Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 13 January 2021
Sec. Psychological Therapies
This article is part of the Research Topic Contemporary Issues in Defining the Mechanisms of Cognitive Behavior Therapy View all 13 articles

The Temporal Associations of Therapeutic Alliance and Manual Adherence With Depressive Symptom Change in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Adult Outpatient Major Depression

\nFrank J. Don,
Frank J. Don1,2*Ellen Driessen,&#x;Ellen Driessen2,3Jaap PeenJaap Peen2Jan Spijker,Jan Spijker1,4Robert J. DeRubeisRobert J. DeRubeis5Matthijs BlankersMatthijs Blankers2Jack J. M. Dekker,Jack J. M. Dekker2,3
  • 1Expert Center for Depression, Pro Persona Mental Health Care, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Research, Arkin Mental Health Care, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 3Department of Clinical, Neuro and Developmental Psychology, Amsterdam Public Health Research Institute, Vrije Universiteit Amsterdam, Amsterdam, Netherlands
  • 4Department of Clinical Psychology, Behavioral Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands
  • 5School of Arts and Sciences, University of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, PA, United States

Background: The therapeutic alliance is considered an important causal agent of psychotherapy efficacy. However, studies in cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) for depression have suggested that alliance might be more of a consequence rather than a cause of depressive symptom change, while adherence to CBT specific techniques was found to be associated with subsequent depression change. We aimed to add to this body of literature by assessing the temporal associations of both therapeutic alliance and manual adherence with depressive symptom change in a relatively large sample of depressed adult outpatients over the full course of CBT.

Methods: Adults with a major depressive episode (n = 98) participating in a randomized clinical trial were offered 22 weeks of CBT and rated the Penn Helping Alliance Questionnaire (HAq-I) at weeks 5 and 22. Therapists rated their adherence to the CBT manual after each session and observers assessed the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale scores at weeks 0, 5, 10, and 22. Linear mixed model analyses were used to assess the associations of alliance and adherence with prior and subsequent depression change.

Results: HAq-I Relationship and manual adherence ratings were not significantly associated with prior nor with subsequent depression change (p > 0.14). Prior depression change was associated with the HAq-I subscale Perceived helpfulness at the end of treatment (r = 0.30, CI = 0.03–0.56, p = 0.03).

Conclusion: We were not able to replicate prior depression change in CBT for depression to be associated with improved quality of the therapeutic alliance when using a more “pure” measure of the therapeutic relationship. Limitations of this study include the subjective alliance and adherence assessments. Our findings indicate the need to appropriately distinguish between the perceived helpfulness and the relationship factors when examining therapeutic alliance.

Introduction

Cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) is one of the best-known and empirically supported psychological treatments for depression. Although CBT has shown to be efficacious in the treatment of depression (e.g., 1), still, up to 50% of patients fail to achieve an adequate response, and even fewer achieve remission following an acute treatment trial (1). Thus, the efficacy of CBT for depression needs to be improved.

One possible way to enhance efficacy is to investigate which treatment factors in CBT result in symptom change (2, 3), so that “we can direct better, stronger, different, or more strategies that trigger the critical change process(es)” (2). Two theories pose how CBT results in depressive symptom change: the behavioral activation theory (4) and the cognitive theory (5). Lewinsohn et al. (4) theorize that depression is caused or maintained by a reduction in satisfying activities. Patients are therefore encouraged to engage in activities that positively influence their mood. On the other hand, the cognitive theory posits that inaccurate beliefs and maladaptive information processing play a causal role in the development and maintenance of depressive symptoms (5). According to this theory, correcting these beliefs and processes is the core working mechanism in CBT for depression. In order to capitalize on these theorized effective treatment factors, it is important that therapies are being conducted as intended, i.e., that the therapist is adherent to the treatment manual (6, 7).

Besides the specific CBT factors mentioned previously, there is a more general—or non-specific—factor that is supposed to contribute to CBT efficacy. The quality of the therapeutic alliance is considered an important factor that can enhance the efficacy of psychotherapy in general (8, 9). According to Bordin's (10) influential definition, therapeutic alliance implies that therapist and patient (a) agree on treatment goals, (b) define a set of therapeutic tasks used to achieve the goals, and (c) form a positively toned emotional bond.

In an attempt to assess the associations of therapeutic alliance as well as therapist's adherence to the treatment manual with treatment effect, Castonguay et al. (11) found treatment adherence and the alliance—both as assessed at the end of treatment—to be associated with a better response to CBT for depression. However, in this study the temporal relation of adherence and alliance with symptom change was not adequately established (2). It did not rule out reverse causality: that it was symptom change that drove adherence and alliance rather than the other way around. Adequately taking reverse causality into account requires addressing the temporal relationship between treatment factors and outcome.

DeRubeis and Feeley (12) were the first to use this strategy when investigating process factors in CBT associated with alleviation of depressive symptoms. In a sample of 25 depressed outpatients, they found that the extent to which therapists adhered to the manual, i.e., they used concrete symptom-focused CBT in an early session, was associated with subsequent change in depression. This finding was replicated by Sasso et al. (13), Strunk et al. (14), and Brotman (15), and provides support for the behavioral activation and cognitive theories of depression. However, an investigation by Snippe et al. (16) among depressed diabetics following CBT failed to identify a significant association between adherence and treatment effect.

DeRubeis and Feeley (12) also found that therapeutic alliance was not correlated with subsequent change in depression, but was correlated with prior symptom change, suggesting that the quality of the therapeutic relation was more of a consequence rather than a cause of depression symptom change. This finding is particularly interesting, because it is contrary to the conventional wisdom that the quality of the therapeutic relation is an important causal agent of psychotherapy efficacy. It received only marginal support in a replication study by the same authors, where prior symptom change predicted therapeutic alliance at trend level only (17), but it was replicated in larger samples by Strunk et al. (14) and by Strunk et al. (18). However, the last two investigations were restricted to the first five and three sessions, respectively, leaving unanswered which changes may have taken place in later phases of treatment. Using a sophisticated repeated-measures design, Falkenström et al. (19) also found that improvement in alliance was associated with a reduction of depressive symptoms in the next CBT or IPT session, but the authors note the relatively small sample size as a limitation of their study (43 patients underwent CBT). Together, the findings of these studies suggest that in CBT for depression, quality of the therapeutic relation might be a consequence of depression symptom change. This is in contrast with studies investigating the alliance in treatments other than CBT for depression, which generally found the alliance associated with subsequent symptom change (2028). It should be noted, however, that several studies also have failed to find a significant relationship between alliance and symptom change in CBT for depression (29, 30) even when applying a high-quality mediator study design. A meta-analysis among several kinds of psychotherapies and disorders by Flückiger et al. (31) revealed a significant relation only in investigations with a specific interest in the alliance.

In this study, we aim to add to the literature mentioned previously by investigating the temporal associations of both therapeutic alliance and manual adherence with depressive symptom change in CBT for adult outpatient major depression in a relatively large sample of patients over the entire course of the treatment. We hypothesize that prior symptom change will be positively associated with therapeutic alliance, but that therapeutic alliance will not be associated with subsequent symptom change. Furthermore, we hypothesize that early manual adherence will be associated with subsequent symptom change, but not with prior symptom change.

Materials and Methods

Design

This paper draws upon data from the CBT condition of a randomized clinical trial in the outpatient treatment of major depression (32). The study design was approved by the Dutch Union of Medical-Ethic Trial Committees for mental health organizations and the study protocol was published (33). Of the 341 participants randomized to treatment in the clinical trial, 164 were assigned to the CBT condition. Of these, 66 had a baseline HDRS score >24 and were offered additional antidepressant medication.

Participants

Participants were referred by their general practitioner for depression treatment to one of three outpatient mental health clinics in Amsterdam, the Netherlands. Inclusion criteria were: (34) main diagnosis of depressive disorder according to DSM-IV criteria (35) as assessed by the MINI-International Neuropsychiatric Interview—Plus (36), (1) a score of 14 or above on the Hamilton Depression Rating Scale (HRSD, 30), (2) age between 18 and 65 years, and (3) a written informed consent after having received a complete description of the study.

Exclusion criteria included presence of psychotic symptoms or bipolar disorder, severe suicidality warranting immediate intensive treatment or hospitalization, substance misuse or abuse in the past 6 months, pregnancy, inability to meet trial demands due to for example medical conditions, and use of psychotropic or other medications that might influence mental functions. Patients on an antidepressant regimen were included only if the medication they were currently taking was judged to be inefficacious by both the patient and the intake psychiatrist. If so, the medication was tapered off under medical supervision, and baseline assessment took place after a washout period of at least 1 week after the medication was completely stopped. Patients with very severe depression (HDRS score >24) at baseline were offered additional antidepressant medication administrated by a psychiatrist. We excluded these patients for this work, because the effects of CBT could not be disentangled from those of the antidepressant medication.

Separate random allocation sequences were generated for each of the three clinics by one of the authors (J.P.) using SPSS random number generator (SPSS, Chicago). Randomization was stratified by gender and age (<32.5 and >32.5 years). Research assistants, aware of the allocation sequence, enrolled participants, and assigned them into interventions.

Intervention

CBT comprised 16 individual sessions within 22 weeks, with the first 10 sessions taking place weekly and the final six taking place 2-weekly. CBT was conducted according to a published treatment manual (37) and consisted of an introductory session, three treatment phases and a concluding session. In the introductory session, acquaintance with the therapist was made, therapy conditions were explained, and a treatment contract was signed by both the patient and the therapist. The first treatment phase (sessions 2–4) focused on behavioral activation by means of planning and registering activities and concurrent mood levels. In the second CBT phase (sessions 5–7), the cognitive model was explained and patients kept a thought diary to identify automatic thoughts. These thoughts were challenged in the third phase (session 8–15), when they were tested on their validity and utility by logical reasoning. Patients were encouraged to identify reasoning errors in their own thinking. In addition, a behavioral experiment was designed and conducted to test the identified automatic thoughts in real life. Depending on the patient's needs, sessions 13–15 could be spent on complementary challenging techniques or conducting additional behavioral experiments. The final session (session 16) concluded treatment by evaluating the therapy and the therapeutic goals, and discussing strategies of action in case of relapse.

CBT therapists were psychiatrists or psychologists with at least a master's degree who completed a 100-h basic CBT training course accredited by the Dutch Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapy. Moreover, all therapists adequately conducted at least one intensively supervised therapy case in accordance with the treatment manual as judged by a study supervisor. Although no formal assessments were conducted, treatment fidelity was checked by means of biweekly supervision sessions, chaired by a study supervisor, in which audiotaped material was discussed. All study supervisors were registered supervisors with the Dutch Association for Behavioral and Cognitive Therapy.

Measures

An overview of the assessments included in this work is provided in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Timeline of assessments during CBT treatment.

Depression severity was assessed with the Dutch version of the 17-item HRSD (38, 39) at weeks 0, 5, 10, and 22. The HRSD is a structured interview designed to quantify the severity of depressive symptoms in patients already diagnosed as suffering from a depressive disorder. Its items cover different depressive symptoms, such as mood, sleep problems, lack of appetite, weight loss, suicide intentions, and feelings of guilt, which are rated on either a 0–2 or 0–4 scale. Trained research assistants (master's-level graduate students in clinical psychology) assessed the HRSD according to the Dutch scoring manual (40). Assessors participated in biweekly 1-h peer supervision sessions, in which audiotaped interviews were discussed. The average intraclass correlation coefficient over 46 audiotaped assessments scored by multiple assessors was 0.97. The HRSD showed good reliability (Cronbach's alpha:0.82).

Therapeutic alliance was assessed from the patient's perspective at weeks 5 and 22 by means of the Penn Helping Alliance Questionnaire Method (HAq-I), which assesses the extent to which the patient experiences the therapist and the therapy as helpful (41, 42). The HAq-I is a self-report instrument including 11 items that are rated on a 6-point scale from −3 (“No, I strongly feel that it is not true”) to 3 (“Yes, I strongly feel that it is true”). The total score equals the sum of the item ratings. The HAq-I correlates well with other measures of therapeutic alliance (4345) and the strength of the association between alliance and outcome assessed with HAq-I is comparable with other measures (46). The Penn Helping Alliance Scales distinguish two types of helping alliance. Helping Alliance Type 1 refers to the patient's perceived helpfulness of the therapist, whereas Helping Alliance Type 2 is defined as the patient's collaboration or bonding with the therapist. The HAq-I Type 2 subscale items have shown to form an independent factor (43, 47) that measures the collaborative nature of the therapeutic relationship. We used this subscale for the main analyses in this study, because, in our opinion, it better reflects Bordin's (10) definition of the therapeutic alliance. The reliability of both the total scores and Type 1 & 2 subscales of the HAq-I was good (Cronbach's alpha: type 1 alliance, 0.92; type 2 alliance, 0.92; total alliance 0.94).

CBT manual adherence was assessed by the therapist, who rated the extent to which he or she had been able to adhere to the treatment manual for that session on a scale from 1 (“not at all”) to 10 (“completely”) after each session.

Data-analysis

We first calculated raw prior depression change scores for each patient by subtracting the HRSD score at week 0 from the HRSD scores at week 5, week 10, and week 22. Similarly, we calculated raw subsequent depression change scores by calculating the differences between the HRSD score at weeks 0, 5, and 10, and the end-of-treatment HRSD score at week 22. Next, we transformed the raw change scores to residualized change scores. More specifically, we used ANOVA to predict each patient's change based on their HRSD-score at week 0. We calculated the difference between the predicted score and the patient's actual change score resulting in a residual. A positive sign of this residual signifies a better-than-expected effect, while a negative sign indicates the effect is less than expected. We added the group mean change to the residuals and transformed these scores into z-scores to center the residual change around the group mean change (48)877–883.

With regard to therapeutic alliance, following Lorenzo-Luaces et al. (49), we used the sum score for the Relationship (Type 2)-subscale (items 1, 6, 7, 8, 9, and 10) for our main analyses, because this measures the collaborative nature of the therapeutic relationship, and not the perceived helpfulness that characterizes items from the Type 1-subscale. However, we also conducted sensitivity analyses using the HAQ-I total and the Type 1 (Perceived helpfulness) subscale sum scores.

With regard to manual adherence, mean adherence scores were calculated for sessions 1–5, sessions 6–10, and sessions 11–16, corresponding with treatment weeks 0–5, weeks 6–10, and weeks 11–22, respectively. We visually inspected Probability-Probability plots and judged all variables to be normally distributed.

We then assessed the associations of alliance and adherence with prior and subsequent depression change using linear mixed model analyses with a two-level structure (patient and therapist). In analyses of prior symptom change, alliance, or adherence served as the dependent variable and symptom change as the independent variable. In analyses of subsequent depression change, this variable served as the dependent variable and alliance or adherence as the independent variable. By design, alliance at week 22 (end of treatment) could only be related to prior depression change. Similarly, adherence in sessions 1–5 could only be related to subsequent symptom change and adherence in sessions 10–16 could only be related to prior symptom change.

In analyses including therapeutic alliance, the number of prior episodes was added as a covariate, because this variable has been found to moderate the alliance-outcome association in CBT for depression (49). In addition, we examined possible other confounders separately for each analysis, by testing whether the independent variable was significantly associated with one of the baseline characteristics (Table 2) using one-way ANOVA. As a result, we added gender as a covariate in the analysis of therapeutic alliance at week 5 and subsequent depression change (Table 3). Before computing the estimate of fixed effects, results were standardized into z-scores in order to get an r-type effect size following Strunk et al. (12, 730), where 0.20 represents a small effect 0.30 a medium sized effect and 0.50 representing a large effect (50). All analyses were conducted using SPSS version 22.0. The significance level used was alpha <0.05, 2-sided.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Baseline characteristics of 98 patients assigned to cognitive behavioral therapy for depression.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Associations of therapeutic alliance and manual adherence with prior and subsequent change in depression imputing all variables.

Missing data for the different measures were imputed at item-level by means of multiple imputation, using the MICE package in “R” statistical software [version 2.25; (51)]. The default settings for the imputation method were applied, meaning that predictive mean matching was used for the imputation of missing numeric data, logistic regression imputation for binary data, polytomous regression imputation for unordered categorical data, and proportional odds model imputation for ordered categorical data. Variables with more than 50% missings were not imputed. Twenty imputed datasets were created. Density plots showed the distribution of the imputed data following the distribution of the original data, indicating adequate imputation. The analyses were performed on the 20 imputed datasets separately, and these results were combined using Rubin's rules (52) in SPSS. We used the imputed data for our main analyses, but we conducted sensitivity analyses using the observed (unimputed) data only. We also conducted sensitivity analyses in which the HRSD-scores at week 22 were not imputed but the other variables were, as imputation of the outcome variable has been disputed (53).

As an additional analysis we examined whether there was an interaction effect between the alliance at week 5 and manual adherence up to that point on subsequent symptom change in order to investigate whether manual adherence has more effect in the context of a good therapeutic alliance.

Results

Participants

From April 2006 to December 2009, 4,866 patients were assessed for eligibility during a standard intake procedure. Ninety-eight participants were included in the present study, 67 (69.8%) of which were female. Their mean age was 37.3 years (SD = 10.79; range 22–64) and their mean pre-treatment HRSD score was 19.96 (SD = 2.68), indicating severe levels of depression (54). The majority of the participants were never married (54.2%), had intermediate (36.5%) to high (42.7%) education levels, were unemployed (54.2%), had a depressive episode duration of <6 months (34.0%), and had reported two or more prior depressive episodes (46.2%). Detailed characteristics of the study sample are described in Table 2.

Thirty-five therapists treated on average 2.8 patients (range 1–9). The majority of patients had a female therapist (75.5%) with a mean age of 40.9 years (SD = 10.2, range 27–57). The average number of CBT sessions attended was 10.8 (SD = 5.5).

Therapeutic Alliance and Symptom Change

The associations of therapeutic alliance with prior and subsequent depression change after imputation of missing data are shown in Table 3. Therapeutic alliance, as assessed with the Type 2 (Relationship) subscale at both week 5 (r = 0.14, CI: −0.15–0.43, p =0.34) and week 22 (r = 0.20, CI: −0.07 to 0.47, p = 0.14) was not associated with prior symptom change. Nor was therapeutic alliance Type 2 at week 5 associated with subsequent symptom change (r = 0.14, CI: −0.16 to 0.45, p = 0.35).

Sensitivity analyses with the Type 1 (Helpfulness) and HAq-I total sum scores also indicated no significant association between prior depression change and therapeutic alliance at week 5 (Table 3). However, prior symptom change was significantly associated with therapeutic alliance Type 1 at week 22 (r = 0.30, CI: 0.03–0.56, p = 0.03), and a similar association was found at the level of a non-significant trend for the HAq-I total score (r = 0.25, CI: −0.01 to 0.54, p = 0.06). HAq-Type 1 at week 5 predicted subsequent change at the level of a non-significant trend (r = 0.24, CI: −0.01 to 0.50, p = 0.06).

Sensitivity analyses using the observed data only and using the dataset in which all but the HRSD-score at week 22 were imputed are described in the (Supplementary Tables 1, 2). In the analysis of the unimputed data only, HAq-I Type 2 at week 22 was significantly associated with prior symptom change (r = 0.43, CI:0.13–0.68, p = 0.01), but not when imputing all variables but the HDRS at week 22 (r = 0.23, CI: −0.09 to 0.58, p = 0.15). We found no association between alliance at week 5 and prior or subsequent change in both sensitivity analyses (ps > 0.10), though HAq-I Type 2 at week 5 was associated with subsequent depression change at the level of a non-significant trend (r = 0.27, CI: −0.05 to 0.74, p = 0.08) when using the observed data.

Manual Adherence and Symptom Change

The associations of CBT manual adherence with prior and subsequent depression change after imputation of missing data are shown in Table 3. Mean CBT manual adherence scores in the first 5 weeks of treatment were not associated with subsequent depressive symptom change (r = −0.05, CI: −2.36 to 1.70, p = 0.75). Mean adherence scores in weeks 5–10 and weeks 10–22 were not associated with prior symptom change up until these weeks either (5 and 10, respectively) (r = 0.11, CI: −0.19 to 0.41, p = 0.46 and = 0.04, CI: −0.22 to 0.31, p = 0.71). Sensitivity analyses using the observed data and the dataset in which all variables but the HRSD-scores at week 22 were imputed are described in the (Supplementary Tables 1, 2) and showed similar results. Again, no significant associations were found between manual adherence and prior or subsequent depressive symptom change (ps > 0.15).

There were no interaction effects between therapeutic alliance (HAq-I type 1, HAq-I type 2, and HAq-I total score) at week 5 and manual adherence up to that point on subsequent symptom change (ps> 0.05).

Discussion

Findings

We examined the temporal associations of therapeutic alliance and manual adherence with depressive symptom change in adult outpatients receiving CBT for depression. We found no association of CBT manual adherence with prior nor with subsequent symptom change. Similarly, we did not find therapeutic alliance to be related with prior nor with subsequent depression change in our primary analyses, in which we used the HAq-I type 2 subscale containing the items that purely tap the collaborative nature of the alliance. We did find prior depression change to be associated with the HAq-I type 1 subscale that assesses the perceived helpfulness, as well as for the total HAq-I scores (which is the sum of both subscales) at the level of a non-significant trend. We take this finding to indicate that patients who have experienced more depressive symptom alleviation over the course of their 22 week treatment also perceive their therapy and therapist as more helpful. Similarly, the perceived helpfulness subscale scores at week 5 were associated with subsequent depression change at the level of a non-significant trend, meaning that patients that perceive their therapist as helpful also may experience more symptom reduction.

Thus, we were not able to replicate previous studies finding prior symptom change to be associated with therapeutic alliance in CBT (12, 14) and CBT combined with antidepressant medication (18), when using a measure that, in our opinion, purely taps the collaborative nature of the alliance. Rather, our findings are in line with previous work in which no significant relationship between alliance and symptom change in CBT for depression was found (29, 30). We also did not find that the alliance is associated with subsequent depression change, which was also found in previous studies in CBT for depression (12, 14, 18), but not for other treatments like alliance fostering therapy (e.g., 15, 16) and supportive-expressive psychotherapy or clinical management combined with pharmacotherapy or clinical management combined placebo (27, 28). Maybe the therapeutic alliance plays a less important role in CBT for Depression than it does in other therapies (19). Concerning manual adherence, we also were not able to replicate prior studies finding adherence in CBT to be associated with subsequent depression change (e.g., 12, 50, 51), but our findings are in line with other work reporting no significant relation between adherence and treatment effect in CBT for depression (16).

Strengths and Limitations

The study has a number of strengths. First, the study includes a relatively a large sample. Second, several elements contribute to the generalizability of the study's findings to general clinical practice. Treatment was provided in regular psychiatric outpatient clinics by a large number of therapists with different experience levels. Patients were not recruited by advertisement but instead were referred by general practitioners, no selection criteria with regard to previous treatment or suitability for psychotherapy were applied, and patients with relatively low socioeconomic status were included. Third, we carefully distinguished the collaborative nature of the therapeutic relationship and the perceived helpfulness of the therapist/therapy in our analyses. Fourth, our study design allowed us to examine the temporal relation between treatment effect and process variables and we used sophisticated statistical techniques to do so, controlling for patient and therapist variance.

This study also has a number of limitations. First, although depression symptom severity was assessed by independent observers, manual adherence and therapeutic alliance were subjectively assessed by, respectively, therapists and patients. Although the patient's perspective is frequently used in alliance research [e.g., (19, 55)], patients and therapists may be biased in their judgements by the improvement (or lack thereof) they experience. Independent raters, blind to outcome (56), may assess alliance and adherence more objectively. Second and related, symptom change and therapeutic alliance were rated later in treatment than in some other studies (e.g., 48) and we cannot rule out the possibility that an interaction between alliance and outcome might have already taken place at week 5. Third, although our research design allowed us to study the temporal associations of therapeutic alliance and manual adherence with symptom change, our design did not allow us to identify either of these variables as mechanisms of change (2). Neither does our study rule out possible third variable causality (that some unmeasured patient characteristic facilitated both the process variable and symptom change with no direct causal link between the two). Fourth, no control condition was included in the study.

Clinical and Research Implications

The fact that we did not find a relation between treatment effect and the alliance does not mean that the alliance is irrelevant in CBT. It has been long suggested that in CBT for depression the alliance is necessary but not sufficient for therapeutic change to take place (e.g., 6). Rather, to put it in the words of DeRubeis and Feeley (1990), our well-trained and “empathetic therapists may undoubtedly have created a proper environment for therapeutic change.”

Concerning the limitations of this study, we recommend researchers investigating relations of alliance and adherence with symptom change to use more objective measures. Recent work has suggested that working mechanisms might be too complex to be captured in simple causal models (57) and this might also apply to the alliance and adherence. Indeed, for example Sasso et al. (58) found that protocol adherence has different aspects and may work differently in specific subgroups. Additionally, Lorenzo-Luaces et al. (49) found that the alliance-outcome association was moderated by the number of previous depressive episodes, also suggesting that the relationship between alliance and outcome can be different for different patients. We advocate further investigation of moderators of alliance-outcome and adherence-outcome relationships. Most importantly, however, our work underlines the importance of distinguishing the perceived helpfulness from the more pure relationship items of the HAq-I when examining therapeutic alliance and we recommend future investigations of therapeutic alliance to use an instrument not containing items that may also measure therapeutic progress as this can potentially bias results.

Conclusions

We examined the temporal associations of therapeutic alliance and manual adherence with depressive symptom change in adult outpatients receiving CBT for depression. We found no association of CBT manual adherence with prior nor with subsequent symptom change. Similarly, we did not find therapeutic alliance to be related with prior nor with subsequent depression change in our primary analyses, in which we used the HAq-I type 2 subscale containing the items that purely tap the collaborative nature of the alliance. Thus, we were not able to replicate prior depression change in CBT for depression to be associated with improved quality of the therapeutic alliance. Our findings indicate the need to appropriately distinguish between perceived helpfulness and the more pure relationship items of the HAq-I when examining therapeutic alliance.

Data Availability Statement

The dataset presented in this article can be accessed upon request. Requests can be directed to Jack J. M. Dekker, jack.dekker@arkin.nl.

Ethics Statement

This study was reviewed and approved by Dutch Union of Medical Ethics Trial Committees for mental health organisations. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

FD and ED wrote the manuscript. ED coordinated the data acquisition. JP performed the statistical analyses. MB performed the imputations. RD contributed to the research design. JD an JS lead the research project. All authors provided comments and read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

This study was supported by an unrestricted grant from Wyeth pharmaceuticals. Arkin Mental Health Care, Amsterdam, ProPersona Mental Health Care, Nijmegen, and the Faculty of Clinical Psychology and Education, Department of Clinical Psychology, VU University, Amsterdam, The Netherlands financially supported the author's contributions to this manuscript. ED's contributions to this work were also supported by The Netherlands Organisation of Scientific Research (NWO; 016.Veni.195.215 6806). None of the sponsors had a role in the design and conduct of the study, collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; nor in the preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest

FD and JD receive fees from Springer Media.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank all participating clients, therapists, and management of Arkin Mental Health Amsterdam for their priceless contributions to the research project. Also teachers and students of the 2015 Clinical Psychologist course at Radboud Centre for Social Sciences Nijmegen, The Netherlands contributed with ideas and suggestions regarding the design and style of the manuscript.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.602294/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Rush AJ, Trivedi MH, Wisniewski SR, Nierenberg AA, Stewart JW, Warden D, et al. Acute and longer-term outcomes in depressed outpatients requiring one or several treatment steps: a STAR*D report. Am J Psychiatry. (2006) 163:1905–17. doi: 10.1176/ajp.2006.163.11.1905

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Kazdin AE. Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. (2007) 3:1–27. doi: 10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.3.022806.091432

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Driessen E, Hollon SD. Cognitive behavioral therapy for mood disorders: efficacy, moderators and mediators. Psychiatr Clin North Am. (2010) 33:537–55. doi: 10.1016/j.psc.2010.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Lewinsohn PM, Biglan A, Zeiss AM. Behavioral treatment of depression. In: Davidson PO, editor. The Behavioral Management of Anxiety, Depression and Pain. New York, NY: Brunner/Mazel (1976) p. 91–146.

5. Beck AT, Rush AJ, Shaw BF, Emery G. Cognitive Therapy of Depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press (1979).

Google Scholar

6. Webb CA, DeRubeis RJ, Hollon SD, Dimidjian S, Amsterdam JD, Shelton RC. Convergence and divergence in the delivery of cognitive therapy in two randomized clinical trials. Behav Res Ther. (2013) 51:493–8. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2013.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Perepletchikova F, Kazdin AE. Treatment integrity and therapeutic change: issues and research recommendations. Clin Psychol Sci Prac. (2005) 12:365–83. doi: 10.1093/clipsy.bpi045

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Horvath AO, Luborsky L. The role of the therapeutic alliance in psychotherapy. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1993) 61:561–73. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.61.4.561

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kazantzis N, Luong HK, Usatoff AS, Impala T, Yew RY, Hofmann SG. The processes of cognitive behavioral therapy: a review of meta-analyses. Cogn Ther Res. (2018) 42:349–57. doi: 10.1007/s10608-018-9920-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Bordin ES. The generalizability of the psychoanalytic concept of the working alliance. Psychotherapy. (1979) 16:252–60. doi: 10.1037/h0085885

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Castonguay LG, Goldfried MR, Wiser S, Raue PJ, Hayes AM. Predicting the effect of cognitive therapy for depression: a study of unique and common factors. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1996) 64:497–504. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.64.3.497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. DeRubeis RJ, Feeley M. Determinants of change in cognitive therapy for depression. Cogn Ther Res. (1990) 14:469–82. doi: 10.1007/BF01172968

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Sasso KE, Strunk DR, Braun JD, DeRubeis RJ, Brotman MA. A re-examination of process-outcome relations in cognitive therapy for depression: disaggregating within-patient and between-patient effects. Psychother Res. (2016) 26:387–98. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2015.1026423

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Strunk DR, Brotman MA, DeRubeis RJ. The process of change in cognitive therapy for depression: predictors of early inter-session symptom gains. Behav Res Ther. (2010) 48:599–606. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Brotman MA. Therapeutic alliance and adherence in cognitive therapy for depression. Dissert Abstracts Int Sec B Sci Eng. (2004) 65: 3146.

Google Scholar

16. Snippe E, Schroevers MJ, Tovote KA, Sanderman R, Emmelkamp PMG, Fleer J. Explaining variability in therapist adherence and patient depressive symptom improvement: the role of therapist interpersonal skills and patient engagement. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2019) 26:84–93. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2332

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Feeley M, DeRubeis RJ, Gelfand LA. The temporal relation of adherence and alliance to symptom change in cognitive therapy for depression. J Consult Clin Psychol. (1999) 67:578–82. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.67.4.578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Strunk DR, Cooper AA, Ryan ET, DeRubeis RJ, Hollon SD. The process of change in cognitive therapy for depression when combined with antidepressant medication: predictors of early intersession symptom gains. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2012) 80:730. doi: 10.1037/a0029281

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Falkenström F, Ekeblad A, Holmqvist R. Improvement of the working alliance in one treatment session predicts improvement of depressive symptoms by the next session. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2016) 84:738–51. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Crits-Christoph P, Gibbons MBC, Hamilton J, Ring-Kurtz S, Gallop R. The dependability of alliance assessments: the alliance-outcome correlation is larger than you might think. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2011) 79:267–78. doi: 10.1037/a0023668

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Crits-Christoph P, Johnson JE, Connolly Gibbons MB, Gallop R. Process predictors of the outcome of group drug counseling. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2013) 81:23–34. doi: 10.1037/a0030101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Falkenström F, Granström F, Holmqvist R. Therapeutic alliance predicts symptomatic improvement session by session. J Counsel Psychol. (2013) 60:317–28. doi: 10.1037/a0032258

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Hoffart A, Øktedalen T, Langkaas TF, Wampold BE. Alliance and outcome in varying imagery procedures for PTSD: a study of within-person processes. J Counsel Psychol. (2013) 60:471–82. doi: 10.1037/a0033604

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Marker CD, Comer JS, Abramova V, Kendall PC. The reciprocal relationship between alliance and symptom improvement across the treatment of childhood anxiety. J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. (2013) 42:22–33. doi: 10.1080/15374416.2012.723261

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Tasca GA, Lampard AM. Reciprocal influence of alliance to the group and outcome in day treatment for eating disorders. J Counsel Psychol. (2012) 59:507–17. doi: 10.1037/a0029947

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Xu H, Tracey TJG. Reciprocal influence model of working alliance and therapeutic outcome over individual therapy course. J Counsel Psychol. (2015) 62:351–9. doi: 10.1037/cou0000089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Zilcha-Mano S, Errázuriz P. One size does not fit all: examining heterogeneity and identifying moderators of the alliance-outcome association. J Counsel Psychol. (2015) 62:579–91. doi: 10.1037/cou0000103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Zilcha-Mano S, Dinger U, McCarthy KS, Barber JP. Does alliance predict symptoms throughout treatment, or is it the other way around?. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2014) 82:931–5. doi: 10.1037/a0035141

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Kaufman NK, Rohde P, Seeley JR, Clarke GN, Stice E. Potential mediators of cognitive-behavioral therapy for adolescents with comorbid major depression and conduct disorder. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2005) 73:38–46. doi: 10.1037/0022-006X.73.1.38

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Klug G, Henrich G, Filipiak B, Huber D. Outcome trajectories and mediators of change in psychoanalytic, psychodynamic, and cognitive behavioral therapy. J Am Psychoanal Assoc. (2012) 60:598–605. doi: 10.1177/0003065116644742

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Flückiger C, Del Re AC, Wampold BE, Symonds D, Horvath AO. How central is the alliance in psychotherapy? A multilevel longitudinal meta-analysis. J Couns Psychol. (2012) 59:10–17. doi: 10.1037/a0025749

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Driessen E, Van HL, Don FJ, Peen J, Kool S, Westra D, et al. The efficacy of cognitive behavioral therapy and psychodynamic therapy in the outpatient treatment of major depression: a randomized clinical trial. Am J Psychiatry. (2013) 170:1041–50. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12070899

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Driessen E, Van HL, Schoevers RA, Cuijpers P, van Aalst G, Don FJ, et al. Cognitive behavioral therapy versus short psychodynamic psychotherapy in the outpatient treatment of depression: a randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry. (2007) 7:58. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-7-58

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Cuijpers P, van Straten A, Driessen E, van Oppen P, Bockting CLH, Andersson G. Depression dysthymic disorders. In: Hersen M, Sturmey P, editors. Handbook of Evidence-Based Practice in Clinical Psychology. Vol II. Adult Disorders. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley (2012) p. 243–84. doi: 10.1002/9781118156391.ebcp002011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) 4th edition. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (1994).

Google Scholar

36. Sheehan DV, Lecrubier Y, Sheehan KH, Amorim P, Janavs J, Weiller E, et al. The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview (M.I.N.I.): the development and validation of a structured diagnostic psychiatric interview for DSM-IV and ICD-10. J Clin Psychiatry. (1998) 59:22–33.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

37. Molenaar PJ, Don FJ, Sterk F, van den Bout J, Sterk F, Dekker J. Cognitieve Gedragstherapie bij Depressie. Houten: Bohn Stafleu van Loghum. doi: 10.1007/978-90-313-7352-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Hamilton M. A rating scale for depression. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. (1960) 23:56–62. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.23.1.56

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kupka RW, de Jonghe F, Koeter M, Vermeulen HDB. Betrouwbaarheid van een semi-gestructureerd interview voor de hamilton-depressieschaal [ reliability of a semi-structured interview for the hamilton depression rating scale. Tijdschr. Psychiatr. (1996) 10:759–65.

40. de Jonghe F. Leidraad voor het scoren van de Hamilton Depression Rating Scale. Amsterdam: Benecke (1994).

41. Luborsky L, McLellan AT, Woody GE, O'Brien CP, Auerbach A. Therapist success and its determinants. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (1985) 42:602–11. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1985.01790290084010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Luborsky L. the penn helping alliance scales. In Greenberg LS, Pinsof WM. editors. The Psychotherapeutic Process: A Research Handbook. New York, NY: Guilford Press (1986). p. 325–66.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

43. de Weert-Van Oene GH, de Jong CA, Jörg F, Schrijvers GJ. The helping alliance questionnaire: psychometric properties in patients with substance dependence. Subst Use Misuse. (1999) 34:1549–69. doi: 10.3109/10826089909039414

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Le Bloc'h Y, De Roten Y, Drapeau M, Despland JN. New, but improved? Comparison between first and revised version of the Helping Alliance questionnaire. Schweizer Archiv für Neurol Psychiatrie. (2006) 157:23–8. doi: 10.4414/sanp.2006.01669

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Munder T, Wilmers F, Leonhart R, Linster HW, Barth J. Working alliance inventory-short revised (WAI-SR): psychometric properties in outpatients and inpatients. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2010) 17:231–9. doi: 10.1002/cpp.658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Horvath A, Del Re AC, Flückiger C, Symonds D. Alliance in Individual Psychotherapy. Psychotherapy. (2011) 48:9–16. doi: 10.1037/a0022186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Hendriksen M, Van R, Peen J, Oudejans S, Schoevers R, Dekker J. Psychometric properties of the helping alliance questionnaire-i in psychodynamic psychotherapy for major depression. Psychother Res. (2010) 20:589–98. doi: 10.1080/10503307.2010.493539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Kraemer HC, Wilson GT, Fairburn CG, Agras W. S. Mediators and moderators of treatment effects in randomized clinical trials. Arch Gen Psychiatry. (2002) 59:877–83. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.59.10.877

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Lorenzo-Luaces L, Driessen E, DeRubeis R, Van HL, Keefe JR, Hendriksen M, et al. Moderation of the alliance-outcome association by prior depressive episodes: differential effects in cognitive-behavioral therapy and short-term psychodynamic supportive psychotherapy. Behav Ther. (2017) 48:581–95. doi: 10.1016/j.beth.2016.11.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Cohen J. A power primer. Psychol Bull. (1992) 112:155–9. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. van Buuren S, Groothuis-Oudshoorn K. MICE: multivariate imputation by chained equations in R. J Statis Softw. 45:1–67. doi: 10.18637/jss.v045.i03

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Rubin DB. (2004) Multiple Imputation for Nonresponse in Surveys. Vol. 81. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

53. Sullivan TR, White IR, Salter AB, Ryan P, Lee KJ. Should multiple imputation be the method of choice for handling missing data in randomized trials? Stat Methods Med Res. (2018) 27:2610–26. doi: 10.1177/0962280216683570

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Katz R, Shaw BF, Vallis TM, Kaiser AS. The assessment of severity and symptom patterns in depression. In: Beckham EE. Leber RW, editors. Handbook of Depression 2nd edition New York, NY: Guilford Press (1995). p. 61–85.

Google Scholar

55. Webb CA, Beard C, Auerbach RP, Menninger E, Björgvinsson T. The therapeutic alliance in a naturalistic psychiatric setting: temporal relations with depressive symptom change. Behav Res Ther. (2014) 61:70–7. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2014.07.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Webb CA, DeRubeis RJ, Amsterdam JD, Shelton RC, Hollon SD, Dimidjian S. Two aspects of the therapeutic alliance: differential relations with depressive symptom change. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2011) 79:279–83. doi: 10.1037/a0023252

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Lemmens LHJM, Müller VNLS, Arntz A, Huibers MJH. Mechanisms of change in psychotherapy for depression. Clin Psychol Rev. (2016) 50:95–107. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2016.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Sasso KE, Strunk DR, Braun JD, DeRubeis RJ, Brotman MA. Identifying moderators of the adherence-outcome relation in cognitive therapy for depression. J Consult Clin Psychol. (2015) 83:976. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: therapeutic alliance, manual adherence, cognitive behavioral therapy, depression, temporal associations

Citation: Don FJ, Driessen E, Peen J, Spijker J, DeRubeis RJ, Blankers M and Dekker JJM (2021) The Temporal Associations of Therapeutic Alliance and Manual Adherence With Depressive Symptom Change in Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Adult Outpatient Major Depression. Front. Psychiatry 11:602294. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.602294

Received: 02 September 2020; Accepted: 11 December 2020;
Published: 13 January 2021.

Edited by:

Nikolaos Kazantzis, Institute for Social Neuroscience Psychology, Australia

Reviewed by:

Alicia Salamanca-Sanabria, Singapur ETH-Zentrum, ETH Zürich, Singapore
Fredrik Falkenström, Linköping University, Sweden

Copyright © 2021 Don, Driessen, Peen, Spijker, DeRubeis, Blankers and Dekker. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Frank J. Don, f.don@propersona.nl

Present address: Ellen Driessen, Expert Center for Depression, Pro Persona Mental Health Care, and Department of Clinical Psychology, Behavioral Science Institute, Radboud University, Nijmegen, Netherlands

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.