Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 18 June 2021
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

Common Venues in Romantic Relationships of Adults With Symptoms of Autism and Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder

  • 1Programa de Pós-Graduação em Medicina Molecular, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
  • 2Department of Mental Health, Universidade Federal de Minas, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
  • 3Department of Psychology, Faculdade de Ciências Médicas de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil
  • 4Department of Pediatrics, Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais, Belo Horizonte, Brazil

Introduction: Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) and Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) figures among the most common neurodevelopmental disorders. Despite having opposite stereotypes, both ADHD and ASD compromise, though in different ways, skills such as social interactions, communication skills, and social thinking, which may underlie difficulties in romantic relationships.

Methods: We evaluated 306 adults about their romantic relationships and the intensity of their love. Participants were from one of four groups:, individuals with ASD-only traits, a group with symptoms of ADHD-only, participants with neither ADHD nor ASD symptoms (control) or from a fourth group of individuals with both ADHD and ASD traits.

Results: The ASD traits group had fewer married people and more people who have never been married, while the rate of divorce was higher in the ADHD traits group. Regarding the intensity of love, the mean scores of the ADHD and the ADHD+ASD traits groups were higher in the Passionate Love Scale than the mean score of the control group. Passionate love in the ASD group was no different from the other groups. Small positive correlations were found between passionate love and ADHD and ASD behavioral problems.

Conclusion: Marital status was distinct depending on symptoms of a neurodevelopmental disorder in adulthood. Also, ADHD and ASD symptoms were associated with greater passionate love. Therefore, ADHD and ASD behavioral dimensions may impact long-term and short-term experiences of a person's relationship with a significant other in distinct ways. Understanding how people with neurodevelopmental disorders experience love might help us to better clarify the mechanisms associated with their relationship patterns.

Introduction

Neurodevelopmental disorders are childhood-onset conditions that commonly have a major impact on the personal, laboral, academic, and social functioning of individuals and their families. Two of the most frequent disorders diagnosed early in life are Autism Spectrum Disorders (ASD) and Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD), with a prevalence of ~1.8 and 5.29% of affected children (1, 2), respectively. Although these conditions are usually associated with childhood, 2.5–5% adults worldwide are affected by ADHD (3, 4), and 80–99% of all children with ASD diagnosis will meet criteria in adulthood (5, 6). Although ASD and ADHD show important differences in core symptoms, they share neurobiological, psychological, and behavioral characteristics, in addition to a high rate of co-occurrence (79).

ADHD is a heterogeneous disorder marked by inattentive and/or hyperactive/impulsive behaviors. Its functional impairment includes poor occupational performance, lower level of education, higher rates of delinquent behaviors, traffic accidents, self-reported happiness, marital dissatisfaction and divorce (1, 1014). Although individuals with ADHD may often seem as outgoing and amusing people, they frequently suffer from social and interpersonal problems (15, 16). These social impairments encompasses a broad range manifestations as deficits in processing negative emotions in facial expressions or perceiving their inadequate behavior and inhibit ongoing actions toward more appropriate ones (17). They have poor social skills being commonly rejected by their peers and having difficulty to establish a relationship with a partner (15, 18). ASD shares some of the functional and social impairments associated with ADHD, such as difficulties in social interactions and communication (19). Impairment in social interaction is a core finding in ASD, coursing with difficulties to share interests, to understand and to describe facial expressions and feelings about yourself and others (20). ASD also is marked by a heterogeneous presentation, varying in cognitive disability, behavioral and functional profiles (21).

Considering that understanding, developing and maintaining intimate relationships requires social interactions, communication skills and social thinking (22, 23), individuals with ADHD and ASD might experience difficulties related to those (22, 24, 25). Indeed, Canu and Carlson (26) found that college students with the inattentive type of ADHD reported less and a later start of dating relationships, compared to college students with the combined type or with typical development. Girls diagnosed with ADHD in childhood reported fewer romantic relationships in late adolescence and early adulthood (27). Young adults with ADHD also presented an increased risk for undesirable pregnancies, higher frequency of sexually transmitted infections, casual sex and a greater number of sexual partners (2832).

Concerning ASD individuals, data on their love life is inconsistent. Lau and Peterson (33) found that the presence of ASD symptoms didn't seem to influence marital satisfaction at a significant level, while Deguchi and Asakura (34) observe that wives of husbands with ASD report feeling lonely and socially excluded. Moreover, there is a general agreement that the frequency of ASD individuals in a romantic relationship is lower when compared with typically developed individuals (23).

Individuals with autism are frequently portrayed as insensitive and cold, that lack the need for love. Some traits, such as dislike being touched and poor comprehension of social signs, contribute to social stereotypes, labeling individuals with autism as asexual or uninterested in romantic relationships (22, 3537), stereotypes which are reinforced by media which frequently consider autism as opposed to romantic love (38). However, researches indicate that adults with ASD would like to have romantic relations and report suffering from having those social impairments (39, 40).

Meanwhile, individuals with ADHD are often viewed as more creative, passionate and intense (12), a portrayal that is probably related to their high impulsivity. Acting without thinking or acting in the “heat of the moment” are very common behaviors among ADHD individuals, and such characteristics may contribute to describing them as passionate people who listen to their hearts or follow their instincts no matter what.

Nevertheless, these characteristics may also contribute to higher rates of divorce and poorer marital adjustment within adults with ADHD (41). Considering the response to undesirable partner's behavior, both the inattentive and the hyperactive/impulsive individuals present poor coping strategies to solve problems (42). Inattentive ADHD symptoms are related to less constructive reactions to the partners' failures and paying more attention to alternative partners. Those individuals presenting symptoms of hyperactivity-impulsivity present failure in inhibit negative responses to their partner and this could lead to the fail the search for an alternative partner.

Previous studies pointed to an association between marital quality and health, with divorce being correlated with poorer outcomes, as a greater risk for early death, for mental illness and with alterations in endocrine and immune function (4345). Wherefore, passionate love, defined as a state of intense longing for union (46), a strong positive feeling toward the other one (47), seems to be an essential element. Although it is known as the first phase of a relationship, passionate love is important not only to initiate but also for maintaining a relationship, being predictive of relationship satisfaction, on both short and long-term relationships (48, 49). Love was also associated with relationship length. Results from Ratelle et al. (50) suggested that the stronger the feelings of love toward the partner, the greater the chances of the couple still being together after 3 months. Moreover, between other aspects, couples who divorced earlier showed lower levels of love when compared to couples who divorce later and with happily married couples (51).

Considering the impact on mental health and well-being of dysfunctional or unsatisfactory romantic relationships, our study aimed to investigate the characteristics of passionate love among individuals with symptoms of ASD and ADHD.

Methods

Participants and Procedures

Participants were 306 Brazilian adults (i.e., ≥18 years) recruited via the internet through a virtual snowball sampling strategy (banner ad posted at the researchers' social media) for a partially online open survey specifically designed to study cognition, behavior, and adaptive functioning depending on ASD traits in adults. The research is under the Helsinki Declaration principles and was approved by the local ethics board (registry: CAAE 56534516.1.0000.5149). Participants were assigned to four distinct groups according to clinical cut-off points on ADHD and ASD screening tools. To assess the ADHD symptoms we used the Adult Self-Report Scale (ASRS-v1.1) (52), and to evaluate autism traits we used the Autism-Spectrum Quotient (AQ) (53), besides a recall of behavioral symptoms' onset. Ninety-two (30%) participants had no criteria for clinical risk attribution (scores below the cut-off points and/or passed the age of onset), so they were grouped as typically developed individuals (i.e., control group). Forty-two (14%) participants were at clinical risk for ASD, 76 (25%) individuals for ADHD, and 96 (31%) showed scores suggesting a simultaneous risk for clinical ASD and ADHD (ASD + ADHD).

Participants voluntarily consented with their participation by clicking a button “I agree” in the electronic questionnaire after reading an invitation explaining the purpose of the study, the duration of the form, data that were being stored, and confidentiality security. No incentives were offered for voluntary participation. No technical issues were reported to our research team during data collection. Data were obtained from July 2016 to July 2017, and all questionnaire items were presented in a fixed order for all participants, independent of their response choices. Responses could be changed before submission, but not after. A second submission was not readily available for participants. Duplicates were identified by participants' full name, which was visible only for one researcher (JJP) before masking (each participant received a numeric ID), and only the first response was kept. Participants took an average of 30 min to respond to the electronic questionnaire.

Instruments

Passionate Love Scale

To assess the passionate love intensity we used the short version of the Passionate Love Scale (PLS) (46), a 15-item instrument which, summed up, suggests a global measure on how much in love the respondent is. PLS is an unidimensional instrument, which evaluates cognitive, emotional and behavioral features of passionate love (46). The cognitive elements comprise the concerns related to the special other, as intrusive thinking and the partner or the relationship idealization. The behavioral and emotional features include aspects associated with the attraction toward the special one, as physiological arousal, physical proximity, positive and negative feelings, and availability to the other (46, 54, 55).

Sociodemographic Characteristics

The Brazilian Economic Classification Criteria (CCEB) was used to characterize the participants' socioeconomic status. Scores can vary from 0 to 100 and classified in one of six socioeconomic strata: A (monthly household income estimation of U$ 20888.00), B1 (monthly household income estimation of U$ 9254.00), B2 (average household income of U$ 4852.00), C1 (average household income of U$ 2705.00), C2 (average household income of U$ 1625.00), and DE (average household income of U$ 768.00) (56).

Statistical Procedures

All analyses were performed with SPSS 22.0. Descriptive statistics and Spearman's correlation analyses were conducted within the entire sample to investigate variables distribution and their associations. Comparisons between the groups regarding passionate love, ADHD and ASD traits, instruction, sociodemographic measures and age, were performed by the Kruskal-Wallis test. Then, for further details of the results, we used Mann-Whitney tests (Bonferroni corrected) to specific group comparisons. Differences between groups regarding marital status and sex distribution were performed by chi-square tests.

Results

In this survey, respondents' age varied from 18 to 58 years (M = 31.8; SD = 8.5) being predominantly female (73.9%). This distribution is in accordance with some results that suggest a gender bias, especially in surveys related to mental health and emotional issues (57, 58). Nevertheless, we found no significant differences in the distribution of sex (χ2 = 4.81 p = 0.186) between groups. The sample was classified according to the participants' scores on ADHD and ASD screening scales. Thus, 30.1% (Group 1, n = 92) had negative scores for both ADHD and ASD, 13.8% (Group 2, n = 42) had high scores for autism traits, 24.8% (Group 3, n = 76) had clinical scores only for ADHD, in which 34.2% were inattentive, 14.5% hyperactive, and 51.6% combined, and 31.4% (Group 4, n = 96) had clinical scores for both conditions. The participants' description is shown in Table 1. We found no significant differences between the distribution of age (Z = 2.44, p = 0.486), education (Z = 5.85, p = 0.119) and sociodemographic measures (Z = 0.68, P = 0.876) between groups. Of the total sample, 45.7% (n = 139) was in a steady relationship (married or living with their partner), while the others 54.3% (n = 165) had never been married, or were widowed, separated or divorced at the time. There were also significant differences in the marital status distribution (Table 2; χ2 = 36.948, p = 0.001) between groups (Figure 1 and Table 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Participant's description and group comparison.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Marital status' distribution among groups.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Marital status distribution by group. 1 = Control group, 2 = ASD-traits group, 3 =A DHD-traits group, 4 = ADHD + ASD-traits group.

Regarding the intensity of passionate love, Table 3 shows a significant correlation with symptoms of inattention (r = 0.253, p < 0.01) and hyperactivity/impulsivity (r = 0.204, p < 0.01), assessed by ASRS-18, and with Autism Quotient (r = 0.212, p < 0.01) (Table 3). Comparing the groups' intensity of love, we found differences between the ADHD (H = −42.05, p = 0.013) and the ADHD + ASD-traits group (H = −70.55, p < 0.001) when compared with the control group (Table 1). Within the ADHD-traits group, we also found a significant difference, with a higher PLS score in the inattentive group (Z = −52.31, p = 0.001) and in the combined group (Z = −40.24, p = 0.007) when compared to the control group, while the hyperactive/impulsive group showed no significant difference (p = 0.058) in this comparison.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Participants' correlations among passionate love and symptoms of autism and ADHD.

Discussion

Previous studies have suggested that individuals with ADHD and ASD may experience difficulties related to romantic relationships, whether to initiate or maintain it (22, 25, 40). Previously, individuals with ASD have declared to have fewer romantic partners when compared to typically developed individuals (23). Individuals with ASD symptoms seem to have less exposition in any kind of relationship. In the ASD-traits group, there is a lower frequency of married people and a higher proportion of people who have never been married, while the ADHD appears to have a four times higher chance to have a divorce, confirming an impact in relationships. Both divorce and marital separation are two of the most stressful life events an individual can undergo, being considered more stressful than the death of a close relative or being in jail (59, 60).

Here we focused on the potential for any difference regarding the intensity of love feelings between people with high ADHD and autism traits and people without symptoms of any of these diagnoses. Romantic or passionate love is an intense emotional state typical of the beginning of romantic relationships, marked by profound feelings of attraction and commitment, as well as by obsessive characteristics, such as a jealous dependence and intrusive thoughts about the partner (48, 61). Some researches point to an association between romantic love, mental health and overall well-being and quality of life (48, 62), as well as with both marital and life satisfaction (63, 64). Moreover, if we exclude the obsessional aspects of early-stage love, it seems to be correlated with relationship satisfaction even in long-term relationships, suggesting that passionate love might be important not only in the formation but also in the maintenance phases of a relationship (48).

Our results suggest that the intensity of passionate love in ADHD-traits and the ADHD+ASD-traits group is greater than in the group with no symptoms, which could be interpreted as a trace of emotion dysregulation, a very common aspect among people with ADHD (65). Emotional regulation comprises a range of mechanisms associated with self-regulation, which encompasses some features as intensity, duration and stability of emotions, and skills of affection recognition, modulation and responsiveness (6668). Although most researchers may focus on aspects related to emotional responsiveness, with special attention to reactivity to negative emotions, results from Rapport et al. (69) indicate that adults with ADHD appear to experience their own emotions with greater intensity, when compared to adults without the disorder, suggesting that individuals with ADHD may not only respond more intensely to their emotions but also feel that way.

Further analysis also suggested that the difference found in the ADHD-traits group was probably related to the inattentive dimension of the disorder. In previous studies, the ADHD inattentive presentation was associated with lower romantic satisfaction and less expression of love and affection (70). Indeed, behaviors that provoked the most negative reactions in partners of individuals with ADHD correspond to inattentive symptoms more than to hyperactivity (71). Moreover, inattentive individuals might be rated as unassertive and uninterested, which could explain their rejection rates regarding the beginning of romantic relationships (72).

These results are in accordance with the proposition that the point of difficulty in the interpersonal interaction might be in self-regulation of emotion as it is experienced and expressed (69). People with ADHD might have an accurate perception of social cues and their own emotions but may fail to act properly (65, 69), as individuals with ADHD seem to engage less in steady relationships and more likely to divorce, although they feel love more intensely.

The ASD-traits group does not differ from the ADHD, control and ADHD+ASD-traits group in the romantic love intensity, suggesting that adults with high autism traits do not experience less or more passionate love in their lives compared to the other groups. Although ASD has a very heterogeneous manifestation, difficulties with social interaction, in general, are a core trait, which includes deficits with verbal and non-verbal communication (21). They usually report difficulties expressing their feelings and emotions, to understand and predict other people's mental states and facial expressions (20). In addition, an individual with autism could have difficulties to initiate or to maintain a conversation, sharing their interests, or to modulate behavior considering the context (25, 73). Indeed, those aspects will impact the development of any kind of relationship, including romantic ones. A study developed by Stokes et al. (74), investigated the nature and predictors of social and romantic functioning in adolescents and adults with ASD. Their results suggested that the autism group reported less access to peers and friends compared to the control group, and this fact probably impacts the learning of social rules and romantic skills.

The common image of an individual with ASD is a person for whom love has no meaning, no draw, no neurochemical reward related to love, however, nothing of this picture was sustained by our data. Despite this, feeling in love is not a lacking emotion for those who have plenty of symptoms compatible with ASD. Our findings agreed with other reports, that individuals with autism, as well as those with a neurotypical development, also desire to be in an intimate and romantic relationship, even when they lack understanding of them, do not have skills, previous experiences, or knowledge to initiate those interactions (23, 75).

fMRI findings suggest that passionate love recruits brain regions involved in complex cognitive processing, such as social cognition, body image, self-representation and attention (76). We observe more or at least the same intensity of passionate love in those who have ASD and/or ADHD symptoms, but the mechanisms of ASD and ADHD symptoms impact in romantic relationships are still to be addressed in further studies.

An important limitation is the fact that we analyzed the relationship effects under the presence of symptoms without a diagnostic interview and evaluated only the romantic love effect without any information about the long-term features of love or less stable relationships. Another limitation was the survey's sex distribution, which was predominantly female. Since both ADHD and ASD are more prevalent among males, this could imply a sampling bias. Furthermore, there was also a difference in the sample distribution within the ADHD group, so the results should be interpreted with caution. A strength of our study was the evaluation of the dimensional effect presented in the individuals.

In conclusion, the relationship seems to be impacted by the presence of symptoms of neurodevelopmental disorders, having more impact on individuals with symptoms compatible with ASD. However, the impact apparently is not related to the intensity of love in the individual's point of view. Subjects with higher symptoms of ADHD and with high ADHD and ASD traits described having a higher intensity of romantic love, and nevertheless have less stable relationships. They are more likely to have a divorce, pointing toward a possible gap between feeling and expressing their emotions. Further studies are necessary to understand the mechanisms of differences and mechanisms of the relationship deficits. Improvement in relationships showed a beneficial impact and a universal protective factor, however, preventing bad relationships is a more important goal than getting an average relationship to a more satisfactory level (77). Interventions focused on preventing relationship dysfunction should have great potential. Thus, understanding how these population experiences love might help us to clarify points to be addressed in social training and mechanisms associated with their relationship patterns.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by UFMG Ethical Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

Material preparation, data collection, and analysis were performed by LS and AA. The first draft of the manuscript was written by LS. All authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript, contributed to the study conception and design, and read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was financed in part by the Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior—Brasil (CAPES)—Finance Code 001, CNPq, PRPq da Universidade Federal de Minas Gerais.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Polanczyk G, De Lima MS, Horta BL, Biederman J, Rohde LA. The worldwide prevalence of ADHD: a systematic review and metaregression analysis. Am J Psychiatry. (2007) 164:942–8. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2007.164.6.942

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Baio J, Wiggins L, Christensen DL, Maenner MJ, Daniels J, Warren Z, et al. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years—autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 Sites, United States, 2014. MMWR Surveill Summ. (2018) 67:1. doi: 10.15585/mmwr.ss6706a1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Simon V, Czobor P, Bálint S, Mészáros Á, Bitter I. Prevalence and correlates of adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: meta-analysis. Br J Psychiatry. (2009) 194:204–11. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.107.048827

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Willcutt EG. The prevalence of DSM-IV attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analytic review. Neurotherapeutics. (2012) 9:490–9. doi: 10.1007/s13311-012-0135-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Billstedt E, Gillberg C, Gillberg C. Autism after adolescence: population-based 13- to 22-year follow-up study of 120 individuals with autism diagnosed in childhood. J Autism Dev Disord. (2005) 35:351–60. doi: 10.1007/s10803-005-3302-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. McGovern CW, Sigman M. Continuity and change from early childhood to adolescence in autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2005) 46:401–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00361.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Taurines R, Schwenck C, Westerwald E, Sachse M, Siniatchkin M, Freitag C. ADHD and autism: differential diagnosis or overlapping traits? A selective review. Atten Deficit Hyperact Disord. (2012) 4:115–39. doi: 10.1007/s12402-012-0086-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Christakou A, Murphy CM, Chantiluke K, Cubillo AI, Smith AB, Giampietro V, et al. Disorder-specific functional abnormalities during sustained attention in youth with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and with Autism. Mol Psychiatry. (2013) 18:236–44. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Craig F, Lamanna AL, Margari F, Matera E, Simone M, Margari L. Overlap between autism spectrum disorders and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: searching for distinctive/common clinical features. Autism Res. (2015) 8:328–37. doi: 10.1002/aur.1449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Faraone SV, Biederman J. What is the prevalence of adult ADHD? Results of a population screen of 966 adults. J Atten Disord. (2005) 9:384–91. doi: 10.1177/1087054705281478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Shaw P, Stringaris A, Nigg J, Leibenluft E. Emotion dysregulation in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2014) 171:276–93. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13070966

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. de Schipper E, Mahdi S, Coghill D, de Vries PJ, Gau SSF, Granlund M, et al. Towards an ICF core set for ADHD: a worldwide expert survey on ability and disability. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2015) 24:1509–21. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0778-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Stickley A, Koyanagi A, Takahashi H, Ruchkin V, Kamio Y. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms and loneliness among adults in the general population. Res Dev Disabil. (2017) 62:115–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2017.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Aduen PA, Kofler MJ, Sarver DE, Wells EL, Soto EF, Cox DJ. ADHD, depression, and motor vehicle crashes: a prospective cohort study of continuously-monitored, real-world driving. J Psychiatr Res. (2018) 101:42–9. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2018.02.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Paulson JF, Buermeyer C, Nelson-Gray RO. Social rejection and ADHD in young adults: an analogue experiment. J Atten Disord. (2005) 8:127–35. doi: 10.1177/1087054705277203

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Nijmeijer JS, Minderaa RB, Buitelaar JK, Mulligan A, Hartman CA, Hoekstra PJ. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and social dysfunctioning. Clin Psychol Rev. (2008) 28:692–708. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2007.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Coutinho TV, Reis SPS, da Silva AG, Miranda DM, Malloy-Diniz LF. Deficits in response inhibition in patients with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: the impaired self-protection system hypothesis. Front Psychiatry. (2018) 8:e00299. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00299

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Weiss G, Hechtman L. Hyperactive Children Grown Up: ADHD in Children, Adolescents, and Adults. (1993). Available online at: https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1993-98233-000 (accessed July 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

20. Hill EL. Evaluating the theory of executive dysfunction in autism. Dev Rev. (2004) 24:189–233. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2004.01.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Doernberg E, Hollander E. Neurodevelopmental disorders (ASD and ADHD): DSM-5, ICD-10, and ICD-11. CNS Spectr. (2016) 21:295–9. doi: 10.1017/S1092852916000262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Byers ES, Nichols S, Voyer SD. Challenging stereotypes: sexual functioning of single adults with high functioning autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2013) 43:2617–27. doi: 10.1007/s10803-013-1813-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Strunz S, Schermuck C, Ballerstein S, Ahlers CJ, Dziobek I, Roepke S. Romantic relationships and relationship satisfaction among adults with asperger syndrome and high-functioning autism. J Clin Psychol. (2017) 73:113–25. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Howlin P, Mawhood L, Rutter M. Autism and developmental receptive language disorder—a follow-up comparison in early adult life. II: social, behavioural, and psychiatric outcomes. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2000) 41:561–78. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00643

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Orsmond GI, Krauss MW, Seltzer MM. Peer relationships and social and recreational activities among adolescents and adults with autism. J Autism Dev Disord. (2004) 34:245–56. doi: 10.1023/B:JADD.0000029547.96610.df

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Canu WH, Carlson CL. Differences in heterosocial behavior and outcomes of ADHD-symptomatic subtypes in a college sample. J Atten Disord. (2003) 6:123–33. doi: 10.1177/108705470300600304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Babinski DE, Pelham WE, Molina BSG, Waschbusch DA, Gnagy EM, Yu J, et al. Women with childhood ADHD: comparisons by diagnostic group and gender. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2011) 33:420–9. doi: 10.1007/s10862-011-9247-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Flory K, Molina B, Pelham WE, Smith B. Childhood ADHD predicts risky sexual behavior in young adulthood. Artic J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2007) 35:571–7. doi: 10.1207/s15374424jccp3504_8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Winters KC, Botzet AM, Fahnhorst T, Baumel L, Lee S. Impulsivity and its relationship to risky sexual behaviors and drug abuse. J Child Adolesc Subst Abus. (2009) 18:43–56. doi: 10.1080/15470650802541095

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. White JW, Buehler C. Adolescent sexual victimization, ADHD symptoms, and risky sexual behavior. J Fam Violence. (2012) 27:123–32. doi: 10.1007/s10896-012-9411-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Huggins SP, Rooney ME, Chronis-Tuscano A. Risky sexual behavior among college students with ADHD: is the mother-child relationship protective? J Atten Disord. (2015) 19:240–50. doi: 10.1177/1087054712459560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Lau W, Peterson CC. Adults and children with Asperger syndrome: exploring adult attachment style, marital satisfaction and satisfaction with parenthood. Res Autism Spectr Disord. (2011) 5:392–9. doi: 10.1016/j.rasd.2010.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Deguchi N, Asakura T. Qualitative study of wives of husbands with autism spectrum disorder: subjective experience of wives from marriage to marital crisis. Psychology. (2018) 09:14–33. doi: 10.4236/psych.2018.91002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Koller R. Sexuality and adolescents with autism. Sex Disabil. (2000) 18:125–35. doi: 10.1023/A:1005567030442

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Hénault I. Asperger's Syndrome and Sexuality: From Adolescence Through Adulthood. (2006). Available online at: https://books.google.com.br/books?hl=pt-BRandlr=andid=YdkPBQAAQBAJandoi=fndandpg=PP1andots=HGcfmhgGdyandsig=TJ8twM_mnnviaMgUmqDaH23zpAU (accessed July 15, 2020).

Google Scholar

37. Willey A, Subramaniam B, Hamilton JA, Couperus J. The mating life of geeks: love, neuroscience, and the new autistic subject. Signs (Chic). (2015) 40:369–91. doi: 10.1086/678146

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Brooks E. “Healthy sexuality”: opposing forces? Autism and dating, romance, and sexuality in the mainstream media. Can J Disabil Stud. (2018) 7:161–86. doi: 10.15353/cjds.v7i2.428

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Engström I, Ekström L, Emilsson B. Psychosocial functioning in a group of Swedish adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. Autism. (2003) 7:99–110. doi: 10.1177/1362361303007001008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Howlin P, Goode S, Hutton J, Rutter M. Adult outcome for children with autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry Allied Discip. (2004) 45:212–29. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00215.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Eakin L, Minde K, Hechtman L, Ochs E, Krane E, Bouffard R, et al. The marital and family functioning of adults with ADHD and their spouses. J Atten Disord. (2004) 8:1–10. doi: 10.1177/108705470400800101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. VanderDrift LE, Antshel KM, Olszewski AK. Inattention and hyperactivity-impulsivity: their detrimental effect on romantic relationship maintenance. J Atten Disord. (2019) 23:985–94. doi: 10.1177/1087054717707043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Sbarra DA, Law RW, Portley RM. Divorce and death: a meta-analysis and research agenda for clinical, social, and health psychology. Perspect Psychol Sci. (2011) 6:454–74. doi: 10.1177/1745691611414724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Sbarra DA, Hasselmo K, Bourassa KJ. Divorce and health: beyond individual differences. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. (2015) 24:109–13. doi: 10.1177/0963721414559125

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Kiecolt-Glaser JK. Marriage, divorce, and the immune system. Am Psychol. (2018) 73:1098–108. doi: 10.1037/amp0000388

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Hatfield E, Sprecher S. Measuring passionate love in intimate relationships. J Adolesc. (1986) 9:383–410. doi: 10.1016/S0140-1971(86)80043-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Sternberg RJ. A triangular theory of love. Psychol Rev. (1986) 93:119–35. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.93.2.119

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Acevedo BP, Aron A. Does a long-term relationship kill romantic love? Rev Gen Psychol. (2009) 13:59–65. doi: 10.1037/a0014226

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Graham JM. Measuring love in romantic relationships: a meta-analysis. J Soc Pers Relat. (2011) 28:748–71. doi: 10.1177/0265407510389126

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Ratelle CF, Carbonneau N, Vallerand RJ, Mageau G. Passion in the romantic sphere: a look at relational outcomes. Motiv Emot. (2013) 37:106–20. doi: 10.1007/s11031-012-9286-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Huston TL, Houts RM, Caughlin JP, Smith SE, George LJ. The connubial crucible: newlywed years as predictors of marital delight, distress, and divorce. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2001) 80:237–52. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.2.237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Kessler RC, Adler L, Ames M, Demler O, Faraone S, Hiripi E, et al. The World Health Organization adult ADHD self-report scale (ASRS): a short screening scale for use in the general population. Psychol Med. (2005) 35:245–56. doi: 10.1017/S0033291704002892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Skinner R, Martin J, Clubley E. The autism-spectrum quotient (aq): evidence from asperger syndrome/high-functioning autism, males and females, scientists and mathematicians. J Autism Dev Disord. (2001) 31:5–17. doi: 10.1023/A:1005653411471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Cacioppo S, Bianchi-demicheli F, Hatfield E, Rapson RL. Social neuroscience of love. Clin Neuropsychiatry. (2012) 9. Available online at: https://www.clinicalneuropsychiatry.org/

Google Scholar

55. Hernandez JAE. Validade de Construto da Escala de Amor Apaixonado. Psicol Teor e Prática. (2015) 17:133–45. doi: 10.15348/1980-6906/psicologia.v17n3p133-145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. ABEP. Brazilian Criteria 2015 and social class distribution update for 2016. Critério Classif econômica Bras. (2016) 1–6. Available online at: http://www.abep.org/ (accessed April, 2018).

Google Scholar

57. Tay R. Drivers' perceptions and reactions to roadside memorials. Accid Anal Prev. (2009) 41:663–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

58. Smith G. Does Gender Influence Online Survey Participation?: A Record-Linkage Analysis of University Faculty Online Survey Response Behavior. ERIC Document Reproduction Service No. ED 501717 (2008).

Google Scholar

59. Holmes TH, Rahe RH. The social readjustment rating scale. J Psychosom Res. (1967) 11:213–8. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(67)90010-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. McAndrew FT, Akande A, Turner S, Sharma Y. A cross-cultural ranking of stressful life events in Germany, India, South Africa, and The United States. J Cross Cult Psychol. (1998) 29:717–27. doi: 10.1177/0022022198296003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Hatfield E, Rapson RL. The neuropsychology of passionate love. In: Cuyler E, and Ackhart M, editors. Psychology of Social Relationships. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science (2009) 1–47.

Google Scholar

62. Kim J, Hatfield E. Love types and subjective well-being: a cross-cultural study. Soc Behav Pers. (2004) 32:173–82. doi: 10.2224/sbp.2004.32.2.173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Traupmann J, Eckels E, Hatfield E. Intimacy in older women's lives. Gerontologist. (1982) 22:493–8. doi: 10.1093/geront/22.6.493

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Masuda M. Meta-analyses of love scales: do various love scales measure the same psychological constructs? Jpn Psychol Res. (2003) 45:25–37. doi: 10.1111/1468-5884.00030

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions: Constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. (1997) 121:65–94. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.121.1.65

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Thompson RA. Emotion regulation: a theme in search of definition. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev. (1994) 59:25. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-5834.1994.tb01276.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Skirrow C, McLoughlin G, Kuntsi J, Asherson P. Behavioral, neurocognitive and treatment overlap between attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder and mood instability. Expert Rev Neurother. (2009) 9:489–503. doi: 10.1586/ern.09.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Bruner MR, Kuryluk AD, Whitton SW. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptom levels and romantic relationship quality in college students. J Am Coll Heal. (2015) 63:98–108. doi: 10.1080/07448481.2014.975717

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Rapport LJ, Friedman SL, Tzelepis A, Van Voorhis A. Experienced emotion and affect recognition in adult attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder. Neuropsychology. (2002) 16:102–10. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.16.1.102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Overbey GA, Snell WE, Callis KE. Subclinical ADHD, stress, and coping in romantic relationships of university students. J Atten Disord. (2010) 15:67–78. doi: 10.1177/1087054709347257

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Robin AL, Payson E. The impact of ADHD on marriage. ADHD Rep. (2002) 10:9–14. doi: 10.1521/adhd.10.3.9.20553

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Canu WH, Tabor LS, Michael KD, Bazzini DG, Elmore AL. Young adult romantic couples' conflict resolution and satisfaction varies with partner's attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder type. J Marital Fam Ther. (2014) 40:509–24. doi: 10.1111/jmft.12018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Sigman M, Ungerer J, Sherman T. Defining the social deficits of autism: the contribution of non-verbal communication measures. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (1986) 27:657–69. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00190.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Stokes M, Newton N, Kaur A. Stalking, and social and romantic functioning among adolescents and adults with autism spectrum disorder. J Autism Dev Disord. (2007) 37:1969–86. doi: 10.1007/s10803-006-0344-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Stokes MA, Kaur A. High-functioning autism and sexuality: a parental perspective. Autism. (2005) 9:266–89. doi: 10.1177/1362361305053258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Cacioppo S, Frum C, Asp E, Weiss RM, Lewis JW, Cacioppo JT. A quantitative meta-analysis of functional imaging studies of social rejection. Sci Rep. (2013) 3:2027. doi: 10.1038/srep02027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Braithwaite S, Holt-Lunstad J. Romantic relationships and mental health. Curr Opin Psychol. (2017) 13:120–5. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.04.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: autism, ADHD, romantic (love), passionate love, attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder

Citation: Soares LS, Alves ALC, Costa DS, Malloy-Diniz LF, Paula JJ, Romano-Silva MA and Miranda DM (2021) Common Venues in Romantic Relationships of Adults With Symptoms of Autism and Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 12:593150. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.593150

Received: 24 September 2020; Accepted: 21 May 2021;
Published: 18 June 2021.

Edited by:

Jeffrey I. Hunt, Brown University, United States

Reviewed by:

Robert Lee Hendren, University of California, San Francisco, United States
Petya Radoeva, Brown University, United States

Copyright © 2021 Soares, Alves, Costa, Malloy-Diniz, Paula, Romano-Silva and Miranda. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Débora Marques de Miranda, debora.m.miranda@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.