- 1Department of Human and Social Sciences, University of Bergamo, Bergamo, Italy
- 2Department of Clinical and Experimental Sciences, University of Brescia, Brescia, Italy
- 3Clinical Neuropsychology, ASST Spedali Civili, Brescia, Italy
- 4Medical Physics Unit, ASST Nord Milano, Milan, Italy
- 5Neuroradiology Unit, Spedali Civili, Brescia, Italy
- 6FOCUS Lab, Department of Psychology, University of Turin, Turin, Italy
Developmental topographical disorientation (DTD) has been defined as a developmental deficit in human navigational skills in the absence of congenital or acquired brain damage. We report the case of Lost In Space Again (LISA), a 22-year-old woman with a normal development and no clinical history of neurological or psychiatric diseases, evaluated twice, with an interval of 5 years. The magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) examination did not reveal any morphological alteration, while diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) showed a structural connectivity deficit (a decreased fractional anisotropy—FA) in the parieto–prefrontal and parieto–premotor pathway. The behavioral assessment showed different deficits in spatial and navigational tasks, which seemed to be connected to a poor ability to form a cognitive map of the environment. Moreover, LISA displayed a poor performance in high-level face encoding and retrieval. The aim of this case report is to share new insight about DTD in order to deepen the knowledge of this specific neurodevelopmental disorder. In conclusion, this novel DTD case (1) supports the hypothesis of the existence of different DTD subtypes; (2) sustains the evidence that DTD can co-occur (or not) with deficit in face recognition; and (3) highlights the need for an in-depth examination from both a neurocognitive and behavioral point of view of a possible common developmental defect between the formation of cognitive maps and the recognition of faces that might be in mental imagery skills. Future directions will be also discussed.
Introduction
In the last years, a newly discovered neurodevelopmental disorder identified as “developmental topographical disorientation” (DTD) has been described. This disorder seems to be widespread (1, 2); however, only few patients affected by DTD have been studied with a comprehensive neuropsychological, behavioral, and neuroimaging assessment. Typically, patients' general intelligence is in the normal range, and they do not report any cognitive complaints except for a really disabling deficit to orient themselves and to find their way in new and/or even in familiar environments since childhood (3–9). They also have no history of neurological or psychiatric disorders and show no brain damage. Moreover, even if the greatest difficulty exhibited by these patients was found in the formation of a cognitive map (2), they have exhibited varying types and degrees of spatial and navigational difficulties [see Fusi et al. (10) for a review] leading researchers to hypothesize the existence of different subtypes of DTD (8). The same emerged for the evaluation of neuroimaging evidence, with patients that showed different patterns of alterations in terms of activations or functional connectivity (FC) (3–6, 8, 11, 12). In addition, interestingly, some of the described DTD cases showed impairment in face recognition tasks (10) or even a clear diagnosis of prosopagnosia (7). The authors have already suggested that a possible common developmental defect could be responsible for this comorbidity.
Here, we present a new DTD case, characterized not only by spatial and navigational deficits but also by impairments in two face recognition tasks. Our aim is to share new insight about DTD by highlighting again the need to provide a new taxonomy and, more specifically, to refocus the discussion about the possible link between navigational deficits and face recognition abilities.
Case Description
We present the case of a 22-year-old woman, Lost In Space Again (LISA). She had a high school diploma and no previous clinical, neurological, or psychiatric disorders. She was referred for a neuropsychological examination because of her disabling life-long difficulties to orient herself in the environment. The patient has been experiencing difficulties in space orientation since she was a child. She reported difficulties in learning new routes and in mentally representing environments, even of familiar places such as her neighborhood. Moreover, she textually described her difficulties as “a struggle to connect two known places,” or to “retrace a path backward:” she therefore reported some episodes in which she got lost and she had to return to “the starting point” (her home) to properly reach the initial destination. The patient was submitted to a clinical psychological interview, which also provided for the administration of the Cognitive Behavioral Assessment 2.0 [CBA 2.0; (13)] and the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 [MMPI-2; (14)] from which no psychological disorders have been evidenced. LISA did not report any other cognitive or emotional efforts, except for a slight status of anxiety due to navigational deficits. Then, a first comprehensive assessment was performed and, 5 years later, due to the persistence of the orientation difficulties, the patient returned to our observation and she was submitted to a complementary evaluation (second assessment). We obtained the participant's written informed consent. The study was designed in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and received the approval from the Ethics Committee of the University of Bergamo.
First Assessment
A magnetic resonance imaging (MRI—a 1.5-T MRI scanner MAGNETOM Avanto, Siemens, Erlangen, Germany) was performed; the examination did not show any morphological alteration. LISA was then submitted to a first standard neuropsychological (NPS) evaluation (see Table 1).
LISA was alert and cooperative and not impaired in general intelligence, reasoning, and language, but she showed selective deficits that will be addressed in the Discussion section.
Second Assessment
Five years later, LISA was submitted to a second complementary evaluation. Her cognitive status remained essentially unchanged (see Supplementary Material), and the psychological measures [i.e., Beck Depression Inventory, BDI—(37); State-Trait Anxiety Inventory, STAI—(38)] were in the normal range. A diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) was then performed with the same scanner of the previous evaluation using the following scanning procedure: 1. DTI (gradient echo EPI) sequence: 2 mm isotropic voxel, 20 encoding directions, 2 avg/dir, effective b value of 1,000 s/mm2 and 2. high-resolution T1 3D MPRAGE: TR/TE 2,050/2.56 ms, 256 mm FOV, 256 ×256 matrix, and 144 sagittal slices for an effective resolution of 1.0 mm3. All data processing was performed using the FMRIB Software Library (FSL) tools (http://www.fmrib.ox.ac.uk/fsl). Source images of each run were corrected for distortions caused by eddy currents and head motion with an affine registration to the first b = 0 image using the FMRIB's Linear Image Registration Tool (FLIRT) and Diffusion Toolbox. Next, the two runs were concatenated and then averaged. The b = 0 mean image was coregistered to the 3D T1-weighted anatomical image, which was normalized to standardized Montreal Neurological Institute 152 (MNI) space. The fractional anisotropy (FA) maps was calculated using the DTIFIT tool. Probabilistic tractography was run using FSL's BedpostX program, based on a multifiber diffusion model (39) with the aim to explore the visuo-spatial networks proposed by Kravitz et al. (40), investigating the anatomical connection between the Brodmann area (BA) 7 and the BA6 and BA9 and between the posterior cingulate and hippocampus. DTI images are shown in Figure 1.
Figure 1. Diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) images. Tracts (red) reconstructed (10% threshold) in LISA: (A) connection between Brodmann area (BA) 7 (green) and BA6 (light blue); (B) connection between BA7 (green) and BA9 (yellow); (C) connection between the posterior cingulum (blue) and the hippocampus (pink). Coordinates are in Montreal Neurological Institute (MNI) space.
The mean FA values along each right and left fiber tract were extrapolated from patient's DTI data. A group average template, constructed by Rohlfing et al. (41), was used in order to compare patient's results with only an observatory purpose. LISA showed global decrease of FA with respect to the normal subjects in the parieto–prefrontal and in the parieto–premotor pathways. However, comparable (in the right hemisphere) or greater (in the left hemisphere) FA values were observed in the patient as concerned the parieto-medial-temporal pathway. The decrease of FA related to a parieto–prefrontal pathway, which is supposed to support spatial working memory and top-down executive control of visuo-spatial processing, and to a parieto–premotor pathway, involved in visually guided actions (40), might have an impact on her ability to integrate and manipulate different frameworks of spatial information and to form a complex internal space representation.
LISA then performed ecological spatial and navigational tasks (see Table 2). Specifically, in order to test wayfinding abilities in familiar surroundings, she was submitted to two tasks in which she had to describe 12 paths from her home to other places (Wayfinding in familiar routes 1) and 12 paths from one place to another in her town (Wayfinding in familiar routes 2). Then, we used two other ecological navigation tests: in the first task, she had to draw a map of her home and of her hometown to test her ability to retrieve cognitive maps of very familiar environments. An adapted version (i.e., related to the city where the patient lived) of the Postcard Test of Palermo et al. (42), named “The Landmark Test,” was also administered to evaluate patient's ability to recognize landmarks (buildings, palaces, and monuments) or views (streets and squares) of known and unknown places and to relocate them on a blank map of the city.
LISA was finally submitted to a battery of online tests assessing different cognitive skills relevant to DTD, which includes both spatial and face recognition tasks [see www.gettinglost.ca and Iaria and Barton (1) for a detailed description], and to the “Plastic City test,” which was used as a small-scale unfamiliar but more ecological environment [see Rusconi et al. (43) for a detailed description]. LISA's performances are shown in Table 3.
Discussion
We described a new DTD case, LISA, whose assessments revealed a complex clinical picture and presented some significant elements that may be considered for future studies. The NPS evaluation showed a normal general cognitive profile and normal abilities to recognize objects, landmarks, and face expressions and to recall directional information from landmarks in the on-line T. Her difficulties seemed to begin when she had to form, have access, or manipulate a cognitive map in order to reach a destination. Indeed, her performances were altered in the formation and use of a cognitive map and in the path reversed subtest, which requires to form a cognitive map and to perform a manipulation to reverse a route from the final position back to the starting point. These difficulties were confirmed also in the landmark test in which, despite good landmark recognition skills, LISA showed significant difficulties in the relocation of these landmarks on a blank map of her city; the same happened with the small-scale ecological task (i.e., Plastic City), in particular in the city landmark replacement subtest. Moreover, her performance was good when she was required to draw a relatively simple or overlearned cognitive map (i.e., her home) but was poor in the reproduction of a more complex environment such as her neighborhood. Again, when she had to describe some paths giving verbal information, her performance was good only when the starting point was her home (i.e., overlearned path). Conversely, a clear impairment was observed when the starting point was in any other place in her hometown, even when the target point was her home, as if she was able to access an overlearned cognitive path/map, but she was unable to manipulate and rotate this information in order to complete the second task. All this evidence (confirmed also by the altered performance in the Rey recall task) seems to suggest that LISA was actually unable to form quickly a complex internal representation of the environment that would allow her to access the information about landmarks and their mutual metric distances; she was able to form a representation only after multiple presentation of the same environment (i.e., overlearned). According to this, but contrary to the findings of the study performed by Burles and Iaria (2), LISA showed a deficit in the mental rotation test. The capability to perform mental rotations has already been linked to the ability to form cognitive maps: some authors, indeed, evidenced how this imagery ability could help subjects to build spatial relationships between landmarks (42). This can be seen as a first indication that DTD subjects can show different patterns of imagery and spatial abilities and therefore that different subtypes of DTD can be hypothesized (8).
Another interesting aspect, about the possible existence of different DTD subtypes, is that LISA revealed also a poor performance in high-level face recognition abilities: only some of the DTD cases showed this deficit [(4, 6, 9, 11); see (10) for a review]. She seemed able to recognize face expression, which required low-level abilities, but showed significant difficulties in the face identity recognition subtest and in the Benton Face Recognition Test. Grounding on the model proposed by Bruce and Young (45), LISA might have a specific deficit in the structural encoding of faces, which concerns the ability to create a complex mental representation of a pattern of invariant face characteristics against a spared ability to recognize face expression, which instead concerns changeable face characteristics (46). It is also worth noting that previous studies showed that subjects affected by both developmental prosopagnosia (DP) and acquired prosopagnosia (AP) showed concurrent topographical deficit (47, 48). Indeed, even if the comorbidity between DP and navigational difficulties or DTD is not yet known, some evidence does suggest that many subjects might experience both types of difficulties [e.g., Corrow et al. (49); Klargaard et al. (47); Piccardi et al. (7)]. It was already hypothesized that this relationship is possibly driven, on the one hand, by the fact that visual processing of faces and places, even if they are distinct enough to be clearly dissociable, are both ventral stream processes (2); however, this discussion has not been continued to date. For example, also the well-known role of the ventral stream in the processing of far space and allocentric representations and its connection with the dorsal stream (50) might be considered more carefully in future studies. Indeed, subjects with DTD seem to be able, like LISA, to recognize spatial landmarks (a deficit usually found in patients with lesions in these areas), but they seem to have higher-level difficulties that could be explained by alterations in the structural or functional connections with other areas belonging to the spatial network. On the other hand, other authors have evidenced the possible role of the hippocampus that is implicated both in the encoding of new faces (51) and of verbal and spatial information (52, 53), so much so that lesions to this structure or surrounding areas can result in higher-order impairments of both face recognition (54) and/or to the inability to encode and retrieve spatial information about newly learned routes (55). This is in line with the few neuroimaging findings, which have revealed a lower hippocampal and retrosplenial cortex activations during map formation in some DTD subjects (3, 5).
Moreover, from a cognitive point of view, Bate et al. (48) suggested that there could be also an overlapping of cognitive processes involved in both cognitive map formation and face recognition: one of those might be visual imagery skills. Accordingly, we hypothesized that the alteration of mental imagery ability found in LISA might be the possible link between her spatial and face recognition alterations. Consequent to this alteration, LISA was indeed not able to encode, form, and manipulate complex mental images or representations, that could be a face, a scene (environment), or a complex navigational pattern in which the construction of an integrated mental image (cognitive map) was required. It is now clear that some overlaps between the formation of cognitive maps and face recognition abilities do exist and that a possible common developmental defect can be observed; according to this case, mental imagery could then be a prerequisite for the normal development of human navigational skills; however, further studies are needed to investigate this from both cognitive and neural perspectives.
It should be noted that our case presents some limitations: for example, a more in-depth psychological and psychiatric evaluation that could consider the presence of other neurodevelopmental pathologies in comorbidity as well as functional imaging data that could give additional significant information. However, this novel DTD case has certainly the value to bring the attention back to the possible presence of face recognition disorders in patients with DTD [and on the comorbidities with prosopagnosia, see Piccardi et al. (7)] and to highlight again how different forms of this disorder may exist, leading to the hypothesis of the possible existence of a specific taxonomy for individuals with DTD. Future studies should therefore investigate DTD patients both with psychological, psychiatric, behavioral, and functional neuroimaging measures to establish the cognitive and neural profile of different subtypes of DTD; observatory DTI results seemed to suggest also that alterations in the connection between brain areas involved in the human navigational network might be further explored. Comprehensive batteries not only should include specific spatial and navigational tasks but could also investigate the domain of face processing and recognition, as suggested by Burles and Iaria (2), by demanding more ecological stimuli (e.g., short movies, people embedded in spatial and/or social contexts, etc.) in order to deepen our knowledge about the link between these two disorders. Finally, for the future, it could be interesting to evaluate the processing not only of spatial but also of temporal orientation, given the overlapping of processing (i.e., mental cognitive maps) in these domains [see (56)].
Conclusions
Spatial navigation is a complex cognitive skill that plays a key role for the proper functioning of daily activities, allowing individuals to navigate in the environment. Alterations in this capacity, such as those demonstrated in children and adolescents with this specific neurodevelopmental disorder (i.e., DTD), can have adverse developmental consequences and even a reduction in the overall quality of life. Different DTD subtypes seem to exist, and therefore, comprehensive psychological, cognitive, behavioral, and neuroimaging assessments are needed to study in detail the development of different cognitive skills in these patients. This case report indeed suggests that not only spatial ability results to be altered in DTD patients but also concurrent spatial and face recognition deficits can be observed, at least in some cases, and that an alteration in mental imagery skills might represent the developmental defect underlying the two altered abilities. Finally, a better understanding of a specific patient's orientation and face processing alterations could be also fundamental to plan ad hoc rehabilitation programs.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by University of Bergamo. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study. Written informed consent was obtained from the individual(s) for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.
Author Contributions
MLR, AS, FM, and CS designed the experimental procedures. AS administered the tests batteries. CP, CA, and TC worked on the neuroradiological examinations. GF and MLR wrote the article. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
The authors would like to thank LISA for her kind participation.
Supplementary Material
The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.654071/full#supplementary-material
References
1. Iaria G, Barton JJ. Developmental topographical disorientation: a newly discovered cognitive disorder. Exp Brain Res. (2010) 206:189–96. doi: 10.1007/s00221-010-2256-9
2. Burles F, Iaria G. Behavioural and cognitive mechanisms of developmental topographical disorientation. Sci Rep. (2020) 10:1–11. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-77759-8
3. Iaria G, Bogod N, Fox CJ, Barton JJ. Developmental topographical disorientation: case one. Neuropsychologia. (2009) 47:30–40. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.08.021
4. Bianchini F, Incoccia C, Palermo L, Piccardi L, Zompanti L, Sabatini U, et al. Developmental topographical disorientation in a healthy subject. Neuropsychologia. (2010) 48:1563–73. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.01.025
5. Nemmi F, Bianchini F, Piras F, PÈran P, Palermo L, Piccardi L, et al. Finding my own way: an fMRI single case study of a subject with developmental topographical disorientation. Neurocase. (2015) 21:573–83. doi: 10.1080/13554794.2014.960424
6. Conson M, Bianchini F, Quarantelli M, Boccia M, Salzano S, Di Vita A, et al. Selective map-following navigation deficit: a new case of developmental topographical disorientation. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2018) 40:940–50. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2018.1451493
7. Piccardi L, De Luca M, Di Vita A, Palermo L, Tanzilli A, Dacquino C, et al. Evidence of taxonomy for developmental topographical disorientation: developmental landmark agnosia case 1. Appl Neuropsychol Child. (2019) 8:187–98. doi: 10.1080/21622965.2017.1401477
8. Palermo L, Piccardi L, Bianchini F, Nemmi F, Giorgio V, Incoccia C, et al. Looking for the compass in a case of developmental topographical disorientation: a behavioral and neuroimaging study. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2014) 36:464–81. doi: 10.1080/13803395.2014.904843
9. Bianchini F, Palermo L, Piccardi L, Incoccia C, Nemmi F, Sabatini U, et al. Where Am I? A new case of developmental topographical disorientation. J Neuropsychol. (2014) 8:107–24. doi: 10.1111/jnp.12007
10. Fusi G, Crepaldi M, Rusconi ML. Developmental Topographical Disorientation (DTD): A Literature Review. New York, NY: Nova Sc. Pub. (2020).
11. Iaria G, Arnold AE, Burles F, Liu I, Slone E, Barclay S, et al. Developmental topographical disorientation and decreased hippocampal functional connectivity. Hippocampus. (2014) 24:1364–74. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22317
12. Kim JG, Aminoff EM, Kastner S, Behrmann M. A neural basis for developmental topographic disorientation. J Neurosci. (2015) 35:12954–69. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0640-15.2015
13. Zotti AM, Bertolotti G, Michielin P, Sanavio E, Vidotto G. CBA-Cognitive Behavioural Assessment 2.0–Scale primarie. Firenze: Giunti OS (1985).
14. Butcher JN. Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2: Manual for Administration, Scoring, and Interpretation. University of Minnesota Press (2001). doi: 10.1016/B0-08-043076-7/01294-8
15. Nasreddine ZS, Phillips NA, Bédirian V, Charbonneau S, Whitehead V, Collin I, et al. The montreal cognitive assessment, MoCA: a brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J Am Geriatr Soc. (2005) 53:695–9. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005.53221.x
16. Wechsler D. Manual for the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-Revised (WAIS-R). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation (1981).
17. Orsini A, Laicardi JC. WAIS-R. Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale – Revised. Firenze: Giunti Psychometrics, Giunti O.S. (1997).
19. Raven JC. SPM-Standard Progressive Matrices. Firenze: Giunti Psychometrics, Giunti O.S. Ed. (2008).
20. Spinnler H, Tognoni G. Standardizzazione e taratura Italiana di test neuropsicologici. Ital J Neurol Sci. (1987) 8:1–120.
21. Orsini A, Grossi D, Capitani E, Laiacona M, Papagno C, Vallar G. Verbal and spatial immediate memory span: normative data from 1355 adults and 1112 children. Ital J Neurol Sci. (1987) 8:539–48. doi: 10.1007/BF02333660
22. Novelli G, Papagno C, Capitani E, Laiacona M, Vallar G, Cappa SF. Tre test clinici di memoria verbale a lungo termine. Taratura su soggetti normali. Arch Psicol Neurol Psichiatr. (1986) 47:278–96.
23. Carlesimo G, Buccione I, Fadda L, Graceffa A, Mauri M, Lo Russo S, et al. Standardizzazione di due test di memoria per uso clinico: Breve Racconto e Figura di Rey. Riv Neurol. (2002) 12:1–13.
24. Caffarra P, Vezzadini G, Dieci F, Zonato F, Venneri A. Rey-Osterrieth complex figure: normative values in an Italian population sample. Neurol Sci. (2002) 22:443–7. doi: 10.1007/s100720200003
25. Giovagnoli AR, Del Pesce M, Mascheroni S, Simoncelli M, Laiacona M, Capitani E. Trail making test: normative values from 287 normal adult controls. Ital J Neurol Sci. (1996) 17:305–9. doi: 10.1007/BF01997792
26. Zimmerman P, Fimm B. Testbatterie zur Aufmerksamkeitsprufung (TAP). Freiburg: Psytest (1992) (edizione italiana: Batteria di Test per l'esame dell'Attenzione (TEA). Roma: Edizioni Erre, 1994).
27. Mondini S, Mapelli D, Vestri A, Arcara V, Bisiacchi PS. (2012) Esame Neuropsicologico Breve 2. Una batteria di test per lo screening neuropsicologico. Milano: Raffaello Cortina Editore.
28. Della Sala S, MacPherson SE, Phillips LH, Sacco L, Spinnler H. How many camels are there in Italy? Cognitive estimates standardised on the Italian population. Neurol Sci. (2003) 24:10–15. doi: 10.1007/s100720300015
29. Culbertson WC, Zillmer EA. Tower of London. Drexel University. TOLDX. North Tonawanda: Multi-Health Systems (2001).
30. Heaton RK, Chelune GJ, Talley JL. Wisconsin Card Sorting Test Manual-revised and expanded. Lutz, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources (1981).
31. Benton AL, Varney NR, Hamsher KD. Visuospatial judgment. A clinical test. Arch. Neurol (1978) 35:364–7. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1978.00500300038006
32. Benton AL, Abigail B, Sivan AB, Hamsher KD, Varney NR, Spreen O. Contributions to Neuropsychological Assessment: A Clinical Manual. Oxford, MA: Oxford University Press (1994).
33. Ratcliff G. Spatial thought, mental rotation and the right cerebral hemisphere. Neuropsychologia. (1979) 17:49–54. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(79)90021-6
34. Money J, Alexander D, Walker HT. A Standardized Road-Map Test of Direction Sense. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins Press (1965).
35. Marks DF. Visual imagery differences in the recall of pictures. Br J Psychol. (1973) 64:17–24. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1973.tb01322.x
36. Crawford JR, Howell DC. Comparing an individual's test score against norms derived from small samples. Clin Neuropsychol. (1998) 12:482–6. doi: 10.1076/clin.12.4.482.7241
37. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1961) 4:561–71. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004
38. Spielberger CD. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory: Bibliography. 2nd ed. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press (1989).
39. Behrens TE, Berg HJ, Jbabdi S, Rushworth MF, Woolrich MW. Probabilistic diffusion tractography with multiple fibre orientations: what can we gain? Neuroimage. (2007) 34:144–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.09.018
40. Kravitz DJ, Saleem KS, Baker CI, Mishkin M. A new neural framework for visuospatial processing. Nat Rev Neurosci. (2011) 12:217–30. doi: 10.1038/nrn3008
41. Rohlfing T, Zahr NM, Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A. The SRI24 multichannel atlas of normal adult human brain structure. Hum Brain Mapp. (2010) 31:798–819. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20906
42. Palermo L, Iaria G, Guariglia C. Mental imagery skills and topographical orientation in humans: a correlation study. Behav Brain Res. (2008) 192:248–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2008.04.014
43. Rusconi ML, Suardi A, Zanetti M, Rozzini L. Spatial navigation in elderly healthy subjects, amnestic and non amnestic MCI patients. J Neurol Sci. (2015) 359:430–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2015.10.010
44. Crawford JR, Garthwaite PH. Investigation of the single case in neuropsychology: confidence limits on the abnormality of test scores and test score differences. Neuropsychologia. (2002) 40:1196–208. doi: 10.1016/S0028-3932(01)00224-X
45. Bruce V, Young A. Understanding face recognition. Br J Psychol. (1986) 77 (Pt 3):305–27. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1986.tb02199.x
46. Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbin MI. The distributed human neural system for face perception. Trends Cogn Sci. (2000) 4:223–33. doi: 10.1016/S1364-6613(00)01482-0
47. Klargaard SK, Starrfelt R, Petersen A, Gerlach C. Topographic processing in developmental prosopagnosia: preserved perception but impaired memory of scenes. Cogn Neuropsychol. (2016) 33:405–13. doi: 10.1080/02643294.2016.1267000
48. Bate S, Adams A, Bennetts R, Line H. Developmental prosopagnosia with concurrent topographical difficulties: a case report and virtual reality training programme. Neuropsychol Rehabil. (2017) 29:1–23. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2017.1409640
49. Corrow JC, Corrow SL, Lee E, Pancaroglu R, Burles F, Duchaine B, et al. Getting lost: topographic skills in acquired and developmental prosopagnosia. Cortex. (2016) 76:89–103. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.01.003
50. Chen Q, Weidner R, Weiss PH, Marshall JC, Fink GR. Neural interaction between spatial domain and spatial reference frame in parietal-occipital junction. J Cogn Neurosci. (2012) 24:2223–36. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00260
51. Haxby JV, Ungerleider LG, Horwitz B, Maisog JM, Rapoport SI, Grady CL. Face encoding and recognition in the human brain. Proc Nat Acad Sci USA. (1996) 93:922–7. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.2.922
52. Moser MB, Moser EI. Functional differentiation in the hippocampus. Hippocampus. (1998) 8:608–19. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1098-1063(1998)8:6<608::AID-HIPO3>3.0.CO;2-7
53. Eichenbaum H. The role of the hippocampus in navigation is memory. J Neurophysiol. (2017) 117:1785–96. doi: 10.1152/jn.00005.2017
54. Delvenne JF, Seron X, Coyette F, Rossion B. Evidence for perceptual deficits in associative visual (prosop)agnosia: a single-case study. Neuropsychologia. (2004) 42:597–612. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.10.008
55. Habib M, Sirigu A. Pure topographical disorientation: a definition and anatomical basis. Cortex. (1987) 23:73–85. doi: 10.1016/S0010-9452(87)80020-5
Keywords: developmental topographical disorientation, neurodevelopmental disorders, cognitive map, face recognition, spatial navigation, case report
Citation: Rusconi ML, Fusi G, Stampatori C, Suardi A, Pinardi C, Ambrosi C, Costa T and Mattioli F (2021) Developmental Topographical Disorientation With Concurrent Face Recognition Deficit: A Case Report. Front. Psychiatry 12:654071. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.654071
Received: 15 January 2021; Accepted: 17 May 2021;
Published: 25 June 2021.
Edited by:
Laura Piccardi, Sapienza University of Rome, ItalyReviewed by:
Felipe Ortuño, University of Navarra, SpainMaria Arioli, University of Milano-Bicocca, Italy
Copyright © 2021 Rusconi, Fusi, Stampatori, Suardi, Pinardi, Ambrosi, Costa and Mattioli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Maria Luisa Rusconi, marialuisa.rusconi@unibg.it