- Department of Dynamic and Clinical Psychology, Faculty of Medicine and Psychology, Sapienza University of Rome, Rome, Italy
Background: Our study aimed to test the hypotheses that an increased level of loneliness experienced during coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) confinement was predictive of internalizing symptoms and that this pathway was mediated by emotion dysregulation levels.
Methods: To reach this aim, we performed an online longitudinal survey recruiting 1,330 participants at Time 1 (at the beginning of the lockdown) and 308 participants at Time 2 (few days before the end of the lockdown). All filled out a set of questionnaires: demographic data, University of California, Los Angeles Loneliness scale, Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale−18 items, and Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale−21 items. Hypotheses were tested using structural equation modeling in two steps and controlling for age. First, hypotheses were tested on cross-sectional data. Then, a cross-lagged panel analysis was performed on longitudinal data.
Results: Models obtained a good fit and evidenced the predictive role of loneliness levels on the three outcomes (i.e., depression, anxiety, and stress). Moreover, we found that emotion dysregulation levels partially mediated the longitudinal relationship between loneliness and both depression and stress but not between loneliness and anxiety levels.
Conclusions: This study points out that a central goal of clinical intervention could be the ability to regulate negative emotional states.
Introduction
The coronavirus disease 2019 (COVID-19) pandemic poses challenges never before faced in society from many angles. It affects not only the state of health and integrity of our body endangered by COVID-19 but also the health and integrity of our mind, and the effects on the mental health of COVID-19 are expected to be diverse (1).
During a pandemic, not only the virus, with the consequent fear of contagion and death, but also the lockdown measures imposed by the various states contribute as stress factors affecting people's well-being (2–10). Indeed, according to a survey conducted in the United Kingdom on pandemic concerns (1), the prospect of contracting the severe acute respiratory syndrome coronavirus 2 was judged to be less worrying compared with psychological and social responses to the situation.
Furthermore, the answers of governments to fight the spreading of the virus, remaining physically distanced from relatives and friends, have determined a common condition of social isolation. As already known from the literature, social isolation is considered a risk factor for diverse psychopathological manifestations, such as anxiety (5, 6, 11), depressed mood (6, 7, 12, 13), substance abuse, domestic violence, suicide, and self-harm (14–16) and a trigger for some threatening conditions such as loneliness. A rapid review conducted by Brooks et al. (17) provided an overview of psychological effects related to quarantine, which occurred in acute, going from general psychological distress to post-traumatic stress symptoms (8, 18). Some studies have investigated long-term effects of the isolation experience with follow-ups, finding that PTSD and depressive symptoms persisted in a part of the sample even after 3 years from the epidemic outburst (13, 19).
Indeed, a wide body of research suggests that social isolation (i.e., the objective condition of isolation), psychological stress, and loneliness (i.e., subjective condition of isolation) have an unfavorable effect on many health outcomes, including mortality (20, 21). For this reason, the increase in social isolation and loneliness needs to be considered among the most important probable negative consequence of COVID-19, as evidenced by the surveys conducted by Holmes et al. (1).
Loneliness and Emotion Regulation
Loneliness, defined as the pain of feeling alone (22), is a psychological condition characterized by a deep sense of emptiness and uselessness, lack of control, and personal threat (23, 24). Studies have shown that loneliness can lead to more serious physical and mental health problems such as internet addiction, suicide ideation, and substance use (25, 26). Loneliness also seems to be linked to internalizing symptoms such as depression in both adolescents (27–31) and adults (32–35), anxiety (36, 37), and—moreover—social anxiety (32, 38, 39).
However, research (40) highlights that diverse factors are involved in loneliness, such as individual (i.e., personality features) and contextual facets (i.e., social isolation). For instance, a meta-analysis study focused on adolescence found that the most powerful predictors of loneliness were individual characteristics such as low self-esteem and social anxiety (41). Instead, from a review that investigated the phenomenon in older adults, it came out that the psychological characteristics strongly linked to loneliness were cognitive deficits, poor mental health, negative life events, and low self-efficacy beliefs (42).
From an individual perspective, emotion regulation, defined as the complex set of psychic processes that translate into one's ability to influence his/her emotions, how he/she experiences them and how he/she expresses them (43), has a decisive role in promoting environmental adaptation and the following well-being (44). Literature has also highlighted the role of difficulties in emotion regulation as a risk factor for behavioral (45–47) and emotional problems such as substance abuse, gambling disorder, anxiety, and depression (48–51).
Nevertheless, individuals with the same level of loneliness may not experience similar psychological outcomes (52). This suggests that there might be mechanisms underlying the relationship between loneliness and internalizing symptoms, one of which seems to be emotion regulation difficulties. In fact, it has been shown that lonelier individuals make less use of adaptive emotional regulation strategies compared with individuals who suffer less from loneliness (53).
In light of these pieces of evidence, the main purpose of this study was to examine—through a structural equation model—the relationship between loneliness and internalizing symptoms, considering the mediating effect of difficulties in emotion regulation and the longitudinal link between loneliness and internalizing symptoms.
Methods
Participants and Procedure
For the purposes of the study, an online survey was created and diffused online 3 days after the beginning of the confinement in XXX. At the beginning of the survey, an exhaustive presentation of the study's aims and scopes was delivered and information concerning anonymity and privacy. Then, the participant was asked to sign an informed consent. Three days before the end of the national lockdown, participants were sent an email asking them to fulfill the same battery of self-report questionnaires. The procedure of the study applied with the American Psychological Association official guidelines and was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of XXX, XXXX (N. 356/20).
Measures
Demographic Information
For the purpose of the study, an initial questionnaire was created to evaluate information such as age, sex, and socioeconomic status.
University of California, Los Angeles Loneliness Scale
University of California, Los Angeles (UCLA) Loneliness Scale [(54), Italian version by (55) was used to measure the perceived level of loneliness. The instrument is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 20 items on a four-point Likert-type scale. Empirical literature evidenced the existence of three factors underlying the structure of the instrument, namely Intimate Others (intimate and interpersonal loneliness), Social Others (lack of social networks or social friendships), and Affiliative Environment (a lack of belonging to the affiliative environment). In the present study, the instrument showed good internal consistency with a Cronbach alpha coefficient of 0.82.
Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale 18 Items
Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale 18 items (56) is the short version of the Difficulties in Emotion Regulation Scale (DERS) (57, 58). As DERS, the instrument asks the participant to answer on a five-point Likert-type scale. The scale measures emotion dysregulation levels providing a total score and six separated scores related to the subscales of the instrument being Non-Acceptance (difficulty to accept in a non-judgmental way one's negative emotional states), Awareness (lack of awareness of one's negative emotions), Clarity (difficultly to discriminate between different negative emotional states), Strategies (perception of a lack of available emotion regulation strategies), Goals (difficulty to pursue goal-directed behavior when experiencing negative emotional states), and Impulse (tendency to act rashly when experiencing a negative emotion). In our study, DERS showed good internal consistency with a Cronbach alpha coefficient reaching 0.90.
Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale 21 Items
Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale 21 items (59, 60) is a self-report questionnaire consisting of 21 items on a four-point Likert-type scale. The instrument provides scores for three subscales evaluating levels of depression, anxiety, and stress. In our study, each of these subscales evidenced a good internal consistency with Cronbach's alpha coefficients being 0.91 (Stress), 0.88 (Depression), and 0.84 (Anxiety).
Statistical Analyses
The statistical analysis plan consisted of several steps. First, descriptive analyses were performed calculating Cronbach's alphas, frequencies, means, and standard deviations of the main variables of the study. Then, the normality of the main variables involved in the study at Time 1 was checked throughout the computation of skewness and kurtosis (61). Normality indexes all fall in the acceptable range except for the Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale Anxiety variable, which showed a normality index slightly upon the acceptable cutoff (Kurtosis = 3.04). Also, bivariate r-Pearson correlations were calculated to examine zero-order correlations between continuous variables involved in the study. These analyses were performed with SPSS v.24 for Windows. Then, to test the hypotheses of the study, a structural equation model was designed and tested using the lavaan package of the R software for Mac. In the models, we inserted age, sex, and education level as control variables on levels of depression, anxiety, and stress measured at Time 2. Also, we added the estimation of the covariances between the levels of depression, anxiety, and stress at Time 1 on the one hand and age, sex, and education level on the other hand. Regarding sex and education variables, they were treated as a dummy variable with 0 being females and 1 being male for sex variable and 0 being having a high school diploma or lower educational level and 1 having a higher educational level for the education variable. The method used evaluates the consistency of a dataset with a model previously defined throughout the robust maximum likelihood method of estimation. Results brought by these statistical analyses are typically judged using several goodness of fit indexes such as root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA), Tucker–Lewis coefficient (TLI), and comparative fit index (CFI). A 0.05 < RMSEA >0.08 (62) and both TLI and CFI being >0.90 (63) are generally interpreted as an adequate fit. In addition, we examined the lower and upper boundaries of the 90% confidence interval for RMSEA, with an upper boundary of more than 0.10, indicating that the model should be rejected (62).
Results
Descriptive Analyses
A total of 1,323 respondents answered the survey on Time 1. The age of participants ranged from 18 to 91 years (Mage = 35.38; SD = 14.08), and 23% of the sample were male. As for the characteristics of the residence, 49.2% of participants live in a big city, 33.8% in a medium-size city, and 17% in a small city or rural area. Of the participants, 52.3% achieved a high school diploma or a higher level of education. Also, almost half of the sample reported having an income of less than € 36,000 per year, and 26.5% of the participants referred to have one or more children. Because of a COVID-19 emergency, 52.6% reported having to interrupt their working activity. From this first sample, 308 of the respondents (Mage = 35.31; SD = 13.91; 22.7% males) participated in the second part of the study. Means and standard deviations were computed for the main variables involved in the study, both for the Total sample (Time 1) and the subsample (Time 2). These are displayed in Table 1.
Table 1. Means and Standard Deviations of the main variables involved in the study for the full sample (Time 1) and the subsample (Time 2).
Correlations Between Variables
r-Pearson correlations between all variables involved in the study at Time 1 were calculated. Results are fully displayed in Table 2.
Structural Equation Model at Time 1
To reach the aims of the study, we tested the goodness of fit of a Structural Equation Model on the whole sample participating at Time 1. Specifically, we tested, controlling for age, sex, and education levels, if UCLA scores predicted depression, anxiety, and stress levels both directly and indirectly throughout emotion dysregulation levels. To create depression, anxiety, and stress latent variables, we used the scores obtained on the items converging on the subscales of Depression Anxiety and Stress Scale-21. Loneliness Latent Variable was produced using the scores obtained on the three subscales (Intimate, Social, and Affiliative) of UCLA. Finally, the emotion dysregulation latent variable was formed using the scores obtained on the six subscales of DERS. Regarding covariates, we observed that age was significantly related to stress, education level with depression, and sex with both anxiety and stress levels.
As illustrated in Figure 1, the model obtained an acceptable fit as indicated by the CFI (0.91), TLI (0.90), and RMSEA indexes [0.06; confidence interval 90% (0.055–0.060)]. Moreover, all manifest variables loaded significantly on the respective latent variables. Also, we observed that UCLA scores predicted both, directly and indirectly, depression, anxiety, and stress levels.
Figure 1. Structural equation model testing the cross-sectional meditational effect of emotion dysregulation in the relationship linking loneliness with anxiety, depression, and stress. Solid lines indicate statistically significant paths. Dashed line indicate not statistically significant paths. ED, Emotion Dysregulation; I, Depression Anxiety Stress Scale item.
Cross Lagged Panel Model on Longitudinal Data
To test if emotion dysregulation levels actually mediated the longitudinal link between loneliness and internalizing outcomes (depression, anxiety, and stress), we test a cross-lagged panel model throughout structural equation modeling on the subsample of longitudinal data. The tested paths and the hypothesized model are fully illustrated in Figure 2.
Figure 2. Representation of the full tested longitudinal model. Solid lines indicate statistically hypothesized significant paths. Dashed line indicate paths inserted in the statistical mode but not expected by hypotheses. DERS, Emotion Dysregulation; T1, Scores obtained at Time 1; T2, Scores obtained at Time 2.
The first model test obtained an adequate fit according to the RMSEA index [0.052; CI 90% (0.049–0.056)] but was slightly below the acceptable CFI cutoff (0.88). The full model is illustrated in Figure 2. Consequently, we consulted the modification indexes and freed some parameters, further estimating some covariance between residual errors of some items involved in the measurement model. To obtain an adequate fit, nine parameters had to been added to the model. All of the additional covariances estimated were between manifest variables converging toward the same latent variable, maintaining coherence with our conceptual model.
The final model [RMSEA = 0.50; CI 90% (0.046–0.053); CFI = 0.90] is illustrated in Figure 3. Directions and statistical significances of coefficients did not differ from the first model tested. For clarity purposes, we did not display coefficients between manifest and latent variables, but all of them were positive and significant. Moreover, we found that emotion dysregulation levels partially mediated the longitudinal relationship between loneliness and both depression (β = 0.006; p = 0.043) and stress (β = 0.007; p = 0.017) but not between loneliness and anxiety levels (β = 0.002; p = 0.061).
Figure 3. First structural equation model testing the longitudinal meditational effect of emotion dysregulation in the relationship linking loneliness with anxiety, depression, and stress. Solid lines indicate statistically significant paths. Dashed line indicate not statistically significant paths. DERS, Emotion Dysregulation; T1, Scores obtained at Time 1; T2, Scores obtained at Time 2; a*b = 0.01 (p = 0.043); a*c < 0.01 (p = 0.061); a*d = 0.01 (p = 0.017).
Discussion
It is widely stated that in times of crisis, psychological research should address and provide answers on risk and protective factors involved in the well-being and adjustment of individuals. Empirical and anecdotal contributions have highlighted that the emergency related to COVID-19 has been very powerful in physical and psychological impacts. In the wide range of sudden changes undergone by the world population, most of them have had to face restrictive measures related to their interpersonal interactions: in many countries, the population has been asked to maintain rigorous and lasting lockdown measures to contain the pandemic. One of the consequences for most people was a reduced frequency of social interactions, which was probably translated into an increase in the level of loneliness perceived for most of them. Our focus on loneliness is linked to results that suggest that one of the most direct consequences of lockdown measures is isolation and the associated feeling of loneliness (64). Because previous research has already highlighted the negative consequences of loneliness on mental health, particularly on the maintenance, worsening or even onset of internalizing symptoms, clinical psychologists have been asked to address this central issue during the COVID-19 emergency.
However, the available empirical evidence that can provide useful clinical indications for dealing with psychological problems caused by “isolation” is limited. Our study aimed to test the hypotheses that an increase in loneliness levels would predict an increase in internalizing symptom levels and that the ability to regulate emotions may mediate these relationships. Our focus on emotion regulation strategies as a further mediator of these associations has its roots in the widely coherent literature that attests to their association with psychopathology and, more specifically, with reactions to distressing events and situations (65, 66).
The first equation structural model was based on an evaluation of all the variables at Time 1 when 1,323 participants were at the beginning of the lockdown. In this model, a significant relationship has been observed between the dimensions of loneliness, emotional dysregulation, and internalizing symptoms. Specifically, loneliness dimensions directly predicted depression, anxiety, and stress levels. This datum is in line with existing literature concerning the association between loneliness and anxiety, depression and other psychological outcomes, and even suicide (25, 67). However, if the majority of past studies focused their attention on chronic loneliness (68), in this study, we can start a reflection on the similar impact of intense and constrictive isolation.
Furthermore, we found that loneliness predicts these results in terms of depression, anxiety, and stress levels also indirectly, mediated by emotion regulation strategies. In fact, solitude, in its aspects of the intimate, social, and affiliative components, has its most distressing effect if associated with difficulties in emotion regulation. Other studies have shown this strong association (69) and have investigated the role of emotion regulation strategies as a mediator of the sense loneliness perceived (70), whereas other authors have investigated loneliness itself as a mediator in the association between emotion regulation strategies and psychological outcomes (71), showing that loneliness mediates the association between the difficulties in regulating emotions and psychopathology.
The second equation structural model aimed at testing the role of time in a subsample of 308 participants concerning these associations. The results of this model only partially supported what was found in the cross-sectional analysis: the levels of emotion dysregulation mediated only the path that connects perceived loneliness and depression and stress but not anxiety. Regarding anxiety, we may speculate that other factors may explain the longitudinal relationship between loneliness and anxiety levels. For instance, the lack of social confrontation on the topic generating anxiety (such as news related to the pandemic) and the fear of getting sick in a situation of loneliness may better explain the pathways linking perceived loneliness to anxiety. An additional explanation of this result can be found in methodological considerations. Indeed, the important dropout of participants between Time 1 and Time 2 of the study has induced an important loss of statistical power, thus making not significant a precedent significant path. We may speculate so that a bigger sample size at Time 2 (almost equal to those used at Time 1) would have evidenced a significant influence of emotion dysregulation in the path between perceived loneliness and anxiety, in line with the high number of studies highlighting the role played by this variable in anxiety symptomatology.
Regarding the role of emotional dysregulation in the relationship between loneliness and stress levels, several considerations can be made. First, our results are in line with previous literature showing that adaptive emotion regulation strategies, such as reappraisal, may buffer the negative impacts of daily stress on positive emotions (72) and that emotion dysregulation and coping stress seem to share a common neurobiological basis (73) underlying the tight relationship between emotion regulation capacities and stress resilience. Moreover, medical literature documented the association between loneliness status and stress, operationalized throughout physiological variables, including indicators such as stress hormones (74). Indeed, researchers brought relevant pieces of evidence highlighting the central role of social support in resilience to stress (75). In the context of the pandemic and confinement, we can hypothesize that loneliness may have deprived individuals of basic emotion regulation strategies such as exercise (76) and, in turn, account for increased levels of excessive arousal (e.g., fatigue). Similarly, lack of perceived social support associated with loneliness would probably heighten the feeling of being more easily overwhelmed by external threats, such as health problems, job loss, or domestic violence (77, 78). Besides, loneliness may interrupt the buffering role of social interactions toward maladaptive rumination that would, in turn, lead to increased levels of stress (79). Thus, owning good emotion regulation capacities is likely to play a protective role in the pathways leading loneliness to stress, balancing the deprivation of interpersonal protective factors in relation to stress.
Regarding the mediating role of emotion dysregulation in the relationship between loneliness and depression, several explanations can be provided. For instance, a difficulty to accept negative emotions triggered by loneliness feelings in a non-judgmental way may lead to depressive symptoms, eliciting thoughts on the self-perceived inability to adequately face the situation. In this regard, adequate information provided by the institutions on the potential negative emotional states aroused by the situation can be of great use in legitimizing the onset of this emotion in the entire population. Furthermore, an alleged lack of self-efficacy in regulating emotions can be reasonably explained by our observations. This may be especially true of individuals with low self-efficacy in regulating emotions, who have perceived themselves as accustomed to relying on others to regulate their negative emotional states. Feeling lonely for individuals with a propensity for this emotional regulation deficit can be particularly inspiring. In this regard, a prevention program that aims to provide some tips for regulating negative emotions can help avoid a major increase in psychological distress in the general population. Furthermore, from this perspective of understanding the associations between vulnerability factors and psychological well-being, other interesting discoveries offer ideas for further research on these topics. For example, an interesting fact concerns the role of age in the mediation of these associations. In fact, we have found significant correlations between age and outcomes in terms of depression, anxiety, and stress. Young people show multiple levels of stress during this blocking period. It can be suggested that abandoning important aspects of their daily lives for young people could be more stressful.
Other studies (80, 81) have pointed out that the closure of schools, universities, and businesses has led to negative feelings and has greatly impacted the population in terms of mental health. We could suggest that the closure of the productive sectors mentioned and also of other important sectors, e.g., cultural and sports centers and other recreational activities, have a greater impact on young people who have greater difficulty in abandoning their previous lifestyle. However, this preliminary data need further sociodemographic insights to be better explained.
Despite the insightful nature of our findings, several limitations of the study need to be considered. First, despite longitudinal studies often suffer from relevant dropout in the participation of subjects, this issue is likely to introduce a bias in our study. Indeed, we are not aware of the reasons for dropping, and dropped participants may be more resilient or, conversely, more vulnerable to some of the mental health issues investigated in our study. This issue may limit the generalizability of our findings. Then, due to the growing research on COVID-19, we have chosen to focus attention on the general population, and we have not been able to define specific reactions between subsamples of more exposed and less exposed people. However, our idea is that it is equally important that research may focus on the most influential risk factors. In this line, as suggested by other authors (80), the possibility of capturing psychological responses to the pandemic represents itself a crucial element in identifying people who may need psychological intervention.
Conclusion
Overall, our results indicate the need to improve research in defining specific risk factors implicated in well-being and adjustment to this very demanding period. This could suggest implementing intervention programs aimed at improving well-being in population segments most at risk for their functioning and mental health.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Sapienza Ethics Board. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
PV and RT conceived the presented idea. SB, RC, and GR performed the analysis. All authors discussed the results and contributed to the final manuscript.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Acknowledgments
Authors would like to thank the individuals and the Soleterre Foundation who generously shared their time, experience, and materials for the purposes of this project.
References
1. Holmes EA, O'Connor RC, Perry VH, Tracey I, Wessely S, Arseneault L, et al. Multidisciplinary research priorities for the COVID-19 pandemic: a call for action for mental health science. Lancet Psychiatry. (2020) 7:e43. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(20)30168-1
2. Blendon RJ, Benson JM, DesRoches CM, Raleigh E, Taylor-Clark K. The public's response to severe acute respiratory syndrome in Toronto and the United States. Clin Infect Dis. (2004) 38:925–31. doi: 10.1086/382355
3. Braunack-Mayer A, Tooher R, Collins JE, Street JM, Marshall H. Understanding the school community's response to school closures during the H1N1 2009 influenza pandemic. BMC Public Health. (2013) 13:1–15. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-13-344
4. Cava MA, Fay KE, Beanlands HJ, McCay EA, Wignall R. The experience of quarantine for individuals affected by SARS in Toronto. Public Health Nursing. (2005) 22:398–406. doi: 10.1111/j.0737-1209.2005.220504.x
5. Desclaux A, Badji D, Ndione AG, Sow K. Accepted monitoring or endured quarantine? Ebola contacts' perceptions in Senegal. Soc Sci Med. (2017) 178:38–45. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2017.02.009
6. DiGiovanni C, Conley J, Chiu D, Zaborski J. Factors influencing compliance with quarantine in Toronto during the 2003 SARS outbreak. Biosecur Bioterror. (2004) 2:265–72. doi: 10.1089/bsp.2004.2.265
7. Hawryluck L, Gold WL, Robinson S, Pogorski S, Galea S, Styra R. SARS control and psychological effects of quarantine, Toronto, Canada. Emerg Infect Dis. (2004) 10:1206–12. doi: 10.3201/eid1007.030703
8. Reynolds DL, Garay JR, Deamond SL, Moran MK, Gold W, Styra R. Understanding, compliance and psychological impact of the SARS quarantine experience. Epidemiol Infect. (2008) 136:997–1007. doi: 10.1017/S0950268807009156
9. Robertson E, Hershenfield K, Grace SL, Stewart DE. The psychosocial effects of being quarantined following exposure to SARS: a qualitative study of Toronto health care workers. Can J Psychiatry. (2004) 49:403–7. doi: 10.1177/070674370404900612
10. Wilken JA, Pordell P, Goode B, Jarteh R, Miller Z, Saygar BG, et al. Knowledge, attitudes, and practices among members of households actively monitored or quarantined to prevent transmission of ebola virus disease—Margibi County, Liberia: February-March 2015. Prehosp Disast Med. (2017) 32:673–8. doi: 10.1017/S1049023X17006720
11. Bai Y, Lin CC, Lin CY, Chen JY, Chue CM, Chou P. Survey of stress reactions among health care workers involved with the SARS outbreak. Psychiatr Serv. (2004) 55:1055–57. doi: 10.1176/appi.ps.55.9.1055
12. Lee S, Chan LY, Chau AM, Kwok KP, Kleinman A. The experience of SARS-related stigma at amoy gardens. Soc. Sci. Med. (2005) 61:2038–46. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2005.04.010
13. Liu X, Kakade M, Fuller CJ, Fan B, Fang Y, Kong J, et al. Depression after exposure to stressful events: lessons learned from the severe acute respiratory syndrome epidemic. Comp Psychiatry. (2012) 53:15–23. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2011.02.003
14. O'Connor RC, Nock MK. The psychology of suicidal behaviour. Lancet Psychiatry . (2014) 1:73–85. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70222-6
15. John A, Glendenning AC, Marchant A, Montgomery P, Stewart A, Wood S, et al. Self-harm, suicidal behaviours, cyberbullying in children young people: systematic review. J Med Intern Res. (2018) 20:e129. doi: 10.2196/jmir.9044
16. Turecki G, Brent DA, Gunnell D, O'Connor RC, Oquendo MA, Pirkis J, et al. Suicide and suicide risk. Nat Rev Dis Primers. (2019) 5:1–22. doi: 10.1038/s41572-019-0121-0
17. Brooks SK, Webster RK, Smith LE, Woodland L, Wessely S, Greenberg N, et al. The psychological impact of quarantine and how to reduce it: rapid review of the evidence. Lancet. (2020) 395:912–20. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30460-8
18. Sprang G, Silman M. Posttraumatic stress disorder in parents and youth after health-related disasters. Disast Med Public Health Prepared. (2013) 7:105–110. doi: 10.1017/dmp.2013.22
19. Wu P, Fang Y, Guan Z, Fan B, Kong J, Yao Z, et al. The psychological impact of the SARS epidemic on hospital employees in China: exposure, risk perception, and altruistic acceptance of risk. Can J Psychiatry. (2009) 54:302–11. doi: 10.1177/070674370905400504
20. Fakoya OA, McCorry NK, Donnelly M. Loneliness and social isolation interventions for older adults: a scoping review of reviews. BMC Public Health. (2020) 20:129. doi: 10.1186/s12889-020-8251-6
21. Porcelli P. Fear, anxiety and health-related consequences after the COVID-19 epidemic. Clin Neuropsychiatry. (2020) 17:103–11. doi: 10.36131/CN20200215
23. Weiss RS. Loneliness: The Experience of Emotional and Social Isolation. Cambridge MA: The MIT Press (1973).
24. Cacioppo JT, Patrick W. Loneliness: Human Nature and the Need for Social Connection. New York, NY: WW Norton & Company (2008).
25. Cacioppo JT, Hughes ME, Waite LJ, Hawkley LC, Thisted RA. Loneliness as a specific risk factor for depressive symptoms: cross-sectional and longitudinal analyses. Psychol Aging. (2006) 21:140–51. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.21.1.140
26. Marroquín B. Interpersonal emotion regulation as a mechanism of social support in depression. Clin Psychol Rev. (2011) 31:1276–90. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2011.09.005
27. Koenig LJ, Abrams RF. Adolescent loneliness and adjustment: a focus on gender differences. In: Rotenberg KJ, Hymel S. editors. Loneliness in Childhood and Adolescence. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (1999) pp. 296–324. doi: 10.1017/CBO9780511551888.015
28. Koenig LJ, Isaacs AM, Schwartz JA. Sex differences in adolescent depression and loneliness: why are boys lonelier if girls are more depressed?. J Res Pers. (1994) 28:27–43. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.1994.1004
29. Lau S, Chan DW, Lau PS. Facets of loneliness and depression among Chinese children and adolescents. J Soc Psychol. (1999) 139:713–29. doi: 10.1080/00224549909598251
30. Mahon NE, Yarcheski A. Outcomes of depression in early adolescents. Western J Nurs Res. (2001) 23:360–75. doi: 10.1177/01939450122045212
31. Qualter P, Brown SL, Munn P, Rotenberg KJ. Childhood loneliness as a predictor of adolescent depressive symptoms: an 8-year longitudinal study. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2010) 19:493–501. doi: 10.1007/s00787-009-0059-y
32. Anderson CA, Harvey RJ. Brief report: discriminating between problems in living: an examination of measures of depression, loneliness, shyness, and social anxiety. J Soc Clin Psychol. (1988) 6:482–91. doi: 10.1521/jscp.1988.6.3-4.482
33. Barg FK, Huss-Ashmore R, Wittink MN, Murray GF, Bogner HR, Gallo JJ. A mixed-methods approach to understanding loneliness and depression in older adults. J Gerontol Ser B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. (2006) 61:S329–39. doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.6.S329
34. Jackson J, Cochran SD. Loneliness and psychological distress. J Psychol. (1991) 125:257–62. doi: 10.1080/00223980.1991.10543289
35. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Ahrens C. Age differences and similarities in the correlates of depressive symptoms. Psychol Aging, (2002) 17:116–24. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.116
36. Sündermann O, Onwumere J, Kane F, Morgan C, Kuipers E. Social networks and support in first-episode psychosis: exploring the role of loneliness and anxiety. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. (2014) 49:359–66. doi: 10.1007/s00127-013-0754-3
37. Mijuskovic B. Loneliness, anxiety, hostility, and communication. Child Study J. (1986) 6:227–40.
38. Caplan SE. Relations among loneliness, social anxiety, and problematic internet use. CyberPsychol Behav. (2006) 10:234–42. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2006.9963
39. Moore D, Schultz NR. Loneliness at adolescence: correlates, attributions, and coping. J Youth Adolesc. (1983) 12:95–100. doi: 10.1007/BF02088307
40. Vanhalst J, Luyckx K, Goossens L. Experiencing loneliness in adolescence: a matter of individual characteristics, negative peer experiences, or both? Soc Dev. (2014) 23:100–18. doi: 10.1111/sode.12019
41. Mahon NE, Yarcheski A, Yarcheski TJ, Cannella BL, Hanks MM. A meta-analytic study of predictors for loneliness during adolescence. Nursing Res. (2006) 55:308–15. doi: 10.1097/00006199-200609000-00003
42. Cohen-Mansfield J, Hazan H, Lerman Y, Shalom V. Correlates and predictors of loneliness in older-adults: a review of quantitative results informed by qualitative insights. Int Psychogeriatr. (2016) 28:557–76. doi: 10.1017/S1041610215001532
43. Gross JJ. Antecedent-and response-focused emotion regulation: divergent consequences for experience, expression, and physiology. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1998) 74:224–37. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.224
44. Balzarotti S, Biassoni F, Villani D, Prunas A, Velotti P. Individual differences in cognitive emotion regulation: implications for subjective and psychological well-being, J Happiness Stud. (2016) 17:125–43. doi: 10.1007/s10902-014-9587-3
45. Velotti P, Garofalo C, Callea A, Bucks R, Roberton T, Daffern M. Exploring anger among offenders: the role of emotion dysregulation and alexithymia. Psychiatry Psychol Law. (2017) 28:128–38. doi: 10.1080/13218719.2016.1164639
46. Garofalo C, Velotti P, Callea A, Popolo R, Salvatore G, Cavallo F, et al. Emotion dysregulation, impulsivity and personality disorder traits: a community sample study. Psychiatry Res. (2018) 266:186–92. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.05.067
47. Velotti P, Zobel BS, Rogier G, Tambelli R. Exploring relationships: a systematic review on intimate partner violence and attachment. Front Psychol. (2018). 9:1166. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01166
48. Aldao A, Nolen-Hoeksema S, Schweizer S. Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clin Psychol Rev. (2010) 30:217–37. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004
49. Mathews BL, Kerns KA, Ciesla JA. Specificity of emotion regulation difficulties related to anxiety in early adolescence. J Adolesc. (2014) 37:1089–97. doi: 10.1016/j.adolescence.2014.08.002
50. Rogier G, Velotti P. Conceptualising gambling disorder with the process model of emotion regulation, J Behav Addic. (2018) 7:239–51. doi: 10.1556/2006.7.2018.52
51. Rogier G, Velotti P. Narcissistic implications in gambling disorder: the mediating role of emotion dysregulation. J Gambl Stud. (2018) 34:1241–60. doi: 10.1007/s10899-018-9759-x
52. Marroquín B, Czamanski-Cohen J, Weihs KL, Stanton AL. Implicit loneliness, emotion regulation, and depressive symptoms in breast cancer survivors. J Behav Med. (2016) 39:832–44. doi: 10.1007/s10865-016-9751-9
53. Marroquín B, Nolen-Hoeksema S. Emotion regulation and depressive symptoms: close relationships as social context and influence. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2015) 109:836–55. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000034
54. Russell D, Peplau LA, Ferguson ML. Developing a measure of loneliness. J Pers Assess. (1978) 42:290–4. doi: 10.1207/s15327752jpa4203_11
55. Boffo M, Mannarini S, Munari C. Exploratory structure equation modeling of the UCLA loneliness scale: a contribution to the italian adaptation. Testing Psychometr Methodol Appl Psychol. (2012) 19:345–63.
56. Victor SE, Klonsky ED. Validation of a brief version of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale (DERS-18) in five samples. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2016) 38:582–9. doi: 10.1007/s10862-016-9547-9
57. Gratz KL, Roemer L. Multidimensional assessment of emotion regulation and dysregulation: development, factor structure, and initial validation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale. J Psychopathol Behav Assess. (2004) 26:41–54. doi: 10.1023/B:JOBA.0000007455.08539.94
58. Giromini L, Velotti P, de Campora G, Bonalume L, Zavattini GC. Cultural adaptation of the difficulties in emotion regulation scale: reliability and validity of an Italian version. J Clin Psychol. (2012) 68:989–1007. doi: 10.1002/jclp.21876
59. Lovibond SH, Lovibond PF. Manual for the Depression Anxiety Stress Scales. Sydney, NSW: Psychology Foundation. (1995). doi: 10.1037/t01004-000
60. Bottesi G, Ghisi M, Altoe G, Conforti E, Melli G, Sica C. The Italian version of the depression anxiety stress scales-21: factor structure and psychometric properties on community and clinical samples. Compr Psychiatry. (2015) 60:170–81. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2015.04.005
61. Kim H.-Y. Statistical notes for clinical researchers: assessing normal distribution using skewness and kurtosis. Restor Dentist Endodontics. (2013) 38:52–4. doi: 10.5395/rde.2013.38.1.52
62. Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative ways of assessing model fit. Sociol Methods Res. (1992) 21:230–58. doi: 10.1177/0049124192021002005
63. Kline RB. Promise and pitfalls of structural equation modeling in gifted research. In: Thompson B, Subotnik RF, editors. Methodologies for Conducting Research on Giftedness. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. (2010) pp. 147–169. doi: 10.1037/12079-007
64. McBride O, Murphy J, Shevlin M, Gibson Miller J, Hartman TK, Hyland P, et al. Monitoring the Psychological Impact of the COVID-19 Pandemic in the General Population: An Overview of The Context, Design and Conduct of the COVID-19 Psychological Research Consortium (C19PRC) Study. London (2020). Retrieved from. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/wxe2n
65. Dimaggio G, Popolo R, Montano A, Velotti P, Perrini F, Buonocore L, et al. Emotion dysregulation, symptoms and interpersonal problems as independent predictors of a broad range of personality disorders in an outpatient sample. Psychol Psychother. (2017) 90:569–99. doi: 10.1111/papt.12126
66. Gillespie S, Garofalo C, Velotti P. Emotion regulation, mindfulness, and alexithymia: specific or general impairments in sexual, violent, and homicide offenders? J Criminal Justice. (2018) 58:56–66. doi: 10.1016/j.jcrimjus.2018.07.006
67. Stravynski A, Boyer R. Loneliness in relation to suicide ideation and parasuicide: a population-wide study. Suicide Life Threat Behav. (2001) 31:32–40. doi: 10.1521/suli.31.1.32.21312
68. Shi R, Zhang S, Zhang Q, Fu S, Wang Z. Experiential avoidance mediates the association between emotion regulation abilities and loneliness. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0168536. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0168536
69. Kearns SM, Creaven AM. Individual differences in positive and negative emotion regulation: which strategies explain variability in loneliness? Pers Mental Health. (2016) 11:64–74. doi: 10.1002/pmh.1363
70. Koole SL. The psychology of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Cogn Emot. (2009) 23:4–41. doi: 10.1080/02699930802619031
71. Southward MW, Christensen KA, Fettich KC, Weissman J, Berona J, Chen EY. Loneliness mediates the relationship between emotion dysregulation and bulimia nervosa/binge eating disorder psychopathology in a clinical sample. Eat Weight Disord. (2014) 19:509–13. doi: 10.1007/s40519-013-0083-2
72. Richardson CME. Emotion regulation in the context of daily stress: impact on daily affect. Pers Ind Diff. (2017) 112:150–6. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2017.02.058
73. Wang M, Saudino KJ. Emotion regulation and stress. J Adult Dev. (2011) 18:95. doi: 10.1007/s10804-010-9114-7
74. Campagne DM. Stress and perceived social isolation (loneliness). Arch Gerontol Geriatr. (2019) 82:192–9. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2019.02.007
75. Ozbay F, Johnson DC, Dimoulas E, Morgan CA, Charney D, Southwick S. Social support and resilience to stress: from neurobiology to clinical practice. Psychiatry. (2007) 4:35–40.
76. Hawkley LC, Thisted RA, Cacioppo JT. Loneliness predicts reduced physical activity: cross-sectional & longitudinal analyses. Health Psychol. (2009) 28:354–63. doi: 10.1037/a0014400
77. Mazza M, Marano G, Antonazzo B, Cavarretta E, Di Nicola M, Janiri L, et al. What about heart and mind in the covid-19 era? Minerva Cardioangiol. (2020). doi: 10.23736/S0026-4725.20.05309-8. [Epub ahead of print].
78. Mazza M, Marano G, Lai C, Janiri L, Sani G. Danger in danger: interpersonal violence during COVID-19 quarantine. Psychiatry Res. (2020) 289:113046. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2020.113046
79. Puterman E, Delongis A, Pomaki G. Protecting us from ourselves: social support as a buffer of trait and state rumination. J Soc Clin Psychol. (2007) 29:797–820. doi: 10.1521/jscp.2010.29.7.797
80. Chew QH, Wei KC, Vasoo S, Chua HC, Sim K. Narrative synthesis of psychological and coping responses towards emerging infectious disease outbreaks in the general population: practical considerations for the COVID-19 pandemic. Singapore Med J. (2020) 61:350–6. doi: 10.11622/smedj.2020046
81. Wang C, Pan R, Wan X, Tan Y, Xu L, Ho CS, et al. Immediate psychological responses and associated factors during the initial stage of the 2019 coronavirus disease (COVID-19) epidemic among the general population in China. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2020) 17:1729. doi: 10.3390/ijerph17051729
Keywords: loneliness, emotion dysregulation, depression, stress, anxiety
Citation: Velotti P, Rogier G, Beomonte Zobel S, Castellano R and Tambelli R (2021) Loneliness, Emotion Dysregulation, and Internalizing Symptoms During Coronavirus Disease 2019: A Structural Equation Modeling Approach. Front. Psychiatry 11:581494. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.581494
Received: 08 July 2020; Accepted: 30 November 2020;
Published: 08 January 2021.
Edited by:
Paola Aceto, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, ItalyReviewed by:
Maria Picó-Pérez, University of Minho, PortugalMarianna Mazza, Catholic University of the Sacred Heart, Italy
Copyright © 2021 Velotti, Rogier, Beomonte Zobel, Castellano and Tambelli. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: Patrizia Velotti, cGF0cml6aWEudmVsb3R0aSYjeDAwMDQwO3VuaXJvbWExLml0