks to neuronal compensatory processes (10, 12).
A study with African Americans reported comparable findings. Adverse factors involving long-term stress and solution seeking, such as food restrictions in childhood, were associated with a slower rate of cognitive decline (11).
The findings of this study could also be explained from an evolutionary perspective, since surviving to advanced age despite early adversity could be an indicator of evolutionary advantages that make individuals more resistant to adversity itself (43). Such advantages would also be demonstrated by better executive skills, which would have favored individuals' recovery from the effects of an adverse childhood.
Limitations, Strengths, and Conclusions
This study has several limitations. The first one is that the retrospective assessment of traumas may lead to underestimating their prevalence; however, the use of an inventory adapted to and validated with a standardized list of traumatic experiences improves the measurement reliability to determine the exposure to and severity of traumas (44). Additionally, some studies have suggested that trauma memories are reliable, especially in people without depressive disorder (such as in this sample) because possible retrospective falsifications are minimized (45).
Another limitation is that posttraumatic stress symptoms were not measured after the traumatic experience or in adult life; therefore, it is not possible to know clearly whether the lower executive planning performance is associated with the early trauma or is a result of posttraumatic stress disorder. However, given that the CTSI was analyzed for each trauma subscale, this limitation may have been overcome. The severity index measures the total trauma burden, better reflecting the long-term effect of exposure to the traumatic events analyzed.
Since we used a non-probability sample, caution should be exercised when generalizing data. On the other hand, this is the first study assessing, besides the classic child abuse, the severity of situations of conflict and sociopolitical violence in a sample from the general population, with clear inclusion and exclusion criteria.
The study has some strengths. As far as we know, this is the first report on the long-term impact of early exposure to traumatic events related to sociopolitical violence (in the context of a prolonged conflict of varying intensity) on cognitive performance, specifically on executive planning. The simultaneous analysis of traumatic experiences associated with abuse and sociopolitical violence offers a broader spectrum of events that (negatively and positively) affect the neurodevelopment and the adaptive capacity, the effects of which can be traced in adult life.
In short, traumatic childhood events exert differential effects on the executive planning abilities. Early exposure to physical and sexual abuse has a negative impact on the executive skills in adulthood, while some traumatic events related to a long-lasting environment of sociopolitical violence, particularly kidnapping, favor the refinement of executive planning processes, presumably as a mechanism of evolutionary adaptation.
Data Availability Statement
The datasets presented in this article are not readily available because the participants have not given consent to have their data publicly stored, however, de-identified data are available upon request. Requests to access the datasets should be directed to Ana M. Gaviria, amigago@gmail.com.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Bioethics Committee of Universidad de San Buenaventura. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
DM-A oversaw study design and completion of project, with assistance of DL-G. DL-G conducted neuropsychological evaluations. JF oversaw data analysis and manuscript completion. AG oversaw study design, completed all data analyses, and writing of manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Funding
This work was internally financed by the Centro de Investigación para el Desarrollo y la Innovación (CIDI) from the Universidad Pontificia Bolivariana, the Dirección de Investigaciones from the Universidad de San Buenaventura, and the Centro de Investigaciones y Desarrollo Empresarial (CIDE) from the Fundación Universitaria María Cano. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
1. Nowak K, Dacewicz A, Broczek K, Kupisz-Urbanska M, Galkowski T, Szelag E. Temporal information processing and its relation to executive functions in elderly individuals. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:1599. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01599
2. García-Molina A, Enseñat-Cantallops A, Tirapu-Ustárroz J, Roig-Rovira T. Maturation of the prefrontal cortex and development of the executive functions during the first five years of life. Rev Neurol. (2009) 48:435–40. doi: 10.33588/rn.4808.2008265
3. Nemeroff CB. Paradise lost: the neurobiological and clinical consequences of child abuse and neglect. Neuron. (2016) 89:892–909. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.019
4. Petkus AJ, Lenze EJ, Butters MA, Twamley EW, Wetherell JL. Childhood trauma is associated with poorer cognitive performance in older adults. J Clin Psychiatry. (2018) 79:16m11021. doi: 10.4088/JCP.16m11021
5. Carrion VG, Wong SS. Can traumatic stress alter the brain? Understanding the implications of early trauma on brain development and learning. J Adolesc Heal. (2012) 51:10. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2012.04.010
6. Barrera M, Calderón L, Bell V. The cognitive impact of sexual abuse and PTSD in children: A neuropsychological study. J Child Sex Abus. (2013) 22:625–38. doi: 10.1080/10538712.2013.811141
7. Navalta CP, Polcari A, Webster DM, Boghossian A, Teicher MH. Effects of childhood sexual abuse on neuropsychological and cognitive function in college women. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. (2006) 18:45–53. doi: 10.1176/jnp.18.1.45
8. Nolin P, Ethier L. Using neuropsychological profiles to classify neglected children with or without physical abuse. Child Abus Negl. (2007) 31:631–43. doi: 10.1016/j.chiabu.2006.12.009
9. Spann MN, Mayes LC, Kalmar JH, Guiney J, Womer FY, Pittman B, et al. Childhood abuse and neglect and cognitive flexibility in adolescents. Child Neuropsychol. (2012) 18:182–9. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2011.595400
10. Ritchie K, Jaussent I, Stewart R, Dupuy AM, Courtet P, Malafosse A, et al. Adverse childhood environment and late-life cognitive functioning. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. (2011) 26:503–10. doi: 10.1002/gps.2553
11. Barnes LL, Wilson RS, Everson-Rose SA, Hayward MD, Evans DA, De Leon CFM. Effects of early-life adversity on cognitive decline in older African Americans and whites. Neurology. (2012) 79:2321–7. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e318278b607
12. Feeney J, Kamiya Y, Robertson IH, Kenny RA. Cognitive function is preserved in older adults with a reported history of childhood sexual abuse. J Trauma Stress. (2013) 26:735–43. doi: 10.1002/jts.21861
13. Dube SR, Felitti VJ, Dong M, Giles WH, Anda RF. The impact of adverse childhood experiences on health problems: Evidence from four birth cohorts dating back to 1900. Prev Med. (2003) 37:268–77. doi: 10.1016/S0091-7435(03)00123-3
14. Hillis S, Mercy J, Amobi A, Kress H. Global prevalence of past-year violence against children: A systematic review and minimum estimates. Pediatrics. (2016) 137:e20154079. doi: 10.1542/peds.2015-4079
15. Ramírez Herrera C. El impacto del maltrato en los niños y las niñas en Colombia. Rev Infanc Adolesc y Fam. (2006) 1:287–301.
16. Cartagena C. Los estudios de la violencia en Colombia antes de la violentología. Diálogos Rev Electrónica Hist. (2016) 17:63–8. doi: 10.15517/DRE.V17I1.18103
17. Posada S, Londoño NH, Gaviria AM. Propiedades psicométricas de la adaptación para Colombia del inventario de experiencias traumáticas en la infancia (ETI-SRCol). Med UPB. (2019) 38:33–45. doi: 10.18566/medupb.v38n1.a05
18. Salthouse TA. When does age-related cognitive decline begin? Neurobiol Aging. (2009) 30:507–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2008.09.023
20. Gómez-Angulo C, Campo-Arias A. Escala de Yesavage para depresión geriátrica (GDS-15 y GDS-5): estudio de la consistencia interna y estructura factorial. Univ Psychol. (2011) 10:735–43. doi: 10.11144/Javeriana.upsy10-3.eydg
21. Cummings JL, Mega M, Gray K, Rosenberg-Thompson S, Carusi DA, Gornbein J. The neuropsychiatric inventory: comprehensive assessment of psychopathology in dementia. Neurology. (1994) 44:2308–14. doi: 10.1212/WNL.44.12.2308
22. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. “Mini-mental state”. A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. (1975) 12:189–98. doi: 10.1016/0022-3956(75)90026-6
23. Bremner JD, Vermetten E, Mazure CM. Development and preliminary psychometric properties of an instrument for the measurement of childhood trauma: the early trauma inventory. Depress Anxiety. (2000) 12:1–12. doi: 10.1002/1520-6394(2000)12:1<1::AID-DA1>3.0.CO;2-W
24. Bremner JD, Bolus R, Mayer EA. Psychometric properties of the early trauma inventory-self report. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2007) 195:211–8. doi: 10.1097/01.nmd.0000243824.84651.6c
25. Plaza A, Torres A, Martin-Santos R, Gelabert E, Imaz ML, Navarro P, et al. Validation and test-retest reliability of early trauma inventory in spanish postpartum women. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2011) 199:280–5. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31821245b9
26. Jeon J-R, Lee E-H, Lee S-W, Jeong E-G, Kim J-H, Lee D, et al. The early trauma inventory self report-short form: psychometric properties of the Korean version. Psychiatry Investig. (2012) 9:229–35. doi: 10.4306/pi.2012.9.3.229
27. Osório FL, Salum GA, Donadon MF, Forni-dos-Santos L, Loureiro SR, Crippa JAS. Psychometrics properties of early trauma inventory self report – short form (ETISR-SR) for the Brazilian context. PLoS ONE. (2013) 8:e76337. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076337
28. Antonopoulou Z, Konstantakopoulos G, zinieri-Coccosis M, Sinodinou C. Rates of childhood trauma in a sample of university students in Greece: the Greek version of the Early Trauma Inventory-Self Report. Psychiatrike. (2017) 28:19–27. doi: 10.22365/jpsych.2017.281.19
29. Hörberg N, Kouros I, Ekselius L, Cunningham J, Willebrand M, Ramklint M. Early Trauma Inventory Self-Report Short Form (ETISR-SF): validation of the Swedish translation in clinical and non-clinical samples. Nord J Psychiatry. (2019) 73:81–9. doi: 10.1080/08039488.2018.1498127
30. Culbertson WC, Zillmer EA. Tower of London Drexel University (TOLDX). Technical manual. Toronto, ON: Multi-Health Systems Inc. (2001).
31. Montañés P, Espitia A, Duarte L. Neuronorma. Colombia. Protocolo, normas, plataforma de perfiles neuropsicológicos y aplicaciones clínicas. In: Montañés P, Espitia A, Duarte L, editors. Bogotá D.C: Biblioteca Abierta: Colección General, serie Psicología. Facultad de Ciencias Humanas: Universidad Nacional de Colombia (2020). doi: 10.2307/j.ctv224tngh
32. Kales HC, Gitlin LN, Lyketsos CG. Assessment and management of behavioral and psychological symptoms of dementia. BMJ. (2015) 350:h369. doi: 10.1136/bmj.h369
33. Grissom NM, Reyes TM. Let's call the whole thing off: evaluating gender and sex differences in executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2019) 44:86–96. doi: 10.1038/s41386-018-0179-5
34. Saleh A, Potter GG, McQuoid DR, Boyd B, Turner R, MacFall JR, et al. Effects of early life stress on depression, cognitive performance and brain morphology. Psychol Med. (2017) 47:171–81. doi: 10.1017/S0033291716002403
35. Petkus AJ, Wetherell JL, Stein MB, Liu L, Barrett-Connor E. History of sexual assault is associated with greater declines in executive functioning in older adults with APOE ϵ4. J Gerontol Ser B. (2012) 67:653–9. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbr163
36. Douglas Bremner J, Randall P, Scott TM, Capelli S, Delaney R, McCarthy G, et al. Deficits in short-term memory in adult survivors of childhood abuse. Psychiatry Res. (1995) 59:97–107. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02800-5
37. McEwen BS. Sex, stress and the hippocampus: allostasis, allostatic load and the aging process. Neurobiol Aging. (2002) 23:921–39. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(02)00027-1
38. Fors S, Lennartsson C, Lundberg O. Childhood living conditions, socioeconomic position in adulthood, and cognition in later life: exploring the associations. J Gerontol Soc Sci. (2009) 6:750–7. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbp029
39. Korten NCM, Penninx BWJH, Pot AM, Deeg DJH, Comijs HC. Adverse childhood and recent negative life events: Contrasting associations with cognitive decline in older persons. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. (2014) 27:128–38. doi: 10.1177/0891988714522696
40. Madariaga Orozco C, Gallardo Bayona LD, Salas Fernández FM, Santamaría Barrios EM. Violencia política y sus efectos en la identidad psicosocial de los niños desplazados el caso de la Cangrejera. Psicol. desde el Caribe. (2011) 10:88–106.
41. Grainger SA, Crawford JD, Kochan NA, Mather KA, Chander RJ, Draper B, et al. An investigation into early-life stress and cognitive function in older age. Int Psychogeriatrics. (2019) 32:1325–9. doi: 10.1017/S1041610219001583
42. Armony JL, Corbo V, Clément MH, Brunet A. Amygdala response in patients with acute PTSD to masked and unmasked emotional facial expressions. Am J Psychiatry. (2005) 162:1961–3. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.162.10.1961
43. Preston SH, Hill ME, Drevenstedt GL. Childhood conditions that predict survival to advanced ages among African-Americans. Soc Sci Med. (1998) 47:1231–46. doi: 10.1016/S0277-9536(98)00180-4
44. Franklin CL, Sheeran T, Zimmerman M. Screening for trauma histories, posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), and subthreshold PTSD in psychiatric outpatients. Psychol Assess. (2002) 14:467–71. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.14.4.467
Keywords: elderly, child abuse, violence, adverse childhood experiences, executive planning
Citation: Montoya-Arenas DA, Londoño-Guzmán D, Franco JG and Gaviria AM (2022) Effect of Exposure to Socio-Political Violence and Abuse During Childhood on Executive Planning in Adult Life. Front. Psychiatry 12:693741. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.693741
Received: 12 April 2021; Accepted: 30 December 2021;
Published: 15 February 2022.
Edited by:
Nora S. Vyas, Kingston University, United KingdomReviewed by:
Ganiu Oladega Okunnu, Crescent University, NigeriaSrinivas Dannaram, Banner Health, United States
Alessandra Carta, University of Sassari, Italy
Fransiska Kaligis, University of Indonesia, Indonesia
Copyright © 2022 Montoya-Arenas, Londoño-Guzmán, Franco and Gaviria. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Ana M. Gaviria, amigago@gmail.com