Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 21 January 2021
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry
This article is part of the Research Topic Functional Near-infrared Diffuse Optical Spectroscopy (fNIRS) to Explore Mental Health View all 13 articles

Use of Virtual Reality Working Memory Task and Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy to Assess Brain Hemodynamic Responses to Methylphenidate in ADHD Children

  • 1Institute of Behavioral Science in Medicine, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2OBELAB Inc, Seoul, South Korea
  • 3Department of Psychiatry, Yonsei University Gangman Severance Hospital, Seoul, South Korea
  • 4Department of Neurology, Yonsei University College of Medicine, Seoul, South Korea
  • 5College of Medicine, Hallym University, Chuncheon, South Korea
  • 6Department of Psychiatry, National Health Insurance Service Ilsan Hospital, Goyang, South Korea

Virtual reality (VR) neuropsychological tests have emerged as a method to explore drug effects in real-life contexts in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) children. Functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) is a useful tool to measure brain activity during VR tasks in ADHD children with motor restlessness. The present study aimed to explore the acute effects of methylphenidate (MPH) on behavioral performance and brain activity during a VR-based working memory task simulating real-life classroom settings in ADHD children. In total, 23 children with ADHD performed a VR n-back task before and 2 h after MPH administration concurrent with measurements of oxygenated hemoglobin signal changes with fNIRS. Altogether, 12 healthy control (HC) subjects participated in the same task but did not receive MPH treatment. Reaction time (RT) was shortened after MPH treatment in the 1-back condition, but changes in brain activation were not observed. In the 2-back condition, activation of the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) and bilateral medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) was decreased alongside behavioral changes such as shorter RT, lower RT variability, and higher accuracy after MPH administration. Bilateral mPFC activation in the 2-back condition inversely correlated with task accuracy in the pre-MPH condition; this inverse correlation was not observed after MPH administration. In ADHD children, deactivation of the default mode network mediated by mPFC reduced during high working memory load, which was restored through MPH treatment. Our results suggest that the combination of VR classroom tasks and fNIRS examination makes it easy to assess drug effects on brain activity in ADHD children in settings simulating real-life.

Introduction

Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) is one of the most common neurodevelopmental disorders in children. ADHD is defined by age-inappropriate symptoms of inattention, impulsivity, and hyperactivity. Patients with ADHD exhibit complex multisystem impairments in fronto-cingulo-striato-thalamic and fronto-parieto-cerebellar networks that mediate attention, inhibition, working memory, and timing (1). In addition to higher-order cognitive functions, abnormalities in sensorimotor processing and the default mode network have been identified in ADHD (1). ADHD affects approximately 5% of school-aged children and often persists into adolescence and adulthood (2). Consequently, patients with ADHD often experience impaired academic and social function, which increases the risk of other comorbidities including antisocial behavior, oppositional defiant disorder, and substance abuse (3). Hence, early diagnosis of ADHD and appropriate intervention are critical.

To date, administration of psychostimulant drugs, especially methylphenidate (MPH), has been a treatment of choice for ADHD children (4). However, in up to 30% of patients, MPH is ineffective or can be difficult to administer due to side effects such as insomnia, appetite loss, headaches, and irritability (5). Therefore, studies have been conducted to identify objective functional biomarkers by exploring the neural correlates of MPH effects (1). In these studies, various neuropsychological tests have been used to measure cognitive functions of interest in ADHD subjects. However, the utility of such computer-based tests for evaluating real-world performance is limited, because performance in a laboratory research setting may lack real-world translation. Indeed, traditional computerized neuropsychological tests are criticized for their lack of ecological validity. Similarly, there has been limited progress in research on how psychostimulants operate in real-life environments.

To overcome these limitations, the utility of virtual reality (VR) has been recognized in terms of diagnosis and treatment of ADHD. VR facilitates the creation of dynamic, immersive environments with three-dimensional stimuli in which attention can be tested in an environment comparable to that experienced in the real world, improving ecological validity (6). In the field of assessment, numerous studies have confirmed that continuous performance tests embedded in VR (VR-CPT) are as sensitive and accurate as traditional CPT (79). In addition, the efficacy of MPH has been verified through VR-CPT (10) and the effectiveness of VR rehabilitation programs, such as VR neurofeedback or cognitive training, have been demonstrated (6, 11). In a VR environment, training motivation is fostered by providing real-life contexts that connect cognitive training to goals of everyday life.

However, few studies have evaluated brain activity during VR tasks in ADHD patients. One of the reasons for the paucity of research on brain activity during VR tasks is due to difficulties in examination. It is physically challenging to perform functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) or single-photon emission computed tomography (SPECT) scanning while using VR devices, especially for young ADHD patients with motor restlessness. Moreover, research on drug effects requires repeated pre- and post-administration imaging, which necessitates employment of a simple and easy tool to measure brain function. In this context, research using functional near-infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) has been growing. fNIRS is a non-invasive imaging technique that uses near-infrared light to measure functional brain activity through changes in concentration of oxygenated hemoglobin (HbO) and deoxygenated hemoglobin (Hb). The main advantage of fNIRS over other functional neuroimaging modalities such as fMRI, SPECT, or magnetoencephalography (MEG) is its portability (12). The fMRI, MEG, and SPECT involve the use of large-sized equipment and the patient need to be in the supine position (13). In contrast, fNIRS facilitates the investigation of brain activation in ecologically valid settings as well as repetitive measurements with low-cost, safe, transportable instrumentation in the natural position (14). Furthermore, it is less sensitive to movement artifacts and shows greater spatial resolution than EEG. However, fNIRS can provide information on only cortical activity (13). Its inability to provide information on subcortical levels and cortical-subcortical connectivity can limit its use in studies on psychiatric disorders.

Nevertheless, over the last few years, fNIRS has been used extensively to investigate cortical alterations in patients with various psychiatric disorders, such as schizophrenia; mood, anxiety, and eating disorders; and substance use disorder (13). For example, using fNIRS, Kawakubo et al. (2009) found bilateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) hypoactivation during the verbal fluency task in patients on the autism spectrum disorder (15). In addition, it has been used to measure the therapeutic effect and efficacy of treatment and to identify life-time brain function development in patients with neurodevelopmental disorders such as ADHD to neurodegenerative disorders (13). Considering these previous studies and the advantages mentioned above, fNIRS is seen as a particularly suitable tool for evaluating participants who experience unavoidable movements, such as ADHD patients in clinical settings (16).

Various studies using fNIRS have been conducted in ADHD patients. In most studies using tasks for evaluating attention or inhibition, ADHD patients exhibited lower prefrontal activity than that of healthy control (HC) subjects during the task (1720) although the results of studies using working memory tasks have been equivocal (21, 22). Furthermore, many studies have investigated the hemodynamic responses of MPH treatments using fNIRS, demonstrating that reduced right inferior frontal gyrus (IFG) and middle frontal gyrus (MFG) activation in ADHD patients compared to HCs was normalized after single-dose MPH administration (23, 24). These results indicate that the neural correlates of MPH effects can be verified with fNIRS.

However, there has been limited research investigating the effects of medication on brain activity using fNIRS in VR tasks which simulates real-life classroom settings. As cognitive skills are ultimately applied in the classroom setting, especially in school aged children, attention and executive function can be better encapsulated in the virtual classroom environment than using traditional computerized neuropsychological tests. Furthermore, previous studies used the Go/No-go and oddball tasks mainly to explore inhibitory function. However, inhibition alone is insufficient to explain the pathophysiological mechanisms of ADHD. Working memory (WM) impairment is considered a core deficit of ADHD associated with prefrontal dysfunction.

WM is defined as the ability to maintain information in an easily accessible state for a short period of time. According to the state-based model, the working memory contents are determined by perceptual & long-term memory representations being in a particular state of accessibility, maintained by neural activities controlled by the attentional processes (Supplementary Figure 1) (25). Information maintenance, an important element of working memory, is regulated by well-operated attentional processes, established by persistent neural activities in the relevant working areas (26). WM plays an important role in rapid processing and attention, and in turn, a large attention span and fast processing speed promote WM (27, 28). Rather than being determined by single brain region, WM appears to depend on good synchronization with the PFC and other brain areas, for example, the parietal cortex (27). In this process, the PFC is responsible for covering task-relevant information and for organizing fronto-parietal activity for sustained attention (29). For efficient activation of the WM network during task execution, the default mode network (DMN), which is activated in baseline cognitive state, performs deactivation coupling (3032). The DMN is a large-scale brain network that consists of the core region of the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and the postural cingulate/precuneus, along with inferior parietal lobule, lateral temporal cortex, and hippocampus (33). The DMN is largely related to mind wandering, which has known to affect performance of classroom of ADHD patients (34, 35). WM also plays an important role in maintaining focused behavior and improving classroom performance (36).

The N-back task is a well-known working memory paradigm which has been used extensively in functional neuroimaging studies on ADHD (37). A few studies investigated cortical brain activation using fNIRS during the n-back task. Herff et al. (2014) have found that different n-back conditions can be distinguished throughout fNIRS with high accuracy by the changes in hemodynamic response depending on the mental workload (38). Other studies using fNIRS also identified lower prefrontal complexity in patients with ADHD compared to in healthy control during the n-back task (39). However, few studies have explored the effects of MPH in relation to working memory using fNIRS in children with ADHD.

Based on these findings, we aimed to investigate the acute effects of MPH on behavioral performance and brain activity of children with ADHD during a VR n-back task in a virtual classroom setting. To the best of our knowledge, this is the first study using a VR-based working memory task and fNIRS to explore the effects of MPH in ADHD children.

Methods

Subjects

Altogether, 23 right-handed Korean children with ADHD (age range, 7–16 years; mean age, 9.96 ± 2.82) and 12 healthy control (HC) children (age range, 7–14 years; mean age, 11.33 ± 2.93) participated in this study. The number of participants required for adequate statistical power was based on previous studies that investigated drug effects in ADHD patients through fNIRS (23, 4042) and a previous study that described the optimal design for functional brain imaging (43). The detailed demographic and clinical characteristics of subjects are listed in Table 1. ADHD patients were recruited by posting a notice on the outpatient clinic of Gangnam Severance Hospital, and the HC were recruited by posting announcements to the local internet community. All participants were interviewed by a psychiatrist to confirm the ADHD diagnosis according to the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th edition (DSM-5) (44). The Mini-International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID 6.0) was administered to all participants by certified psychologists (45). Exclusion criteria for participants with ADHD were clinically significant medical or neurological disorders, developmental disabilities including autism spectrum disorder, intellectual disabilities, speech impairments, severe learning disabilities, schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, substance and/or alcohol use disorders, IQ <70, or illiteracy to read consent. Participants in the HC group were excluded if they had current or past history of mental illness, clinically significant medical disease or neurological deficits, IQ <70, and/or illiteracy to read consent. IQ was measured with the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for children (WISC-IV).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics by group, comprising attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) vs. healthy control (HC) subjects.

Several measurements were conducted to assess the psychological state of participants. The ADHD Rating Scales (ADHD-RS), an 18-question parent rating scale, was used to identify the presence of ADHD in children. Behavioral problems were assessed with the Korean version of the Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL), an 118-item parent-rated scale which queries behavioral problems in the past 6 months (46); Children's Depression Inventory (CDI) (47), a 27-item self-report scale to assess depressive symptoms (48); and State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children (STAI-C), a 20-item self-report scale were used to measure anxiety symptoms (49, 50). Simulator Sickness Questionnaire (SSQ), a 16-item self-report questionnaire was used to assess participants' subjective discomfort (disorientation, oculomotor symptoms, and nausea) after exposure to VR programs to measure simulator sickness due to discrepancies between vision and motion after VR use (51).

The protocol used for this study was approved by the Institutional Review Board of Yonsei University College of Medicine Gangnam Severance Hospital. Written informed consent and assent were obtained from all participants and one of their parents.

Procedure

We investigated the effects of ADHD medication in a controlled pre- and post-MPH study design, whereby participants performed n-back tasks with the 1-back and 2-back conditions. The experimental protocol is summarized in Figure 1A. Participants underwent a medication washout period of 2 days before the examination. On the examination day, participants were first assessed for demographic, clinical characteristics, and IQ. Participants subsequently performed a practice session to familiarize themselves with the VR environment and tasks. Participants then underwent two test sessions, one before dosing and the other 2 h after dosing. After the first session, MPH (Concerta, Metadate or Medikinet) was administered orally. Experimental doses were the same as the participants' regular dose. Each test session was conducted in the order of the introduction session followed by an n-back task. The HC performed only one test session after the practice session.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Experimental design and virtual reality environment. (A) A schematic representation of the flow of pre- and post-MPH n-back task sessions. (B) Screenshots of the virtual classroom during introduction of the n-back task. (C) The virtual reality theater with the curved screen. MPH, methylphenidate.

Virtual Reality Environment

The background of this VR task was a virtual classroom. The participants began by finding themselves in a typical classroom in a Korean school with desks facing ahead and blackboards in front of them. The participants' point of view was of the first person, facing the teacher, with other avatar classmates seated nearby (Figure 1B). The message “Please feel free to look around before class” offered the participants 15 s to adjust to the VR environment. A teacher avatar entered and informed participants of the rules of the N-back task, introduced as a game called “ABC Dungeon.” The participants first carried out a practice task, called “game tutorial” comprising 10 stimuli for each condition. The practice session was repeated if the participants made more errors than the set standard. Before the actual task, the teacher avatar briefly explained the fNIRS device, and participants were encouraged to remain as still as possible. After receiving instructions, participants were guided to wear fNIRS gear, followed by the main task.

The n-back task consisted of three block sets. Each block set contained alternating 1-back (low WM load) and 2-back (high WM), and in-between 0-back (control) conditions (block length, 40 s; 20 trials with random display of capitalized letters from “A” to “G”). Each stimulus was presented for 500 ms with an interstimulus interval of 1.5 s. Overall block-set time was 160 s, and total session time was 8 min. The total number of trials in experimental conditions was 120. Subjects were instructed to press the button with their right index finger as quickly as possible, when the current stimulus was the same as the previously shown letter (1-back), or with the letter shown two screens back (2-back). Since it is extremely difficult for young ADHD patients to remain still when not performing a task, our baseline (control) task required participants to respond to each stimulus with a button press to rule out motion artifacts and equalize the motion load with the experimental task. To diminish habituation or practice effects in the post-MPH session, two task versions with different stimuli were employed. These two versions were randomly assigned to each subject.

The software was written in Visual Studio 2017(C#) and designed on Unity 2018.2. The avatars and structures comprising the virtual environment were built using a 3Ds Max 2014. We used the 3D development platform (Vizard 5.1; WorldViz, Santa Barbara, CA, USA) to develop the virtual classroom environment. Our VR classroom was implemented in the VR theater which provides a semi-immersive environment with a 2.18-m radius curved screen, providing 150° field-of-view (PACOM Display System Inc, Suwon, Kyungki-do, Republic of Korea; Figure 1C). Two projectors with HDTV resolution (1,960 × 1,080 pixels) were used to project the programs onto the screen. The system was driven by a desktop computer with Microsoft Windows 10 operating system, including a high-end graphics card (NVIDIA GeForce GTX 970) and 16 GB RAM of graphics memory.

fNIRS Measurements

We used a multichannel high density fNIRS device (NIRSIT; OBELAB, Seoul, Korea), which consisted of 24 laser diodes emitting two wavelengths (780/850 nm) and 32 photodetectors separated by a 1.5 cm unit distance. The laser and detector pairs were separated at a 3 cm distance. Sampling rate was 8.138 Hz. The alignment of 48 channels is shown in Figure 2. The fNIRS device was placed on the head according to the relevant standard positions of the International 10–20 system for EEG electrode locations. The center of the bottom line of the measuring channel was located on the FPZ. The threshold of signal-to-noise ratio was 30 dB such that slow drift of physiological noise and environmental noise was removed after filtering through a band-pass filter (0.005–0.1 Hz) of detected light signals. The modified Beer Lambert Law (MBLL) was used to convert raw light intensities into concentration changes in oxygenated hemoglobin (ΔHbO2). The averaged oxy-Hb concentration changes (avgΔHbO2) during the task period baselined from 5 s before task initiation was calculated in each channel after block averaging of multiple trials. Finally, the regional representative value of avgΔHbO2 was extracted by averaging categorized channels based on the specified region of interest (ROI). The selection of the brain ROI was completed before data analysis. The 48 channels were categorized as right and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC), ventrolateral prefrontal cortex (VLPFC), medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC), and orbitofrontal cortex (OFC), which constituted eight ROIs. The channels corresponding to each region are shown in Figure 2. The MNI coordinates for each channel were defined based on the equipment coordinates. Using this information, the ROIs were designated in accordance with the Brodmann area template for each channel. Brain activation maps (Figure 3A) were visualized using the avgΔHbO2 per channel of each group according to 1-back and 2-back tasks.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Spatial profiles of fNIRS channels. The fNIRS probes were attached to the prefrontal area. The included channels are represented according to brain region location. DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; VLPFC, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; Rt, right; Lt, left.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Hemodynamic changes during performance of the n-back task. (A) The overall brain activation patterns are shown as signal maps with avgΔHbO2 values presented in accordance with the color bar. The upper and lower lines show results for 1-back and 2-back condition, respectively, of each group. The mean avgΔHbO2 values for 1-back condition in (B) right and (C) left hemispheres and 2-back condition in (D) right and (E) left hemispheres are represented as bar graphs. ADHD, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder; HC, healthy control; MPH, methylphenidate; Hb, hemoglobin; DLPFC, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex; VLPFC, ventrolateral prefrontal cortex; mPFC, medial prefrontal cortex; OFC, orbitofrontal cortex; Rt, right; Lt, left. *p < 0.05.

Behavioral Data Analysis

For n-back task results, the total reaction time (RT), RT variability, and accuracy were used as dependent variables for analysis. RT variability was calculated by dividing the standard deviation of the individual RT by the mean value, as reported previously (52). We computed accuracy in each condition by dividing the correct answer (correct response and appropriate rejection) by total number of stimuli.

Statistical Analysis

Group differences in clinical characteristics between ADHD and HC groups were compared using an independent sample t-test for numerical variables or Chi square (χ2) test for categorical variables. The means and standard deviations of avgΔHbO2 were calculated for each ROI and in each group to compare group differences and verify the effects of pre- and post-MPH effects on fNIRS results. For comparison of behavioral performance and fNIRS data between pre- and post-treatment conditions in ADHD participants, we used a two-tailed paired t-test. An independent two-sample two-tailed t-test was used for comparing variables between ADHD and HC groups. The normality of the data was evaluated by visual inspection of quantile-quantile plots and the Shapiro-Wilk test. To examine the association between behavioral performance and fNIRS data, we conducted correlation analysis using the Pearson's method. The statistical threshold was set at p < 0.05. All statistical analyses were completed with IBM SPSS version 25 (IBM Corporation, Armonk, NY, USA).

Results

Demographic and Clinical Characteristics

Baseline characteristics of the study participants are presented in Table 1. ADHD and HC groups did not differ significantly in mean age, sex ratio, full scale IQ(FSIQ), CDI, STAI-C, or SSQ scores (Table 1). With regard to comorbidities, only one patient with ADHD had a tic disorder. Both ADHD patients and HC showed lower CDI and STAI-C scores than the cut-off (ADHD, CDI = 8.52, STAI-C trait = 29.09, STAI-C state = 28.07; HC, CDI = 10.25, STAI-C trait = 30, STAI-C state = 30.83; cut-off, CDI = 13, STAI-C trait = 36, STAI-C state = 36), indicating the absence of clinical depression and anxiety. As expected, significant differences were observed in the ADHD-RS (t = −4.64, p < 0.001), ADHD-RS-inattention and hyperactive-impulsive subscales scores (inattention, t = −4.62, p < 0.001; hyperactive-impulsive, t = −4.137, p < 0.001), CBCL total and attention problem subscale scores (total, t = −4.15, p < 0.001; attention problem, t = −4.21, p < 0.001). The mean ADHD_RS score of the ADHD patients was 20.96, which indicates relatively mild level of ADHD severity. The SSQ score was low in both groups (ADHD, 18.14; HC, 39.31) with few symptoms. The typical mean MPH dose of ADHD subjects was 29.35 mg (SD, 16.23 mg; range, 10–64 mg).

Behavioral Performance

The average accuracy rates and RTs in each n-back task for HC and ADHD participants are summarized in Table 2. Within-ADHD-subject analysis revealed shorter RTs in the 1-back condition and shorter RT, lower RT variability, and higher accuracy in the 2-back condition after MPH administration compared to that of pre-MPH. No significant differences in behavioral performance between conditions were observed for ADHD and HC groups.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. N-back task performance for ADHD and control subjects.

fNIRS Results

Changes in avgΔHbO2 measured using fNIRS are presented in Figure 3 according to brain area and group. For the (low WM load) 1-back condition, no significant avgΔHbO2 changes were observed in any area when comparing pre- and post-MPH conditions (Figures 3B,C; Supplementary Table 1). Compared to the HC group, both pre-MPH and post-MPH ADHD subjects showed significantly fewer avgΔHbO2 signals in the right DLPFC (pre-MPH vs HC, t = −2.443, p < 0.05; post-MPH vs. HC, t = −2.233, p < 0.05; Figure 3B; Supplementary Table 1). For the (high WM load) 2-back condition, avgΔHbO2 in pre-MPH ADHD subjects was significantly higher than that in post-MPH ADHD subjects in the left DLPFC and bilateral mPFC (left DLPFC, t = 2.838, p < 0.05, uncorrected; left mPFC, t = 2.334, p < 0.05, uncorrected; right mPFC, t = 2.496, p < 0.05, uncorrected; Figures 3D,E; Supplementary Table 1). No significant differences were observed when comparing the HC group with both pre- and post-MPH ADHD subjects. In the 2-back task, decreased activation was noted in several brain areas after MPH administration in ADHD subjects.

Association Between Behavioral Performance and fNIRS Results

We investigated the relationship between 2-back task accuracy (% correct) and mPFC activation. There was a negative correlation between accuracy and avgΔHbO2 in the right and left mPFC in pre-MPH ADHD subjects (right mPFC, r = −0.518, p < 0.05; left mPFC, r = −0.591, p < 0.05; Figures 4A,B). After MPH administration, this correlation was not observed.

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Correlation between 2-back task accuracy and avgΔHbO2 signals in bilateral mPFC. Correlations between accuracy (%) for 2-back task and avgΔHbO2 values in (A) right mPFC and (B) left mPFC in pre-(red dot) and post-MPH (white dot) ADHD subjects were analyzed with Pearson's correlation test. Line and R2 value indicate the regression of the dataset (dashed, pre-MPH ADHD; dotted, post-MPH ADHD).

Discussion

The present study aimed to investigate the feasibility of using fNIRS to investigate the neural substrates of single-dose MPH effects in children with ADHD during a VR working memory task. The combination of VR and fNIRS in this study revealed that this model was able to easily measure the effects of various drugs and interventions on children with ADHD in an environment simulating a real-world setting. Furthermore, it enables a setting that explores changes in brain activity during a neuro-rehabilitation program using VR environments. To our knowledge, this is the first study using a VR environment to measure the impact of MPH on working memory with fNIRS in ADHD subjects.

In the current study, task performance improved after MPH treatment; this was verified by shorter RT in the 1-back condition and shorter RT, lower RT variability, and higher accuracy in the 2-back condition compared to that in pre-medicated ADHD subjects. This result is generally consistent with previous studies reporting performance enhancing effects of MPH in ADHD patients (5355). Conversely, there was no performance differences between pre- and post-medicated ADHD subjects and the HC group in the VR n-back task. This result was also reported in our previous study that examined VR-CPT performance in ADHD and HC subjects. In this study, scores were lower in the ADHD group than in the HC group in traditional CPT, but VR-CPT performance was not significantly different. Similarly, it is possible that VR increased the motivation of ADHD patients by presenting them with a more immersive environment in the current study, thereby improving their task performance, as reported in our previous VR study (56). Although the enjoyment or motivation of patients was not measured in this study, previous studies have shown that VR neuropsychological tests were perceived as more enjoyable to patients and increased motivation (9, 57). For precise interpretation, further research is needed on motivation and performance differences in ADHD patients when using VR.

We observed increased activation in the left DLPFC and bilateral mPFC in pre-medicated ADHD subjects during the VR 2-back task, which decreased after MPH administration. However, there were no significant differences between HC and ADHD children. In most NIRS studies comparing children with ADHD to HCs in different executive functioning tasks, altered prefrontal activity was reported: some studies reported reduced activity in ADHD (1719), whereas others reported increased activity in ADHD (58, 59). The cause of these conflicting results is not clear, but a possible reason is the use of different cognitive tasks with different stimulus characteristics. Therefore, the brain areas associated with task performance in each study were not concordant, rendering the neural correlates of WM in ADHD subjects unclear. Previous meta-analyses revealed significant hypoactivation in the left inferior frontal gyrus/anterior insula and right middle frontal gyrus (60), bilateral superior frontal gyrus, and left medial frontal gyrus (61) in adult ADHD patients during the N-back task compared to that in HCs. Nevertheless, meta-analyses on the neural substrates related to WM in children with ADHD are limited. One meta-analysis on healthy children found no concordance in prefrontal regions related to the n-back task (62). In addition, the only study exploring the effects of MPH on brain function during a WM task reported that no specific brain area was activated by stimulants (54). These discrepancies may be due to varying brain maturity and subjective n-back difficulty according to age (62). Further, medication status may differentially affect brain activation. A previous study reported that medication-naïve ADHD patients showed a tendency toward lower activation than that of HCs, whereas non-naïve and HCs did not differ significantly but showed similar activation (42). Given these conflicting results, it is difficult to conclude whether our findings agree with the literature.

Increased activation in bilateral mPFC was negatively correlated with accuracy of the 2-back task in pre-medicated ADHD subjects, whereas this association disappeared after MPH treatment. This result implies that mPFC was abnormally hyper-activated in ADHD subjects during the 2-back task, which was accompanied by low performance. The mPFC is a brain region that constitutes the default mode network (DMN) along with the medial and lateral parietal and temporal cortices (63). The DMN is associated with intrinsic brain activity and commonly deactivates during attention-demanding or goal-directed activity. Abnormal hyperactivity in DMN areas, indicating unsuccessful task-induced deactivation, is characteristic of ADHD patients (55, 64, 65). Impairment of sustained attention in ADHD may result from abnormal persistence or intrusion of DMN activity (64). In addition, several studies reported that psychostimulants normalized abnormal hyperactivity in the DMN, including the mPFC, in ADHD subjects, and even in children (55, 65, 66). Our study also demonstrated that MPH administration reduced mPFC activation while improving task performance, which indicated normalization of attenuated DMN deactivation in ADHD children. However, a double-blind placebo-controlled study involving other areas comprising the DMN should be performed to understand the relevance of these findings.

By contrast, in the 1-back condition, we found no difference in brain activation between pre- and post-MPH conditions. Given that only RT was shorter after MPH treatment for behavioral performance, and there was no significant difference in accuracy or RT variability, it can be assumed that the difference before and after drug was minimal due to the low level of 1-back difficulty, reflected by a ceiling effect from the pre-test. The only study that explored the effect of MPH during the n-back task in ADHD children reported no differences in brain function in the 1-back condition between pre- and post-drug administration, which is concordant with our study (67). In addition to exploring MPH effects in a VR environment, brain activity was also measured with fNIRS in this study in a VR-based WM task. Compared to the HC group, ADHD children showed lower activation of the right DLPFC, consistent with previous studies on ADHD and HC participants in which the right DLPFC was a ROI identified via fNIRS (37).

In the present study, the VR classroom environment provided a more ecologically valid and motivating task than that of traditional computerized cognitive tasks, suggested by the improved task performance of ADHD children. We propose several advantages of using a VR task over traditional computerized WM tasks. Previous studies have indicated that ADHD children find digital technology environments more enjoyable and are more immersed in the task (9, 68). None of the participants complained of side effects in SSQ, suggesting that the virtual classroom is a user-friendly tool. This may be regarded as an alternative for estimating how brain activity changes with WM in ADHD children after drug administration in a setting that simulates real-world classrooms.

Limitations

Our study has several limitations which we hope to address in future studies. First, the study design was not optimized for neuropharmacological analysis. A double-blind placebo-controlled or a cross-over design with drug-naive ADHD children should be conducted. Since we did not include a placebo group, the superior task performance in post-MPH ADHD subjects could be due to a repeat effect. In addition, unlike the ADHD patients, the HC did not repeat the tests, making it difficult to compare the HC group with the post-MPH group or to eliminate the time effect. Therefore, it is not entirely clear whether the changes in brain activation in the ADHD group were truly induced by the treatment or were simply a function of time. However, considering the NIRS imaging results, the improved behavioral performance was accompanied by changes in brain activity such as reduced hyperactivity of bilateral mPFC in post-MPH ADHD subjects, making it difficult to fully explain solely based on repeat effects. Further, WM tasks are generally considered to have strong test-retest reliability and to be relatively unaffected by practice effects (69). In addition, previous studies suggested that the effects of MPH on brain activity differ between drug-naive and non-naive ADHD subjects (42, 70, 71). Although our subjects may have exhibited weakened pure drug effects because they were a drug-non-naive group, it is relevant for feasibility because it is necessary to measure drug effects in non-naive subjects in real clinical practice. Further studies including both drug-naïve and non-naïve children could lead to accurate interpretations. Second, due to the limitations in our experimental design, such as the relatively small sample size and lack of a cross-over design, a liberal statistical threshold was adopted for this study. Third, the relatively wide age range of the subjects (7–16 years) and the lack of sex stratification could be problematic. A meta-analysis of fMRI studies related to the n-back task revealed widespread variability of prefrontal activation patterns across ages due to a protracted, step-wise maturation pattern of the prefrontal cortex (62). Although most ADHD research is conducted with similar age ranges to ours, it will help to narrow down the age range to elicit more accurate results on prefrontal function in child participants. In addition, although previous studies have shown sex differences, such as hypofrontality only in males, we did not conduct a subgroup analysis by sex due to the small sample size of this study (72, 73). Future studies with large sample sizes to analyze group effects by age and sex are needed. Fourth, the brain regions evaluated in this study were not wide enough to cover WM-related areas or DMN. The fNIRS probe covered only the prefrontal cortex and was unable to detect activity in deeper cortical structures unreachable by near-infrared light. Further studies covering wider areas or exploring connectivity between brain areas are needed to clearly interpret the results of this study. Fifth, we cannot determine if the VR n-back task is actually more effective or motivation-enhancing because we lack a comparison of a VR paradigm to a 2D version of the n-back task. Finally, we used a semi-immersive VR display, instead of a more immersive HMD version of virtual reality, since simultaneously wearing HMD and fNIRS devices is difficult due to space overlap.

Conclusions

To our knowledge, this is the first study to explore the effects of MPH on brain activity during a VR-based WM task reproducing real-classroom settings in ADHD children. We observed that activation of bilateral mPFC decreased after MPH treatment in a high-load WM task. Further, bilateral mPFC activation was negatively correlated with task accuracy in pre-MPH condition; this correlation disappeared after MPH administration. These findings suggest that mPFC-mediated inappropriately excessive mind-wandering during a high-load WM task in ADHD children may have disappeared after MPH administration. Taken together, these results suggest that the combination of VR tasks and fNIRS examination is a technique that enables examination of the effects of interventions within a real-life setting in ADHD children and adolescents.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Institutional Review Board of Yonsei University College of Medicine Gangnam Severance Hospital. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants' legal guardian/next of kin. Written informed consent was obtained from the minor(s)' legal guardian/next of kin for the publication of any potentially identifiable images or data included in this article.

Author Contributions

EK and NL devised the project, the main conceptual ideas, and proof of the outline. EK, NL, and JY designed the detailed study design. JY, NH, JK, and JH examined participants, performed the experiments, and acquired and organized the data. SJ and JC conducted analyses of the experimental data, drafted the manuscript, and designed the figures with supervision from EK. JO and J-jK contributed to interpreting the results and worked on the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and commented on the manuscript. EK supervised the project. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This research was supported by the Brain Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT & Future Planning (NRF-2018R1A2B6006971).

Conflict of Interest

JC was employed by OBELAB Inc.

The remaining authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

This work was supported by the Brain Research Program through the National Research Foundation of Korea (NRF) funded by the Ministry of Science, ICT & Future Planning (NRF-2018R1A2B6006971).

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2020.564618/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Rubia K. Cognitive neuroscience of Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder (ADHD) and its clinical translation. Front Hum Neurosci. (2018) 12:100. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2018.00100

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Faraone SV, Asherson P, Banaschewski T, Biederman J, Buitelaar JK, Ramos-Quiroga JA, et al. Attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Nat Rev Dis Primers. (2015) 1:15020. doi: 10.1038/nrdp.2015.20

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Wehmeier PM, Schacht A, Barkley RA. Social and emotional impairment in children and adolescents with ADHD and the impact on quality of life. J Adolesc Health. (2010) 46:209–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2009.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Arnsten AF. Stimulants: therapeutic actions in ADHD. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2006) 31:2376–83. doi: 10.1038/sj.npp.1301164

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Spencer TJ. ADHD treatment across the life cycle. J Clin Psychiatry. (2004) 65(Suppl. 3):22–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

6. Bashiri A, Ghazisaeedi M, Shahmoradi L. The opportunities of virtual reality in the rehabilitation of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a literature review. Korean J Pediatr. (2017) 60:337–43. doi: 10.3345/kjp.2017.60.11.337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Nolin P, Stipanicic A, Henry M, Lachapelle Y, Lussier-Desrochers D, Albert R, et al. Classroom-CPT: a virtual reality tool for assessing attention and inhibition in children and adolescents. Comput Hum Behav. (2016) 59:327–33. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2016.02.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Parsons TD, Bowerly T, Buckwalter JG, Rizzo AA. A controlled clinical comparison of attention performance in children with ADHD in a virtual reality classroom compared to standard neuropsychological methods. Child Neuropsychol. (2007) 13:363–81. doi: 10.1080/13825580600943473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Pollak Y, Weiss PL, Rizzo AA, Weizer M, Shriki L, Shalev RS, et al. The utility of a continuous performance test embedded in virtual reality in measuring ADHD-related deficits. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2009) 30:2–6. doi: 10.1097/DBP.0b013e3181969b22

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Muhlberger A, Jekel K, Probst T, Schecklmann M, Conzelmann A, Andreatta M, et al. The Influence of methylphenidate on hyperactivity and attention deficits in children with ADHD: a virtual classroom test. J Atten Disord. (2020) 24:277–89. doi: 10.1177/1087054716647480

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Cho BH, Kim S, Shin DI, Lee JH, Lee SM, Kim IY, et al. Neurofeedback training with virtual reality for inattention and impulsiveness. Cyberpsychol Behav. (2004) 7:519–26. doi: 10.1089/cpb.2004.7.519

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Scarapicchia V, Brown C, Mayo C, Gawryluk JR. Functional magnetic resonance imaging and functional near-infrared spectroscopy: insights from combined recording studies. Front Hum Neurosci. (2017) 11:419. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Ehlis AC, Schneider S, Dresler T, Fallgatter AJ. Application of functional near-infrared spectroscopy in psychiatry. NeuroImage. (2014) 85:478–88. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Scholkmann F, Kleiser S, Metz AJ, Zimmermann R, Mata Pavia J, Wolf U, et al. A review on continuous wave functional near-infrared spectroscopy and imaging instrumentation and methodology. NeuroImage. (2014) 85:6–27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Kawakubo Y, Kuwabara H, Watanabe K, Minowa M, Someya T, Minowa I, et al. Impaired prefrontal hemodynamic maturation in autism and unaffected siblings. PLoS ONE. (2009) 4:e6881. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0006881

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Irani F, Platek SM, Bunce S, Ruocco AC, Chute D. Functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS): an emerging neuroimaging technology with important applications for the study of brain disorders. Clin Neuropsychol. (2007) 21:9–37. doi: 10.1080/13854040600910018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Inoue Y, Sakihara K, Gunji A, Ozawa H, Kimiya S, Shinoda H, et al. Reduced prefrontal hemodynamic response in children with ADHD during the Go/NoGo task: a NIRS study. Neuroreport. (2012) 23:55–60. doi: 10.1097/WNR.0b013e32834e664c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Negoro H, Sawada M, Iida J, Ota T, Tanaka S, Kishimoto T. Prefrontal dysfunction in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder as measured by near-infrared spectroscopy. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. (2010) 41:193–203. doi: 10.1007/s10578-009-0160-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Xiao T, Xiao Z, Ke X, Hong S, Yang H, Su Y, et al. Response inhibition impairment in high functioning autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: evidence from near-infrared spectroscopy data. PLoS ONE. (2012) 7:e46569. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0046569

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Yasumura A, Kokubo N, Yamamoto H, Yasumura Y, Nakagawa E, Kaga M, et al. Neurobehavioral and hemodynamic evaluation of Stroop and reverse Stroop interference in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Brain Dev. (2014) 36:97–106. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2013.01.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Arai S, Okamoto Y, Fujioka T, Inohara K, Ishitobi M, Matsumura Y, et al. Altered frontal pole development affects self-generated spatial working memory in ADHD. Brain Dev. (2016) 38:471–80. doi: 10.1016/j.braindev.2015.11.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Schecklmann M, Romanos M, Bretscher F, Plichta MM, Warnke A, Fallgatter AJ. Prefrontal oxygenation during working memory in ADHD. J Psychiatr Res. (2010) 44:621–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2009.11.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Monden Y, Dan H, Nagashima M, Dan I, Tsuzuki D, Kyutoku Y, et al. Right prefrontal activation as a neuro-functional biomarker for monitoring acute effects of methylphenidate in ADHD children: an fNIRS study. Neuroimage Clin. (2012) 1:131–40. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2012.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Nagashima M, Monden Y, Dan I, Dan H, Tsuzuki D, Mizutani T, et al. Neuropharmacological effect of methylphenidate on attention network in children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder during oddball paradigms as assessed using functional near-infrared spectroscopy. Neurophotonics. (2014) 1:015001. doi: 10.1117/1.NPh.1.1.015001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Eriksson J, Vogel EK, Lansner A, Bergström F, Nyberg L. Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron. (2015) 88:33–46. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Sreenivasan KK, Curtis CE, D'Esposito M. Revisiting the role of persistent neural activity during working memory. Trends Cogn Sci. (2014) 18:82–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2013.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gazzaley A, Rissman J, D'Esposito M. Functional connectivity during working memory maintenance. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. (2004) 4:580–99. doi: 10.3758/CABN.4.4.580

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Fry AF, Hale S. Relationships among processing speed, working memory, and fluid intelligence in children. Biol Psychol. (2000) 54:1–34. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0511(00)00051-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. D'Esposito M, Postle BR. The cognitive neuroscience of working memory. Annu Rev Psychol. (2015) 66:115–42. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Mayer JS, Roebroeck A, Maurer K, Linden DE. Specialization in the default mode: task-induced brain deactivations dissociate between visual working memory and attention. Hum Brain Mapp. (2010) 31:126–39. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20850

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Tomasi D, Ernst T, Caparelli EC, Chang L. Common deactivation patterns during working memory and visual attention tasks: an intra-subject fMRI study at 4 Tesla. Hum Brain Mapp. (2006) 27:694–705. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20211

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Fox MD, Snyder AZ, Vincent JL, Corbetta M, Van Essen DC, Raichle ME. The human brain is intrinsically organized into dynamic, anticorrelated functional networks. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. (2005) 102:9673–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0504136102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Spreng RN, Stevens WD, Chamberlain JP, Gilmore AW, Schacter DL. Default network activity, coupled with the frontoparietal control network, supports goal-directed cognition. Neuroimage. (2010) 53:303–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Fox KC, Spreng RN, Ellamil M, Andrews-Hanna JR, Christoff K. The wandering brain: meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage. (2015) 111:611–21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Alali-Morlevy K, Goldfarb L. The connection between mind wandering, ADHD, and level of performance on an attention task. J Atten Disord. (2020). doi: 10.1177/1087054720946371. [Epub ahead of print].

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Kane MJ, Gross GM, Chun CA, Smeekens BA, Meier ME, Silvia PJ, et al. For whom the mind wanders, and when, varies across laboratory and daily-life settings. Psychol Sci. (2017) 28:1271–89. doi: 10.1177/0956797617706086

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Gu Y, Miao S, Han J, Liang Z, Ouyang G, Yang J, et al. Identifying ADHD children using hemodynamic responses during a working memory task measured by functional near-infrared spectroscopy. J Neural Eng. (2018) 15:035005. doi: 10.1088/1741-2552/aa9ee9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Herff C, Heger D, Fortmann O, Hennrich J, Putze F, Schultz T. Mental workload during n-back task-quantified in the prefrontal cortex using fNIRS. Front Hum Neurosci. (2013) 7:935. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00935

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Gu Y, Miao S, Han J, Zeng K, Ouyang G, Yang J, et al. Complexity analysis of fNIRS signals in ADHD children during working memory task. Sci Rep. (2017) 7:829. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-00965-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Nakanishi Y, Ota T, Iida J, Yamamuro K, Kishimoto N, Okazaki K, et al. Differential therapeutic effects of atomoxetine and methylphenidate in childhood attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder as measured by near-infrared spectroscopy. Child Adolesc Psychiatry Ment Health. (2017) 11:26. doi: 10.1186/s13034-017-0163-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Sanefuji M, Yamashita H, Torisu H, Takada Y, Imanaga H, Matsunaga M, et al. Altered strategy in short-term memory for pictures in children with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a near-infrared spectroscopy study. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 223:37–42. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2014.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Ishii-Takahashi A, Takizawa R, Nishimura Y, Kawakubo Y, Hamada K, Okuhata S, et al. Neuroimaging-aided prediction of the effect of methylphenidate in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: a randomized controlled trial. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2015) 40:2676–85. doi: 10.1038/npp.2015.128

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Carter CS, Heckers S, Nichols T, Pine DS, Strother S. Optimizing the design and analysis of clinical functional magnetic resonance imaging research studies. Biol Psychiatry. (2008) 64:842–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric Association (2013). doi: 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Sheehan DV, Sheehan KH, Shytle RD, Janavs J, Bannon Y, Rogers JE, et al. Reliability and validity of the Mini International Neuropsychiatric Interview for Children and Adolescents (MINI-KID). J Clin Psychiatry. (2010) 71:313–26. doi: 10.4088/JCP.09m05305whi

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Oh K, Lee H. Development of Korean Version of Child Behavior Checklist (K-CBCL). Seoul: Korean Research Foundation (1990).

PubMed Abstract

47. Kovacs M. The Children's Depression, Inventory (CDI). Psychopharmacol Bull. (1985) 21:995–8.

PubMed Abstract

48. Cho S, Lee Y. Development of the Korean form of the Kovacs' Children's Depression Inventory. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc. (1990) 29:943–56.

49. Spielberger CD, Gorsuch RL, Lushene R, Vagg PR, and Jacobs GA. State-Trait Anxiety Inventory for Children: How I Feel Questionnaire: Professional Manual. Menlo Park, CA: Mind Garden. (1973).

Google Scholar

50. Cho S, Choi J. Development of the Korean form of the state-trait anxiety inventory for children. Seoul J Psychiatry. (1989) 14:150–7.

51. Kennedy RS, Lane NE, Berbaum KS, Lilienthal MG. Simulator sickness questionnaire: an enhanced method for quantifying simulator sickness. Int J Aviat Psychol. (1993) 3:203–20. doi: 10.1207/s15327108ijap0303_3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Epstein JN, Langberg JM, Rosen PJ, Graham A, Narad ME, Antonini TN, et al. Evidence for higher reaction time variability for children with ADHD on a range of cognitive tasks including reward and event rate manipulations. Neuropsychology. (2011) 25:427–41. doi: 10.1037/a0022155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Epstein JN, Casey BJ, Tonev ST, Davidson MC, Reiss AL, Garrett A, et al. ADHD- and medication-related brain activation effects in concordantly affected parent-child dyads with ADHD. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2007) 48:899–913. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2007.01761.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Kobel M, Bechtel N, Weber P, Specht K, Klarhofer M, Scheffler K, et al. Effects of methylphenidate on working memory functioning in children with attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. (2009) 13:516–23. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpn.2008.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Liddle EB, Hollis C, Batty MJ, Groom MJ, Totman JJ, Liotti M, et al. Task-related default mode network modulation and inhibitory control in ADHD: effects of motivation and methylphenidate. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2011) 52:761–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02333.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Eom H, Kim KK, Lee S, Hong YJ, Heo J, Kim JJ, et al. Development of virtual reality continuous performance test utilizing social cues for children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2019) 22:198–204. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2018.0377

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. van Bennekom MJ, de Koning PP, Denys D. Virtual reality objectifies the diagnosis of psychiatric disorders: a literature review. Front Psychiatry. (2017) 8:163. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00163

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Weber P, Lutschg J, Fahnenstich H. Cerebral hemodynamic changes in response to an executive function task in children with attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder measured by near-infrared spectroscopy. J Dev Behav Pediatr. (2005) 26:105–11. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200504000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Jourdan Moser S, Cutini S, Weber P, Schroeter ML. Right prefrontal brain activation due to Stroop interference is altered in attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder - a functional near-infrared spectroscopy study. Psychiatry Res. (2009) 173:190–5. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Cortese S, Kelly C, Chabernaud C, Proal E, Di Martino A, Milham MP, et al. Toward systems neuroscience of ADHD: a meta-analysis of 55 fMRI studies. Am J Psychiatry. (2012) 169:1038–55. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2012.11101521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. McCarthy H, Skokauskas N, Frodl T. Identifying a consistent pattern of neural function in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:869–80. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713001037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Yaple Z, Arsalidou M. N-back working memory task: meta-analysis of normative fMRI studies with children. Child Dev. (2018) 89:2010–22. doi: 10.1111/cdev.13080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Raichle ME. The brain's default mode network. Annu Rev Neurosci. (2015) 38:433–47. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Sonuga-Barke EJ, Castellanos FX. Spontaneous attentional fluctuations in impaired states and pathological conditions: a neurobiological hypothesis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. (2007) 31:977–86. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Peterson BS, Potenza MN, Wang Z, Zhu H, Martin A, Marsh R, et al. An FMRI study of the effects of psychostimulants on default-mode processing during Stroop task performance in youths with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. (2009) 166:1286–94. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.08050724

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Fassbender C, Zhang H, Buzy WM, Cortes CR, Mizuiri D, Beckett L, et al. A lack of default network suppression is linked to increased distractibility in ADHD. Brain Res. (2009) 1273:114–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2009.02.070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Cubillo A, Smith AB, Barrett N, Giampietro V, Brammer M, Simmons A, et al. Drug-specific laterality effects on frontal lobe activation of atomoxetine and methylphenidate in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder boys during working memory. Psychol Med. (2014) 44:633–46. doi: 10.1017/S0033291713000676

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Prins PJ, Dovis S, Ponsioen A, Ten Brink E, van Der Oord S. Does computerized working memory training with game elements enhance motivation and training efficacy in children with ADHD? Cyberpsychol Behav Soc Netw. (2011) 14:115–22. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2009.0206

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Hockey A, Geffen G. The concurrent validity and test–retest reliability of a visuospatial working memory task. Intelligence. (2004) 32:591–605. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2004.07.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Shaw P, Sharp WS, Morrison M, Eckstrand K, Greenstein DK, Clasen LS, et al. Psychostimulant treatment and the developing cortex in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Am J Psychiatry. (2009) 166:58–63. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08050781

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Frodl T, Skokauskas N. Meta-analysis of structural MRI studies in children and adults with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder indicates treatment effects. Acta Psychiatr Scand. (2012) 125:114–26. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2011.01786.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Valera EM, Brown A, Biederman J, Faraone SV, Makris N, Monuteaux MC, et al. Sex differences in the functional neuroanatomy of working memory in adults with ADHD. Am J Psychiatry. (2010) 167:86–94. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09020249

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Dickstein SG, Bannon K, Castellanos FX, Milham MP. The neural correlates of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: an ALE meta-analysis. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. (2006) 47:1051–62. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01671.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: virtual reality, fNIRS, ADHD, working memory, n-back, Methylphenidate, mPFC

Citation: Jang S, Choi J, Oh J, Yeom J, Hong N, Lee N, Kwon JH, Hong J, Kim J-j and Kim E (2021) Use of Virtual Reality Working Memory Task and Functional Near-Infrared Spectroscopy to Assess Brain Hemodynamic Responses to Methylphenidate in ADHD Children. Front. Psychiatry 11:564618. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.564618

Received: 22 May 2020; Accepted: 23 December 2020;
Published: 21 January 2021.

Edited by:

David Cohen, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, France

Reviewed by:

Isabelle Esther Bauer, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, United States
Iliyan Ivanov, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, United States

Copyright © 2021 Jang, Choi, Oh, Yeom, Hong, Lee, Kwon, Hong, Kim and Kim. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Eunjoo Kim, RUpLSU05NiYjeDAwMDQwO3l1aHMuYWM=

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.