Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 15 March 2021
Sec. Psychopathology

Self-Schemas and Self-Esteem Discrepancies in Subclinical Paranoia: The Essential Role of Depressive Symptoms

  • 1Departament de Psicologia Clínica i de la Salut, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona, Cerdanyola del Vallès, Spain
  • 2Department of Psychology, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA, United States
  • 3School of Psychology, Complutense University of Madrid, Pozuelo de Alarcón, Spain
  • 4Department of Psychology, University of Camilo José Cela, Villanueva de la Cañada, Spain
  • 5Department of Psychology, University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign, Champaign, IL, United States
  • 6Sant Pere Claver - Fundació Sanitària, Barcelona, Spain
  • 7Centre for Biomedical Research in Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Madrid, Spain

Background : Self-concepts are being intensively investigated in relation to paranoia, but research has shown some contradictory findings. Studying subclinical phenomena in a non-clinical population should allow for a clearer understanding given that clinical confounding factors are avoided. We explored self-esteem, self-schemas, and implicit/explicit self-esteem discrepancies in three non-clinical groups with different psychopathological traits and a control group.

Methods: Participants with elevated trait-paranoia (n = 41), depressive symptoms (n = 34), a combination of both traits (n = 32), and a control group (n = 71) were assessed on implicit and explicit self-esteem, self-schemas, depression, and paranoia. A dimensional approach with the total sample (n = 208) was also used to complement the information provided by the group approach.

Results: All groups presented similar and positive levels of implicit self-esteem. Trait-paranoia participants had similar levels of explicit self-esteem and self-schemas compared with the control group. However, the group with a combination of trait-paranoia and depressive symptoms showed the lowest levels of positive self-schemas and self-esteem. Furthermore, this group and the control group displayed implicit/explicit self-esteem discrepancies, although in opposite directions and with different implications. The dimensional approach revealed associations of trait-paranoia and depressive symptoms with poor explicit self-esteem and self-schemas but not with implicit self-esteem.

Conclusions: Trait-paranoia participants showed different self-representations depending on whether depressive symptoms were present or not. The interaction between subclinical neurotic and psychotic traits entailed a detrimental self-representation that might increase the risk for psychopathology.

Introduction

Understanding psychopathological phenomena as a dimensional continuum, which range from mental health to severe mental illness, have a long tradition (15). Psychosis is currently conceptualized as a dynamic continuum that ranges from individual differences in schizotypy traits and subtle psychotic-like experiences through at-risk mental states (ARMS) or prodromal states to schizophrenia-spectrum disorders (68). Likewise, depression can be conceived as a dynamic continuum that extends from non-clinical experiences or feelings of depression to severe depressive disorders (911). Studying subclinical phenomena in non-clinical populations complements the information obtained from ill participants and provides a “cleaner” laboratory to grasp subtle psychological constructs by avoiding confusion factors such as elevated symptom severity, high comorbidity, medication side-effects, and the chronicity of the disorder itself, thus allowing to elucidate mechanisms of disorder risk, resilience, and onset (12).

Two psychological constructs that have attracted considerable attention are self-esteem (SE) and self-schemas (SC), which are widely involved not only in depression but also in psychosis, and more specifically, in paranoia (13, 14). Low SE is one of the diagnostic criteria for a DSM-5 diagnosis of major depressive disorder (15). However, it has also been demonstrated that neuroticism and low SE are risk factors for psychosis (16, 17). Besides, it is well-known that there is a high association between depression, low SE, negative SC, and persecutory delusions (18, 19), and it has even been suggested that some forms of paranoid schizophrenia might be camouflaged depression (20). Depression is also associated with severity, distress, prognosis, and relapse of psychotic symptoms (21), and it is estimated that comorbidity with schizophrenia occurs in 50% of patients (22). It has also been shown that negative SC (in combination with anxiety and negative-other self-evaluations), but not positive SC or low SE, were associated with non-clinical paranoia (23). However, other studies have found that low positive SC (24, 25) and low SE (2628) correlated with paranoia in non-clinical populations. Thus, the precise pattern of associations between SC, SE, and paranoia in non-clinical populations remains unclear.

Regarding persecutory delusions, Bentall et al. (29) proposed a model based on a cycle of mutual influences between causal attributions and self-representations. They argued that in order to maintain deactivated implicit negative beliefs about the self, people with persecutory delusions attribute negative events to external agents. Although this “defensive model” did not intend to make any prediction about the role of explicit SE, some studies have ruled it out because low levels of explicit SE in paranoid patients have been documented. This defensive model only assumed, and showed evidence from implicit measures, that paranoid patients, like depressed people, have latent negative beliefs about the self. Nevertheless, recent findings are inconsistent with this assumption regarding paranoia, showing mixed results [see reviews: (18, 30, 31)]. In contrast, there is mounting evidence indicating that depressed patients show positive implicit SE, actually similar to that of healthy controls (3235). One study assessed implicit SE using three different paradigms and found positive implicit SE for depressed participants in the three measures (32). Indeed, it has been demonstrated that high implicit SE is not necessarily advantageous for psychological health, and that discrepancies between explicit SE and implicit SE could be more damaging than high or low SE per se (36).

The study of discrepancies between explicit SE and implicit SE seems critical and intimately connected with paranoia, but the evidence is scarce (37). Discrepancies between explicit SE and implicit SE can occur in two different ways: higher explicit SE than implicit SE or higher implicit SE than explicit SE (36, 38), and it has been found that SE discrepancies in either direction could be damaging for psychological health (36, 37). As Bentall and colleagues proposed (29), measuring discrepancies between implicit SE and explicit SE might be especially relevant for testing the defensive model of paranoia. It could be expected that paranoid patients would have lower implicit SE than explicit SE. In addition, comparing levels of implicit SE between patients with persecutory delusions and patients without paranoia or healthy controls should provide meaningful evidence for a defensive model. To date, research has yielded contradictory results in clinical populations, with some studies supporting or partially supporting the defensive model (3941), and others not (4245). However, two of the three studies that assessed discrepancies directly comparing z-scores of implicit SE and explicit SE within groups of paranoid and depressed patients, and healthy controls reported no SE discrepancies in paranoid patients. Curiously, though, Kesting and colleagues (42) found differences between levels of explicit SE and implicit SE in healthy controls and depressed patients but not in acute and remitted deluded patients. Vázquez et al. (45) found similar results using identical groups but a different measure of implicit SE. In both studies, the depressed group showed the same pattern of discrepancy: higher implicit SE than explicit SE. Conversely, higher explicit SE than implicit SE was found in the healthy control group. Thus, it remains unclear whether differences between explicit SE and implicit SE characterize clinical and subclinical paranoid samples. To the best of our knowledge, no studies have tested SE discrepancies in non-clinical paranoia by directly comparing z-scores of implicit SE and explicit SE. Therefore, the present study was the first to examine SE discrepancies in subclinical paranoia. Moreover, it is the first time that levels of explicit SE and implicit SE, as well as SC, were compared among three non-clinical groups with elevations in different psychopathological traits: paranoia, depression, and a combination of the two.

The first goal of the present study was to examine levels and differential patterns of implicit SE and explicit SE, as well as SC, in non-clinical subjects with elevated levels of trait-paranoia with and without elevated levels of depressive symptoms, non-clinical subjects with high levels of depressive symptoms, and a control group. Secondly, discrepancies between z-scores of implicit SE and explicit SE were also explored within these four groups. Finally, a dimensional approach using the total sample (n = 208) was employed to explore whether trait-paranoia, depressive symptoms, and their interaction predicted SE, SC, positive discrepant SE (i.e., higher implicit SE than explicit SE), and negative discrepant SE (i.e., higher explicit SE than implicit SE).

Firstly, and according to Bentall et al. (29), it was hypothesized that trait-paranoia would be associated with negative implicit SE, whereas in line with recent research, depressive symptoms would be related to positive implicit SE. It was also predicted that subclinical paranoia and depression would be associated with low levels of explicit SE and SC. Specifically, we expected to find lower levels of explicit SE and SC in groups with elevated levels of depressive symptoms. Secondly, regarding the discrepancies between z-scores of explicit SE and implicit SE within groups, no discrepancies were expected in the trait-paranoia group. However, positive discrepant SE was predicted for the groups with depressive symptoms, while negative discrepant SE was expected for the control group. Finally, when using dimensional scores in the total sample, we hypothesized that trait-paranoia and depressive symptoms would be associated with explicit SE and SC, although a larger association with depressive symptoms was expected. We also predicted that trait-paranoia would not be related to any form of discrepant SE, whereas depressive symptoms would be related to positive discrepant SE.

Methods

Participants

The present study is part of an ongoing longitudinal project examining risk and resilience for psychosis (BLISS) (46). At time one, an unselected sample of 589 undergraduates at the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona was screened for schizotypy traits. Usable screening data was obtained from 547 participants (42 were excluded due to invalid protocols). At time two, a smaller sample was selected for intensive assessment and participants with high schizotypy scores were oversampled in order to ensure an adequate representation of schizotypy variance. A detailed description of the sample selection procedure has been provided elsewhere (46, 47). A total of 208 (out of 214) participants, which successfully completed the implicit self-esteem assessment as well as other measures described above, were included in this study. The mean age of this sample was 19.7 years (SD = 2.3) and 77.9% were women. Four different groups were also yielded:

The Depression group (DepG) included those 34 participants of the total sample (n = 206) who scored in the top quartile of the Beck Depression Inventory-II (BDI-II) (48) and had trait-paranoia levels below the top quartile (percentile 73) as measured by the Suspiciousness subscale (SPQ-S) of the Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire (SPQ) (49).

The Paranoia group (ParG) included those 41 participants of the total sample who scored in the top quartile of the SPQ-S and had levels of depressive symptoms below the top quartile as measured by the BDI-II.

The Mixed group (MixG) included those 32 participants of the total sample who scored in the top quartile on both trait-paranoia and depressive symptom measures (SPQ-S and BDI-II, respectively).

The Control group (ConG) included those 71 participants of the total sample who scored below the percentile 50 on both measures.

Materials and Procedure

The interviews were conducted by psychologists and advanced graduate students in clinical psychology who were trained in the administration of the measures and were unaware of participants' scores on the screening questionnaires. The study was approved by the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona (Spain) Ethics Committee and conformed to the Helsinki Declaration.

Implicit SE

The Go/No-Go Association Task (GNAT) (50) was employed to assess implicit SE. The GNAT is conceptually based on the Implicit Association Test (IAT) (51), but it does not need the direct involvement of an opposed target category to make inferences (50, 52). Therefore, the GNAT has the advantage of analyzing automatic responses between the attribute concepts (e.g., positive, negative) and a single target category (e.g., the self). Williams and Kaufman (53) have demonstrated its reliability and some studies have shown its convergent, discriminant, and predictive validity (54, 55).

The GNAT self-esteem version used in this study was presented using Inquisit (Millisecond Software, 1996–2007). It comprised 28 words (of which 14 were positive and 14 negative) and assessed the strength of the automatic associations between words related to the concept of “Self” (e.g., myself, I, participant's first name) and positive attributes (e.g., smart, competent) and negative attributes (e.g., unable, stupid) (41). The 28 stimulus words were validated in a study of positive and negative adjectives related to self-worth (56). The GNAT had two critical blocks (self-positive and self-negative) randomly presented; each block contains the first 20 practice trials and then 60 critical trials. For each trial, one word appears in the middle of the screen while informative labels for the correct response are fixed in the upper left and right corners. Participants had to press the space bar only if the word that appears in the middle of the screen (e.g., smart) belongs to one of the two informative labels (e.g., self, positive). If the word did not match the informative labels (e.g., unable), participants did not have to respond. Words appear for a maximum of up to 1,200 ms or until the participant presses the space bar. Participants are instructed to respond as fast and accurately as possible and they had immediate feedback in each trail: A green O followed a correct response while a red X followed an incorrect response.

To calculate the implicit SE score, reaction times in the positive-self blocks were subtracted from reaction times in the negative-self blocks. A positive score means that the participant is faster associating the self with positive adjectives than the self with negative adjectives. This would be interpreted as positive implicit SE, whereas a negative score suggests negative implicit SE. Although other indices (e.g., d') can be computed, reaction time indices have shown higher internal consistency (41) and much more internal reliability (50).

Explicit SE

To assess global explicit SE, a Spanish version (57) of the Rosenberg self-esteem scale (RSE) (58) was used. The RSE consists of five positively worded items and five negatively worded items (e.g., “I take a positive attitude toward myself”) measured on a four-point scale. Scores range from 0 to 30, where higher scores reflect higher explicit SE.

Discrepant SE

Following previous studies [e.g., (36, 37)], we created an index of discrepant SE. For each participant, standardized values of explicit SE were subtracted from standardized values of implicit SE. Then, in order to test whether trait-paranoia, depressive symptoms, and their interaction predicted a combination of higher levels of implicit SE than explicit SE (positive discrepant SE), or on the contrary, a combination of higher levels of explicit SE than implicit SE (negative discrepant SE), two continuous variables of discrepant SE were calculated. Positive discrepant SE variable included those participants with positive scores on discrepant SE (higher levels of implicit SE than explicit SE), whereas negative discrepant SE variable included those participants with negative scores on discrepant SE (higher levels of explicit SE than implicit SE). A score of 0 on discrepant SE (no differences between levels of implicit SE and explicit SE) indicated congruent SE.

Self-Schemas

The Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS) is a self-reported measure that assesses beliefs about the self and others (23), which yields subscale scores for positive-self, negative-self, positive-others, and negative-others. For the purpose of this study, only positive-self and negative-self subscales were used. Each subscale contains six items rated on a five-point scale, ranging from 0 to 4, where lower scores mean low positive or negative SC, respectively.

Schizotypal Personality Questionnaire

The SPQ (49) is a self-reported scale to evaluate DSM-III-R schizotypal personality disorder. It consists of 70 dichotomous yes-no questions and yields nine subscales, one for each DSM schizotypal trait. The eight-item SPQ-S was used to measure trait-paranoia in this sample (e.g.: “I am sure I am being talked about behind my back”).

Beck Depression Inventory-II

The BDI-II (48) was used to dimensionally measure depressive symptoms. It contains 21 items that are rated on a four-point scale ranging from 0 to 3. Total score ranges between 0 and 63, where higher scores indicate greater severity of depressive symptoms.

Statistical Analyses

All analyses were conducted using the Statistical Package for Social Sciences (SPSS), Version 19.0. Potential sex differences were tested with the Student's t test. To test for differences in sex and age among groups, a chi-square test and one-way ANOVA were respectively performed. Analyses of variance were used to compare groups on implicit SE, explicit SE, and discrepant SE and SC. To compare means, post hoc Tukey HSD or Tamhane tests were employed depending on the assumption of homogeneity of variances between groups. A mixed-model ANOVA with two within-subject variables (implicit SE and explicit SE) and one between-subject variable with four groups (DepG, ParG, MixG, and ConG) was conducted to test discrepancies between z-cores of implicit SE and explicit SE. Bonferroni correction for multiple comparisons was employed. Effects sizes were interpreted following Cohen (59): η2 = 0.01 as small, η2 = 0.06 as medium, and η2 = 0.14 as large. The strategy of comparing groups with “pure” trait-paranoia, depression, and mixed profiles allows for a comparison with clinical samples with depression and paranoia. At the same time, the continuous nature of measures and the non-clinical nature of the sample allowed for a complementary dimensional approach. Thus, a series of hierarchical linear regression were performed to explore the linear effect of paranoid traits, depressive symptoms, and their interaction term on the frequency of implicit SE, explicit SE, and discrepant SE and SC. Paranoia and depression scores were entered simultaneously at the first step to evaluate their unique contribution, and then the interaction term was entered at the second step to evaluate its effect beyond the main effects.

Results

No sex differences were found for implicit SE (t = 0.996, p = 0.312), explicit SE (t = 0.132, p = 0.895), negative SC (t = −0.544, p = 0.587), positive SC (t = 0.694, p = 0.488), positive discrepant SE (t = 0.238, p = 0.813), negative discrepant SE (t = −0.169, p = 0.866), SPQ-S (t = −0.027, p = 0.978), and BDI-II (t = −0.141, p = 0.888) in the overall sample. Only a weak association between age and positive SC (r = −0.183, p = 0.008) was found. Regarding the GNAT, the means of the reaction times on the positive-self and negative-self blocks were 534.34 ms (range: 371.63–727.69) and 561.16 ms (range: 407.18–769.42), respectively, for the whole sample. Therefore, the mean of the implicit SE score was 26.82 ms (range: −137.80–136.67) for the whole sample. There were no significant differences among groups on age (F[3,174] = 0.980, p = 0.404) or sex (χ2 = 2.396, p = 0.494). Descriptive data on paranoia and depression measures for all groups are presented in Table 1. As expected, the ParG and the MixG groups had higher levels of paranoia than the DepG and the ConG (F[3,174] = 235.65, η2 = 0.80, p < 0.001). Similarly, the depression and the mixed groups had higher levels of depressive symptoms than the paranoia and the control groups (F[3,174] = 129.91, η2 = 0.69, p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Means (standard deviations) and score ranges on paranoia and depression measures for groups.

Firstly, the results of ANOVAs comparing implicit SE and explicit SE, discrepant SE, as well as SC across groups are presented in Table 2. No significant differences were found among groups for implicit SE (F[3,174], η2 < 0.01); all groups displayed positive implicit SE. Conversely, there was a large group effect for explicit SE (F[3,171], η2 = 0.37). Post hoc tests indicated that the MixG had lower levels of explicit SE than the other groups (ConG and ParG: p < 0.001; DepG: p = 0.033), whereas the DepG showed lower levels than the ParG (p = 0.011) and the ConG (p < 0.001). Thus, the paranoia and control groups had similar levels of explicit SE (p = 0.306). There was also a large group effect for negative SC (F[3,174], η2 = 0.17), with the MixG showing higher negative SC than the ParG and ConG (all p-values <0.005). Positive SC was also significantly different across groups (F[3,174], η2 = 0.08). The MixG had a lower positive SC than the ConG (p = 0.002). Thus, the depression, paranoia, and control groups showed no significant differences for positive and negative SC. There was a large group effect for positive discrepant SE (F[3,74], η2 = 0.15), indicating that the MixG had a higher positive discrepancy than the ConG. A medium group effect for negative discrepant SE (F[3,93], η2 = 0.09) was also found, with the ParG showing a higher negative discrepancy than the MixG. No other differences were found among groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Mean differences (and standard deviations) in self-esteem and self-schema variables among groups.

Secondly, the analysis of differences between z-cores of implicit SE and explicit SE revealed that the main effect was not significant (F = 1.61, p = 0.207, η2 = 0.00), whereas a significant effect for group was found (F = 17.93, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.24). The ConG had a higher overall SE than the DepG and the MixG, while the ParG showed higher SE than the MixG (all p-values <0.001). A significant two-way interaction between SE and group was also obtained (F = 10.64, p < 0.001, η2 = 0.16). Within-group pairwise comparisons revealed a discrepancy between explicit SE and implicit SE in the ConG (p < 0.001, η2 = 0.06) and the MixG (p < 0.001, η2 = 0.09). The ConG had higher explicit SE than implicit SE, whereas the opposite pattern was found in the MG. The depression and paranoia groups did not show a statistically significant discrepancy (p = 0.138, η2 = 0.01; p = 0.182, η2 = 0.01; respectively). Between-group pairwise comparisons for explicit SE and implicit SE revealed similar results to those described above with one-way ANOVA analyses. Means of implicit SE and explicit SE z-scores for all groups are depicted in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Means of explicit and implicit self-esteem z-scores in the control (CG), depressive (DG), paranoia (PG), and mixed (MG) groups.

Finally, the results of the regression models conducted with the total sample (i.e., dimensional approach) are presented in Table 3. Of the total sample (n = 208), three participants had RSE invalid data; therefore, for explicit SE and discrepant SE variables, the total sample was 205 participants. Of these, 96 had positive discrepant SE (higher levels of implicit SE than explicit SE), 108 had negative discrepant SE (higher levels of explicit SE than implicit SE), and one participant had congruent SE (no differences among implicit SE and explicit SE). For each predictor (trait-paranoia, depressive symptoms, and their interaction), the standardized regression coefficients (β), semipartial r2, and effect sizes f 2 are reported. Trait-paranoia predicted explicit SE and negative SC, while a trend was observed for positive SC (p = 0.059). In contrast, trait-paranoia was not associated with implicit SE or with any type of discrepant SE. Depressive symptoms were negatively associated with explicit SE and positive SC but positively associated with negative SC and positive and negative discrepant SE. Conversely, depressive symptoms did not predict implicit SE. Lastly, the interaction term between trait-paranoia and depressive symptoms predicted levels of explicit SE beyond the main effects (and a trend was observed for negative SC; p = 0.057). No more significant associations were found.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Linear regressions of self-esteem and self-schemas measures (n = 208).

Discussion

The present study compared three non-clinical groups with paranoia, depression symptoms, and a combination of the two features, in addition to a control group, to shed light on the association of these constructs with implicit SE, explicit SE, and discrepant SE and SC. In line with our hypotheses, we found no discrepancies between explicit SE and implicit SE in the trait-paranoia group; although explicit SE was slightly higher than implicit SE, both measures were positive and did not significantly differ. Likewise, dimensional analyses revealed that neither trait-paranoia nor the interaction between trait-paranoia and depressive symptoms were associated with implicit SE, thus replicating and expanding the results found by Cicero and Kerns (26) who found that implicit SE was unassociated with paranoia in non-clinical participants using a different measure of implicit SE. Finally, positive and similar levels of implicit SE were found across all groups. Therefore, these findings do not support Bentall's defensive model in a non-clinical population with trait-paranoia, regardless of whether depressive symptoms were present or not. It is likely that some phenomena in the psychotic spectrum may differ in their expression depending on the severity of the associated impairment. Thus, it could be argued that while subclinical paranoid experiences do not reach the severity of persecutory delusions implicit SE would remain preserved and psychotic defenses would not be triggered. However, two of three studies with paranoid patients that directly compared z-cores of implicit SE and explicit SE (42, 45) also found no discrepancies in the paranoid group. Furthermore, the fact that these studies, and the present study, employed different paradigms to assess implicit SE seems to strengthen the hypothesis that paranoia is not associated with SE discrepancies or low implicit SE. Alternatively, it has been suggested that SE discrepancies would differ depending on the person's belief about the deservedness of the persecutory delusion. Whereas, “bad-me” paranoia patients believe that the persecution is deserved, basically because the self is viewed as bad, “poor-me” paranoia patients believe that the persecution is not deserved (60). Nakamura and collaborators (40) found that poor-me paranoia patients, but not Bad-me, showed a SE discrepancy. However, this finding should be interpreted with caution given the small sample size recruited of the poor-me paranoia group (n = 14) and the fact that when both paranoid groups were jointly assessed (n = 35), no SE discrepancy was found. Actually, it has been claimed that the concept of deservedness in paranoia is not a specific categorical distinction (61), but rather a dimensional facet of paranoia (62).

As expected, the control group presented a statistically significant discrepancy, with higher levels of explicit than implicit SE. Nevertheless, both measures of SE were positive, reflecting optimal levels of implicit SE and explicit SE. Thus, it could be inferred that the control group had secure SE, as there is no need to adopt defensive strategies to protect the self from eventual hazards (63). Kesting et al. (42) speculated that representing oneself explicitly in a more positive way than oneself implicitly feels might be protective. Then, this pattern of self-representation would be adaptive or normative to healthy people reflecting normal cognitive processes. However, it might also simply reflect a social desirability bias when responding to personality or attitudinal questionnaires (64). In contrast, the group with depressive symptoms showed higher implicit SE than explicit SE; however, this difference was not statistically significant, contrary to what was found in clinical samples (42, 45). Probably, the fact that our participants did not have clinical depression and, therefore, their levels of explicit SE remained relatively preserved may explain the differences in SE discrepancies found among clinical and non-clinical samples with depressive symptoms. Nevertheless, in line with other studies (32, 34, 35, 65) and in accordance with our hypothesis, the depression group showed positive or normal levels of implicit SE. Finally, the mixed group did have a significant positive discrepancy (as expected for the depression group), showing low explicit SE and positive implicit SE. Thus, the mixed group was the only group that presented insecure SE (36), specifically, damaged SE (66). However, this pattern of unbalanced self-representation where implicit SE was normal or positive and explicit SE was low does not match with the pattern of discrepancy predicted by Bentall and colleagues in paranoia patients (29). In sum, our participants with a combination of depressive symptoms and trait-paranoia showed a similar pattern to that of clinically depressed patients (33, 41, 42, 45) and indicate different self-representations in trait-paranoia individuals depending on whether depressive symptoms were present or not. This emphasizes the relevance of taking into account the interaction between depressive and paranoid symptoms and suggests that this interaction should be considered regarding the etiology and prognosis of paranoid ideation. Notably, patients with schizophrenia who had suffered depression at the prodromal stage exhibited more severe first psychotic episodes and more depressive and positive symptoms over the initial 5-year course (67).

Regarding explicit SE, our hypotheses were partially supported, as the depression and mixed groups showed the lowest levels. In contrast, the paranoia group had similar levels of explicit SE to the control group. Although recent reviews (18, 31) indicated an association of low explicit SE with paranoia in patients and non-clinical populations, some studies did not control for depressive symptoms while others found positive or normal levels of SE in paranoid patients (41, 60, 68). Furthermore, one study with a large sample size also found no differences in explicit SE between high and low non-clinical paranoid participants (27). It might be that low levels of explicit SE in paranoia were mainly dependent on the neurotic processes that, as many studies pointed out [e.g., (69, 70)], are frequently involved in the formation and maintenance of psychotic delusions.

A slightly different picture appears concerning positive and negative SC. The depression and paranoia groups had slightly low levels of positive SC and high levels of negative SC than the control group; however, this difference did not reach statistical significance. Thus, contrary to our expectations, the depression and paranoia groups showed similar levels of SC to the control group. This seems to indicate that as long as paranoia traits and depressive symptoms do not exceed the clinical threshold, SC will remain relatively preserved. Our results are in agreement with Taylor et al. (71), who found very similar levels of SC in non-clinical participants who endorsed some schizotypy experiences, and with Espinosa and colleagues (72), who reported similar levels of negative beliefs about the self between depressed and paranoid patients. Conversely, levels of positive and negative SC in the group with a combination of trait-paranoia and depressive symptoms were lower than in the control group and comparable with those found in individuals at clinical high risk of developing psychosis (71, 73, 74). Therefore, once again, the mixed group displayed an unfavorable conscious self-representation that seems relevant for both the negative and the positive dimensions of SC.

Finally, whereas the dimensional approach with the total sample revealed significant associations of trait-paranoia with explicit SE and negative SC (only a trend was present for positive SC), depressive symptoms were related to explicit SE, positive and negative SC, and SE discrepancies. The fact that in the dimensional approach, where the whole sample was examined, trait-paranoia was not associated with any type of discrepant SE is in line with the finding that the paranoia group did not show a discrepancy between explicit SE and implicit SE. Instead, depressive symptoms in the total sample were related to both positive and negative discrepant SE, although the association of depressive symptoms with the positive discrepancy was larger, which is in line with findings in clinical depression (33, 42, 45). Participants with negative discrepant SE would be more susceptible to have fluctuating SE discrepancies because having low implicit SE and high explicit SE is associated with an unstable explicit SE (75), which can partly explain the association of depressive symptoms with negative discrepant SE. It is also noteworthy that the interaction term negatively predicted explicit SE beyond the main effects of trait-paranoia and depressive symptom, and was very close to statistical significance in negative SC. This seems to confirm, also in the total sample, that the interaction between trait-paranoia and depressive symptoms aggravates the negative self-concept of individuals, which might increase the risk for psychopathology. The association of depressive symptoms with explicit SE was of a large magnitude, and very close to a medium with positive discrepant SE, whereas the other effect sizes were small. Numerous studies in clinical and non-clinical populations also found significant associations of SE and SC with paranoia but, as this study pointed out, depressive symptoms boosted these associations, and effect sizes must be reported to quantify the magnitude of the relationship (76, 77). Only then a clearer picture of the association between self-representation and paranoia can be obtained.

Concerning the limitations of this study, its cross-sectional nature prevents drawing conclusions about causal effects. Furthermore, given that the participants are pursuing a Psychology degree, it is possible that their psychological knowledge and the awareness of some phenomena may bias their reports and also limits the generalizability of the findings to community samples. Another limitation of the study is that an a priori power analysis to determine sample sizes was not performed. Finally, it has been argued that implicit SE is only an impoverished measure of explicit SE and it should not be labeled as such (78). Although the GNAT overcame some of the weaknesses of previous measures of implicit SE, the precise nature of the unconscious self-related association that the GNAT, or other implicit SE tasks, assesses is still a matter of debate. Further research is needed for a better understanding of the measures that seek to delve into the non-conscious psychological processes. Similarly, further studies should be carried out in order to gain more in-depth knowledge about SE discrepancies in clinical and subclinical paranoia, as reducing maladaptive SE discrepancies could be an effective strategy to reduce paranoid ideation (37, 79).

The present study showed that levels of positive and negative SC, explicit SE, and SE discrepancies in subclinical paranoia depend on the presence and levels of depressive symptoms. In contrast, positive and similar levels of implicit SE were found in subclinical paranoia regardless of the presence of depressive symptoms. In line with recent research, our findings indicate that paranoia was related to negative explicit SE, but no association was found between paranoia and implicit SE (30, 43). Thus, individuals with subclinical paranoia would present different self-representations depending on whether depressive symptoms were present or not. The combination of trait-paranoia and depressive symptoms yielded an unfavorable conscious self-concept, suggesting that the interaction between subclinical neurotic and psychotic traits entailed a detrimental self-representation that could entail an increased risk for the development of psychopathology.

Ethical Standards

The authors assert that all procedures contributing to this work comply with the ethical standards of the relevant national and institutional committees on human experimentation and with the Helsinki Declaration of 1975, as revised in 2008. All participants provided an informed consent.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Committee of the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona (Comissió d'Ética en l'Experimentació Animal i Humana (CEEAH); number 701H-JS). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

MM, SB, and NB-V conceived the study. SB, TS, and NB-V interviewed participants and collected data. MM and TK analyzed the data. MM, TK, and NB-V wrote the paper. TS, CV, and RE critically revised the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Bleuler E. Textbook of Psychiatry. Oxford: Macmillian (1924).

Google Scholar

2. Bowlby J. Personality and Mental Illness: An Essay in Psychiatric Diagnosis. London: Paul, Trench, Trubner (1940).

Google Scholar

3. Claridge G. Origins of Mental Illness. Oxford: Basil Blackwell Ltd (1985).

4. Hoch A. A Guide to the Descriptive Study of Personality: With Special Reference to the Taking of Anamneses of Cases With Psychoses. Utica, N.Y.: State hospital Press (1913).

5. Kretschmer E. A Text-Book of Medical Psychology. London: Oxford University Press, H. Milford (1934).

Google Scholar

6. Claridge G, Beech T. Fully and quasi-dimensional constructions of schizotypy. In: Raine A, Lencz T, Mednick S, editors. Schizotypal Personality Disorder. Cambridge, England: Cambridge University Press (1995). p. 192–216. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511759031.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Kwapil TR, Barrantes-Vidal N. Schizotypy: looking back and moving forward. Schizophr Bull. (2015) 41:S366–73. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. van Os J, Linscott RJ, Myin-Germeys I, Delespaul P, Krabbendam L. A systematic review and meta-analysis of the psychosis continuum: evidence for a psychosis proneness–persistence–impairment model of psychotic disorder. Psychol Med. (2009) 39:179–95. doi: 10.1017/S0033291708003814

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Arieti SA, Bemporad J. Severe and Mild Depression. New York: Basic Books (1978).

10. Blatt SJ, D'Affilitti JP, Quinland DM. Experiences of depression in normal young adults. J Abnorm Psychol. (1976) 85:383–9. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.85.4.383

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Bowins B. Depression: discrete or continuous? Psychopathology. (2015) 48:69–78. doi: 10.1159/000366504

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Barrantes-Vidal N, Grant P, Kwapil TR. The role of schizotypy in the study of the etiology of schizophrenia-spectrum disorders. Schizophr Bull. (2015) 41:S408–16. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbu191

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Bentall RP, Kinderman P, Kaney S. The self, attributional processes and abnormal beliefs: Towards a model of persecutory delusions. Behav Res Ther. (1994) 32:331–41. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(94)90131-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Fowler D, Hodgekins J, Garety P, Freeman D, Kuipers E, Dunn G, et al. Negative cognition, depressed mood, and paranoia: a longitudinal pathway analysis using structural equation modeling. Schizophr Bull. (2012) 38:1063–73. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. American Psychiatric Publishing (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Krabbendam L, Janssen I, Bak M, Bijl RV, de Graff R, van Os J. Neuroticism and low self-esteem as risk factors for psychosis. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2002) 37:1–6. doi: 10.1007/s127-002-8207-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Ben-Zeev D, Granholm E, Cafri G. Symptoms of depression and anxiety mediate the relationship between self-esteem and paranoia. Schizophr Res. (2009) 115:377–78. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2009.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Kesting M-L, Lincoln TM. The relevance of self-esteem and self-schemas to persecutory delusions: a systematic review. Compr Psychiatry. (2013) 54:766–89. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2013.03.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Smith B, Fowler DG, Freeman D, Bebbington P, Bashforth H, Garety P, et al. Emotion and psychosis: links between depression, self-esteem, negative schematic beliefs and delusions and hallucinations. Schizophr Res. (2006) 86:181–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2006.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Zigler E, Glick M. Is paranoid schizophrenia really camouflaged depression? Am Psychol. (1988) 43:284–90. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.43.4.284

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Hartley S, Barrowclough C, Haddock G. Anxiety and depression in psychosis: a systematic review of associations with positive psychotic symptoms. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2013) 128:327–46. doi: 10.1111/acps.12080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Buckley PF, Miller BJ, Lehrer DS, Castle DJ. Psychiatric comorbidities and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. (2009) 35:383–402. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Fowler D, Freeman D, Smith B, Kuipers E, Bebbington P, Bashforth H, et al. The Brief Core Schema Scales (BCSS) : psychometric properties and associations with paranoia and grandiosity in non-clinical and psychosis samples. Psychol Med. (2006) 36:749–59. doi: 10.1017/S0033291706007355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Gracie A, Freeman D, Green S, Garety PA, Kuipers E, Hardy A, et al. The association between traumatic experience, paranoia and hallucinations: a test of the predictions of psychological models. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2007) 116:280–89. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2007.01011.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Green CEL, Freeman D, Kuipers E, Bebbington P, Fowler D, Dunn G, et al. Paranoid explanations of experience: a novel experimental study. Behav Cogn Psychother. (2011) 39:21–34. doi: 10.1017/S1352465810000457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Cicero DC, Kerns JG. Is paranoia a defence against or an expression of low self-esteem? Eur J Pers. (2011) 25:326–35. doi: 10.1002/per.794

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Combs DR, Penn DL, Chadwick PD, Trower P, Michael CO, Basso MR. Subtypes of paranoia in a nonclinical sample. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. (2007) 12:537–53. doi: 10.1080/13546800701707306

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Ellett L, Lopes B, Chadwick P. Paranoia in a nonclinical population of college students. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2003) 191:425–30. doi: 10.1097/01.NMD.0000081646.33030.EF

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Bentall RP, Corcoran R, Howard R, Blackwood N, Kinderman P. Persecutory delusions: a review and theoretical integration. Clin Psychol Rev. (2001) 21:1143–92. doi: 10.1016/S0272-7358(01)00106-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Murphy P, Bentall RP, Freeman D, O'Rourke S, Hutton P. The “paranoia-as-defence” model of persecutory delusions: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry. (2018) 5:1–16. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(18)30339-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Tiernan B, Tracey R, Shannon C. Paranoia and self-concepts in psychosis: a systematic review of the literature. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 216:303–13. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. De Raedt R, Schacht R, Franck E, De Houwer J. Self-esteem and depression revisited: Implicit positive self-esteem in depressed patients? Behav Res Ther. (2006) 44:1017–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Franck E, De Raedt R, De Houwer J. Activation of latent self-schemas as a cognitive vulnerability factor for depression: the potential role of implicit self-esteem. Cogn Emot. (2008) 22:1588–99. doi: 10.1080/02699930801921271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Greenwald AG, Banaji MR, Rudman LA, Farnham SD, Nosek BA, Mellot DS. An unified theory of implicit attitudes, stereotypes, self-esteem, and self-concept. Psychol Rev. (2002) 109:3–25. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.109.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Risch AK, Buba A, Birk U, Morina N, Steffens MC, Stangier U. Implicit self-esteem in recurrently depressed patients. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2010) 41:199–206. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Schröder-Abé M, Rudolph A, Schütz A. High implicit self-esteem is not necessarily advantageous: discrepancies between explicit and implicit self-esteem and their relationship with anger expression and psychological health. Eur J Pers. (2007) 21:319–39. doi: 10.1002/per.626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Valiente C, Espinosa R, Villavicencio P, Cantero D, Fuentenebro F. Adversity and persecutory ideation: a moderated mediational model. Psychiatry Res. (2017) 258:51–8. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2017.09.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Zeigler-Hill V. The Connections between self-esteem and psychopathology. J Contemp Psychother. (2011) 41:157–64. doi: 10.1007/s10879-010-9167-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Moritz S, Werner R, von Collani G. The inferiority complex in paranoia readdressed: a study with the Implicit Association Test. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry. (2006) 11:402–15. doi: 10.1080/13546800444000263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Nakamura M, Hayakawa T, Okamura A, Kohigashi M, Fukui K, Narumoto J. Defensive function of persecutory delusion and discrepancy between explicit and implicit self-esteem in schizophrenia: study using the Brief Implicit Association Test. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. (2015) 11:33–40. doi: 10.2147/NDT.S73906

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Valiente C, Cantero D, Vázquez C, Sánchez A, Provencio M, Espinosa R. Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancies in paranoia and depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (2011) 120:691–99. doi: 10.1037/a0022856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Kesting M-L, Mehl S, Rief W, Lindenmeyer J, Lincoln TM. When paranoia fails to enhance self-esteem: explicit and implicit self-esteem and its discrepancy in patients with persecutory delusions compared to depressed and healthy controls. Psychiatry Res. (2011) 186:197–202. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Monsonet M, Kwapil TR, Barrantes-Vidal N. Deconstructing the relationships between self-esteem and paranoia in early psychosis: an experience sampling study. Br J Clin Psychol. (2020) 59:503–23. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12263

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. MacKinnon K, Newman-Taylor K, Stopa L. Persecutory delusions and the self: an investigation of implicit and explicit self-esteem. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2011) 42:54–64. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Vázquez C, Diez-Alegría C, Hernández-Lloreda MJ, Moreno MN. Implicit and explicit self-schema in active deluded, remitted deluded, and depressed patients. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2008) 39:587–99. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2008.01.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Barrantes-Vidal N, Chun CA, Myin-Germeys I, Kwapil TR. Psychometric schizotypy predicts psychotic-like, paranoid, and negative symptoms in daily life. J Abnorm Psychol. (2013) 122:1077–87. doi: 10.1037/a0034793

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Barrantes-Vidal N, Gross GM, Sheinbaum T, Mitjavila M, Ballespí S, Kwapil TR. Positive and negative schizotypy are associated with prodromal and schizophrenia-spectrum symptoms. Schizophr Res. (2013) 145:50–5. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2013.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. BDI–II. Beck Depression Inventory–Second Edition Manual. San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation (1996). doi: 10.1037/t00742-000

CrossRef Full Text

49. Raine A. The SPQ: a scale for the assessment of schizotypal personality based on DSM-III-R criteria. Schizophr Bull. (1991) 17:555–64. doi: 10.1093/schbul/17.4.555

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Nosek BA, Banaji MR. The Go/No-Go association task. Soc Cogn. (2001) 19:625–64. doi: 10.1521/soco.19.6.625.20886

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Greenwald AG, Farnham SD. Using the implicit association test to measure self-esteem and self-concept. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2000) 79:1022–38. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.1022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. De Houwer J. The Implicit Association Test as a tool for studying dysfunctional associations in psychopathology: strengths and limitations. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2002) 33:115–33. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7916(02)00024-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Williams BJ, Kaufman LM. Reliability of the Go/No Go Association Task. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2012) 48:879–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jesp.2012.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Spalding LR, Hardin CD. Unconscious unease and self- handicapping: behavioral consequences of individual differences in spontaneous and deliberate self-esteem. Psychol Sci. (1999) 10:535–39. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.00202

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Teachman BA. Evaluating implicit spider fear associations using the Go/No Go Association Task. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2007) 38:156–67. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Jiménez F, Vázquez C, Hernangómez L. Adjetivos en castellano de contenido depresivo autorreferente y de contenido neutral: normas de emocionalidad y frecuencia subjectiva de uso. Rev Psicopatol Psicol Clín. (1998) 3:199–215. doi: 10.5944/rppc.vol.3.num.3.1998.3866

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Martín-albo J, Núñez JL, Navarro JG, Grijalvo F. The rosenberg self-esteem scale: translation and validation in University Students. Span J Psychol. (2007) 10:458–67. doi: 10.1017/S1138741600006727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press (1965). doi: 10.1515/9781400876136

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. (1992) 112:155–59. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Chadwick PDJ, Trower P, Juusti-Butler T-M, Maguire N. Phenomenological evidence for two types of paranoia. Psychopathology. (2005) 38:327–33. doi: 10.1159/000089453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Bentall RP, Rowse G, Kinderman P, Blackwood N, Howard R, Moore R, et al. Paranoid delusions in schizophrenia spectrum disorders and depression: the transdiagnostic role of expectations of negative events and negative self-esteem. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2008) 196:375–83. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0b013e31817108db

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Freeman D. Suspicious minds: the psychology of persecutory delusions. Clin Psychol Rev. (2007) 27:425–57. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Kernis MH. Toward conceptualization of Self-esteem. Psychol Inq. (2003) 14:27–31. doi: 10.1207/S15327965PLI1401_02

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Paulhus DL. Measurement and control of response bias. In: Robinson JP, Shaver PR, Wrightsman LS, editors. Measures of Personality and Social Psychological Attitudes. San Diego: Academic Press (1991). p. 17–59. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-590241-0.50006-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Gemar MC, Segal ZV, Sagrati S, Kennedy SJ. Mood-induced changes on the Implicit Association Test in recovered depressed patients. J Abnorm Psychol. (2001) 110:282–89. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Schröder-Abé M, Rudolph A, Wiesner A, Schütz A. Self-esteem discrepancies and defensive reactions to social feedback. Int J Psychol. (2007) 42:174–83. doi: 10.1080/00207590601068134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Häfner H, Maurer K, an der Heiden W. ABC Schizophrenia study: an overview of results since 1996. Soc Psyquiatry Psyquiatr Epidiemiol. (2013) 48:1021–31. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0700-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Lyon HM, Kaney S, Bentall RP. The defensive function of persecutory delusions. Evidence from attribution tasks. Br J Psychiatry. (1994) 164:637–46. doi: 10.1192/bjp.164.5.637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Freeman D, Garety PA. Connecting neurosis and psychosis: the direct influence of emotion on delusions and hallucinations. Behav Res Ther. (2003) 41:923–47. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(02)00104-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Salokangas RK, Schultze-Lutter F, Hietala J, Heinimaa M, From T, Ilonen T. Depression predicts persistence of paranoia in clinical high-risk patients to psychosis: results of the EPOS project. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2016) 51:247–57. doi: 10.1007/s00127-015-1160-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Taylor HE, Stewart SLK, Dunn G, Parker S, Fowler D, Morrison AP. Core schemas across the continuum of psychosis: a comparison of clinical and non-clinical groups. Behav Cogn Psychother. (2014) 42:718–30. doi: 10.1017/S1352465813000593

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Espinosa R, Valiente C, Bentall RP. Beliefs about self and others in persecutory delusions. Clín Salud. (2014) 25:187–95. doi: 10.1016/j.clysa.2014.10.005

CrossRef Full Text

73. Carol EE, Mittal VA. Resting cortisol level, self-concept, and putative familial environment in adolescents at ultra high-risk for psychotic disorders. Psychoneuroendocrinology. (2015) 57:26–36. doi: 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2015.03.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Stowkowy J, Liu L, Cadenhead KS, Cannon TD, Cornblatt BA, McGlashan TH, et al. Core schemas in youth at clinical high risk for psychosis. Behav Cogn Psychother. (2015) 44:203–13. doi: 10.1017/S1352465815000144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Zeigler-Hill V. Discrepancies between implicit and explicit self-esteem: Implications for narcissism and self-esteem instability. J Pers. (2006) 74:119–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2005.00371.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Kelley K, Preacher KJ. On effect size. Psychol Methods. (2012) 17:137–52. doi: 10.1037/a0028086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Willkinson L. Task force on statistical inference. Statistical methods in psychology journals. Am Psychol. (1999) 54:594–604. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.54.8.594

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Buhrmester MD, Blanton H, Swann WB. Implicit self-esteem: nature, measurement, and new way forward. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2011) 100:365–85. doi: 10.1037/a0021341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Espinosa R, Valiente C, Varese F, Bentall RP. Can we ameliorate psychotic symptoms by improving implicit self-esteem? A proof-of-concept experience sampling study of an evaluative classical conditioning intervention. J Nerv Ment Dis. (2018) 206:699–704. doi: 10.1097/NMD.0000000000000858

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: paranoia, self-esteem, self-schemas, depressive symptoms, self-esteem discrepancies, implicit self-esteem

Citation: Monsonet M, Ballespí S, Sheinbaum T, Valiente C, Espinosa R, Kwapil TR and Barrantes-Vidal N (2021) Self-Schemas and Self-Esteem Discrepancies in Subclinical Paranoia: The Essential Role of Depressive Symptoms. Front. Psychiatry 12:623755. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.623755

Received: 30 October 2020; Accepted: 02 February 2021;
Published: 15 March 2021.

Edited by:

Henry W. Chase, University of Pittsburgh, United States

Reviewed by:

Lyn Ellett, University of London, United Kingdom
Robert Christian Wolf, Heidelberg University, Germany

Copyright © 2021 Monsonet, Ballespí, Sheinbaum, Valiente, Espinosa, Kwapil and Barrantes-Vidal. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Neus Barrantes-Vidal, bmV1cy5iYXJyYW50ZXMmI3gwMDA0MDt1YWIuY2F0

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.