- 1Department of Psychology, Faculty of Psychology and Education, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran
- 2Iranian National Center for Addiction Studies, Tehran University of Medical Sciences, Tehran, Iran
- 3Department of Cognitive Psychology, Institute of Cognitive Sciences Studies, Tehran, Iran
- 4Addiction Recovery Research Center, Center for Transformative Research on Health Behaviors, Fralin Biomedical Research Institute at Virginia Tech Carilion, Roanoke, VA, United States
- 5Laureate Institute for Brain Research, Tulsa, OK, United States
Introduction
Continuous maladaptive drug-related memories that are resistant to extinction and cause drug-seeking behaviors to be triggered are known to be one of the hallmarks of drug addiction (1). These drug-related memories are salient, strong, and persistent due to chronic maladaptive consolidation processes. Due to the salient content of drug-related memories formed during drug-taking behaviors, certain stimuli (e.g., peers, locations, paraphernalia) become encoded with reward contingencies associated with drugs. As a result of this learning processing, drug-paired stimuli acquire incentive motivational properties that change them into salient cues (2). According to Pavlovian conditioning, consequent exposure to these stimuli (Henceforth called drug cues) activates the original memories and evokes craving. This enhanced retrieval co-occurs with the activation of limbic cortico-striatal pathways involved in reward processing (3). A serious question in addiction neuroscience is whether these memories could be actively erased/reshaped in favor of the recovery process. Different research groups suggested various treatment strategies during the last decade to modulate these memories. Here in this short opinion paper, we propose a novel framework titled “Cue-induced Retrieval and Reconsolidation with Episodic Foresight” (CIREF) that aims to combine three different cognitive interventions, i.e., cue-exposure, memory reconsolidation, and episodic future thinking, to reshape these maladaptive drug-related memories toward more adaptive memories to support addiction recovery.
Memory Reconsolidation and Cue Exposure
Several studies revealed that when old consolidated memories are reactivated, they may become transiently liable to change their content and salient features (4, 5). This reconsolidation stage that lasts between 1 and 6 h, followed by memory reactivation (6), provides a critical time window to reduce memories' motivational and emotional salience. This “destabilization process” necessitates a subsequent period of restabilization, or memory reconsolidation, during which the reactivated memory could be updated, strengthened, modified, disrupted, or erased (7, 8). Some addiction researchers and clinicians hope that renewal of the drug-related memories and alteration of their related motivational and emotional salience via memory reconsolidation could reduce the risk of relapse (9).
To begin the drug-related memory reconsolidation process, patients could be presented with drug cues that make them mentally travel back in time and retrieve the emotional experiences for further elaboration. Previously conducted trials that implemented behavioral memory reconsolidation interventions were often supplemented by extinction-enhancing pharmacological treatments such as D-cycloserine (10–12) or β-adrenergic antagonists like Propranolol (13, 14) and have shown to be impactful in terms of drug cue reactivity extinction (15–17). Positive results in the extinction of drug cue reactivity are suggested to be highly dependent on the efficiency of reactivating maladaptive drug memories (18, 19), and that comes as no surprise that drug cue-exposure seems like a promising strategy for aiding retrieval and reactivation of such memories.
In the experimental context of addiction research, drug cue-exposure, in which the drug-related cues are repeatedly presented in the absence of reinforcement (20), is supposed to reduce drug saliency cues. In the clinical context, Cue-Exposure Therapies (CET) for addiction recovery have been developed to extinguish the conditioned craving-provoking effects of drug cues using extinction procedures (21).
At the same time, despite the promising findings of experimental trials that utilized CET and memory reconsolidation paradigms for addiction treatment, mixed results have been obtained regarding the efficacy of these approaches in terms of craving, relapse rate, and the number of abstinent days in the actual treatment setting (22–24). More recently, skeptical arguments have questioned the efficacy as well as the assumption underlying CET trials—which is drug cue extinction in the lab settings could be translated to a reduction of cue-reactivity in real-life settings, leading to lessened problematic drug use—questioning the efficacy and the ecological validity of CET for drug addiction (25). Moreover, a guided approach that leads to opting for healthier “alternatives” is lacking through learning to react to drug-cues in a neutral way (i.e., CET's ideal outcome) and modifying persistent maladaptive drug-related memories (i.e., memory reconsolidation's ideal outcome). Therefore, this shortcoming raises the need for a rigorous, multi-faceted add-on to these approaches to target not only the past and present-oriented aspects of cognitive processing of drug-related memories but also implement a tool for choosing healthier alternatives in response to drug-cues and the reactivation of drug-related memories. Similarly, given the key contribution of future decision-making in four main phases of addiction (initiation, progression, treatment-seeking, and recovery), interventions that effectively target aberrant decision-making and ultimately effectuate foreseeing the steps leading to recovery are crucial additions to reshape those aspects of cognitive processing modified in aid of CET and memory reconsolidation prospectively (26).
In this opinion paper and for the first time, we propose a new framework as a cognitive intervention termed Cue-induced Retrieval and Reconsolidation with Episodic Foresight (CIREF) for utilizing a combination of episodic future thinking with cue-induced memory reconsolidation to confer greater benefits by adding a future-oriented cognitive training modality.
Episodic Future Thinking: How Could it Put Extinction into Practice?
Despite the widely conceived notion about memory and its retrospective nature over decades ago, cognitive psychologists and neuroscientists' attention has been recently drawn to the future-oriented aspect of memory. This heed was majorly inspired by Tulving's conception of episodic memory and mental time travel, highlighting the prospective facet of human memory (27, 28). Future thinking or prospection (29) has four primary steps: simulation, prediction, intention, and planning; 28), which provide the capacity to imagine and project oneself forward in time and to pre-experience personal events that might happen in the future (30–32).
EFT has become a focus of growing interest among neuroscientists and psychologists, most probably owing to its vast contributions to various cognitive functions and adaptive behaviors, such as decision-making, planning, self-control, goal-attainment, goal-directed behavior, and psychological well-being in general (33–36). Moreover, EFT has considerable implications in “implementation intentions” as a deliberate self-regulatory strategy. Pre-deciding how to implement one's goals, simulating the mental representations of probable future events related to a specific goal, and specifying the fully detailed steps leading to goal attainment take place with the aid of EFT (30, 37). Hence, the ability to elaborately simulate possible future events stands as an essential factor in the treatment of mental health issues such as addictive behaviors, given their associations with impaired value-based decision-making and goal-directed behaviors.
EFT has been recently utilized as an intervention in both clinical and non-clinical populations (38). This dynamism mainly results from evidence showing the adaptive function of the EFT, allowing individuals to simulate distant outcomes and desires (39). In other words, the ability to envision future events may result in more accurate predictions of future behaviors and outcomes by allowing one to mentally “try” various potential ways to react to upcoming situations without engaging in actual behaviors (40). Across different populations, EFT has been shown to enhance the prospective memory—remembering to do something in the future at a specific time, which comprises planning, coordinating, and executing one's intention in an appropriate time in the future; for instance, remembering to take a medicine at a specific time of the day (e.g., tomorrow at 10 a.m.) (41–45). Studies suggest that individuals with drug use or other addictive behaviors experience difficulties with prospective memory that could reduce their ability to form a memory-dependent strategy, such as forming the intention and plans to quit drug use. Hence, cognitive training interventions that target prospective memory in the context of drug addiction could be effectively implemented by rehearsing the simulation and planning self-initiated strategies within probable risky situations to achieve intention completion and control drug-seeking behavior in these populations (46).
Another cognitive mechanism that EFT has effectively targeted in several cognitive enhancement studies in samples with addictive behaviors is intertemporal value-based decision-making—choosing between options associated with rewarding outcomes at different time points in the future (47). Numerous theories have proposed that the discounting of delayed rewards with a preference for immediate payoffs compared to greater but delayed ones (i.e., delay discounting) is impaired decision-making that contributes to the development of addictive behaviors [e.g., (48–51)]. Peters and Büchel were the first to show that engaging in EFT reduces delay discounting rates by modulating decision-making and EFT neural networks (including the anterior cingulate cortex, hippocampus, and amygdala). They further showed that these networks enable future-minded choices allowing one to opt for options that maximize future payoffs (52). Moreover, these critical insights contributed to the formation of Reinforcer Pathology Theory (RPT) (53, 54).
Simply put, RPT states that reinforcers are integrated over a temporal window, measured by delay discounting. The length of that window in part determines the relative reinforcing value of substances vs. the other positive pro-social events. Importantly, this perspective recognizes the important temporal features of these different reinforcers. Drugs are brief, immediate, intense, and reliable. At the same time, pro-social reinforcers are less intense, variable in their outcome (e.g., good, bad, or neutral day at work), and that value accrues over time and investment. When the temporal window is short, brief, intense, reliable reinforcers would have greater value. In contrast, a longer temporal window will decrease substance valuation and increase the valuation of pro-social reinforcers.
In light of these advances and seminal findings, several experimental studies and clinical trials investigated the therapeutic effects of EFT on reducing delay discounting and consequent maladaptive behaviors and reported positive health-related outcomes as a result of engaging in EFT in people with alcohol use disorder, overweight, obese and prediabetic individuals, cigarette smokers, cannabis users, and people with cocaine use disorder (55–61). Moreover, EFT training for individuals with addictive behaviors is suggested to improve the efficiency of other psychosocial interventions aiming to attain emotional reappraisal and correction (62). Lastly, the repeated regeneration of episodic future thinking events has been shown to progressively increase the temporal window in those with alcohol use disorder (63). Since addictive behaviors are primarily associated with the pervasive preference of smaller immediate rewards in lieu of larger delayed ones (i.e., steep discounting), and this preference often leads to impulsive maladaptive behaviors such as drug-seeking and drug use (64), the therapeutic effects of EFT potentially arise from its ability to reduce discounting rates. The studies that implemented EFT as an intervention suggested that pre-experiencing future actions broadens one's temporal window by simulating the value of the reward and therefore facilitating the evaluation of behavior's long-term outcomes (e.g., becoming overweight resulting from excessive calorie intake, developing lung cancer resulting from smoking) (65). These findings indicate that EFT has therapeutic effects on addictive behaviors by changing the excessive discounting of the future while promoting healthy and adaptive decisions resulting in positive behavior change. Considering the aforementioned positive effects, the current paper proposes a new framework for integrating EFT with cue-induced memory reconsolidation in the context of addiction treatment.
Episodic Future Thinking in Cue Exposure Context
As we discussed before, drug-related memories could be retrieved and reactivated as a result of drug cue-exposure. During this context, patients could be asked to imagine themselves in a hypothetical drug-related situation associated with the presented cue (e.g., being offered to use drugs, passing by a group of drug-users in a park, etc.) taking place in the future and elaborate on it in episodic details. The five stages of the CIREF intervention take place in the same order as the EFT stages and subsequent to the cue-exposure as follows:
1) Activating Past Memories With Cue Exposure: Patients initially become exposed to drug-related stimuli using formerly validated pictorial cues (66). This drug cue-exposure process leads to reactivation of maladaptive drug-related memories that happened in the past, which causes the patients to retrieve the drug-related memories and possibly re-experience the emotional arousal associated with them. Past memories become unstable during the reactivation stage and become ready to undergo potential modifications throughout the next stages.
2) Simulating Future Cue Exposures: Patients are prompted to vividly simulate a novel future event that may happen in response to drug-cue encounter in as much detail as possible and verbally describe who they are with, what they are doing (and thinking), where they exactly are, and how they feel (Simulation phase of EFT). Imagining the probable future events in the proposed manner would improve the ecological validity of the intervention.
3) Predicting Response to Cue Exposure and Its Outcomes: Subsequently, patients are asked to predict their associated emotions and behaviors in the simulated event (Prediction phase of EFT). As the patients verbally express their predictions, different options of how to deal with the potential drug-related situation should be predicted and vividly imagined. During this stage, patients expect both positive and negative scenarios that may happen due to being exposed to drug-related situations. The probable future behaviors and emotions (both positive and negative) undergo a pre-appraisal stage by the patient based on the predicted outcomes.
4) Making Intentions in the Context of Cue Exposure: Upon prediction of their reactions, patients are guided to replace immediate rewards that may be chosen impulsively with later self-controlled reward choices (Intention phase of EFT). The intention formation phase in this framework is similar to the “goal-setting” exercises taking place in psychotherapy settings and implementation of intentions (37) in which the patient specifies the when, where, and how of responses leading to goal attainment.
5) Developing Executive Plan for Adaptive Response: Finally, the unstable retrieved drug-related memory, therefore, will be updated with memory reconsolidation strategies that are not limited to modification of the retrospective memory per se but also supplemented with the reconstruction of the prospective memory leading to optimal planning for the future and behaving upon it (Planning phase of EFT). During the planning phase, patients are guided to plan the organization of steps needed to arrive at a specific autobiographical future outcome (67).
To put it differently, while the patients undergo the CIREF cognitive intervention (multiple sessions of individual or group-based therapy meetings), the maladaptive drug-related memories become triggered by a stepwise exposure using a large database of drug-related stimuli. After exposure to each individually validated drug cue set, the patients are asked to imagine themselves in a hypothetical cue-associated drug-related situation that could be happening in the future and elaborate on it in episodic details, mentally predicting and “trying out” different options and their outcomes and planning their future actions upon them. Then, the planned activities based on reconsolidated memories become stored as a new prospective memory guiding the patients to recall their planned intentions at some future point in time. Figure 1 illustrates the conceptual process of the proposed framework and the implications of each stage of this approach in real-life settings.
Figure 1. A schematic diagram illustrating the cognitive processes underlying “Cue-induced Retrieval and Reconsolidation with Episodic Foresight (CIREF),” which applies episodic future thinking to cue-induced memory reconsolidation. The maladaptive drug-related memory that has been consolidated and stored in the past (Left) becomes reactivated as a result of drug cue exposure (Middle). The reactivated memory becomes transiently labile and prone to modification. The extinction process of maladaptive drug-related memories takes place as a result of repetitive exposure to relevant drug cues. Implementing episodic future thinking during this stage engages the person in (1) simulation of a future drug-related event, (2) prediction of emotions and reactions related to it, (3) intention of modifying the drug-seeking behavior triggered by cue exposure, and (4) planning for future actions in response to drug cue exposure maintaining healthy behavior (Top). The boxes in oval shapes depict processes underlying each stage. The reconsolidated memory is therefore updated and re-encoded in aid of episodic future thinking, and the drug cue has undergone extinction as a result of cue exposure therapy (Right).
CIREF benefits from EFT enhancement as a translatable approach in clinical settings (68) that is also necessitated by pieces of evidence showing that individuals with addictive behaviors have difficulties imagining future events and implementing intentions based on them (69–72). Moreover, the suggested framework could fill in the gaps of CET and memory reconsolidation interventions by taking a step further from classical conditioning and updating past drug-related memories by implementing goal-based strategies. Individuals struggling with addiction could develop their “future sightedness” and increase the length of their temporal window trained via EFT within this framework and consequently make healthier decisions, possibly by viewing future events as more connected to their present (73).
Future Directions and Conclusion
Theoretically, two sets of clinical outcomes are expected to be accomplished at both neural and behavioral levels after individuals with addictive behaviors undergo the CIREF intervention. The first set of which are short-term outcomes comprising cue reactivity—the physiological and subjective reactions while being exposed to drug-related stimuli—and drug craving (i.e., feeling the urge to use drugs or be engaged with addictive behavior). These outcomes are expected to be immediate changes in patients' behavior after completion of the CIREF intervention and could be measured with self-report measures (e.g., craving scales and questionnaires), as well as brain imaging techniques (e.g., cue reactivity fMRI task) (74). Ideally, we are expecting that the CIREF approach would lead to some long-term outcomes as well. The long-term clinical outcomes include changes in abstinence measures, such as duration of abstinence (usually measured by biochemical validation methods like urine drug tests in the context of substance use disorders), type of abstinence (i.e., point prevalence, continuous, or prolonged), and relapse rates (75). Therefore, the clinical outcomes of the CIREF approach could be validated at multiple levels using measurements of the neural and cognitive targets (as mediators) and ultimate behavioral outcomes in future studies.
There are potential limitations to the CIREF approach. For instance, the person who is guiding the intervention (i.e., the therapist) has to be conscious of the cue-induced craving levels and ensure that the patients' cue-reactivity and craving that are triggered by the drug cue encounter and simulation (step 1 and 2 of the CIREF intervention) will be managed and mitigated effectively before the patient starts to form intentions and plan for healthier outcomes (step 4 and 5 of the CIREF intervention). A self-report assessment of craving before and after each session of the intervention and ending the session with common psychological craving management strategies (76) could potentially address this limitation as it helps the therapist to gain more control over patients' cue-elicited craving.
Furthermore, there is a thorough and in-depth protocol paper in preparation by our team of authors elaborating on each stage of the CIREF framework that provides the detailed considerations that should be taken into account while implementing each stage of this multicomponent intervention and its translational limitations.
In sum, addiction is a complex disorder that may persist due to a lack of proper integration of past memories and new learning. We propose a novel cognitive interventional framework for drug addiction titled “Cue-Induced Retrieval and Reconsolidation with Episodic Foresight (CIREF),” aiming to supplement cue-induced memory reconsolidation strategies focused on retrieval-extinction procedures with episodic future thinking for optimal results. Episodic future thinking guides patients with addictive behaviors to simulate future events that trigger cue-induced drug craving and mentally rehearse coping strategies that lead to addiction recovery. CIREF provides a multi-faceted approach for addiction treatment in light of targeting both past and future-oriented cognition affected by addiction. Further research is needed to bridge the gap between fundamental laboratory research and applied research to translate the presented framework's basic idea into an actual manualized or computerized intervention for future clinical investigations.
Author Contributions
PR, TR, and HE conceived the conceptual framework of the paper. PR and TR wrote the first draft of the manuscript. HE and WB edited the manuscript and gave conceptual advice. All authors discussed the implications and commented on the final manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
References
1. Tronson NC, Taylor JR. Addiction: a drug-induced disorder of memory reconsolidation. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2013) 23:573–80. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.022
2. Milton AL, Everitt BJ. The psychological and neurochemical mechanisms of drug memory reconsolidation: implications for the treatment of addiction: mechanisms of drug memory reconsolidation. Eur J Neurosci. (2010) 31:2308–19. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2010.07249.x
3. Garavan H, Pankiewicz J, Bloom A, Cho J-K, Sperry L, et al. Cue-induced cocaine craving: neuroanatomical specificity for drug users and drug stimuli. Am J Psychiatry. (2000) 157:1789–98. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.157.11.1789
4. Dudai Y. Reconsolidation: the advantage of being refocused. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2006) 16:174–8. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2006.03.010
5. Becker JE, Price JL, Leonard D, Suris A, Kandil E, Shaw M, et al. The efficacy of lidocaine in disrupting cocaine cue-induced memory reconsolidation. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2020) 212:108062. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.108062
6. Chiamulera C, Hinnenthal I, Auber A, Cibin M. Reconsolidation of maladaptive memories as a therapeutic target: pre-clinical data and clinical approaches. Front Psychiatry. (2014) 5:107. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00107
7. Winters BD, Tucci MC, DaCosta-Furtado M. Older and stronger object memories are selectively destabilized by reactivation in the presence of new information. Learn Memory. (2009) 16:545–53. doi: 10.1101/lm.1509909
8. Lee JLC, Nader K, Schiller D. An update on memory reconsolidation updating. Trends Cogn Sci. (2017) 21:531–45. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.04.006
9. Milton A. Drink, drugs and disruption: memory manipulation for the treatment of addiction. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2013) 23:706–12. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2012.11.008
10. Kantak KM, Nic Dhonnchadha BÁ. Pharmacological enhancement of drug cue extinction learning: translational challenges: enhancement of drug cue extinction. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2011) 1216:122–37. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05899.x
11. Kamboj SK, Joye A, Das RK, Gibson AJW, Morgan CJA, Curran HV. Cue exposure and response prevention with heavy smokers: a laboratory-based randomised placebo-controlled trial examining the effects of D-cycloserine on cue reactivity and attentional bias. Psychopharmacology. (2012) 221:273–84. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2571-2
12. Kiefer F, Kirsch M, Bach P, Hoffmann S, Reinhard I, Jorde A, et al. Effects of d-cycloserine on extinction of mesolimbic cue reactivity in alcoholism: a randomized placebo-controlled trial. Psychopharmacology. (2015) 232:2353–62. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-3882-5
13. Otis JM, Werner CT, Mueller D. Noradrenergic regulation of fear and drug-associated memory reconsolidation. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2015) 40:793–803. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.243
14. Zhao L-Y, Sun L-L, Shi J, Li P, Zhang Y, Lu L. Effects of β-adrenergic receptor blockade on drug-related memory reconsolidation in abstinent heroin addicts. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2011) 118:224–9. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2011.03.025
15. Walsh KH, Das RK, Saladin ME, Kamboj SK. Modulation of naturalistic maladaptive memories using behavioural and pharmacological reconsolidation-interfering strategies: a systematic review and meta-analysis of clinical and 'sub-clinical' studies. Psychopharmacology. (2018) 235:2507–27. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-4983-8
16. Paulus DJ, Kamboj SK, Das RK, Saladin ME. Prospects for reconsolidation-focused treatments of substance use and anxiety-related disorders. Curr Opin Psychol. (2019) 30:80–6. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.03.001
17. Gale G, Walsh K, Hennessy VE, Stemerding LE, Ni KS, Thomas E, et al. Long-term behavioural rewriting of maladaptive drinking memories via reconsolidation-update mechanisms. Psychol Med. (2020) 1–11. doi: 10.1017/S0033291720001531
18. Das Ravi K, Hindocha C, Freeman TP, Lazzarino AI, Curran HV, Kamboj SK. Assessing the translational feasibility of pharmacological drug memory reconsolidation blockade with memantine in quitting smokers. Psychopharmacology. (2015) 232:3363–74. doi: 10.1007/s00213-015-3990-2
19. Kamboj SK, Das RK. Behavioral and pharmacological strategies for weakening maladaptive reward memories: a new approach to treating a core disease mechanism in tobacco use disorder. JAMA Psychiatry. (2017) 74:209. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3937
20. Auber A, Tedesco V, Jones CE, Monfils M-H, Chiamulera C. Post-retrieval extinction as reconsolidation interference: methodological issues or boundary conditions? Psychopharmacology. (2013) 226:631–47. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3004-1
21. Xue YX, Luo YX, Wu P, Shi HS, Xue LF, Chen C, et al. A memory retrieval-extinction procedure to prevent drug craving and relapse. Science. (2012) 336:241–5. doi: 10.1126/science.1215070
22. Saladin ME, Gray KM, McRae-Clark AL, LaRowe SD, Yeatts SD, Baker NL, et al. A double blind, placebo-controlled study of the effects of post-retrieval propranolol on reconsolidation of memory for craving and cue reactivity in cocaine dependent humans. Psychopharmacology. (2013) 226:721–37. doi: 10.1007/s00213-013-3039-3
23. Germeroth LJ, Carpenter MJ, Baker NL, Froeliger B, LaRowe SD, Saladin ME. Effect of a brief memory updating intervention on smoking behavior: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry. (2017) 74:214. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2016.3148
24. Das RK, Walsh K, Hannaford J, Lazzarino AI, Kamboj SK. Nitrous oxide may interfere with the reconsolidation of drinking memories in hazardous drinkers in a prediction-error-dependent manner. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2018) 28:828–40. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2018.05.001
25. Byrne SP, Haber P, Baillie A, Giannopolous V, Morley K. Cue exposure therapy for alcohol use disorders: what can be learned from exposure therapy for anxiety disorders? Subst Use Misuse. (2019) 54:2053–63. doi: 10.1080/10826084.2019.1618328
26. Ekhtiari H, Victor TA, Paulus MP. Aberrant decision-making and drug addiction—How strong is the evidence? Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2017) 13:25–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.09.002
27. Tulving E. On the uniqueness of episodic memory. In: Nilsson LG, Markowitsch HJ, editors. Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory. Göttingen: Hogrefe & Huber Publishers (1999). p. 11–42.
28. Tulving E. Episodic memory and autonoesis: Uniquely human? In: Terrace HS, Metcalfe J, editors. The Missing Link in Cognition. New York: Oxford University Press (2005). p. 4–56. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195161564.003.0001
29. Gilbert DT, Wilson TD. Prospection: experiencing the future. Science. (2007) 317:1351–4. doi: 10.1126/science.1144161
30. Atance CM, O'Neill DK. Episodic future thinking. Trends Cogn Sci. (2001) 5:533–9. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01804-0
31. Schacter DL, Benoit RG, Szpunar KK. Episodic future thinking: mechanisms and functions. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2017) 17:41–50. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.06.002
32. Schacter DL, Addis DR. The cognitive neuroscience of constructive memory: remembering the past and imagining the future. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. (2007) 362:773–86. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2007.2087
33. Boyer P. Evolutionary economics of mental time travel? Trends Cogn Sci. (2008) 12:219–24. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.03.003
34. D'Argembeau A, Van der Linden M. Predicting the phenomenology of episodic future thoughts. Conscious Cogn. (2012) 21:1198–206. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2012.05.004
35. Schacter DL. Adaptive constructive processes and the future of memory. Am Psychol. (2012) 67:603–13. doi: 10.1037/a0029869
36. Seligman MEP, Railton P, Baumeister RF, Sripada C. Navigating into the future or driven by the past. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2013) 8:119–41. doi: 10.1177/1745691612474317
37. Gollwitzer PM. Implementation intentions: strong effects of simple plans. Am Psychol. (1999) 54:493–503. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.7.493
38. Rezapour T, Aupperle RL, Paulus MP, Ekhtiari H. Clinical translation and implementation neuroscience for novel cognitive interventions in addiction medicine. In: Rezapour T, Aupperle RL, Paulus MP, Ekhtiari H, editors. Cognition and Addiction. San Diego: Elsevier (2020). p. 393–404. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-815298-0.00029-0
39. Benoit RG, Gilbert SJ, Burgess PW. A neural mechanism mediating the impact of episodic prospection on farsighted decisions. J Neurosci. (2011) 31:6771–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.6559-10.2011
40. Spreng RN, Madore KP, Schacter DL. Better imagined: neural correlates of the episodic simulation boost to prospective memory performance. Neuropsychologia. (2018) 113:22–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.025
41. Paraskevaides T, Morgan CJA, Leitz JR, Bisby JA, Rendell PG, Curran HV. Drinking and future thinking: acute effects of alcohol on prospective memory and future simulation. Psychopharmacology. (2010) 208:301–8. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1731-0
42. Neroni MA, Gamboz N, Brandimonte MA. Does episodic future thinking improve prospective remembering? Conscious Cogn. (2014) 23:53–62. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2013.12.001
43. Altgassen M, Rendell PG, Bernhard A, Henry JD, Bailey PE, Phillips LH, et al. Future thinking improves prospective memory performance and plan enactment in older adults. Q J Exp Psychol. (2015) 68:192–204. doi: 10.1080/17470218.2014.956127
44. Mioni G, Bertucci E, Rosato A, Terrett G, Rendell PG, Zamuner M, et al. Improving prospective memory performance with future event simulation in traumatic brain injury patients. Br J Clin Psychol. (2017) 56:130–48. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12126
45. Lloyd B, Oudman E, Altgassen M, Walvoort SJW, Kessels RPC, Postma A. Episodic future thinking together with observational learning benefits prospective memory in high-functioning Korsakoff's syndrome patients. Br J Clin Psychol. (2020) 59:369–83. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12251
46. Griffiths A, Hill R, Morgan C, Rendell PG, Karimi K, Wanagaratne S, et al. Prospective memory and future event simulation in individuals with alcohol dependence: prospective memory and alcohol dependence. Addiction. (2012) 107:1809–16. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03941.x
47. Peters J, Büchel C. The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability. Trends Cogn Sci. (2011) 15:227–39. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.002
48. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts discount delayed rewards at higher rates than non-drug using controls. J Exp Psychol. (1999) 128:78–87. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78
49. Bickel WK, Odum AL, Madden GJ. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology. (1999) 146:447–54. doi: 10.1007/PL00005490
50. Kobiella A, Ripke S, Kroemer NB, Vollmert C, Vollstädt-Klein S, Ulshöfer DE, et al. Acute and chronic nicotine effects on behaviour and brain activation during intertemporal decision making: discounting under nicotine. Addict Biol. (2014) 19:918–30. doi: 10.1111/adb.12057
51. Bickel WK, Koffarnus MN, Moody L, Wilson AG. The behavioral- and neuro-economic process of temporal discounting: a candidate behavioral marker of addiction. Neuropharmacology. (2014) 76:518–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2013.06.013
52. Peters J, Büchel C. Episodic future thinking reduces reward delay discounting through an enhancement of prefrontal-mediotemporal interactions. Neuron. (2010) 66:138–48. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.03.026
53. Bickel WK, Athamneh LN. A reinforcer pathology perspective on relapse. J Exp Anal Behav. (2019) 113:48–56. doi: 10.1002/jeab.564
54. Bickel WK, Athamneh LN, Snider SE, Craft WH, DeHart WB, Kaplan BA, et al. Reinforcer pathology: implications for substance abuse intervention. In: deWit H, Jentsch JD, editors. Recent Advances in Research on Impulsivity and Impulsive Behaviors. Cham: Springer (2020). p. 139–62. doi: 10.1007/7854_2020_145
55. Daniel TO, Stanton CM, Epstein LH. The future is now: comparing the effect of episodic future thinking on impulsivity in lean and obese individuals. Appetite. (2013) 71:120–5. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2013.07.010
56. Snider SE, LaConte SM, Bickel WK. Episodic future thinking: expansion of the temporal window in individuals with alcohol dependence. Alcoholism. (2016) 40:1558–66. doi: 10.1111/acer.13112
57. Stein JS, Wilson AG, Koffarnus MN, Daniel TO, Epstein LH, Bickel WK. Unstuck in time: episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and cigarette smoking. Psychopharmacology. (2016) 233:3771–8. doi: 10.1007/s00213-016-4410-y
58. Athamneh LN, Stein MD, Lin EH, Stein JS, Mellis AM, Gatchalian KM, et al. Setting a goal could help you control: comparing the effect of health goal versus general episodic future thinking on health behaviors among cigarette smokers and obese individuals. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2020) 29:59–72. doi: 10.1037/pha0000351
59. Chang BPI, Claassen MA, Klein O. The time is ripe: thinking about the future reduces unhealthy eating in those with a higher BMI. Foods. (2020) 9:1391. doi: 10.3390/foods9101391
60. Sofis MJ, Lemley SM, Lee DC, Budney AJ. A web-based episodic specificity and future thinking session modulates delay discounting in cannabis users. Psychol Addict Behav. (2020) 34:532–40. doi: 10.1037/adb0000557
61. Forster SE, Steinhauer SR, Ortiz A, Forman SD. Evaluating effects of episodic future thinking on valuation of delayed reward in cocaine use disorder: a pilot study. Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse. (2021) 47:199–208. doi: 10.1080/00952990.2020.1865997
62. Noël X, Jaafari N, Bechara A. Addictive behaviors: why and how impaired mental time matters? Prog Brain Res. (2017) 235:219–37. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2017.07.011
63. Mellis AM, Snider SE, Deshpande HU, LaConte SM, Bickel WK. Practicing prospection promotes patience: repeated episodic future thinking cumulatively reduces delay discounting. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2019) 204:107507. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2019.06.010
64. Levitt E, Sanchez-Roige S, Palmer AA, MacKillop J. Steep discounting of future rewards as an impulsivity phenotype: a concise review. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2020) 47:113–38. doi: 10.1007/7854_2020_128
65. Stein JS, Tegge AN, Turner JK, Bickel WK. Episodic future thinking reduces delay discounting and cigarette demand: an investigation of the good-subject effect. J Behav Med. (2018) 41:269–76. doi: 10.1007/s10865-017-9908-1
66. Ekhtiari H, Kuplicki R, Pruthi A, Paulus M. Methamphetamine and opioid cue database (MOCD): development and validation. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2020) 209:107941. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2020.107941
67. Szpunar KK, Spreng RN, Schacter DL. A taxonomy of prospection: Introducing an organizational framework for future-oriented cognition: Fig. 1. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2014) 111:18414–21. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1417144111
68. Rung JM, Epstein LH. Translating episodic future thinking manipulations for clinical use: development of a clinical control. PLoS ONE. (2020) 15:e0237435. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0237435
69. Mercuri K, Terrett G, Henry JD, Bailey PE, Curran HV, Rendell PG. Episodic foresight deficits in long-term opiate users. Psychopharmacology. (2015) 232:1337–45. doi: 10.1007/s00213-014-3772-2
70. Moustafa AA, Morris AN, Nandrino JL, Misiak B, Szewczuk-Bogusławska M, Frydecka D, et al. Not all drugs are created equal: impaired future thinking in opiate, but not alcohol, users. Exp Brain Res. (2018) 236:2971–81. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5355-7
71. Rafei P, Rezapour T, Batouli SAH, Verdejo-Garcia A, Lorenzetti V, Hatami J. How do cannabis users mentally travel in time: evidence from an fMRI study of episodic future thinking [Preprint]. PsyArXiv. (2020). doi: 10.31234/osf.io/ksvhz
72. Noël X, Saeremans M, Kornreich C, Jaafari N, D'Argembeau A. Future-oriented mental time travel in individuals with disordered gambling. Conscious Cogn. (2017) 49:227–36. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2017.02.006
73. Thorstad R, Wolff P. A big data analysis of the relationship between future thinking and decision-making. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2018) 115:E1740–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1706589115
74. Ekhtiari H, Zare-Bidoky M, Sangchooli A, Janes AC, Kaufman MJ, Oliver J, et al. A methodological checklist for fMRI drug cue reactivity studies: development and expert consensus. MedRxiv. (2020) doi: 10.1101/2020.10.17.20214304
75. Cheung KL, de Ruijter D, Hiligsmann M, Elfeddali I, Hoving C, Evers S, et al. Exploring consensus on how to measure smoking cessation. A Delphi study. BMC Public Health. (2017) 17:890. doi: 10.1186/s12889-017-4902-7
76. Ekhtiari H, Nasseri P, Yavari F, Mokri A, Monterosso J. Neuroscience of drug craving for addiction medicine: from circuits to therapies. Prog Brain Res. (2016) 223:115–41. doi: 10.1016/bs.pbr.2015.10.002
77. VandenBos GR. APA Dictionary of Psychology. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association (2007).
78. Mellentin AI, Skøt L, Nielsen B, Schippers GM, Nielsen AS, Stenager E, et al. Cue exposure therapy for the treatment of alcohol use disorders: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2017) 57:195–207. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.07.006
79. Alberini CM, Ledoux JE. Memory reconsolidation. Curr Biol. (2013) 23:R746–50. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.06.046
80. Bickel WK, Stein JS, Moody LN, Snider SE, Mellis AM, Quisenberry AJ. Toward narrative theory: interventions for reinforcer pathology in health behavior. Nebraska Symp Motiv. (2017) 64:227–67. doi: 10.1007/978-3-319-51721-6_8
81. Robinson TE, Berridge KC. Review. The incentive sensitization theory of addiction: some current issues. Philos Trans R Soc London Ser B Biol Sci. (2008) 363:3137–46. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2008.0093
82. Olney JJ, Warlow SM, Naffziger EE, Berridge KC. Current perspectives on incentive salience and applications to clinical disorders. Curr Opin Behav Sci. (2018) 22:59–69. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.01.007
83. MacKillop J, Amlung MT, Few LR, Ray LA, Sweet LH, Munafò MR. Delayed reward discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis. Psychopharmacology. (2011) 216:305–21. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2229-0
84. Amlung M, Vedelago L, Acker J, Balodis I, MacKillop J. Steep delay discounting and addictive behavior: a meta-analysis of continuous associations. Addiction (Abingdon, England). (2017) 112:51–62. doi: 10.1111/add.13535
85. Gollwitzer PM, Sheeran P. Implementation intentions and goal achievement: a meta-analysis of effects and processes. Adv Exp Soc Psychol. (2006) 38:69–119. doi: 10.1016/S0065-2601(06)38002-1
Glossary
Keywords: cue-exposure therapy, episodic future thinking, addiction, memory reconsolidation, cognitive training, episodic foresight
Citation: Rafei P, Rezapour T, Bickel WK and Ekhtiari H (2021) Imagining the Future to Reshape the Past: A Path to Combine Cue Extinction and Memory Reconsolidation With Episodic Foresight for Addiction Treatment. Front. Psychiatry 12:692645. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.692645
Received: 08 April 2021; Accepted: 15 June 2021;
Published: 21 July 2021.
Edited by:
Mehdi Farokhnia, National Institutes of Health (NIH), United StatesReviewed by:
Chloe Jordan, McLean Hospital, United StatesWarren Bernhard Logge, Sydney Local Health District, Australia
Copyright © 2021 Rafei, Rezapour, Bickel and Ekhtiari. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Hamed Ekhtiari, hekhtiari@laureateinstitute.org