Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 28 January 2021
Sec. Addictive Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Control and Reward Processing in Addiction View all 10 articles

Monetary Reward Discounting, Inhibitory Control, and Trait Impulsivity in Young Adults With Internet Gaming Disorder and Nicotine Dependence

\nWan-Sen Yan
Wan-Sen Yan*Ruo-Ting ChenRuo-Ting ChenMeng-Meng LiuMeng-Meng LiuDan-Hui ZhengDan-Hui Zheng
  • Department of Psychology, School of Medical Humanitarians, Guizhou Medical University, Guiyang, China

Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) has been considered a potential behavioral or non-substance addiction that requires further investigation. Recognition of the commonalities between IGD and Substance Use disorders (SUD) would be of great help to better understand the basic mechanisms of addictive behaviors and excessive Internet gaming. However, little research has targeted a straightforward contrast between IGD and SUD on neuropsychological aspects. The present study thus aimed to explore the associations of reward processing and inhibitory control with IGD and nicotine dependence (ND) in young adults. Fifty-eight IGD and 53 ND individuals, as well as 57 age- and gender-matched healthy controls, were assessed with a series of measurements including the Delay-discounting Test (DDT), Probability Discounting Test (PDT), the Stroop Color-Word Task, a revised Go/No Go Task, and the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11). Multivariate analysis of variance (mANOVA) models revealed that both IGD and ND groups scored higher than healthy controls on the BIS-11 attentional, motor, and non-planning impulsiveness (Cohen's d = 0.41–1.75). Higher degrees of delay discounting on the DDT were also found in IGD and ND groups compared to healthy controls (Cohen's d = 0.53–0.69). Although IGD group did not differ from healthy controls on the PDT, ND group had a lower degree of probability discounting than healthy controls (Cohen's d = 0.55), suggesting a reduction in risk aversion. Furthermore, ND subjects showed a lower correct accuracy in the incongruent trials of the Stroop task than healthy controls (Cohen's d = 0.61). On the Go/No Go task, both IGD and ND groups had a lower correct accuracy in the No-Go trials than healthy controls (Cohen's d = 1.35–1.50), indicating compromised response inhibition. These findings suggested that IGD was linked to both anomalous reward discounting and dysfunctional inhibitory control, which was comparable with one typical SUD category (i.e., ND). This study might promote a better understanding of the pathogenesis of IGD as a potential addictive disorder similar to SUD.

Introduction

Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) has been included as a tentative behavioral or non-substance addiction that warrants further research before it can be accepted as a full disorder in the latest revision of the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (i.e., DSM-5) (1). More recently, IGD was proposed in the list of addictive conditions and was formally recognized as Gaming Disorder in the 11th revision of the International Classification of Diseases (i.e., ICD-11) (24). Both in the DSM-5 and ICD-11, IGD is characterized by a pattern of persistent and disordered gaming behavior, which leads to significant clinical impairments within a period of at least 12 months (5, 6). To be diagnosed as IGD in the DSM-5, five of the nine diagnosis criteria (i.e., preoccupation, withdrawal, tolerance, loss of control, loss of interest or giving up other activities, continued overuse, deception, escape of negative feelings, negative consequences) must be endorsed within a 12-month period (1). Prevalence estimates of IGD among general samples have been always below 5% (7, 8), with a low of 0.5% and a high of 10% (9). In recent meta-analysis studies, the global prevalence of IGD was reported to be about 3.05% with significant variability (10), ranging from 0.21 to 57.5% in general populations, 3.2–91.0% in clinical populations, and 50.42–79.25% in populations undergoing intervention (i.e., severe cases) (11).

As a putative non-substance addiction, IGD has led to a large number of issues, concerns, and scientific dialogues from experts in the field (1216). Although IGD seems to share many clinical manifestations with Substance Use disorders (SUD) in terms of etiology, biology, and treatment (13, 1719), it remains a highly controversial topic whether IGD should qualify as a new clinical disorder (12, 20, 21), and a wider range of empirical studies are needed to clarify the theoretical underpinnings of IGD (2). In a manner, understanding the biological, psychological, and social processes underlying different forms of addictive behaviors stands to capture the core elements of IGD, such as on the commonalities and distinctions between IGD and SUD (22).

However, little research by now has targeted a straightforward contrast between IGD and other well-identified addictions on neuropsychological aspects. Considering the core features of impulsivity and compulsivity involved in addictive behaviors, one prior study has tried to detect the similarities and differences among male patients with IGD, Gambling Disorder (GD), and Alcohol Use Disorder (AUD) compared to healthy controls with a small sample size (23). It was reported that the IGD and AUD groups had higher impulsivity scores on the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) and showed decreased proportions of successful stops on the Stop-Signal Test than the healthy controls, while only the GD group made more errors on the compulsivity test (i.e., the Intra-Extra Dimensional Set Shift Test) compared with healthy controls (23). Another latest study assessed trait impulsivity, delay discounting, and decision making between patients with IGD and GD compared to healthy controls, reporting that IGD and GD groups did not differ from healthy controls on the BIS-11, but both groups displayed a steeper delay curve (i.e., a higher discounting degree) on the Delay Discounting Task (DDT) (24). Despite these preliminary evidence, recognition of the commonalities between IGD and SUD/GD would be of great help to better understand the basic mechanisms of IGD from a cross-spectrum view (25).

Relative to other populations, adolescents and young adults have been found to be more susceptible to IGD because of their age-related immaturity of cognitive control as well as their easy access to the Internet during this period (2631). Analogously, cigarette smoking behavior (or even worse, Nicotine Dependence) as one kind of SUD categories has also been available and increasing in youths from middle schools to universities (3235), sometimes equally between males and females (36). In many cases, Nicotine Dependence (ND) and IGD tend to co-occur in young men (37, 38), and there is a high co-occurrence of cigarette smoking with IGD in both adults and adolescents (39). Interestingly, although significant correlations of IGD with various forms of SUD including nicotine, alcohol, caffeine, and cannabis use were found in the adult and elder populations (39), cigarette smoking, rather than other substance use, was strongly associated with IGD in the adolescent and younger populations (40). Moreover, cue-induced smoking craving and gaming urge showed similar neurobiological correlates (e.g., higher parahippocampus activation) in young adult subjects comorbid with ND and IGD (37), and young ND and IGD individuals shared decreased resting-state functional connectivity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex with the right insula and left inferior frontal gyrus, which are related to craving and impulsive inhibitions (41). Nevertheless, the common and distinct aspects of neuropsychological characteristics between IGD and ND are not well-acknowledged given the scarce evidence with a direct comparison between them.

According to recent neurocognitive models of addiction, the neural substrates implicated in addictive behaviors might include multiple brain systems that govern reward seeking/risk taking, craving and cognitive control (42, 43). Indeed, individuals with IGD are often characterized by heightened reward-seeking, persistent craving, and decreased executive control (4446). Furthermore, the developmental theories of adolescent brains highlighted the imbalance between a salient reward-seeking system and a hypoactive executive-control system, which is closely associated with various risky behaviors including IGD during adolescence and early adulthood (47, 48). Thus, it is necessary to extend the neurocognitive underpinnings of IGD by evaluating both reward processing and cognitive control among adolescents and young adults with IGD, especially in direct contrast to those with other addictive behaviors (e.g., ND).

The Delay-discounting Test (DDT) (49) is a widely-used reward choice task that assesses the ability of delay of gratification by choosing between immediate and prospective monetary rewards (50). Similarly, the Probability Discounting Test (PDT) (51) evaluates the propensity of taking a risk for gaining more valuable rewards by choosing between one smaller reward delivered “for sure” and another larger but probabilistic reward (52). Previous case-control studies have consistently revealed a higher degree of delay discounting among adolescents and young adults with IGD (5358), though some data showed no differences between problematic and normal Internet users on the DDT (25). More interestingly, treatment seekers diagnosed with IGD displayed a similar tendency on discounting long-term rewards faster with those diagnosed with gambling disorder (24). In heavy smokers and nicotine-dependent individuals, greater delay-discounting rates were also found (5963), and the degree of delay discounting was significantly related to the severity of ND (64, 65). However, regarding probability discounting, limited data have been discrepant. Some studies showed that IGD participants preferred the probabilistic rewards to those delivered “for sure,” compared to recreational Internet game users and healthy controls (6668). Nonetheless, some adolescents and college students with IGD revealed no differences on the PDT compared to healthy controls (53, 54). Analogously, some data revealed that heavy smokers showed a shallower rate of probability discounting than never- smokers (63, 68), while more studies displayed no differences between heavy/habitual smokers and never-smokers on the PDT (60, 6971), and acute smoking abstinence did not reveal an increase in probability discounting of money or cigarettes (72).

Together with reward processing, cognitive control is believed to play a critical role in the transition from recreational drug use to drug addiction, given the fact that some individuals who use addictive drugs finally develop an addiction while others do not (7377). Abnormalities in cognitive control have been observed in IGD samples (67, 78, 79), accompanied by neural alterations in the prefrontal regions (18, 80, 81). Although cognitive control consists of a series of cognitive processes that regulate goal-directed actions and adaptive responses to complex situations, such as response inhibition, performance monitoring, and working memory (82), most studies concerning IGD mainly investigated inhibitory control or response inhibition (83, 84). Previous studies revealed that adolescents with IGD committed more errors in the incongruent conditions than healthy controls on the Stroop tasks (8588). Adolescents and young adults with IGD also made more commission errors in no-go trials on the Go/No-Go tasks, and showed longer stop-signal reaction time (SSRT) on the Stop- Signal tasks (23, 31, 89, 90). Nevertheless, some studies did not reveal differences on the Go/No-Go task between IGD and healthy control groups (9193). Except for the diverse samples, these inconsistent results might also be due to the mixed processing of both stimulus-driven attentional bias and response inhibition in the Go/No-Go task itself (i.e., novelty from 25% No-Go trials vs. 75% Go trials). A newly modified Go/No-Go task, containing 75% frequent-Go trials, 12.5% infrequent-Go trials, and 12.5% No-Go trials, has been developed to directly detect response inhibition (94). A clear association between inhibitory control taxed by this novel task and smoking relapse vulnerability was revealed in treatment-seeking smokers (95). Nonetheless, deficits in inhibitory control were not consistently found in ND. Some data revealed that inhibitory control performance was negatively correlated with smoking behavior (96, 97), and subjects with ND showed impaired inhibitory control following 12-h abstinence (98). However, heavy smokers and non-smokers displayed no differences on the classical Go/No-Go tasks (99, 100), though smokers committed more errors on the Stroop task (101). Given the mixed tasks used in the literature and no direct comparison between IGD and ND, it remains unclear whether inhibitory control dysfunctions are simultaneously connected to IGD and ND in young adults.

Therefore, the main purpose of this current study was to gather more empirical evidence about the associations of reward processing and inhibitory control with both IGD and ND among young adults, targeting a straightforward contrast between IGD individuals, ND individuals, and healthy controls on the Delay-discounting Test (DDT), the Probability Discounting Test (PDT), the Stroop Color-Word Task, and the revised Go/No Go Task. Besides, we also employed the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale (BIS-11) to test trait impulsivity, considering the inconsistent BIS-11 data between IGD and other addictive disorders (23, 24). We generally hypothesized that as a putative non-substance addiction, IGD might share an aberrant pattern of inhibitory control and reward discounting with ND that is one typical kind of SUD categories.

Methods

Participants and Procedure

A total of 168 young adult subjects participated in this study, including 58 individuals with Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD; mean age: 20.19 ± 1.42 years; 35 males, 60.3%), 53 individuals with Nicotine Dependence (ND; mean age: 20.64 ± 1.72 years; 33 males, 62.3%), and 57 age- and gender-matched healthy controls (HC; mean age: 20.19 ± 1.41 years; 36 males, 63.2%). All of them were college students recruited during April and September 2019, through advertisement and flier from two local universities in Guiyang City, China. Participants were invited to complete a person-to-person screening interview conducted by an experienced psychiatrist and a well-trained clinical psychologist in the laboratory, and then they finished a battery of questionnaires and cognitive tasks when enrolled according to the clinical interview.

Inclusion criteria for the IGD group included: (1) ≥18 years of age; (2) meeting five or more of the nine criteria for IGD proposed in the DSM-5 (1); (3) having a score of 50 or more on the Internet Addiction Test (IAT) (102), which indicates severe or problematic Internet use (103); and (4) at least 3 h per day spent on playing Internet games (mainly the Multiplayer Online Battle Arena games, such as the League of Legends, the Arena of Valor, and the Game For Peace/Playerunknown's Battlegrounds) over a 12-month period. The exclusion criteria included current/past major psychiatric disorders (e.g., schizophrenia, bipolar disorder), neurological diseases or mental disorders, brain trauma, use of psychoactive drugs (e.g., cocaine, heroin, methamphetamine), alcohol abuse or dependence, and current/past smoking.

Inclusion criteria for the ND group included: (1) ≥18 years of age; (2) endorsing three or more of the seven criteria for Nicotine Dependence in the DSM-IV-TR (104); (3) having a score of 4 or more on the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND) (105), which is determined as high nicotine dependence (106, 107); and (4) daily smoking with at least 10 cigarettes over a 12-month period. Moreover, the ND group should have no history of regular Internet gaming, with a score of <40 on the IAT indicating normal Internet use (103). The exclusion criteria were same as those for the IGD group (except for the criterion of current/past smoking).

The healthy controls met the following criteria: (1) ≥18 years of age; (2) a score of <40 on the IAT and no experience of Internet gaming; (3) non-smoking and a score of 0 on the FTND; and (4) no current/past major psychiatric disorders, neurological diseases or mental disorders, brain trauma, use of psychoactive drugs, alcohol abuse or dependence. All subjects were right-handed and had normal or rectified eyesight, without any color vision deficiency. All of them gave written informed consent and were compensated with a gift equal to RMB ¥ 50 for their time. The current study was reviewed and approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee at the Guizhou Medical University. The proposed study design, recruitment process, and our plans to compensate the participants were in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Monetary Reward Discounting Tasks

We used the Delay-discounting Test (DDT) (49) and Probability Discounting Test (PDT) (51) to assess discounting degrees of rewards in the context of monetary choice. The DDT contains a set of choices between a smaller immediate reward and a larger delayed reward. The degree of delay discounting is calculated by the hyperbolic equation V = A/(1+kD). In this equation, k is a free parameter, with a larger k-value describing a higher degree of delay discounting. An adapted version of DDT among Chinese students (108) was used in this study, as reported in our previous studies (25, 109). Examples of choices on this task are “A: receiving RMB ¥1000 now; B: receiving RMB ¥10000 one year later” and “A: receiving RMB ¥9000 now; B: receiving RMB ¥10000 one year later.” The k-value was calculated and log-transformed in keeping with the literature. The PDT consists of three parts (i.e., Part A: $20 vs. $80; Part B: $40 vs. $100; Part C: $40 vs. $60), with 10 choices in each part. Subjects have to choose between a smaller amount of money delivered “for sure” and a larger amount of money delivered probabilistically. Examples of choices are “A: $20 for sure; B: a 1-in-10 chance/10% of winning $80” and “A: $40 for sure; B: a 5-in-10 chance/50% of winning $100.” This task has been properly used in our previous study reported elsewhere (109). The degree of probability discounting is calculated by the equation V = A/(1+hΘ), in which the free parameter h refers to the degree of probability discounting. Lower h indicates that the probabilistic rewards are less steeply discounted, thus suggesting a reduction in risk aversion or a higher level of risk-taking (51). The h scores in each part were calculated and log-transformed to get a normal distribution as suggested before.

Inhibitory Control Tasks

The standard Stroop Color-Word Task (110) was used to measure respond inhibition under cognitive interference condition. In this task, participants are instructed to name the color of the words that are printed in a certain ink. There are two kinds of trials. In congruent trials, the word is printed in a concordant color (e.g., the word RED printed in red ink), while in the incongruent trials, the word-color pairs are always conflicting (e.g., the word RED printed in green ink). In our study, the colored words were presented on the black screen in a 7 × 7 cm size. Each word was presented for 1350ms, with a total interstimulus interval of 2000ms, according to previous studies (111). There were 54 word-color pairs each in the congruent and incongruent trials, and the task lasted for about 6 min. Subjects had 8 trails to check up on the response keys (e.g., “1” for RED, “2” for GREEN) before formal experiments. This task was programmed using the E-prime Version 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Sharpsburg, PA, USA). Response time (RT) and correct accuracy in the congruent and incongruent trials were analyzed.

A modified and validated Go/No Go Task (94, 95) was employed to investigate inhibitory control or response inhibition. This task was designed to separate the processing of infrequent stimuli (e.g., stimulus-driven attention) from inhibitory processes by including three different types of colored circles: frequent-go trials (frequent gray, n = 388, about 75%), infrequent-go trials (rare yellow, n = 65, about 12.5%), and no-go trials (rare blue, n = 65, about 12.5%). The contrast of the no-go trials vs. the infrequent-go trials was expected to detect the process of response inhibition. In this task, a colored circle was presented on the black screen for 400 ms with a 400-ms interstimulus interval over 7 min. Participants were told to press a button as quickly as possible with the right index finger in response to gray and yellow circles, but to withhold a response to blue circles. The frequent-go, infrequent-go, and no-go trials were intermixed in pseudo-random order. Prior to the formal experiments, subjects practiced 30 filler trials (10 gray, 10 yellow, and 10 blue circles). This task was also programmed using the E-prime Version 2.0. Reported no-go accuracy was adjusted, including just those no-go trials with a correct response to the preceding go trial, to control for the effects of attentional lapses (94, 95).

Trait Impulsivity Measurement

The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11 (BIS-11) (112) was employed to measure impulsive traits on three dimensions (Motor Impulsiveness, Attentional Impulsiveness, Non-planning Impulsiveness). Each dimension consists of 8 or 11 items that are rated on a 4-point scale (1 = rarely/never, 4 = almost always/always). Scores of each dimension were obtained for analyses, with higher scores indicating higher levels of trait impulsivity. Cronbach's α was 0.69–0.81 for the three dimensions in this study.

Data Analysis

All data were analyzed with the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences for Windows, Version 19.0 (SPSS Inc., Chicago, IL, USA). Categorical data such as gender, ethnicity, and home locality were analyzed with chi-square tests for group comparisons. The 3 (group: IGD, ND, HC) × 2 (gender: male, female) multivariate analysis of variance (mANOVA) models were used to compare task scores. Post-hoc tests were conducted using Fisher's least significant differences protected t-tests. Partial correlations were tested between task performance and gaming/smoking variables in IGD and ND groups, controlling for gender, age, ethnicity, and home locality. Statistical significance was set as p < 0.05, two-tailed.

Results

Demographic Characteristics and Trait Impulsivity

Table 1 describes the demographic characteristics and BIS-11 scores of the Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD), Nicotine Dependence (ND), and healthy controls (HC) groups. No between-group differences were detected on age [F(2, 165)=1.597, p = 0.260], gender (χ2 = 0.101, p = 0.951), ethnicity (χ2 = 0.211, p = 0.900), or home locality (χ2 = 0.090, p = 0.956). IGD group had a higher IAT score than ND and HC groups [F(2, 165)= 752.96, p < 0.001]. On the BIS-11, the 3 (group: IGD, ND, HC) × 2 (gender: male, female) mANOVA model revealed significant between-group effects on all of the three dimensions, including Motor Impulsiveness [F(2, 162) = 8.255, p < 0.001, ηp2= 0.092], Attentional Impulsiveness [F(2, 162) = 44.111, p < 0.001, ηp2=0.353], and Non-planning Impulsiveness [F(2, 162)= 5.867, p = 0.003, ηp2 = 0.068]. Pairwise comparisons showed that both IGD and ND groups scored higher than healthy controls on Motor Impulsiveness (Cohen's d = 0.78, p < 0.001; Cohen's d = 0.53, p = 0.007, respectively), Attentional Impulsiveness (Cohen's d = 1.75, p < 0.001; Cohen's d = 0.92, p < 0.001, respectively), and Non-planning Impulsiveness (Cohen's d = 0.64, p = 0.001; Cohen's d = 0.41, p = 0.049, respectively). The IGD and ND groups did not differ from each other on Motor Impulsiveness (p = 0.253) or Non-planning Impulsiveness (p = 0.172), but IGD group scored higher than ND group on Attentional Impulsiveness (Cohen's d = 0.83, p < 0.001). Main effects of gender and interaction effects of group × gender were not significant on any of these dimensions (ps > 0.05).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic characteristics and BIS-11 scores for the three groups.

Monetary Reward Discounting

The scores on the delay-discounting and probability-discounting tasks of the IGD, ND, and HC groups are displayed in Table 2. The mANOVA models showed that group effects were significant on the DDT score (i.e., log-transformed k value) and on the PDT score (i.e., log-transformed h value) of the Part A [F(2, 162) = 7.505, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.085; F(2, 162) = 7.118, p = 0.001, ηp2 = 0.081, respectively], but not on the PDT Part B or Part C scores [F(2, 162) = 2.975, p = 0.054; F(2, 162) = 2.674, p = 0.072, respectively]. Pairwise comparisons revealed that both IGD and ND groups had a higher degree of delay discounting (i.e., log-transformed k-value) on the DDT than healthy controls (Cohen's d = 0.53, p = 0.002; Cohen's d = 0.69, p = 0.001, respectively), but the difference between IGD and ND groups was not significant (p = 0.253). By contrast, ND group had a lower probability-discounting degree (i.e., log-transformed h-value) on the PDT (Part A), compared with healthy controls (Cohen's d = 0.55, p = 0.004) and IGD group (Cohen's d = 0.79, p < 0.001), but IGD group did not differ from healthy controls (p = 0.546). Main effects of gender and interaction effects of group × gender were not significant on any of the DDT and PDT scores (ps > 0.05).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Discounting degrees of the three groups on the DDT and PDT.

Inhibitory Control Performance

The inhibitory control performance on the Stroop Color-Word Task and Go/No Go Task of the IGD, ND, and HC groups are showed in Table 3. On the Stroop task, the mANOVA models revealed that the group effects on correct accuracy were significant in the incongruent trials [F(2, 162) = 6.351, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.073] but not in the congruent trials [F(2, 162) = 2.648, p = 0.076], and the group effects on response time were not significant in the congruent or incongruent trials [F(2, 162) = 1.104, p = 0.334; F(2, 162) = 0.682, p = 0.507, respectively]. Pairwise comparisons found that ND group had a lower correct accuracy in the incongruent trials compared with healthy controls (Cohen's d = 0.61, p = 0.001) and IGD group (Cohen's d = 0.49, p = 0.017), but IGD group did not differ from healthy controls (p = 0.250). Main effects of gender and interaction effects of group × gender were not significant on the correct accuracy and response time (ps > 0.05). See Figure 1A for a clear portrayal of the Stroop performance.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Inhibitory control performance on the Stroop and Go/No Go tasks (M ± SD).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Inhibitory control performance on the Stroop Color-Word Task (A) and the revised Go/No Go Task (B) of the three groups. IGD, Internet Gaming Disorder; ND, Nicotine Dependence; HC, healthy controls. Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation (M ± SD). *p < 0.05, ***p < 0.001.

On the Go/No Go task, the mANOVA models revealed significant group effects on correct accuracy in the no-go trials [F(2, 162) = 38.160, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.320], but not in the frequent-go trials [F(2, 162) = 1.085, p = 0.350] or the rare-go trials [F(2, 162) = 0.986, p = 0.375]. Group effects on response time were not significant in the frequent-go or rare-go trials [F(2, 162) = 0.884, p = 0.415; F(2, 162) = 0.939, p = 0.393, respectively). Pairwise comparisons found that both IGD and ND groups had a lower correct accuracy in the no-go trials than healthy controls (Cohen's d = 1.35, p < 0.001; Cohen's d = 1.50, p < 0.001, respectively), but the accuracy difference between IGD and ND groups was not significant (p = 0.332). Main effects of gender and interaction effects of group × gender were not significant on the Go/No Go accuracy and response time (ps > 0.05). See Figure 1B for a direct description of the Go/No Go performance.

Correlations Between Gaming/Smoking Variables and Task Performance

Partial correlations were tested between Internet gaming variables (i.e., IAT score, years of regular gaming, daily gaming hours), nicotine use variables (i.e., FTND score, years of smoking, cigarettes per day) and task performance (i.e., BIS-11 scores, DDT and PDT scores, and inhibitory control scores), controlling for gender, age, ethnicity, and home locality. The data showed that most of the correlations were not significant between gaming/smoking variables with trait impulsivity, discounting degrees, and inhibitory control (ps > 0.05), except for that of the BIS-11 Non-planning Impulsiveness with FTND score, years of smoking, and cigarettes per day in the ND group (rp = 0.267, p = 0.05; rp = 0.269, p = 0.049; rp = 0.278, p = 0.042, respectively). Please see more details for the partial correlations in the Supplementary Table 1.

Discussion

The current study contrasted the characteristics of monetary reward discounting, inhibitory control, and trait impulsivity between Internet Gaming Disorder (IGD) and Nicotine Dependence (ND) individuals, with a well-matched sample of healthy controls as the reference group. To our best knowledge, this is the first study that has targeted a straightforward comparison between IGD and ND on reward processing and cognitive control aspects among young adults. Our data demonstrated that both the IGD and ND groups scored higher on the trait impulsivity (i.e., Motor, Attentional, and Non-planning Impulsiveness) and had higher degrees of delay discounting (i.e., poorer capability of delay gratification) than the healthy controls, while only the ND group showed a lower degree of probability discounting (i.e., lower risk aversion) than healthy controls. Moreover, IGD and ND groups displayed similar impaired inhibitory control on the revised Go/No Go task (i.e., a lower correct accuracy in No-Go trials) compared with healthy controls, but on the Stroop task only the ND subjects showed a lower correct accuracy in the incongruent trials than healthy controls. These findings suggested that IGD was linked to anomalous reward discounting and dysfunctional inhibitory control, comparable with one typical SUD category (i.e., ND) in this study.

In regard to trait impulsivity assessed with the BIS-11, plentiful studies have observed elevated scores among adolescents and young adults with IGD on the three dimensions (i.e., Motor, Attentional, and Non-planning Impulsiveness) (23, 31, 54, 89, 113), despite that some studies revealed no differences on these impulsiveness scores between treatment-seeking patients diagnosed with IGD and healthy controls (24). Our data were consistent with the results of most previous studies, revealing an increased level of trait impulsivity on the BIS model among individuals with IGD. Furthermore, our study detected similar elevated scores of the three dimensions in the Nicotine Dependence (ND) group, in line with the literature of trait impulsivity in cigarette smoking (114). These findings, together with our previous cross-sectional data of trait impulsivity in problematic Internet use and smoking behaviors, indicated that IGD showed a tendency of increased impulsivity traits comparable to ND (25). Interestingly, we further found that the BIS-11 scores were not significantly correlated with the severity of Internet gaming (Supplementary Table 1), but more serious nicotine use (e.g., years of smoking) was associated with a higher score on certain impulsivity trait (non-planning impulsiveness), indicating a possible toxic effect of ND on impulsivity. Particularly, nicotine (i.e., the primary component of cigarettes smoking and ND) is a specific agonist of nicotinic acetylcholine receptors (nAChRs), and chronic exposure to nicotine acts as a neuroteratogen by providing excessive cholinergic stimulation in the developing brain (115). Thus, the deleterious effects of ND are mostly connected with nicotine-induced overstimulation that causes overt neurotoxicity and the adaptive desensitization of the nAChRs that causes alterations in cholinergic transmission, which may produce derangements in final neuronal architecture such as the prefrontal cortex, resulting in less prefrontal inhibition and higher levels of impulsive trait (116).

On the monetary reward discounting tasks (i.e., DDT and PDT), our data showed that both IGD and ND groups had a higher delay-discounting degree (log-transformed k value) than the healthy controls, with a medium to large effect size (Cohen's d = 0.53–0.69), and no difference was found on the DDT between IGD and ND groups. These data were consistent with previous reports indicating higher degrees of delay discounting among adolescents and young adults with IGD (5358), as well as among young heavy smokers and nicotine-dependent individuals (5963). In this respect, an inability to delay gratification might be reflected both in IGD and ND subjects, and this similar tendency on discounting long-term rewards faster could play an important role in the development of these two disordered behaviors among the youths (24, 65). Furthermore, the partial correlations in our study did not find significant relationships between gaming/smoking severity with delay-discounting degrees (k values) in IGD and ND groups (Supplementary Table 1), probably suggesting that the poor delay gratification might not be aggravated by the severity of IGD or ND among these individuals. However, given the cross-sectional design of our study, whether the poor delay gratification is a predisposing factor for IGD or ND still needs more powerful longitudinal evidence.

With respect to probability discounting, IGD subjects did not differ from healthy controls on the probability-discounting degrees (log-transformed h values), indicating a normal risk aversion as expected in previous studies (53, 54), though inconsistent with some reports showing that IGD participants chose more probabilistic rewards than recreational Internet game users and controls (6668). By comparison, the ND group had a lower degree of probability discounting than healthy controls with a medium effect size (Cohen's d = 0.55), suggesting a reduction in risk aversion or a greater risk-taking tendency (63, 117). However, as previous research pointed out, the floor effects of low probabilities in different studies might lead to inconsistent results (60, 6971). Considering that our ND group merely scored lower than the controls on the Part A (i.e., $20 vs. $80), but neither on the Part B (i.e., $40 vs. $100) nor on the Part C (i.e., $40 vs. $60) of the PDT, it appears that the magnitudes of the risky and/or the constant monetary rewards might be also important in the choice of these addicted individuals (118), which calls for further investigations on this interesting topic that to what extend the probabilities and the magnitudes of monetary rewards may affect the degrees of probability discounting on the PDT.

Cognitive control or inhibitory control plays a crucial part in our goal-directed behaviors (48, 119), as well as in the uncontrolled addictive behaviors (120, 121). Impairments in cognitive control might affect the daily life, family relations, and occupational status of drug-dependent individuals, and are essential in the treatment and relapse of drug addiction (122). Based on the phenomenological and empirical evidence of decreased executive control in adolescents with IGD, some theoretical models of IGD, such as the tripartite neurocognitive model (46) and the Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution model (I-PACE) (123), coincidentally underlined the key role of reduced cognitive control or inhibitory control in the development and maintenance of IGD. In the present study, we used the Stroop Color-Word Task and a revised Go/No Go task to test inhibitory control. The data on Stroop task revealed that the Nicotine Dependence (ND) subjects showed a lower correct accuracy than healthy controls in the incongruent trials (Cohen's d = 0.61), indicating a dysfunctional inhibitory control in heavy smokers (101), while the IGD subjects had similar performance on the Stroop as the healthy controls did, discordant with previous reports among adolescents showing that high school students with IGD made more errors in the incongruent trials than healthy controls, indicating impaired inhibitory control (8590). Considering the obvious differences of the IGD samples in ours and other studies (i.e., young adult university students vs. adolescent high-school students), more attention should be paid to these contradictory findings in various samples of IGD. Especially, we noticed that although significantly differing from the healthy controls on the Stroop task, our ND subjects seemed not so “impaired,” with an average correct accuracy of 90.81% in the incongruent trials, in contrast to that of 92.80% (IGD group) and 93.50% (HC group). Thus, the task difficulty and complexity issues should be considered in future similar studies using the Stroop task.

More important findings in this study were from the revised Go/No Go task. Our data showed that the IGD and ND groups exhibited similar impaired performance of inhibitory control on this task (i.e., a lower correct accuracy in No-Go trials) than the healthy controls, with a large effect size (Cohen's d = 1.35, 1.50, respectively), yet the performance differences in frequent-Go and infrequent-Go trials among the IGD, ND and healthy control groups were not significant. Because of the potential confusions arising from the dual processing of attentional bias related to the novelty and response inhibition related to the no-go signals in traditional Go/No-Go paradigms (i.e., 25% No-Go trials vs. 75% Go trials), the present literature of the performance on the Go/No-Go task has been greatly inconsistent, with some studies reporting reduced inhibitory control functions in adolescents and young adults with IGD (31, 89), while others indicating intact inhibitory control in IGD subjects (8688) and in heavy smokers (99, 100). Our current study firstly dissociated inhibitory control aspects from attentional bias among IGD and ND samples, using the newly modified and validated Go/No-Go task (89, 90) that contains 75% frequent-Go trials, 12.5% infrequent-Go trials, and 12.5% No-Go trials to directly detect response inhibition by comparing the infrequent-Go trials with No-Go trials. Our data clearly depicted that although the IGD and ND groups performed normally in both frequent-Go and infrequent-Go trials (the average correct accuracy >92%) similar to the healthy controls, these two disordered groups displayed apparent inhibitory impairments in the infrequent No-Go trials (an average correct accuracy of about 60%) compared with healthy controls (about 70% correct) (Figure 1B). Furthermore, the IGD individuals who has never used any addictive substance, still manifested a comparable impairment of inhibitory control on this task with the ND subjects, who probably had the concomitant intoxication consequences due to chronic nicotine use (116). These findings might indicate a basic pathology mechanism of cognitive control implicated in IGD as a potential addictive disorder similar to SUD (84). Nevertheless, we did not find a significant correlation between the inhibitory control impairments and the severity of smoking in the ND group, inconsistent with previous reports (96, 97). In light of the non-clinical samples of ND (i.e., university students) in our study, we speculated that a narrow distribution of smoking severity (e.g., an average score of 5.83 ± 1.03 on the FTND) might negatively affect the correlation results, and further studies with a larger ND sample are warranted to detect their accurate relationships.

There are several limitations that should be noted in the present study. Firstly, despite the fact that we mainly included the IGD and ND groups by person-to-person screening interview conducted by a psychiatrist and a clinical psychologist, according to the clinical criteria for IGD and ND, we also used some self-report scales (i.e., the IAT and the FTND) to evaluate the severity of IGD/ND, which might bring subjective biases, thus the results should be explained carefully. Secondly, our samples of IGD and ND primarily consisted of the young adult university students, which could not represent the whole population, so the findings should be further examined with other different samples (e.g., treatment-seeking populations of IGD and ND). Thirdly, we did not combine any neurophysiology measurement together with our behavioral tasks as did in previous studies [e.g., (68, 89, 95, 101)], thus our findings of deficient reward processing and inhibitory control in IGD and ND were short of powerful converged evidence on neurobiological correlates. Actually, the current literature suggests that functional and structural neural alterations in the fronto-striatal and fronto-cingulate regions are closely associated with IGD (81), and abnormal activities in the prefrontal areas (e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, and orbitofrontal cortex/ventromedial prefrontal cortex) may account for the impaired cognitive control and decreased loss sensitivity in IGD and gambling disorder (124, 125) as well as for the craving and impulsivity in comorbid IGD and SUD (126). Thus, neurophysiology measurements should be better integrated with behavioral tasks in future. Additionally, although we contrasted IGD and ND individuals with well-matched healthy controls on multiple tasks, with a larger sample size relative to some previous studies [e.g., (23)], our case-control study design was cross-sectional in nature, therefore our results could not draw a causal conclusion, which should also be interpreted more discreetly.

Despite these limitations, our findings indicated for the first time that in a straightforward comparison, young adults with IGD and those with ND concurrently shared poorer capability of delay gratification as well as impaired inhibitory control, suggesting that IGD was linked to a neuropsychological pattern of anomalous reward discounting and dysfunctional inhibitory control, which was comparable to a typical SUD category (i.e., ND). This study thus might promote a better understanding of the pathogenesis of IGD as a potential addictive disorder similar to SUD. Furthermore, our first direct findings from a comparison between IGD and ND should be beneficial for potential clinical implications in the prevention and treatment of excessive Internet gaming and IGD, for instance, developing possible non-pharmacological therapeutic methods aimed at the restoration of inhibitory control or cognitive control functions (124) and/or reducing the high-level subjective representations of exciting activities or instant craving (127). In this respect, non-invasive neuromodulation methods, such as the repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS), have been proposed as an effective intervention to target cognitive dysfunctions in substance-related addictive disorders including tobacco, alcohol, and cocaine addiction (128). In view of the similar impaired inhibitory control detected in both IGD and ND in our study, future treatment of pathological Internet gaming might also be inspired to yield encouraging results by combining neuromodulation methods (e.g., rTMS) that could be applied on selected brain areas especially the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), which has been proved to probably improve the prefrontal top-down executive control and reduce drug craving and consumption in SUD, including ND (128, 129). However, there remains a big need for more accurate studies that can provide deeper insight into the core pathogenesis of IGD so as to advance this field, furnishing the foundation for developing an ideal model for practice in the prevention and treatment of IGD (130).

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Human Research Ethics Committee at the Guizhou Medical University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

W-SY designed the study, wrote the protocols, directed the study, and wrote a first draft of the manuscript. R-TC, M-ML, and D-HZ performed the task assessments, data collection, and main data analysis. All of the authors contributed to the writing and have approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was provided by the National Natural Science Foundation of China (Nos.: 32060195, 31560284) to W-SY. These funding sources had no further role in study design; in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data; in the writing of the report; or in the decision to submit the paper for publication.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2021.628933/full#supplementary-material

References

1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Publishing (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Potenza MN, Higuchi S, Brand M. Call for research into a wider range of behavioural addictions. Nature. (2018) 555:30. doi: 10.1038/d41586-018-02568-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Pontes HM, Griffiths MD. A new era for gaming disorder research: time to shift from consensus to consistency. Addict Behav. (2020) 103:106059. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2019.106059

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. World Health Organization. The ICD-11 Classification of Mental and Behavioral Disorders: Diagnostic Criteria for Research. Geneva: World Health Organization (2018).

Google Scholar

5. Pontes HM, Stavropoulos V, Griffiths MD. Emerging insights on internet gaming disorder: conceptual and measurement issues. Addict Behav Rep. (2020) 11:100242. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2019.100242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Jo YS, Bhang SY, Choi JS, Lee HK, Lee SY, Kweon YS. Clinical characteristics of diagnosis for internet gaming disorder: comparison of DSM-5 IGD and ICD-11 GD diagnosis. J Clin Med. (2019) 8:945. doi: 10.3390/jcm8070945

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Wu AMS, Chen JH, Tong KK, Yu S, Lau JTF. Prevalence and associated factors of internet gaming disorder among community dwelling adults in Macao, China. J Behav Addict. (2018) 7:62–9. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Przybylski AK, Weinstein N, Murayama K. Internet gaming disorder: investigating the clinical relevance of a new phenomenon. Am J Psychiatr. (2017) 174:230–6. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16020224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Petry NM, Rehbein F, Ko CH, O'Brien CP. Internet gaming disorder in the DSM-5. Curr Psychiatr Rep. (2015) 17:72. doi: 10.1007/s11920-015-0610-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Stevens MW, Dorstyn D, Delfabbro PH, King DL. Global prevalence of gaming disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. (2020). doi: 10.1177/0004867420962851. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Darvesh N, Radhakrishnan A, Lachance CC, Nincic V, Sharpe JP, Ghassemi M, et al. Exploring the prevalence of gaming disorder and Internet gaming disorder: a rapid scoping review. Systemat Rev. (2020) 9:68. doi: 10.1186/s13643-020-01329-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Aarseth E, Bean AM, Boonen H, Colder Carras M, Coulson M, Das D, et al. Scholars' open debate paper on the World Health organization ICD-11 gaming disorder proposal. J Behav Addict. (2017) 6:267–70. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Petry NM, Rehbein F, Gentile DA, Lemmens JS, Rumpf HJ, Mößle T, et al. An international consensus for assessing internet gaming disorder using the new DSM-5 approach. Addiction. (2014) 109:1399–406. doi: 10.1111/add.12457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Griffiths MD, van Rooij AJ, Kardefelt-Winther D, Starcevic V, Király O, Pallesen S, et al. Working towards an international consensus on criteria for assessing internet gaming disorder: a critical commentary on Petry et al. (2014). Addiction. (2016) 111:167–75. doi: 10.1111/add.13057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD, Pontes HM. DSM-5 diagnosis of Internet Gaming Disorder: Some ways forward in overcoming issues and concerns in the gaming studies field. J Behav Addict. (2017) 6:133–141. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. van den Brink W. ICD-11 Gaming disorder: needed and just in time or dangerous and much too early? J Behav Addict. (2017) 6:290–2. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.040

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Martin PR, Petry NM. Are non-substance-related addictions really addictions? Am J Addict. (2005) 14:1–7. doi: 10.1080/10550490590899808

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kuss DJ, Pontes HM, Griffiths MD. Neurobiological correlates in internet gaming disorder: a systematic literature review. Front Psychiatr. (2018) 9:166. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. King DL, Delfabbro PH, Potenza MN, Demetrovics Z, Billieux J, Brand M. Internet gaming disorder should qualify as a mental disorder. Austral N Zealand J Psychiatr. (2018) 52:615–7. doi: 10.1177/0004867418771189

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. van Rooij AJ, Ferguson CJ, Colder Carras M, Kardefelt-Winther D, Shi J, Aarseth E, et al. A weak scientific basis for gaming disorder: Let us err on the side of caution. J Behav Addict. (2018) 7:1–9. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/kc7r9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Dullur P, Starcevic V. Internet gaming disorder does not qualify as a mental disorder. Austral N Zeal J Psychiatr. (2018) 52:110–1. doi: 10.1177/0004867417741554

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. James RJ, Tunney RJ. The need for a behavioural analysis of behavioural addictions. Clin Psychol Rev. (2017) 52:69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.11.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Choi SW, Kim HS, Kim GY, Jeon Y, Park SM, Lee JY, et al. Similarities and differences among Internet gaming disorder, gambling disorder and alcohol use disorder: a focus on impulsivity and compulsivity. J Behav Addict. (2014) 3:246–53. doi: 10.1556/JBA.3.2014.4.6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Wölfling K, Duven E, Wejbera M, Beutel ME, Müller KW. Discounting delayed monetary rewards and decision making in behavioral addictions - a comparison between patients with gambling disorder and internet gaming disorder. Addict Behav. (2020) 108:106446. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106446

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Liu SJ, Lan Y, Wu L, Yan WS. Profiles of impulsivity in problematic internet users and cigarette smokers. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:772. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Yan W, Li Y, Sui N. The relationship between recent stressful life events, personality traits, perceived family functioning and internet addiction among college students. Stress Health. (2014) 30:3–11. doi: 10.1002/smi.2490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Bonnaire C, Baptista D. Internet gaming disorder in male and female young adults: the role of alexithymia, depression, anxiety and gaming type. Psychiatr Res. (2019) 272:521–30. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.12.158

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sugaya N, Shirasaka T, Takahashi K, Kanda H. Bio-psychosocial factors of children and adolescents with internet gaming disorder: a systematic review. Bio Psycho Soc Med. (2019) 13:3. doi: 10.1186/s13030-019-0144-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Fam JY. Prevalence of internet gaming disorder in adolescents: a meta-analysis across three decades. Scand J Psychol. (2018) 59:524–31. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Xin M, Xing J, Pengfei W, Houru L, Mengcheng W, Hong Z. Online activities, prevalence of Internet addiction and risk factors related to family and school among adolescents in China. Addict Behav Rep. (2018) 7:14–8. doi: 10.1016/j.abrep.2017.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Wang L, Tian M, Zheng Y, Li Q, Liu X. Reduced loss aversion and inhibitory control in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Psychol Addict Behav. (2020) 34:484–96. doi: 10.1037/adb0000549

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Alexander C, Piazza M, Mekos D, Valente T. Peers, schools, and adolescent cigarette smoking. J Adolescent Health. (2001) 29:22–30. doi: 10.1016/S1054-139X(01)00210-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Chen X, Li X, Stanton B, Mao R, Sun Z, Zhang H, et al. Patterns of cigarette smoking among students from 19 colleges and universities in Jiangsu Province, China: a latent class analysis. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2004) 76:153–63. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Reed MB, Wang R, Shillington AM, Clapp JD, Lange JE. The relationship between alcohol use and cigarette smoking in a sample of undergraduate college students. Addict Behav. (2007) 32:449–64. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2006.05.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Wang TW, Gentzke A, Sharapova S, Cullen KA, Ambrose BK, Jamal A. Tobacco product use among middle and high school students - United States, 2011-2017. MMWMorbidity Weekly Rep. (2018) 67:629–33. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.mm6722a3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Di Nicola M, Ferri VR, Moccia L, Panaccione I, Strangio AM, Tedeschi D, et al. Gender differences and psychopathological features associated with addictive behaviors in adolescents. Front Psychiatr. (2017) 8:256. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Ko CH, Liu GC, Yen JY, Yen CF, Chen CS, Lin WC. The brain activations for both cue-induced gaming urge and smoking craving among subjects comorbid with Internet gaming addiction and nicotine dependence. J Psychiatr Res. (2013) 47:486–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2012.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Marmet S, Studer J, Wicki M, Bertholet N, Khazaal Y, Gmel G. Unique versus shared associations between self-reported behavioral addictions and substance use disorders and mental health problems: a commonality analysis in a large sample of young Swiss men. J Behav Addict. (2019) 8:664–77. doi: 10.1556/2006.8.2019.70

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Burleigh TL, Griffiths MD, Sumich A, Stavropoulos V, Kuss DJ. A systematic review of the co-occurrence of gaming disorder and other potentially addictive behaviors. Curr Addic Rep. (2019) 6:383–401. doi: 10.1007/s40429-019-00279-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Mérelle S, Kleiboer A, Schotanus M, Cluitmans TL, Waardenburg CM, Kramer D, et al. Which health-related problems are associated with problematic video-gaming or social media use in adolescents? Clin Neuropsychiatr. (2017) 14:11–9.

Google Scholar

41. Ge X, Sun Y, Han X, Wang Y, Ding W, Cao M, et al. Difference in the functional connectivity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex between smokers with nicotine dependence and individuals with internet gaming disorder. BMC Neurosci. (2017) 18:54. doi: 10.1186/s12868-017-0375-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Noël X, Brevers D, Bechara A. A neurocognitive approach to understanding the neurobiology of addiction. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2013) 23:632–8. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Turel O, Bechara A. A triadic reflective-impulsive-interoceptive awareness model of general and impulsive information system use: behavioral tests of neuro-cognitive theory. Front Psychol. (2016) 7:601. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Zheng H, Hu Y, Wang Z, Wang M, Du X, Dong G. Meta-analyses of the functional neural alterations in subjects with Internet gaming disorder: Similarities and differences across different paradigms. Progr Neuro-Psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatr. (2019) 94:109656. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2019.109656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Wang M, Dong H, Zheng H, Du X, Dong GH. Inhibitory neuromodulation of the putamen to the prefrontal cortex in Internet gaming disorder: how addiction impairs executive control. J Behav Addict. (2020) 9:312–24. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Wei L, Zhang S, Turel O, Bechara A, He Q. A tripartite neurocognitive model of internet gaming disorder. Front Psychiatr. (2017) 8:285. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00285

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Ernst M, Pine DS, Hardin M. Triadic model of the neurobiology of motivated behavior in adolescence. Psychol Med. (2006) 36:299–312. doi: 10.1017/S0033291705005891

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Somerville LH, Casey BJ. Developmental neurobiology of cognitive control and motivational systems. Curr Opin Neurobiol. (2010) 20:236–241. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2010.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Kirby KN, Petry NM, Bickel WK. Heroin addicts have higher discount rates for delayed rewards than non-drug-using controls. J Exp Psychol. (1999) 128:78–87. doi: 10.1037/0096-3445.128.1.78

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Dixon MR, Marley J, Jacobs EA. Delay discounting by pathological gamblers. J Appl Behav Anal. (2003) 36:449–58. doi: 10.1901/jaba.2003.36-449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Madden GJ, Petry NM, Johnson PS. Pathological gamblers discount probabilistic rewards less steeply than matched controls. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2009) 17:283–90. doi: 10.1037/a0016806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Rachlin H, Raineri A, Cross D. Subjective probability and delay. J Exp Analysis Behav. (1991) 55:233–44. doi: 10.1901/jeab.1991.55-233

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Li Q, Tian M, Taxer J, Zheng Y, Wu H, Sun S, et al. Problematic internet users' discounting behaviors reflect an inability to delay gratification, not risk taking. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2016) 19:172–8. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2015.0295

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Tian M, Tao R, Zheng Y, Zhang H, Yang G, Li Q, et al. Internet gaming disorder in adolescents is linked to delay discounting but not probability discounting. Computers Human Behav. (2018) 80:59–66. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2017.10.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Weinstein A, Abu HB, Timor A, Mama Y. Delay discounting, risk-taking, and rejection sensitivity among individuals with internet and video gaming disorders. J Behav Addict. (2016) 5:674–82. doi: 10.1556/2006.5.2016.081

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Wang Y, Hu Y, Xu J, Zhou H, Lin X, Du X, et al. Dysfunctional prefrontal function is associated with impulsivity in people with internet gaming disorder during a delay discounting task. Front Psychiatr. (2017) 8:287. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Wang Y, Wu L, Wang L, Zhang Y, Du X, Dong G. Impaired decision-making and impulse control in Internet gaming addicts: evidence from the comparison with recreational Internet game users. Addict Biol. (2017) 22:1610–21. doi: 10.1111/adb.12458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Wang Y, Wu L, Zhou H, Lin X, Zhang Y, Du X, et al. Impaired executive control and reward circuit in Internet gaming addicts under a delay discounting task: independent component analysis. Eur Arch Psychiatr Clin Neurosci. (2017) 267:245–55. doi: 10.1007/s00406-016-0721-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Bickel WK, Odum AL, Madden GJ. Impulsivity and cigarette smoking: delay discounting in current, never, and ex-smokers. Psychopharmacology. (1999) 146:447–54. doi: 10.1007/PL00005490

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Białaszek W, Marcowski P, Cox DJ. Differences in delay, but not probability discounting, in current smokers, e-cigarette users, never smokers. Psychol Record. (2017) 67:223–30. doi: 10.1007/s40732-017-0244-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Amlung M, MacKillop J. Clarifying the relationship between impulsive delay discounting and nicotine dependence. Psychol Addict Behav. (2014) 28:761–8. doi: 10.1037/a0036726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. García-Rodríguez O, Secades-Villa R, Weidberg S, Yoon JH. A systematic assessment of delay discounting in relation to cocaine and nicotine dependence. Behav Proces. (2013) 99:100–5. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2013.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Reynolds B, Richards JB, Horn K, Karraker K. Delay discounting and probability discounting as related to cigarette smoking status in adults. Behav Proces. (2004) 65:35–42. doi: 10.1016/S0376-6357(03)00109-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Sweitzer MM, Donny EC, Dierker LC, Flory JD, Manuck SB. Delay discounting and smoking: association with the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence but not cigarettes smoked per day. Nicotine Tobacco Res. (2008) 10:1571–5. doi: 10.1080/14622200802323274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Weidberg S, Secades-Villa R, García-Pérez Á, González-Roz A, Fernández-Hermida JR. The synergistic effect of cigarette demand and delay discounting on nicotine dependence among treatment-seeking smokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2019) 27:146–52. doi: 10.1037/pha0000248

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Lin X, Zhou H, Dong G, Du X. Impaired risk evaluation in people with Internet gaming disorder: fMRI evidence from a probability discounting task. Progr Neuro-psychopharmacol Biol Psychiatr. (2015) 56:142–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2014.08.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Wang L, Wu L, Lin X, Zhang Y, Zhou H, Du X, et al. Dysfunctional default mode network and executive control network in people with Internet gaming disorder: independent component analysis under a probability discounting task. Eur Psychiatr. (2016) 34:36–42. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2016.01.2424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Wang Z, Liu X, Hu Y, Zheng H, Du X, Dong G. Altered brain functional networks in Internet gaming disorder: independent component and graph theoretical analysis under a probability discounting task. CNS Spectrums. (2019) 24:544–56. doi: 10.1017/S1092852918001505

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Reynolds B, Patak M, Shroff P, Penfold RB, Melanko S, Duhig AM. Laboratory and self-report assessments of impulsive behavior in adolescent daily smokers and nonsmokers. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol. (2007) 15:264–71. doi: 10.1037/1064-1297.15.3.264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Ohmura Y, Takahashi T, Kitamura N. Discounting delayed and probabilistic monetary gains and losses by smokers of cigarettes. Psychopharmacology. (2005) 182:508–15. doi: 10.1007/s00213-005-0110-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Yi R, Chase WD, Bickel WK. Probability discounting among cigarette smokers and nonsmokers: molecular analysis discerns group differences. Behav Pharmacol. (2007) 18:633–9. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0b013e3282effbd3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Yi R, Landes RD. Temporal and probability discounting by cigarette smokers following acute smoking abstinence. Nicotine Tobacco Res. (2012) 14:547–58. doi: 10.1093/ntr/ntr252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Bechara A, Berridge KC, Bickel WK, Morón JA, Williams SB, Stein JS. A neurobehavioral approach to addiction: implications for the opioid epidemic and the psychology of addiction. Psychol Sci Public Interest. (2019) 20:96–127. doi: 10.1177/1529100619860513

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hester R, Lubman DI, Yücel M. The role of executive control in human drug addiction. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. (2010) 3:301–18. doi: 10.1007/7854_2009_28

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Tanabe J, Regner M, Sakai J, Martinez D, Gowin J. Neuroimaging reward, craving, learning, and cognitive control in substance use disorders: review and implications for treatment. Br J Radiol. (2019) 92:20180942. doi: 10.1259/bjr.20180942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Smith DG, Jones PS, Bullmore ET, Robbins TW, Ersche KD. Cognitive control dysfunction and abnormal frontal cortex activation in stimulant drug users and their biological siblings. Translational Psychiatr. (2013) 3:e257. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.32

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Goldstein RZ, Volkow ND. Dysfunction of the prefrontal cortex in addiction: neuroimaging findings and clinical implications. Nature Rev. (2011) 12:652–69. doi: 10.1038/nrn3119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Dong G, Li H, Wang L, Potenza MN. Cognitive control and reward/loss processing in Internet gaming disorder: results from a comparison with recreational Internet game-users. Eur Psychiatr. (2017) 44:30–8. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2017.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Wang H, Jin C, Yuan K, Shakir TM, Mao C, Niu X, et al. The alteration of gray matter volume and cognitive control in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Front Behav Neurosci. (2015) 9:64. doi: 10.3389/fnbeh.2015.00064

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Meng Y, Deng W, Wang H, Guo W, Li T. The prefrontal dysfunction in individuals with Internet gaming disorder: a meta-analysis of functional magnetic resonance imaging studies. Addiction Biol. (2015) 20:799–808. doi: 10.1111/adb.12154

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Yao YW, Liu L, Ma SS, Shi XH, Zhou N, Zhang JT, et al. Functional and structural neural alterations in Internet gaming disorder: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 83:313–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.10.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Cools R, D' Esposito. M. Inverted-U-shaped dopamine actions on human working memory and cognitive control. Biol Psychiatr. (2011) 69:e113–25. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2011.03.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Brand M, Young KS, Laier C. Prefrontal control and internet addiction: a theoretical model and review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging findings. Front Human Neurosci. (2014) 8:375. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00375

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Antons S, Brand M, Potenza MN. Neurobiology of cue-reactivity, craving, and inhibitory control in non-substance addictive behaviors. J Neurol Sci. (2020) 415:116952. doi: 10.1016/j.jns.2020.116952

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Cai C, Yuan K, Yin J, Feng D, Bi Y, Li Y, et al. Striatum morphometry is associated with cognitive control deficits and symptom severity in internet gaming disorder. Brain Imaging Behav. (2016) 10:12–20. doi: 10.1007/s11682-015-9358-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Xing L, Yuan K, Bi Y, Yin J, Cai C, Feng D, et al. Reduced fiber integrity and cognitive control in adolescents with internet gaming disorder. Brain Res. (2014) 1586:109–117. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2014.08.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Yuan K, Qin W, Yu D, Bi Y, Xing L, Jin C, et al. Core brain networks interactions and cognitive control in internet gaming disorder individuals in late adolescence/early adulthood. Brain Structure Funct. (2016) 221:1427–42. doi: 10.1007/s00429-014-0982-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Yuan K, Yu D, Cai C, Feng D, Li Y, Bi Y, et al. Frontostriatal circuits, resting state functional connectivity and cognitive control in internet gaming disorder. Addict Biol. (2017) 22:813–22. doi: 10.1111/adb.12348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Li Q, Wang Y, Yang Z, Dai W, Zheng Y, Sun Y, et al. Dysfunctional cognitive control and reward processing in adolescents with Internet gaming disorder. Psychophysiology. (2020) 57:e13469. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Kim YJ, Lim JA, Lee JY, Oh S, Kim SN, Kim DJ, et al. Impulsivity and compulsivity in Internet gaming disorder: a comparison with obsessive-compulsive disorder and alcohol use disorder. J Behav Addic. (2017) 6:545–53. doi: 10.1556/2006.6.2017.069

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Ding WN, Sun JH, Sun YW, Chen X, Zhou Y, Zhuang ZG, et al. Trait impulsivity and impaired prefrontal impulse inhibition function in adolescents with internet gaming addiction revealed by a Go/No-Go fMRI study. Behav Brain Funct. (2014) 10:20. doi: 10.1186/1744-9081-10-20

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text

92. Chen CY, Huang MF, Yen JY, Chen CS, Liu GC, Yen CF, et al. Brain correlates of response inhibition in internet gaming disorder. Psychiatr Clin Neurosci. (2015) 69:201–9. doi: 10.1111/pcn.12224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Ko CH, Hsieh TJ, Chen CY, Yen CF, Chen CS, Yen JY, et al. Altered brain activation during response inhibition and error processing in subjects with Internet gaming disorder: a functional magnetic imaging study. Eur Arch Psychiatr Clin Neurosci. (2014) 264:661–72. doi: 10.1007/s00406-013-0483-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Chikazoe J, Jimura K, Asari T, Yamashita K, Morimoto H, Hirose S, et al. Functional dissociation in right inferior frontal cortex during performance of go/no-go task. Cereb Cortex. (2009) 19:146–52. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhn065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Froeliger B, McConnell PA, Bell S, Sweitzer M, Kozink RV, Eichberg C, et al. Association between baseline corticothalamic-mediated inhibitory control and smoking relapse vulnerability. JAMA Psychiatr. (2017) 74:379–86. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2017.0017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Spinella M. Correlations between orbitofrontal dysfunction and tobacco smoking. Addiction Biol. (2002) 7:381–4. doi: 10.1080/1355621021000005964

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Yakir A, Rigbi A, Kanyas K, Pollak Y, Kahana G, Karni O, et al. Why do young women smoke? IIAttention I, and impulsivity as neurocognitive predisposing factors. Eur Neuropsychopharmacol. (2007) 17:339–51. doi: 10.1016/j.euroneuro.2006.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Charles-Walsh K, Furlong L, Munro DG, Hester R. Inhibitory control dysfunction in nicotine dependence and the influence of short-term abstinence. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2014) 143:81–6. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Dinn WM, Aycicegi A, Harris CL. Cigarette smoking in a student sample: neurocognitive and clinical correlates. Addict Behav. (2004) 29:107–26. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2003.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Lesage E, Sutherland MT, Ross TJ, Salmeron BJ, Stein EA. Nicotine dependence (trait) and acute nicotinic stimulation (state) modulate attention but not inhibitory control: converging fMRI evidence from Go-Nogo and Flanker tasks. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2020) 45:857–65. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0623-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Yuan K, Yu D, Zhao M, Li M, Wang R, Li Y, et al. Abnormal frontostriatal tracts in young male tobacco smokers. NeuroImage. (2018) 183:346–55. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.08.046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Young KS. Internet addiction: the emergence of a new clinical disorder. CyberPsychol Behav. (1998) 1:237–44. doi: 10.1089/cpb.1998.1.237

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Khazaal Y, Billieux J, Thorens G, Khan R, Louati Y, Scarlatti E, et al. French validation of the internet addiction test. Cyberpsychol Behav. (2008) 11:703–6. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2007.0249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed., text revision (DSM-IV-TR). Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2000).

Google Scholar

105. Heatherton TF, Kozlowski LT, Frecker RC, Fagerström KO. The Fagerström Test for Nicotine dependence: a revision of the Fagerström Tolerance Questionnaire. Br J Addict. (1991) 86:1119–27. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.1991.tb01879.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Fagerström KO, Kunze M, Schoberberger R, Breslau N, Hughes JR, Hurt RD, et al. Nicotine dependence versus smoking prevalence: comparisons among countries and categories of smokers. Tobacco Control. (1996) 5:52–6. doi: 10.1136/tc.5.1.52

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Ríos-Bedoya CF, Snedecor SM, Pomerleau CS, Pomerleau OF. Association of withdrawal features with nicotine dependence as measured by the Fagerström Test for Nicotine Dependence (FTND). Addict Behav. (2008) 33:1086–9. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Sun Y, Li S. Testing the effect of risk on intertemporal choice in the Chinese cultural context. J Soc Psychol. (2011) 151:517–522. doi: 10.1080/00224545.2010.503719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Yan WS, Zhang RR, Lan Y, Li ZM, Li YH. Questionnaire-based maladaptive decision-coping patterns involved in binge eating among 1013 college students. Front Psychol. (2018) 9:609. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00609

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Golden CJ. A group version of the Stroop Color and Word Test. J Personal Assessment. (1975) 39:386–8. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa3904_10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Adleman NE, Menon V, Blasey CM, White CD, Warsofsky IS, Glover GH, et al2. A developmental fMRI study of the Stroop color-word task. NeuroImage. (2002) 16:61–75. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.1046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Patton JH, Stanford MS, Barratt ES. Factor structure of the Barratt impulsiveness scale. J Clin Psychol. (1995) 51:768–74. doi: 10.1002/1097-4679(199511)51:6<768::aid-jclp2270510607>3.0.co;2-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Ryu H, Lee JY, Choi A, Park S, Kim DJ, Choi JS. The relationship between impulsivity and internet gaming disorder in young adults: mediating effects of interpersonal relationships and depression. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2018) 15:458. doi: 10.3390/ijerph15030458

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Kale D, Stautz K, Cooper A. Impulsivity related personality traits and cigarette smoking in adults: a meta-analysis using the UPPS-P model of impulsivity and reward sensitivity. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2018) 185:149–67. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Zahalka EA, Seidler FJ, Lappi SE, McCook EC, Yanai J, Slotkin TA. Deficits in development of central cholinergic pathways caused by fetal nicotine exposure: differential effects on choline acetyltransferase activity and [3H] hemicholinium-3 binding. Neurotoxicol Teratol. (1992) 14:375–82. doi: 10.1016/0892-0362(92)90047-E

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. DeBry SC, Tiffany ST. Tobacco-induced neurotoxicity of adolescent cognitive development (TINACD): a proposed model for the development of impulsivity in nicotine dependence. Nicotine Tobacco Res. (2008) 10:11–25. doi: 10.1080/14622200701767811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Poltavski DV, Weatherly JN. Delay and probability discounting of multiple commodities in smokers and never-smokers using multiple-choice tasks. Behav Pharmacol. (2013) 24:659–67. doi: 10.1097/FBP.0000000000000010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Sloan FA, Wang Y. Economic theory and evidence on smoking behavior of adults. Addiction. (2008) 103:1777–85. doi: 10.1111/j.1360-0443.2008.02329.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Dalley JW, Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Impulsivity, compulsivity, and top-down cognitive control. Neuron. (2011) 69:680–94. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.01.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Naish KR, Vedelago L, MacKillop J, Amlung M. Effects of neuromodulation on cognitive performance in individuals exhibiting addictive behaviors: a systematic review. Drug Alcohol Depend. (2018) 192:338–51. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2018.08.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Everitt BJ, Robbins TW. Drug addiction: updating actions to habits to compulsions ten years on. Annual Rev Psychol. (2016) 67:23–50. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Teichner G, Horner MD, Roitzsch JC, Herron J, Thevos A. Substance abuse treatment outcomes for cognitively impaired and intact outpatients. Addict Behav. (2002) 27:751–63. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4603(01)00207-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Brand M, Young KS, Laier C, Wölfling K, Potenza MN. Integrating psychological and neurobiological considerations regarding the development and maintenance of specific Internet-use disorders: an Interaction of Person-Affect-Cognition-Execution (I-PACE) model. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2016) 71:252–66. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.08.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Moccia L, Pettorruso M, De Crescenzo F, De Risio L, di Nuzzo L, Martinotti G, et al. Neural correlates of cognitive control in gambling disorder: a systematic review of fMRI studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2017) 78:104–16. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.04.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Fauth-Bühler M, Mann K. Neurobiological correlates of internet gaming disorder: similarities to pathological gambling. Addict Behav. (2017) 64:349–56. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2015.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Di Nicola M, Tedeschi D, De Risio L, Pettorruso M, Martinotti G, Ruggeri F, et al. Co-occurrence of alcohol use disorder and behavioral addictions: relevance of impulsivity and craving. Drug Alcohol Dependence. (2015) 148:118–25. doi: 10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2014.12.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. King DL, Delfabbro PH, Griffiths MD, Gradisar M. Assessing clinical trials of Internet addiction treatment: a systematic review and CONSORT evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. (2011) 31:1110–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Antonelli M, Fattore L, Sestito L, Di Giuda D, Diana M, Addolorato G. Transcranial magnetic stimulation: a review about its efficacy in the treatment of alcohol, tobacco and cocaine addiction. Addictive behav. (2020) 114:106760. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2020.106760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Lefaucheur JP, Aleman A, Baeken C, Benninger DH, Brunelin J, Di Lazzaro V, et al. Evidence-based guidelines on the therapeutic use of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS): An update (2014-2018). Clin Neurophysiol. (2020) 131:474–528. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2020.02.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. King DL, Delfabbro PH, Wu A, Doh YY, Kuss DJ, Pallesen S, et al. Treatment of Internet gaming disorder: An international systematic review and CONSORT evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. (2017) 54:123–33. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2017.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: Internet Gaming Disorder, Nicotine Dependence, inhibitory control, reward discounting, impulsivity

Citation: Yan W-S, Chen R-T, Liu M-M and Zheng D-H (2021) Monetary Reward Discounting, Inhibitory Control, and Trait Impulsivity in Young Adults With Internet Gaming Disorder and Nicotine Dependence. Front. Psychiatry 12:628933. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.628933

Received: 13 November 2020; Accepted: 05 January 2021;
Published: 28 January 2021.

Edited by:

Xingchao Wang, Shanxi University, China

Reviewed by:

Marco Di Nicola, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy
Hollis C. Karoly, Colorado State University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Yan, Chen, Liu and Zheng. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Wan-Sen Yan, yanwansen@163.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.