Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 05 March 2021
Sec. Aging Psychiatry

Differential Impact of Education on Gray Matter Volume According to Sex in Cognitively Normal Older Adults: Whole Brain Surface-Based Morphometry

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Seoul St. Mary's Hospital, College of Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Yeouido St. Mary's Hospital, College of Medicine, The Catholic University of Korea, Seoul, South Korea

Background: The effect of educational status on brain structural measurements depends on demographic and clinical factors in cognitively healthy older adults.

Objectives: The current study aimed to evaluate the impact of interaction between years of education and sex on gray matter volume and to investigate whether cortical volume has a differential impact on cognitive function according to sex.

Methods: One hundred twenty-one subjects between 60 and 85 years old were included in this study. Gray matter volume was evaluated by whole brain surface-based morphometry. Multiple regression analysis was used to analyze the effects of sex-cortical volume interactions on cognitive functions.

Results: There was a significant interaction between years of education and sex on the cortical volume of the left inferior temporal gyrus after adjusting for age, APOE ε4 allele prevalence, and total intracranial volume. In addition, we found a significant impact of the interaction between adjusted left inferior temporal volume and sex on CERAD-K total scores.

Conclusion: These findings have significant implications for the understanding of how sex could affect the role of cognitive reserve for cortical atrophy in cognitively intact older adults.

Introduction

The concept of reserve has emerged to explain the differential impact of neuropathological changes due to the aging process and neurodegenerative disease on individual clinical outcome, including cognitive decline and decreased ability to perform daily functions (1). Brain reserves, which are based on the brain's quantitative characteristics, and cognitive reserves, which focus on the brain's adaptive functions, are the two broad classifications of the reserve (2). The cognitive reserve has been reported to play a neuroprotective and compensatory role for neuropathologic change accompanying the aging process (3). Educational status, one of the representative proxies of cognitive reserve, can be evaluated as an objective number that represents the ease of acquiring information (4, 5). Previous studies have demonstrated that fewer years of education increase the prevalence of dementia (6), and higher levels of education delay the occurrence of Alzheimer's disease (AD) through tolerance against the neuropathologic change of AD (7).

Among the brain's structural parameters, gray matter volume is a representative surrogate reflecting the aging process and pathological change (8, 9). Years of education have also been correlated with the gray matter volume of several brain regions, including the medial and frontal gyri, superior temporal gyrus, inferior parietal lobule, and the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex in older adults with normal cognitive function (1012). Moreover, another previous study has reported the interaction between education and total brain volume on cognitive decline in cognitively intact older adults (13).

Prior studies have reported that the effect of educational status on brain structural measurements depends on demographic and clinical factors in older adults with healthy cognitive function. Age has been shown to interact with education in relation to cortical gray matter volume in the left middle frontal gyrus and right medial cingulate cortex (12). In addition, the impact of education on hippocampal volume differs according to the presence of amyloid-beta (Aβ) retention, and the degree of Aβ deposition has been associated with cortical volume (14).

Available evidence has described the effect of sex on cortical volume in cognitively normal older adults. Female subjects are shown to have greater superior temporal, inferior frontal gyrus, and cingulate cortex volume; male subjects demonstrate increased cortical volume in bilateral temporal lobes and entorhinal and perirhinal cortices (15). Additionally, the relevance of sex for age-related changes in cortical volume has also been documented (16). Whereas, males showed greater volume loss in frontal and temporal lobes, females displayed greater atrophy of the hippocampus and parietal lobe. Despite the significant effects of sex on age-related volume loss, few studies have determined whether the effect of education on gray matter volume differs according to sex.

The current study aimed to evaluate the impact of the interaction between years of education and sex on gray matter volume by whole brain analysis in cognitively intact older adults. Additionally, we set out to investigate whether the cortical volume of brain regions showing significant interaction had a differential impact on cognitive function according to sex.

Materials and Methods

Participants

One hundred twenty-one subjects between 60 and 85 years old were included in this study. Subjects were recruited from the Catholic Brain Health Center MRI database, which was built through the outpatient psycho-geriatric clinic of Yeouido Saint Mary's Hospital located in Seoul, Republic of Korea, from October 2018 through December 2020. The cognitive function of all subjects was assessed using the Korean version of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease (CERAD-K) (17). Measures included assessment in the Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE-K) (18), Word List Memory (WLM), Word List Recall (WLR), Word List Recognition (WLRc). In addition, total memory (TM) domain scores were obtained by summing scores from the CERAD-K, WLM, WLR, and WLRc. Total CERAD-K scores were calculated by summing all CERAD-K subcategory scores, excluding the MMSE-K score. Details surrounding the usage of specific tests and the reviewing process are described in Supplementary Material. Inclusion criteria were as follows: (1) Participants with or without subjective memory complaints, beyond what would be expected for age, (2) Normal memory function, quantified by scoring above age, sex, and education adjusted cutoffs on the WLM, WLR, and WLRc domain, (3) MMSE-K score between 24 and 30, (4) Clinical Dementia Rating score of 0, (5) Memory Box score of 0, and (6) normal cognitive function, based on the absence of significant impairment in cognitive functions or activities of daily living. We excluded participants with any history of alcoholism, drug abuse, head trauma, or psychiatric disorders, those taking any psychotropic medications (e.g., cholinesterase inhibitors, antidepressants, benzodiazepines, and antipsychotics), those with multiple vascular risk factors, and those with extensive cerebrovascular disease. T2-weighted fluid-attenuated inversion recovery (FLAIR) data were acquired to objectively exclude vascular lesions or other diseases. The study was conducted under the ethical and safety guidelines set forth by the Institutional Review Board of The Catholic University of Korea, which approved all research activity. Informed and written consent was obtained from all participants.

Data Acquisition and Preparation

Imaging data from the Catholic Brain Health Center MRI database were collected by the Department of Radiology of Yeouido Saint Mary's Hospital at the Catholic University of Korea using a 3 T Siemens Skyra MRI machine and a 32-channel Siemens head coil (Siemens Medical Solutions, Erlangen, Germany). One hundred twenty-one subjects were imaged with the T1-weighted magnetization-prepared rapid gradient-echo (MP-RAGE) sequence using the following parameters: image size = 224 × 224 × 256, voxel size = 0.9 × 0.9 × 0.9 mm3, repetition time (TR) = 1,940 ms, echo time (TE) = 2.6 ms, and flip angle = 9°. In addition, FLAIR data were collected using a 2D fast spin-echo sequence with the following parameters: TE = 76 ms, TR = 9 s, inversion time = 2.5 s, FOV = 22 cm × 22 cm, 42 oblique axial slices, slice thickness = 5 mm, image size = 224 × 224 × 256.

Image Processing

FreeSurfer software (version 7.1.0, https://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu) was used to reconstruct and co-register the cortical surfaces and estimate brain structural features including cortical thickness, cortical volume, and surface area. Image processing for the cortical model was done in the following order: removal of non-brain tissue using a hybrid watershed algorithm (19), bias field correction, automated Talairach transformation, segmentation of subcortical white matter and deep gray matter structures (20, 21), intensity normalization, tessellation of the gray/white matter boundary and gray/cerebrospinal fluid (CSF) boundary, automated topology correction (19, 22), and surface deformation following intensity gradients to optimally place the gray/white and gray/CSF borders at the location where the greatest shift in intensity defines the transition to the other tissue class (23). Individual cortical folding patterns were then registered to a spherical atlas in order to match cortical geometry across subjects. Thickness was calculated at each location of the cortex as the distance between white matter and the pial surface (23). Procedures for the measurement of cortical thickness have been validated against histological analysis and manual measurements (24, 25). The cortical volume is defined as the product of cortical thickness and area. All data were smoothed with a 10 mm full width half maximum (FWHM) Gaussian kernel, and the cerebral cortex was parcellated based on gyral and sulcal information derived from manually traced brains (21, 26). These procedures are well-prescribed in a related paper (27).

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses for demographic data were performed with R software (version 2.15.3). Assumptions of normality were tested for continuous variables using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test; all demonstrated a normal distribution. Two sample t-tests and chi-square (χ2) tests were used to probe for differences between the male (n = 41) and female groups (n = 80) in terms of demographic variables, clinical data, and total intracranial volume (ICV). All statistical analyses used a two-tailed level of 0.05 for defining statistical significance.

FreeSurfer software (Version 7.1.0, http://surfer.nmr.mgh.harvard.edu/) was used for group analysis. Surface-based normalization was computed to map cortical volume data for each subject onto a common group space to allow comparison across subjects at homologous points on the cortex. Cortical anatomy measurements were then smoothed with FWHM = 10 mm, and the GLM was fit to the data with age, considering the APOE ε4 allele and total ICV as covariates. The vertex-wise statistical test was performed to evaluate the impact of the interaction between years of education and sex on cortical volumes. The uncorrected significance map with p < 0.001 was then overlapped onto the fsaverage brain template surface for visualization with a cluster-extent threshold of 100 mm2. In addition, we performed the False Discovery Rate (FDR) procedure for the multiple comparison correction (corrected p < 0.05). Furthermore, we performed multiple linear regression analyses to evaluate the effect of adjusted cortical volumes in regions of interest-by-sex interaction on neuropsychological test scores with a backward stepwise regression analysis (p < 0.05). Adjusted cortical volume = raw cortical volume – β × (intracranial volume [ICV]–mean ICV), where β is the regression slope of the region of interest ICV vs. neuropsychological test scores (28).

Results

Baseline Demographic and Clinical Data

Table 1 shows the baseline demographic and clinical data for the male and female groups. There were no significant differences in age, years of education, and APOE ε4 genotype between two groups. In regard to the neuropsychological battery, female group showed higher scores of CERAD-K WLM, WLR, and TM than the male group. In addition, the male group exhibited larger total intracranial volume than the female group.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of cognitively normal older adults.

Sex-By-Years of Education Interaction for Gray Matter Volumes

After adjusting for age, APOE ε4 genotype, and total intracranial volume, the interaction between educational status and sex demonstrated a significant effect on the cortical volume of the left inferior temporal gyrus, right postcentral gyrus, and right superior parietal lobule (Figure 1). However, only the cluster size of the left inferior temporal gyrus exceeded 100 mm2 (Table 2). None of these interactions survived FDR correction for multiple comparisons. In addition, the regression slopes between years of education and cortical volume differed between the sexes. As expected from the visual inspection of the results shown in Figure 2, higher educational level was correlated with the greater cortical volume of the left inferior temporal gyrus in the male group. However, in the female group, the higher years of education were associated with the decreased volume of the left inferior temporal gyrus.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Brain regions with a significant impact of an interaction between years of education and sex on a cortical volume, adjusted for age, APOE ε4 allele, and total intracranial volume (uncorrected p < 0.001, false discovery rate-corrected p > 0.05). General linear model using QDEC tool available in the Freesurfer software. Blue color represents a negative association of an education year with cortical volumes in female participants and a positive association in male participants. Red color represents an opposing association with that described by red color.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Brain regions significantly impacted by the interaction between years of education year and sex on a cortical volume, adjusted for age, APOE ε4 allele prevalence, and total intracranial volume (uncorrected p < 0.001, false discovery rate-corrected p > 0.05).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Impact of interaction between years of education and sex on cortical volume of the left inferior temporal gyrus. General linear model using QDEC tool available in the Freesurfer software. Uncorrected p < 0.001, false discovery rate-corrected p > 0.05.

Gray Matter Volume-By-Sex Interaction for Cognitive Functions

After controlling for age, years of education and APOE ε4 allele presence, we found a significant impact of interaction between adjusted left inferior temporal volume and sex on CERAD-K total scores (Figure 3). However, there was no main effect of interaction on other memory domains of the CERAD-K.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Impact of interaction between adjusted left inferior temporal volume and sex on cognitive function. Multiple linear regression analysis was used to predict the impact of interaction between adjusted left inferior temporal volume and sex on cognitive function after adjusting for age, years of education, and APOE ε4 allele prevalence. Stepwise backward elimination and all subsets regression were performed to select final model.

Discussion

The present study aimed to evaluate whether the association between years of education and gray matter volume differs depending on sex in cognitively normal older adults. In addition, this study was designed to examine the brain regions that show years of education-by-sex interaction for gray matter volume through whole-brain analysis based on surface-morphometry.

The current paper found that the relationship between educational status and the cortical volume of the left inferior temporal gyrus differed significantly according to sex in cognitively healthy older adults. In older male participants, the correlation slope between years of education and cortical volume was positive, however, this slope was negative for older female participants. Although the cluster size was <100 mm3, the interaction found in the right postcentral gyrus and superior parietal lobe was observed in the opposite direction compared to that of the left inferior temporal gyrus. While the left inferior temporal gyrus has been reported to be generally tolerant to healthy aging (29) yet vulnerable to AD pathology (30, 31), the postcentral gyrus and superior parietal lobe have been demonstrated to show accelerated loss of cortical volume more than global atrophy present during the aging process (32). In this regard, the findings of this study suggest that greater years of education are associated with lesser cortical volume in brain regions that are relatively sensitive to AD pathology compared to the healthy aging process for older female participants.

Although the present research does not evaluate the presence of Aβ and tau retention, older female participants with healthy cognition have displayed a higher prevalence of Aβ and tau deposition than males between the ages of 60 and 85 (33). Additionally, prior studies have indicated that cognitively normal older adults with Aβ accumulation show greater atrophy in the hippocampus and supramarginal gyrus compared to those without Aβ retention (14). According to the cognitive reserve hypothesis, older adults with higher years of education could tolerate more AD-induced pathological change through compensatory mechanisms than those with comparable cognitive status (14). With the aforementioned propositions, it is estimated that more prevalent brain pathological changes in AD-vulnerable regions of older female subjects could have an effect on the negative association between years of education and cortical volume in the female group of the current study. Moreover, the inferior temporal gyrus is a Braak stage IV area, where neurofibrillary tangles associated with Aβ retention and cognitive impairment are accumulated (34, 35). Previous evidence has also demonstrated the relationship between this aforementioned tauopathy and cortical atrophy in the aging brain (36). Therefore, it is necessary to interpret the results of this study through the application of the cognitive reserve hypothesis for the two representative pathologies of AD. Since these pathologies were not evaluated in this study, additional studies to complement them should be conducted to enhance our understanding of the differential role of education in cortical atrophy according to sex.

Another important finding uncovered by the present study was the significant interaction between the inferior temporal gyrus volume and sex for global cognitive function in cognitively normal older adults. In a more detail, the cognitive function scores decreased more in the male group than in the female group when the cortical atrophy in the left inferior temporal gyrus progressed. This finding is consistent with the previous research, confirming that male sex is related to worse memory in cognitively intact older adults (37). Given that the inferior temporal gyrus is more vulnerable to AD pathology than to normal aging (30, 31), the compensatory process through cognitive reserve might be more activated to delay cognitive decline in females with relatively high AD pathology. However, as mentioned above, since AD pathology was not evaluated in this study, attention should be paid to interpretation. In addition, further research should be conducted to evaluate a brain functional change, which is closely related to AD pathology and cognition (38, 39).

Finally, several limitations need to be considered. Firstly, lifestyle factors (alcohol consumption, smoking, physical exercise etc.) (40, 41) and vascular risk factors (42), which might show differences between male and female subjects, could influence brain structure. In particular, physical exercise has been reported to increase the size of hippocampus, improve memory performances, and delay the onset of dementia (43, 44). In this regard, physical exercise has been also suggested as another proxy for cognitive reserve (3). Therefore, the possibility that these factors may have influenced the results of this study cannot be excluded. Secondly, this study evaluated only years of education as a proxy for cognitive reserve. Therefore, an additional study to comprehensively evaluate other brain/cognitive reserve proxies, including the aforementioned lifestyle and vascular risk factors, is necessary for further understanding.

The purpose of the current study was to determine the effect of education years-by sex interaction on gray matter volume by whole-brain analysis in cognitively intact older adults and to evaluate the distinctive sex-dependent impact of cortical volume on cognitive function. This study has identified the relevancy of the interaction between education and sex for gray matter volume of AD-vulnerable regions and has shown the differential impact of cortical volume on cognitive function according to sex. These findings have significant implications for the understanding of how sex may affect the role of cognitive reserve in differential cortical atrophy for cognitively intact older adults. In addition, alongside the supplementation of the aforementioned limitations, this research serves as a base for the deeper understanding of cognitive reserve in the aging and neurodegenerative disease processes.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Institutional Review Board of The Catholic University of Korea. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

DK, S-MW, CL, and HL conceived and designed the research. DK, S-MW, H-RN, NK, and HL recruited subjects, and followed subjects to get clinical results. NK performed the in vivo MRI experiments. DK and S-MW performed the image preprocessing and image analysis. DK, S-MW, and NK performed statistical analysis. DK wrote the manuscript. CL and HL provided scientific mentorship throughout the project. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) grant funded by the Korea government (Ministry of Science and ICT) (No. 2019R1A2C2009100 and 2019R1C1C1007608). The funding organizations did not participate in study design or data interpretation.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.644148/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve. Neuropsychologia. (2009) 47:2015–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Stern Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer's disease. Lancet Neurol. (2012) 11:1006–12. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(12)70191-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Wirth M, Chételat G. Cognitive reserve and lifestyle: moving towards preclinical Alzheimer's disease. Front Aging Neurosci. (2015) 7:134. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2015.00134

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Stern Y, Alexander GE, Prohovnik I, Mayeux R. Inverse relationship between education and parietotemporal perfusion deficit in Alzheimer's disease. Ann Neurol. (1992) 32:371–5. doi: 10.1002/ana.410320311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Garibotto V, Borroni B, Kalbe E, Herholz K, Salmon E, Holtoff V, et al. Education and occupation as proxies for reserve in aMCI converters and AD: FDG-PET evidence. Neurology. (2008) 71:1342–9. doi: 10.1212/01.wnl.0000327670.62378.c0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Zhang M, Katzman R, Salmon D, Jin H, Cai G, Wang Z, et al. The prevalence of dementia and Alzheimer's disease in Shanghai, China: impact of age, gender, and education. Ann Neurol. (1990) 27:428–37. doi: 10.1002/ana.410270412

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Meng X, D'arcy C. Education and dementia in the context of the cognitive reserve hypothesis: a systematic review with meta-analyses and qualitative analyses. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e38268. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0038268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Storsve AB, Fjell AM, Tamnes CK, Westlye LT, Overbye K, Aasland HW, et al. Differential longitudinal changes in cortical thickness, surface area and volume across the adult life span: regions of accelerating and decelerating change. J Neurosci. (2014) 34:8488–98. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0391-14.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Storandt M, Mintun MA, Head D, Morris JC. Cognitive decline and brain volume loss as signatures of cerebral amyloid-β peptide deposition identified with Pittsburgh compound B: cognitive decline associated with Aβ deposition. Arch Neurol. (2009) 66:1476–81. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2009.272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Landeau B, La Joie R, Mevel K, Mézenge F, Perrotin A, et al. Relationships between years of education and gray matter volume, metabolism and functional connectivity in healthy elders. Neuroimage. (2013) 83:450–7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.06.053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Solé-Padullés C, Bartrés-Faz D, Junqué C, Vendrell P, Rami L, Clemente IC, et al. Brain structure and function related to cognitive reserve variables in normal aging, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. (2009) 30:1114–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Boller B, Mellah S, Ducharme-Laliberté G, Belleville S. Relationships between years of education, regional grey matter volumes, and working memory-related brain activity in healthy older adults. Brain Imaging Behav. (2017) 11:304–17. doi: 10.1007/s11682-016-9621-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Roe CM, Fagan AM, Grant EA, Marcus DS, Benzinger TL, Mintun MA, et al. Cerebrospinal fluid biomarkers, education, brain volume, and future cognition. Arch Neurol. (2011) 68:1145–51. doi: 10.1001/archneurol.2011.192

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Arenaza-Urquijo EM, Molinuevo J-L, Sala-Llonch R, Sole-Padulles C, Balasa M, Bosch B, et al. Cognitive reserve proxies relate to gray matter loss in cognitively healthy elderly with abnormal cerebrospinal fluid amyloid-β levels. J Alzheimer's Dis. (2013) 35:715–26. doi: 10.3233/JAD-121906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Good CD, Johnsrude I, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. Cerebral asymmetry and the effects of sex and handedness on brain structure: a voxel-based morphometric analysis of 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. (2001) 14:685–700. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0857

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Murphy DG, DeCarli C, Mclntosh AR, Daly E, Mentis MJ, Pietrini P, et al. Sex differences in human brain morphometry and metabolism: an in vivo quantitative magnetic resonance imaging and positron emission tomography study on the effect of aging. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1996) 53:585–94. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1996.01830070031007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Lee JH, Lee KU, Lee DY, Kim KW, Jhoo JH, Kim JH, et al. Development of the Korean Version of the Consortium to Establish a Registry for Alzheimer's Disease Assessment Packet (CERAD-K) clinical and neuropsychological assessment batteries. J Gerontol Ser B. (2002) 57:47–53. doi: 10.1093/geronb/57.1.P47

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Park J-H. Standardization of Korean version of the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE-K) for use in the elderly. Part II. Diagnostic validity. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc. (1989) 28:508–13.

Google Scholar

19. Ségonne F, Dale AM, Busa E, Glessner M, Salat D, Hahn HK, et al. A hybrid approach to the skull stripping problem in MRI. Neuroimage. (2004) 22:1060–75. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Fischl B, Salat DH, Busa E, Albert M, Dieterich M, Haselgrove C, et al. Whole brain segmentation: automated labeling of neuroanatomical structures in the human brain. Neuron. (2002) 33:341–55. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00569-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Fischl B, Salat DH, Van Der Kouwe AJ, Makris N, Ségonne F, Quinn BT, et al. Sequence-independent segmentation of magnetic resonance images. Neuroimage. (2004) 23:69–84. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.07.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Fischl B, Liu A, Dale AM. Automated manifold surgery: constructing geometrically accurate and topologically correct models of the human cerebral cortex. IEEE Trans Med Imaging. (2001) 20:70–80. doi: 10.1109/42.906426

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Fischl B, Dale AM. Measuring the thickness of the human cerebral cortex from magnetic resonance images. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2000) 97:11050–5. doi: 10.1073/pnas.200033797

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Rosas H, Liu A, Hersch S, Glessner M, Ferrante R, Salat D, et al. Regional and progressive thinning of the cortical ribbon in Huntington's disease. Neurology. (2002) 58:695–701. doi: 10.1212/WNL.58.5.695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Kuperberg GR, Broome MR, McGuire PK, David AS, Eddy M, Ozawa F, et al. Regionally localized thinning of the cerebral cortex in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2003) 60:878–88. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.878

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Desikan RS, Ségonne F, Fischl B, Quinn BT, Dickerson BC, Blacker D, et al. An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. (2006) 31:968–80. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Fischl B. FreeSurfer. Neuroimage. (2012) 62:774–81. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.01.021

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Raz N, Lindenberger U, Rodrigue KM, Kennedy KM, Head D, Williamson A, et al. Regional brain changes in aging healthy adults: general trends, individual differences and modifiers. Cereb Cortex. (2005) 15:1676–89. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhi044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Raz N, Torres IJ, Spencer WD, Acker JD. Pathoclysis in aging human cerebral cortex: evidence from in vivo MRI morphometry. Psychobiology. (1993) 21:151–60.

Google Scholar

30. McDonald C, McEvoy L, Gharapetian L, Fennema-Notestine C, Hagler D, Holland D, et al. Regional rates of neocorticalatrophy from normal aging to early Alzheimer disease. Neurology. (2009) 73:457–65. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3181b16431

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Chételat G, Landeau B, Eustache F, Mézenge F, Viader F, de La Sayette V, et al. Using voxel-based morphometry to map the structural changes associated with rapid conversion in MCI: a longitudinal MRI study. Neuroimage. (2005) 27:934–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage. (2001) 14:21–36. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Jack CR Jr, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Therneau TM, Knopman DS, Lowe V, et al. Age-specific and sex-specific prevalence of cerebral β-amyloidosis, tauopathy, and neurodegeneration in cognitively unimpaired individuals aged 50–95 years: a cross-sectional study. Lancet Neurol. (2017) 16:435–44. doi: 10.1016/S1474-4422(17)30077-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Cho H, Choi JY, Hwang MS, Kim YJ, Lee HM, Lee HS, et al. In vivo cortical spreading pattern of tau and amyloid in the Alzheimer disease spectrum. Ann Neurol. (2016) 80:247–58. doi: 10.1002/ana.24711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Johnson KA, Schultz A, Betensky RA, Becker JA, Sepulcre J, Rentz D, et al. Tau positron emission tomographic imaging in aging and early Alzheimer disease. Ann Neurol. (2016) 79:110–9. doi: 10.1002/ana.24546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Sepulcre J, Schultz AP, Sabuncu M, Gomez-Isla T, Chhatwal J, Becker A, et al. In vivo tau, amyloid, and gray matter profiles in the aging brain. J Neurosci. (2016) 36:7364–74. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0639-16.2016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Jack CR, Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Knopman DS, Vemuri P, Mielke MM, et al. Age, sex, and APOE ε4 effects on memory, brain structure, and β-amyloid across the adult life span. JAMA Neurol. (2015) 72:511–9. doi: 10.1001/jamaneurol.2014.4821

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Mormino EC, Smiljic A, Hayenga AO, H. Onami S, Greicius MD, Rabinovici GD, et al. Relationships between beta-amyloid and functional connectivity in different components of the default mode network in aging. Cereb Cortex. (2011) 21:2399–407. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhr025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Damoiseaux JS, Prater KE, Miller BL, Greicius MD. Functional connectivity tracks clinical deterioration in Alzheimer's disease. Neurobiol Aging. (2012) 33:828. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Gow AJ, Bastin ME, Maniega SM, Hernández MCV, Morris Z, Murray C, et al. Neuroprotective lifestyles and the aging brain: activity, atrophy, and white matter integrity. Neurology. (2012) 79:1802–8. doi: 10.1212/WNL.0b013e3182703fd2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Bugg JM, Head D. Exercise moderates age-related atrophy of the medial temporal lobe. Neurobiol Aging. (2011) 32:506–14. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2009.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Barnes J, Carmichael OT, Leung KK, Schwarz C, Ridgway GR, Bartlett JW, et al. Vascular and Alzheimer's disease markers independently predict brain atrophy rate in Alzheimer's Disease Neuroimaging Initiative controls. Neurobiol Aging. (2013) 34:1996–2002. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2013.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Erickson KI, Voss MW, Prakash RS, Basak C, Szabo A, Chaddock L, et al. Exercise training increases size of hippocampus and improves memory. Proc Natl Acad Sci. (2011) 108:3017–22. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1015950108

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Cheng S-T. Cognitive reserve and the prevention of dementia: the role of physical and cognitive activities. Curr Psychiatry Rep. (2016) 18:1–12. doi: 10.1007/s11920-016-0721-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: education, sex, gray matter volume, cognitively normal older adults, surface-based morphometry

Citation: Kang DW, Wang S-M, Na H-R, Kim N-Y, Lim HK and Lee CU (2021) Differential Impact of Education on Gray Matter Volume According to Sex in Cognitively Normal Older Adults: Whole Brain Surface-Based Morphometry. Front. Psychiatry 12:644148. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.644148

Received: 20 December 2020; Accepted: 08 February 2021;
Published: 05 March 2021.

Edited by:

Francesco Panza, University of Bari Aldo Moro, Italy

Reviewed by:

Luca Cravello, ASST Rhodense, Italy
Giulia Donzuso, University of Catania, Italy

Copyright © 2021 Kang, Wang, Na, Kim, Lim and Lee. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Chang Uk Lee, jihan@catholic.ac.kr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.