Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 09 June 2021
Sec. Mood Disorders

An Exploratory Study on Cross-Cultural Differences in Facial Emotion Recognition Between Adults From Malaysia and Australia

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine and Health Sciences, Universiti Putra Malaysia, Seri Kembangan, Malaysia
  • 2School of Psychological Sciences, Turner Institute for Brain and Mental Health, Monash University, Clayton, VIC, Australia

While culture and depression influence the way in which humans process emotion, these two areas of investigation are rarely combined. Therefore, the aim of this study was to investigate the difference in facial emotion recognition among Malaysian Malays and Australians with a European heritage with and without depression. A total of 88 participants took part in this study (Malays n = 47, Australians n = 41). All participants were screened using The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Clinician Version (SCID-5-CV) to assess the Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) diagnosis and they also completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI). This study consisted of the facial emotion recognition (FER) task whereby the participants were asked to look at facial images and determine the emotion depicted by each of the facial expressions. It was found that depression status and cultural group did not significantly influence overall FER accuracy. Malaysian participants without MDD and Australian participants with MDD performed quicker as compared to Australian participants without MDD on the FER task. Also, Malaysian participants more accurately recognized fear as compared to Australian participants. Future studies can focus on the extent of the influence and other aspects of culture and participant condition on facial emotion recognition.

Introduction

Emotions are of interest to researchers and clinicians because of their complex nature (1). They are fundamental to human survival and serve two primary functions; social purposes and coping with life tasks and experiences (1, 2).

In aiding social functions, emotions tell others how we feel, build, sustain, and end relationships by affecting how other people may interact with us and encourage and enable social interaction (3, 4). One of the ways that human beings communicate how we feel is through facial expressions. Humans have inherent facial expressions and body languages that are part of and adhere to both primary and complex emotions (5, 6). Facial expressions are an innate function of expressing and communicating internal emotional states (6, 7). The ability to identify and understand emotions from facial expressions is called facial emotion recognition.

Individuals with major depressive disorder (MDD) are known to have deficits in several areas of functioning like regulation and processing of emotions (810). More specifically, clinical researchers have demonstrated that MDD influences facial emotion recognition. When compared with healthy controls, those suffering from major depressive disorder (MDD) have impaired facial emotion recognition abilities (1116). Specifically, one such study found that adults with MDD had difficulties recognizing disgust (11). Furthermore, in studies investigating reaction times, it was found that those diagnosed with MDD had a slower reaction time in recognizing facial expression as compared to healthy controls (8, 17, 18).

Although research has shown that there is a certain level of universality in emotions, culture does influence emotions in many ways (1921). Individuals from different cultures differ in their construals, which in turn affects how individuals feel and process these feelings (22). Of the many areas of emotion influenced by culture, facial emotion recognition has been found to vary across cultures (21, 23). Research examining cultural differences in facial emotion recognition demonstrates that basic emotions are recognized across cultures at above chance levels (24). However, research has also shown that some cultures or populations recognize certain emotions better when compared to other cultural groups (2528). Research has tended to demonstrate that Caucasians may be better at recognizing emotions than Asians [e.g., (23, 28)], and that emotions such as happiness and surprise are easier to recognize and have fewer cultural differences than emotions like fear (23). This was further supported by Yang et al. (29) who found that fear was least recognized while happiness was most recognized by East-Asian participants when Asian stimuli was used. Similar results were found by Chen et al. (30) and Jack et al. (7) in their studies comparing Asian and Western participants; Asians had difficulties recognizing fear. The recognition of emotions is influenced by cultural factors such as the perception of emotion (23), increased understanding and familiarity of another culture, including the expressive patterns and communication styles (7, 31, 32), emotion display rules (2), contextual information (33), and socio-cultural norms regarding appropriate and proper display of emotions (24).

Despite research demonstrating cultural variation in facial emotion recognition and deficits associated with depression, these two bodies of research have rarely been combined. It is therefore essential that the influence of culture and depression on facial emotion recognition be examined. Researchers have ascertained that cultural values, norms and beliefs impacts how emotion is perceived, experienced, comprehended, interpreted and responded to (3438). Therefore, conducting a cross-cultural investigation, of facial emotion recognition in the context of MDD will allow us to better understand MDD.

The aim of the current study was to investigate the cultural differences between Malaysian Malays and Australian Caucasians, with and without depression, in facial emotion recognition. We hypothesized that there would be a significant difference between Malaysian Malay and Australian Caucasian participants in the overall accuracy and average response times of facial recognition of emotion. Specifically, Australian Caucasians participants would have better performance (more accurate response and faster response times) on facial recognition of emotion than Malaysian Malays. Second, participants without MDD would have better performance (more accurate responses and faster response times) on facial recognition of emotion compared to participants with MDD. Thirdly, participants with MDD would recognize faces depicting disgust better than participants without MDD. Lastly, Malaysian participants would recognize faces depicting happiness better than Australian participants, while Australian participants would recognize faces depicting fear better than Malaysian participants.

Materials and Methods

Participants

There were 88 participants, aged between 18 and 60 years of age, who participated in this study. Forty-seven participants were Malaysian Malays (i.e., both parents and all four grandparents identified as Malaysian Malay descent), whereby there were 25 participants without MDD and 22 participants with MDD. There were 41 Australian with European heritage participants (i.e., both parents and all four grandparents identifies as having Western European descent), of which 22 were without MDD and 19 were participants who had MDD. Participants were literate in either Malay or English and completed the study in Malay or English.

Inclusion Criteria

All participants had to be citizens of Malaysia or Australia. Only individuals of Malaysian Malay and Australian Caucasian background between the ages of 18 and 60 years old were included in the study. These participants had to be literate in Malay, and English, respectively.

Exclusion Criteria

Participants who had been diagnosed with substance dependence, had a history of psychosis, had organic brain injuries, were unable to understand simple spoken or written Malay or English, had a permanent physical injury that would impede responding on the task, had non-corrected vision and were of mixed parentage were excluded from the study.

Recruitment of Participants

The Australian Caucasian participants with MDD were recruited from the general community and clinics in Melbourne, while participants without MDD were recruited by posting advertisements around the Monash University Melbourne campus, and on community noticeboards, and social media sites. All Malaysian Malay participants with MDD were recruited from Hospital Kajang, Hospital Kuala Lumpur and Psychiatry Clinic, Universiti Putra Malaysia (UPM). Participants without MDD were recruited through advertisements on the UPM staff website, around the campus and on social media. Participants with MDD from both countries were outpatient participants. All participation was voluntary and informed consent forms had to be provided prior to the commencement of the clinical diagnostic assessment conducted by the clinical psychologist.

Sample Size Estimate

G*Power was used to calculate the sample size of this study (39, 40). A minimum sample size of 17 participants per group was sufficient in detecting a moderate effect size (α = 0.05, power = 0.80). However, a minimum of 19 participants per group was required to account for drop out and 10% attrition rates.

Measures

Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and Beck Depression Inventory-Malay (BDI-Malay)

The BDI is a self-report tool to assess the severity of depression (41, 42). Each item has a 4-point rating from 0 (no symptom) to 3 (severe symptom). The BDI score is obtained by summing the scores of all the items. The BDI has an internal consistency reliability coefficient ranging from 0.76 to 0.92 (42). The BDI-Malay is a self-report tool to assess the severity of depression (43). The Malay version of this tool has internal consistency values ranging from 0.71 to 0.91 (43).

Stimuli

The Facial Emotion Recognition (FER) task was developed and presented using the E-Prime software (Psychological Software Tools). We used pictures from the Montreal Set of Facial Displays of Emotion (26). Participants were shown two sets of pictures; Caucasian and Asian faces, in random order. Individuals in the pictures were expressing one of seven different emotions (neutral, happy, sad, angry, fear, disgust and shame). The participants were required, as quickly as possible, to correctly identify the emotion that was expressed by the individual in the picture. Prior to the presentation of the experimental stimuli the participants were presented with a set of test stimuli in order to familiarize participants with the task.

Procedure

Ethical approval was obtained from Universiti Putra Malaysia Research Ethics Committee, National Medical Research Register (NMRR), Malaysia and Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee (MUHREC). Data collection in both countries commenced in January 2016 and ended in January 2018. Data collection in Malaysia was carried out at the Behavioral Sciences Lab, Universiti Putra Malaysia. The Australian data was collected at the Behavioral Lab, Monash University Australia. In both cultural contexts the experimental testing were quiet and had optimal lighting. A computer, which was placed ~50 cm away from the participants, was used to run the FER task.

Prior to the study, all participants were screened using The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Clinician Version [SCID-5-CV; (44)] to assess MDD diagnosis. These interviews were audio recorded and assessed by an independent clinician. There was complete agreement between the raters. Following informed consent, participants completed the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) and then the FER task.

Analyses Plan

Data was analyzed using the Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS 25). Prior to data analysis, the dataset was screened for missing data and errors. Participants that had more than 20% missing data were excluded from the analysis (n = 1). After the dataset was cleaned, preliminary analyses were conducted to obtain the frequencies and sociodemographic characteristics of the sample. Each variable was then analyzed to check for any general violations of normality according to participant groups; kurtosis values were lower than 10 and skewness values were lower than 3 (45). Due to the reasons like inability to meet inclusion criteria and data removed due to missing data, equal numbers of participants were not able to be obtained for each study group. Hence, for the multivariate analysis of variances (MANOVAs) the Pillai's trace was used instead of the Wilk's lambda, as it is more robust (46, 47).

In order to test our hypotheses, two 2 (cultural group; Malay, Australian) × 2 (depression status; with MDD, without MDD) analysis of variances (ANOVAs) were conducted with FER accuracy rates or average response times as the dependent variables. We also conducted exploratory analyses to investigate recognition of specific emotion types. Here we used a 2 (cultural group; Malay Australian) × 2 (depression status; with MDD, without MDD) multivariate ANOVA (MANOVA), with the seven emotion types as the dependent variables. Given age, gender and education levels have been found to influence accuracy of recognizing emotions and reaction times (4853) we also conducted these analyses controlling for age, gender and education. In each instance a similar pattern of results emerged to that reported below; specifically, participant education levels, gender and age did not appear to have a significant effect on accuracy rates and reaction times.

Results

Demographic Data

Demographic data is presented in Table 1. The Malaysian participants (21.3% male, 78.7% female) were aged between 19 and 53 years old (M = 30.17, SD = 8.06). Australian participants (29.3% male, 68.3% female) were between 18 and 57 years old (M = 31.95, SD = 14.13). Mean BDI score for Malaysian participants without MDD was 2.72 (SD = 1.62) and participants with MDD was 32.64 (SD = 4.67). Mean BDI score for the Australian participants without MDD was 4.68 (SD = 4.65) and participants with MDD was 27.37 (SD = 10.30).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic data of participants.

A 2 (cultural group) × 2 (depression status) ANOVA with depression symptoms as the dependent variable revealed that there was a significant interaction effect, F(1, 84) = 8.54, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.09. Follow-up analyses revealed that, as expected, the Malaysian participants with MDD had greater depressive symptoms than the Malaysian participants without MDD, t(45) = −30.10, p = 0.00, d = 8.56. Similarly, the Australian participants with MDD scored significantly higher on the BDI than the Australian participants without MDD, t(39) = −9.31, p = 0.00, d = 2.84. While there was no significant difference between Malaysian and Australian participants without MDD in depressive symptoms, t(45) = −1.98, p = 0.05, d = 0.56, the Malaysian participants with MDD had significantly higher BDI scores than the Australian participants with MDD, t(39) = 2.16, p = 0.04, d = 0.66.

In terms of gender, there were no significant differences between Malaysians and Australians, χ2(1, 110) = 2.43, p = 0.12, and between participants with and without MDD, χ2(1, 110) = 1.95, p = 0.16.

A 2 (cultural group) × 2 (depression status) analysis of variance (ANOVA) was conducted with age as the dependent variable. There was a significant interaction effect between depression status and cultural group, F(1, 87) = 5.05, p = 0.03, η2 = 0.06. However, there were no significant main effects of cultural group, F(1, 87) = 0.29, p = 0.59, η2 = 0.004 or depression status, F(1, 87) = 2.30, p = 0.13, η2 = 0.03. Follow-up analysis of the interaction revealed that the only significant difference in age was between participants with and without MDD from Australia, t(38) = 2.06, p = 0.046, d = 0.66.

Performance on FER

The 2 (cultural group) × 2 (depression status) ANOVA with overall FER accuracy as the dependent variable revealed there was no significant interaction effect, F(1, 84) = 0.61, p = 0.44, η2 = 0.01. The depression status main effect, F(1, 84) = 2.05, p = 0.16, η2 = 0.02 and the cultural group main effect, F(1, 84) = 2.25, p = 0.14, η2 = 0.03, were also both non-significant. Thus, there was no evidence that Australian participants performed better than Malaysian participants or that those with MDD had better performance than those without MDD on the overall accuracy of the recognition of emotions.

Table 2 shows the interaction and main effects of all the seven emotions. In terms of recognition of specific emotions, the MANOVA revealed that the depression × cultural group interaction was not significant, F(7, 77) =1.73, p = 0.12, η2 = 0.14. The main effect of depression status was not significant, F(7, 77) = 1.11, p = 0.37, η2 = 0.09. However, there was a significant main effect of cultural group, F(7, 77) = 2.88, p = 0.01, η2 = 0.21. Follow-up analyses revealed that the cultural group main effect was only significant for fear, F(1, 87) = 12.05 p = 0.001, η2 = 0.13. In contrast to our hypothesis, Malaysian participants (M = 4.06, SD = 3.29) had greater accuracy in recognizing fear expressions on the FER task than Australian participants (M = 1.68, SD = 2.98), t(86) = 3.54, p = 0.001, d = 0.76.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Means, Standard Deviations, and Multivariate ANOVA Statistics for Malaysian and Australian participants with and without MDD according to the type of emotion.

Response Time on FER

The ANOVA revealed that there was a significant interaction effect, F(1, 84) = 8.54, p = 0.004, η2 = 0.09. Follow-up analyses found that Malaysian participants without MDD (M = 300.51, SD = 134.05) had a shorter response time than Australian participants without MDD (M = 819.78, SD = 699.04), t(45) = −2.94, p = 0.005, d = 1.03. There was no significant difference between Malaysian participants with MDD and Australian participants with MDD, t(39) = 0.94, p = 0.35, d = 0.29. Also, there was no significant difference between Malaysian participants with and without MDD, t(45) = –.95, p = 0.35. However, Australian participants with MDD (M = 370.37, SD = 301.01) had a shorter response time than Australian participants without MDD (M = 819.78, SD = 699.04), t(39) = 2.60, p = 0.01, d = 0.84.

Discussion

The aim of the current study was to investigate the cultural differences between Malaysian Malays and Australian with European heritage, with and without depression, in facial emotion recognition. Contrary to our first and second prediction, those with and without MDD, and Malays and Caucasians did not differ in their accuracy performance on the overall FER task. In terms of response time, there was no support for our first and second hypothesis. For Australian participants those with MDD provided faster response times than those without MDD and for the Malay group there was no significant difference between those with and without MDD. Therefore, the third hypothesis was also not supported. However, contrary to our last hypothesis, Malay participants more accurately recognized fear expressions than Australian participants.

Our finding for the overall accuracy of the FER supports the Biocultural Model of Emotion (54). This model proposes that priming reactions are spontaneous emotional reactions to stimuli which include expressive behavior and changes in physiology. This domain is mainly influenced by biology and has little influence of culture. This concurs with previous studies that endorse the universality of expressed emotions (55, 56). Nonetheless, according to several studies, there were certain features or nuances of expressions that are specific to particular cultures especially for the more complex emotions (7, 31, 5759).

Delving further into the seven specific emotions used in this study, we can see the extent of the influence of culture. Malay participants could recognize the fear expressions better than the Australians. This finding opposed what was found by Matsumoto (23), Chung and Robins (60), and Jack et al. (7) whereby they found that Asians could not identify fear expressions as well as their Caucasian counterparts. One explanation for this would be that as Malaysia is a multiracial country; there are the three main races with several ethnicities including Europeans, the Malays are exposed to a variety of cultures in everyday life and this has lead them to learn and recognize emotions especially that of fear, better. However, the studies by Matsumoto (23), Chung and Robins (60), and Jack et al. (7) differed from this study in terms of methodology. The stimuli used in the study by Jack et al. (7) were dynamic and the participants in all three studies were students or young adults below the age of 25. These findings and differences in study methodology highlight that these cultural influences need to be further examined before conclusions are drawn.

Contrary to past studies, there was no significant difference between participants with and without MDD in accuracy on the FER task (1116). Despite the non-significant findings, participants with MDD did score worse than those without MDD. The effect size of the difference was 0.30, in which according to Cohen (61), although this is a small effect size, it displays a difference.

There are studies that have reported that the greater the severity of MDD, the greater the impairment of emotion recognition (6264). Demenescu, Kortekaas, den Boer, and Aleman (65), asserted that most studies that investigated the influence of severity employed participants with moderate to severe or purely severe depressive symptoms. In this study, the severity of symptoms of the MDD participants ranged between mild and moderate. Furthermore, this study did not account for participants taking medication or attending therapy. There are medications that may alleviate the negative biases when identifying emotions through faces (66). Also, those attending therapy may have learnt some techniques on ways to reduce biases when perceiving emotions shown by others. This may explain the insignificant results obtained in this experiment.

On the other hand, the literature on reaction times of participants showed that participants with MDD had a slower reaction time as compared to participants without MDD (6769). One explanation for this being adults with MDD show symptoms of psychomotor retardation (70) and that they take more time when identifying sad faces (18). However, there is conflicting evidence that point out that participants with MDD are quicker when identifying negative stimuli especially sad faces (70, 71). Conversely, the results of this study showed that participants without MDD performed slower more specifically slower than Australian participants without MDD. Hence, further investigation should be carried out to ascertain the results obtained from this study. Other factors like fatigue, exposure and practice may have an influence on the reaction time of the participants and this can be examined further in future research.

A major strength of this study was the stimuli used that were adopted from the Montreal Set of Facial Displays of Emotion (MSFDE) developed by Beaupre and Hess (26). This has been validated and widely used in many research studies. In addition, this study is a cross-cultural study, which is the current area of interest owing to the growth of mental health issues across the globe. Another strength was the experimental nature of this study, which was conducted in a laboratory setting.

One limitation of this study was the small sample size. Although the sample size was relatively small, according to the sample size calculations the minimum number required for each group was met. Besides, the post-hoc power analysis showed that the power of this study was between 78 and 94.5%. Another limitation would be the heterogenous population of the participants with MDD. Not only were the participants recruited from different hospitals in Selangor and Melbourne, but some of them were receiving psychotherapy or medication or both. In addition, some were newly diagnosed patients who were not on medication or receiving therapy. The heterogeneity was not controlled mainly due to time and financial constraints. Although scarcely examined, several prior researches investigating the effects of medication on FER did not yield conclusive results; probable reasons being the presence of practice effects, only examining the effects of anti-depressants with no control group or presence of confounding factors (7275). Not only that, most of the participants were from the urban areas of Selangor and Melbourne. Those from similar geographic regions may share similar experiences and unique exposure. Thus, the study outcomes may be generalized to adults from urban areas, but not representative for the whole population.

Despite these limitations, further research is needed. Future studies could include eye-tracking devices to examine if there were differences in the way different cultures identify the expressions on faces or movement. It could also be used to investigate the influences of age and gender on FER. Apart from that, research can include comparing healthy adults with those who have other psychological disorders like mood disorders, schizophrenia or personality disorders.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author/s.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Universiti Putra Malaysia Ethics Committee (JKEUPM), National Medical Research Register (NMRR), Malaysia, Monash University Human Research Ethics Committee (MUHREC). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SNM: methodology, software, formal analysis, data curation, writing-original draft, visualization, project administration, and funding acquisition. FM: methodology, validation, investigation, resources, writing-review and editing, supervision, and funding acquisition. LJ: conceptualization, investigation, resources, writing-review and editing, supervision, and funding acquisition. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This work was supported by the Universiti Putra Malaysia (UPM) Putra-IPS grant (GP-IPS/2017/9520800).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank all participants who took part in this study, and Ms. Aishah Farhana Shahbuddin for assisting with publication matters.

References

1. Reeve J. Understanding Motivation and Emotion, 4th ed. New Jersey, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. (2005).

Google Scholar

2. Matsumoto D, Yoo SH, Nakagawa S, 37 members of the Multinational Study of Cultural Display Rules. Culture, emotion regulation and adjustment. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2008) 94:925–73. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.94.6.925

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Plutchik R. A psychoevolutionary theory of emotions. Soc Sci Inform. (1982) 21:529–53. doi: 10.1177/053901882021004003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Reeve J. Understanding Motivation and Emotion, 4th ed. New Jersey, NJ: John Wiley & Sons, Inc. (2005).

Google Scholar

5. Ekman P. Darwin, deception, and facial expression. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2003) 1000:205–21. doi: 10.1196/annals.1280.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hess U, Thibault P. Darwin and emotion expression. Am Psychol. (2009) 2:120–28. doi: 10.1037/a0013386

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Jack RE, Garrod OGE, Yu H, Caldara R, Schyns PG. From the cover: facial expressions of emotion are not culturally universal. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. (2012) 109:7241–4. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1200155109

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lepannen JM. Emotional information processing in mood disorders: a review of behavioral and neuroimaging findings. Curr Opin Psychiatry. (2006) 19:4–9. doi: 10.1097/01.yco.0000191500.46411.00

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Rosenblau G, Sterzer P, Stoy M, Park S, Friedel E, Heinz A, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of emotion processing in major depressive disorder, is altered after successful antidepressant therapy. J Psychopharmacol. (2012) 26:1424–33. doi: 10.1177/0269881112450779

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Ritchey M, Dolcos F, Eddington KM, Strauman TJ, Cabeza R. Neural correlates of emotional processing in depression: changes with cognitive behavioral therapy and predictors of treatment response. J Psychiatr Res. (2011) 45:577–87. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.09.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Grant FAL. Emotion Recognition in Chronic Treatment Resistant Depression: Before and After Neurosurgical Treatment (Unpublished Master's Dissertation). Scotland: University of St. Andrews, United Kingdom (2013).

Google Scholar

12. Langenecker SA, Bieliauskas LA, Rapport LJ, Zubieta JK, Wilde EA, Berent S. Face emotion perception and executive functioning deficits in depression. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. (2005) 27:320–33. doi: 10.1080/13803390490490515720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Milders M, Bell S, Platt J, Serrano R, Runcie O. Stable expression recognition abnormalities in unipolar depression. Psychiatry Res. (2010) 179:38–42. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2009.05.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Naranjo C, Kornreich C, Campanella S, Noël X, Vandriette Y, Gillain B, et al. Major depression is associated with impaired processing of emotion in music as well as in facial and vocal stimuli. J Affect Disord. (2011) 128:243–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2010.06.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Wright SL, Langenecker SA. Differential risk for emotion processing difficulties by gender age in major depressive disorder. In: Hernandez P, Alonso S, editors. Women and Depression, New York, NY: Nova Science Publishers, Inc. (2008). p. 1–33.

16. Wright SL, Kornreich C, Campanella S, Noël X, Vandriette Y, Gillain B, et al. Gender specific disruptions in emotion processing in younger adults with depression. Depress Anxiety J. (2009) 26:182–9. doi: 10.1002/da.20502

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Fu CHY, Williams SC, Brammer MJ, Suckling J, Kim J, Cleare AJ, et al. Neural responses to happy facial expressions in major depression following antidepressant treatment. Am J Psychiatry. (2007) 164:599–607. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.4.599

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Gollan JK, Pane HT, McCloskey MS, Coccaro EF. Identifying differences in biased affective information processing in major depression. Psychiatry Res. (2008) 159:18–24. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2007.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Ekman P. An argument for basic emotions. Cogn Emot. (1992) 6:169–200. doi: 10.1080/02699939208411068

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Matsumoto D, Yoo SH, Hirayama S, Petrova G. Development and initial validation of a measure of display rules: the display rule assessment inventory (DRAI). Emotion. (2005) 5:23–40. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.5.1.23

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Matsumoto D, Juang L. Culture & Psychology, 4th ed. Belmont, CA: Thomson Wadsworth. (2008).

Google Scholar

22. Wierzbicka A. The “History of Emotions” and the future of emotion research. Emot Rev. (2010) 2:269–73. doi: 10.1177/1754073910361983

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Matsumoto D. American-Japanese cultural differences in the recognition of universal facial expressions. J Cross Cult Psychol. (1992) 23:72–84. doi: 10.1177/0022022192231005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hareli S, Kafetsios K, Hess U. A cross-cultural study on emotion expression and the learning of social norms. Front Psychol. (2015) 6:1501. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Alaerts K, Nackaerts E, Meyns P, Swinnen SP, Wenderoth N. Action and emotion recognition from point light displays: An investigation of gender differences. PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e20989. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0020989

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Beaupre MG, Hess U. Cross-cultural emotion recognition among Canadian ethnic groups. J Cross Cult Psychol. (2005) 36:355–70. doi: 10.1177/0022022104273656

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Elfenbein HA, Beaupré M, Lévesque M, Hess U. Toward a dialect theory: cultural differences in the expression and recognition of posed facial expression. Emotion. (2007) 7:131–46. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.7.1.131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Schimmack U. Cultural influences on the recognition of emotion by facial expressions. J Cross Cult Psychol. (1996) 27:37–50. doi: 10.1177/0022022196271003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Yang T, Yang Z, Xu G, Gao D, Zhang Z, Wang H, et al. Tsinghua facial expression database – A database of facial expressions in Chinese young and older women and men: development and validation. PLoS ONE. (2020) 15:e0231304. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0231304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Chen J, Chen Z, Chi Z, Fu H. Facial expression recognition in video with multiple feature fusion. IEEE Trans Affect Comput. (2018) 9:38–50. doi: 10.1109/TAFFC.2016.2593719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Jack RE, Sun W, Delis I, Garrod OG, Schyns PG. Four not six: revealing culturally common facial expressions of emotion. J Exp Psychol Gen. (2016) 145:708–30. doi: 10.1037/xge0000162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Prado C, Mellor, D, Byrne, LK, Wilson, et al. Facial emotion recognition: a cross-cultural comparison of Chinese, Chinese living in Australia, and Anglo-Australians. Motiv Emot. (2014) 38:420–8. doi: 10.1007/s11031-013-9383-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Stanley JT, Zhang X, Fung HH, Isaacowitz DM. Cultural differences in gaze and emotion recognition: americans contrast more than Chinese. Emotion. (2013) 13:36–46. doi: 10.1037/a0029209

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. De Vaus J, Hornsey MJ, Kuppens P, Bastian B. Exploring the east-west divide in prevalence of affective disorder: a case for cultural differences in coping with negative emotion. Pers Soc Psychol Rev. (2018) 22:285–304. doi: 10.1177/1088868317736222

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Jobson L, Mirabolfathi V, Moshirpanahi S, Parhoon H, Gillard J, Mukhtar F, et al. Investigating emotion in malay, Australian and Iranian individuals with and without depression. Sci Rep. (2019) 9:18344. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-54775-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Juhasz G, Eszlari N, Pap D, Gonda X. Cultural differences in the development and characteristics of depression. Neuropsychopharmacol Hung. (2012) 14:259–65. doi: 10.5706/nph201212007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Kessler RC, Bromet EJ. The epidemiology of depression across cultures. Annu Rev Public Health. (2013) 34:119–38. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-031912-114409

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kleinmann A. Culture and depression. N Engl J Med. (2004) 351:951–3. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp048078

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. (2007) 39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/BF03193146

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. (2009) 41:1149–60. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1961) 4:561–71. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. Psychometric properties of the beck depression inventory twenty-five years of evaluation. Clin Psychol Rev. (1988) 8:77–100. doi: 10.1016/0272-7358(88)90050-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Mukhtar F, Oei TPS. Exploratory and confirmatory factor validation and psychometric properties of the beck depression inventory for malays (BDI-Malay) in Malaysia. Malays J Psychiatry. (2008) 17:51–64. Available online at: https://www.mjpsychiatry.org/index.php/mjp/article/view/27

Google Scholar

44. First MB, Williams JBW, Karg RS, Spitzer RL. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-5 Disorders, Clinician Version (SCID-5-CV). Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2016).

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

45. Kline, RB. Methodology in the Social Sciences: Principles and Practice of Structural Equation Modelling. New York, NY: Guilford Press (1998).

Google Scholar

46. Pallant J. SPSS Survival Manual: A Step by Step Guide to Data Analysis Using IBM SPSS. Berkshire: Open University Press (2016).

47. Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS. Using Multivariate Statistics, 5th ed. New York: Allyn and Bacon. (2007).

Google Scholar

48. Abbruzzese L, Magnani N, Robertson IH, Mancuso M. Age and gender differences in emotion recognition. Front Psychol. (2019) 10:2371. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Isaacowitz D, Löckenhoff C, Lane R, Wright R, Sechrest L, Riedel R, et al. Age differences in recognition of emotion in lexical stimuli and facial expressions. Psychol Aging. (2007) 22:147–59. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.22.1.147

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Mill A, Allik J, Realo A, Valk R. Age-related differences in emotion recognition ability: a cross-sectional study. Emotion. (2009) 9:619–30. doi: 10.1037/a0016562

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Cortes DS, Tornberg C, Bänziger T, Tornberg C, Bänziger T, Elfenbein HA, et al. Effects of aging on emotion recognition from dynamic multimodal expressions and vocalizations. Sci Rep. (2021) 11:2647 doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-82135-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Demenescu LR, Mathiak KA, Mathiak K. Age and gender related variations of emotion recognition in pseudowords and faces. Exp Aging Res. (2014) 40:187–207. doi: 10.1080/0361073X.2014.882210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Demenescu LR, Stan A, Kortekaas R, van der Wee NJA, Veltman DJ, Aleman A. On the connection between level of education and the neural circuitry of emotion perception. Front Hum Neurosci. (2014) 8:866. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Matsumoto D, Hwang HS. Culture and emotion: the integration of biological and cultural contributions. J Cross Cult Psychol. (2012) 43:91–118. doi: 10.1177/0022022111420147

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ekman P, Cordaro D. What is meant by calling emotions basic. Emot Rev. (2011) 3:364–70. doi: 10.1177/1754073911410740

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Pica P, Jackson S, Blake R, Troje NF. Comparing biological motion perception in two distinct human societies. PLoS ONE. (2011) 6:e28391. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028391

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Chen C, Jack RE. Discovering cultural differences (and similarities) in facial expression of emotion. Curr Opin Psychol. (2017) 17:61–6. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Marsh AA, Elfenbein HA, Ambady N. Nonverbal “accents”: Cultural differences in facial expressions of emotion. Psychol Sci. (2003) 14:373–76. doi: 10.1111/1467-9280.24461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Parkinson, C, Walker, TT, Memmi S, Wheatley T. Emotions are understood from biological motion across remote cultures. Emotion. (2017) 17:459–77. doi: 10.1037/emo0000194

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Chung JM, Robins RW. Exploring cultural differences in the recognition of the self-conscious emotions. PLoS ONE. (2015) 10:e0136411. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Pallant J. SPSS Survival Manual: A Step by Step Guide to Data Analysis Using IBM SPSS. Berkshire: Open University Press (2016).

62. Gollan JK, McCloskey M, Hoxha D, Coccaro EF. How do depressed and healthy adults interpret nuanced facial expressions? J Abnorm Psychol. (2010) 119:804–10. doi: 10.1037/a0020234

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Air T, Weightman MJ, Baune BT. Symptom severity of depressive symptoms impacts on social cognition performance in current but not remitted major depressive disorder. Front Psychol. (2015) 6:1–9. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01118

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Hale WW. Judgement of facial expressions and depression persistence. Psychiatry Res. (1998) 80:265–74. doi: 10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00070-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Demenescu LR, Kortekaas R, den Boer JA, Aleman A. Impaired attribution of emotion to facial expression in anxiety and major depression. PLoS ONE. (2010) 5:105. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0015058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Pringle A, McCabe C, Cowen PJ, Harmer CJ. Antidepressant treatment and emotional processing: can we dissociate the roles of serotonin and noradrenaline? J Psychopharmacol. (2013) 27:719–31. doi: 10.1177/0269881112474523

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Hu B, Rao J, Li X, Cao T, Li J, Majoe D, et al. Emotion regulating attentional control abnormalities in major depressive disorder: an event-related potential study. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:13530. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-13626-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Jiang H, Hua L, Dai Z, Tian S, Yao Z, Lu Q, et al. Spectral fingerprints of facial affect processing bias in major depression disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2019) 14:1233–42. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsz096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Liedtke C, Kohl W, Kret ME, Koelkebeck K. Emotion recognition from faces with in- and out-group features in patients with depression. J Affect Disord. (2018) 227:817–23. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2017.11.085

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Ahorsu DK, Tsang HWH. Do people with depression always have decreased cognitive processing speed? Evidence through electrophysiological lens. J Neuropsychiatry. (2018) 8:1227–31. doi: 10.4172/Neuropsychiatry.1000451

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Wu L, Pu J, Allen JJ, Pauli P. Recognition of facial expressions in individuals with elevated levels of depressive symptoms: an eye-movement study. Depress Res Treat. (2012) 2012:249030–7. doi: 10.1155/2012/249030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Anderson IM, Shippen C, Juhasz G, Chase D, Thomas E, Downey D, et al. State-dependent alteration inface emotion recognition in depression. Br J Psychiatry. (2011) 198:302–8. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.110.078139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Dunn BD, Dalgleish T, Lawrence A, Cusack R, Ogilvie A. Categorical and dimensional reports of experienced affect to emotion-enducing pictures in depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (2004) 113:654–60. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.113.4.654

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Rottenberg J, Kasch KL, Gross JJ, Gotlib IH. Sadness and amusement reactivity differently predict concurrent and prospective functioning in major depressive disorder. Emotion. (2002) 2:135–46. doi: 10.1037/1528-3542.2.2.135

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Tranter R, Bell D, Gutting P, Harmer C, Healy D, Anderson IM. The effect of serotonergic and noradrenergic antidepressants on face emotion processing in depressed patients. J Affect Disord. (2009) 118:87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.01.028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cross-cultural differences, facial emotion recognition, Malaysia, Australia, emotion recognition

Citation: Mohan SN, Mukhtar F and Jobson L (2021) An Exploratory Study on Cross-Cultural Differences in Facial Emotion Recognition Between Adults From Malaysia and Australia. Front. Psychiatry 12:622077. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.622077

Received: 13 November 2020; Accepted: 07 May 2021;
Published: 09 June 2021.

Edited by:

Yuan-Pang Wang, University of São Paulo, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Sheila A. M. Rauch, Emory University, United States
Smita Deshpande, Atal Bihari Vajpayee Institute of Medical Sciences and Dr. Ram Manohar Lohia Hospital, India

Copyright © 2021 Mohan, Mukhtar and Jobson. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Sindhu Nair Mohan, c2luZGh1JiN4MDAwNDA7dXBtLmVkdS5teQ==

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.