Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 03 August 2020
Sec. Schizophrenia
This article is part of the Research Topic Genetic Mechanisms of Biomarkers in Schizophrenia, Bipolar Disorder and Depression View all 18 articles

Hyper-Activated Brain Resting-State Network and Mismatch Negativity Deficit in Schizophrenia With Auditory Verbal Hallucination Revealed by an Event-Related Potential Evidence

Qiaoling Sun&#x;Qiaoling Sun1†Yehua Fang&#x;Yehua Fang2†Xuemei PengXuemei Peng3Yongyan ShiYongyan Shi1Jinhong ChenJinhong Chen4Lifeng WangLifeng Wang5Liwen Tan*Liwen Tan1*
  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Mental Health Institute of the Second Xiangya Hospital, Central South University, Changsha, China
  • 2Department of Clinical Psychology, Zhuzhou Central Hospital, Zhuzhou, China
  • 3Department Psychology, Xiangtan Central Hospital, Xiangtan, China
  • 4Department of Sleeping Disorders & Neurosis, Brain Hospital of Hunan Province, Changsha, China
  • 5Department of Clinical Psychology, The Third Xiangya Hospital of Central South University, Changsha, China

Schizophrenia is a holergasia with unclear mechanism and high heterogeneity. Auditory verbal hallucination (AVH) study might help in understanding schizophrenia from the perspective of individual symptoms. This study aimed to investigate the activities of the resting-state networks (RSN) in the electroencephalogram (EEG) and mismatch negativity (MMN) in task-related state of schizophrenia patients with AVH. We recruited 30 schizophrenia patients without any medication for more than 4 weeks (15 AVH patients and 15 Non-AVH patients) and 15 healthy controls. We recorded the EEG data of the participants in the resting-state for 7 min and the event-related potential (ERP) data under an auditory oddball paradigm. In the resting-state EEG network, AVH patients exhibited a higher clustering coefficient than Non-AVH patients and healthy controls on delta and beta bands and a shorter characteristic path length than Non-AVH patients and healthy controls on all frequency bands. For ERP data, AVH patients showed a lower MMN amplitude than healthy controls (p = 0.017) and Non-AVH patients (p = 0.033). What’s more, MMN amplitude was positively correlated with clustering coefficient, and negatively correlated with characteristic path length on delta, theta, beta and gamma band in AVH patients. Our results indicate that AVH patients showed a hyper-activity in resting-state and may have impaired higher-order auditory expectations in the task-related state than healthy controls and Non-AVH patients. And it seems reasonable to conclude that the formation of AVH may occupy certain brain resources and compete for brain resources with external auditory stimuli.

Introduction

Schizophrenia, which typically starts between late adolescence and early adulthood, is characterised by incompatibility among thought, behaviour, emotion and the surrounding environment (1, 2). The disease has an unclear cause, long course, tendency to relapse, and poor treatment effect, which bring a serious burden to society (3). Schizophrenia has always been regarded as a whole in research; however, this approach is inaccurate owing to the heterogeneity of the disorder (4). Schizophrenia from the perspective of individual symptoms may be easily understood (5). One of the most prevalent symptoms of schizophrenia is hallucination (6), which refers to an illusory perception (7) that occurs in the absence of external stimulus and with a clear consciousness state. Auditory verbal hallucination (AVH) involves the perception of speech in the absence of external sensory stimulation, occurs with a 50%–80% probability (8) and is the most common hallucination of schizophrenia. The study of independent AVH may be conducive to understanding schizophrenia (9).

In order to explore how AVH occurs spontaneously from the brain’s intrinsic activity. Amount works had been done by studying the brain in its so-called “resting state,” which refers to the intrinsic patterns of brain activity that are observable in the absence of an external task (10). Many early studies have found increased resting activity in the upper or middle temporal lobe in the presence of AVH (11). Dierks compared activation level of resting-state in the same schizophrenia patients group on and offset AVH and found that the activation level of auditory cortex in schizophrenia patients on AVH was increased (12). Allen et al. observed that the secondary auditory cortex was particularly active when AVH occurred (13). In a case study involving a patient with chronic AVH, elevated activity of the right medial temporal and left superior temporal gyri without external speech was observed (14). The bilateral temporal cortex is overactive when AVH occurs in schizophrenia (15).

The mismatch negativity (MMN) is an electrophysiological response that is elicited when a sequence of identical auditory stimuli is infrequently interrupted by a stimulus that deviates from the standard stimulus along one or more dimensions, which appears to represent the automatic change detection process that occurs when an acoustic event violates expectations maintained by the active auditory trace (16, 17). Some studies showed that the decreased amplitude and prolonged latency of MMN are related to the positive symptoms of schizophrenia (18), and MMN abnormality in schizophrenia patients is mainly manifested in the decrease of amplitude (19). MMN in the fronto-temporal lobe is atypical in schizophrenia patients with AVH (20).

Spontaneous potential is a kind of electroencephalogram (EEG) recorded without stimulation. Brain network analysis is a data processing method and brain network topology can be established from EEG data (21). When stimulation is introduced, the event-related potential (ERP), an EEG recorded during the advanced cognitive processing of an object (attention, memory or judgment), is obtained (22). ERP with an excellent temporal resolution is an ideal tool for studying the cognitive function of patients with psychiatric problems and has an extremely sensitive response to cognitive processing. The latency of ERP component refers to the time interval between the stimulus point and ERP component, reflecting the speed at which the brain processes stimuli, whereas the amplitude reflects the brain’s ability to respond to stimuli and resources (such as the number of neurons initiated) devoted to it.

Amount of works had been done on resting-state or task-related state activities in schizophrenia patients with AVH. Various articles from functional magnetic resonance imaging have posited a specific link between RSN [such as default mode network (23, 24), central executive network (25), auditory, and language regions (2628)] and AH. Few EEG/magnetoencephalography studies have examined the resting state in relation to AVH (29) and found that the occurrence of AVH has been associated with increased beta oscillations in left frontoparietal regions (30) and increased gamma-theta in frontotemporal areas (31).But inadequate AVH severity and the effects of antipsychotic drugs have not been considered in these studies (32), and it is still unclear how this hyper-activated resting-state can affect the activity of the task-related state.

In this present study, we recruited drug-free schizophrenia patients with or without AVH and age-matched healthy controls. We recorded the EEG data in the resting-state and their corresponding ERP data during the auditory oddball paradigm. We aimed to examine the activities of the resting EEG brain networks and MMN in task-related state of schizophrenia patients with AVH and explore whether there were some relations between these two states.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Patients were recruited from the psychiatric outpatient department of the Second Xiangya Hospital of Central South University from June to December 2014. The inclusion criteria of the patients were as follows: (a) aged between 18 to 60 years, (b) met ICD-10 criteria for schizophrenia, (c) drug-free or has washed out more than 4 weeks and (d) normal hearing and right-handed. The exclusion criteria were as follows: (a) history of head injury resulting in loss of consciousness, (b) diseases of physical illness or neurological disorders, (c) comorbid with other mental disorders and with other forms of hallucinations, (d) alcohol or drug abuse history, and (e) received electroconvulsive therapy treatment in the past year. Patients meeting the diagnostic criteria for schizophrenia, according to ICD-10, were assessed by two senior clinical psychiatrists by using the Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (33).

We divided the schizophrenia patients into AVH patients and Non-AVH patients. The AVH patients consisted of 15 patients with a current history of AVH, as evidenced by a score ≥ 3 (“mild or greater hallucinatory experiences”) on the hallucination item of the PANSS positive symptom scale and the Non-AVH patients consisted of 15 patients with no lifetime history of AVH. Healthy controls (n = 15) were recruited from the local community by advertisement. The study was approved by the local ethics committee, and written informed consent was obtained from each participant.

Auditory Oddball Task

The program was performed using the E-prime 2.0 software. Briefly, the subjects received auditory stimulus sequences consisting of 540 standard stimuli and 60 deviant stimuli delivered randomly. MMN can be elicited by auditory oddball paradigm and is the negative waveform peaking between 100 and 250 ms after stimulation without subjective effort or subjective interference (16, 17). The processing of auditory stimuli is complex, especially the encoding of duration. If AVH and shortages of auditory resources are found, the brain is most likely to make mistakes in the encoding of duration. Therefore, this study chose duration deviation to induce MMN (34). The probabilities of hearing standard and deviant stimuli were 90% and 10%, respectively. The inter-stimulus interval was 500 ms. The stimulus was delivered binaurally through Sennheiser headphones. The subjects were instructed to watch a neutral exposition to ignore the stimuli. The auditory stimulus had a pure tone and was applied in different durations (standard: 100 ms, deviant: 50 ms). The frequency of the pure-tone stimuli was 1,000 Hz, and the loudness was 70 dB (35).

Data Recording

EEG data were recorded with an electrode cap with Ag/AgCl electrodes at 64 scalp sites according to the modified 10–20 system of electrode placement. Vertical electro-oscillogram was recorded from one electrode fixed below the right eye. The reference electrode was at FCz. All the electrode impedances were maintained below 5 kΩ. Electrical activity was recorded by using the Brain Vision Recorder software (Germany, Brain Products) with an amplifier band pass of 0.1 and 1,000 Hz and digitised at 500 Hz. The first block of the experiment was to record the resting-state EEG data for 7 min when the participants were asked to sit quietly with their eyes closed. After the first block, the participants had a break for 1–2 min. In the second block, the ERP data were recorded when the subjects received the auditory oddball task.

Analyses of the Resting-State EEG Data and ERP Data

We estimated Coherence (Coh), which is the linear relationship at a specific frequency between the two signals x(t) and y(t) on the basis of their cross-spectrum (36). In brief, Coh is expressed as follows,

Cohxy(f)=|Sxy(f)|2Sxx(f)Syy(f)

Sxy(f) indicates the cross-spectrum of x(t) and y(t) at the frequency f; Sxx(f) and Syy(f) denote the autospectrum from the fast Fourier transformation on x(t) and y(t), respectively.

Network analysis was performed using electrodes as the nodes. The coherence between electrode pairs was used to measure the interactions between two regions. After the weighted network was calculated, a threshold (0.200) that can guarantee the connection of network was used to binarize the network. Based on the binarised network, the characteristics of the network can be quantitatively denoted by the network measurements, including clustering coefficient (C) and characteristic path length (L) (21). The weighted matrix obtained from Coh is referred to as w, and wij represents the connectivity between nodes i and j. Node number is denoted by N, and the set of all nodes in the network is denoted by Ω. For a given threshold T, the adjacent matrix w is binarized as,

wij={1,wijT0,wij<T

The threshold is 0.200. ki is the degree of the node i that was defined as,

ki=jΩwij

ti is the number of triangles that can be formed between node i and its neighboring nodes, and was defined as,

ti12j,hΩwijwihwjh

dij is the shortest path between node i and node j. The characteristic path length L for a graph can then be defined as,

L=1NiΩLi=1NiΩΣjΩ,jidijN1

The clustering coefficient can be calculated as,

C=1NiΩ2tiki(ki1)

Five frequency bands including delta (1–4 Hz), theta (4–8 Hz), alpha (8–13 Hz), beta (13–30 Hz) and gamma (30–60 Hz), were selected for analysis.

MMN was acquired at Fz electrode, because MMN was predominantly distributed in the frontal–central area. The offline data of the second block was processed by using the Brain Vision Analyser 2.0 system (Brain Products GmbH, Germany). EEG data were referenced to the average of mastoids (TP9 and TP10). EEG signals were bandpass filtered using a 0.5–30 Hz (50 Hz notch). Eye movements and eye blinks were removed using an independent component analysis (ICA). Artefact rejection procedures were applied to all epochs (−200 ms pre-stimulus to 450 ms post-stimulus), with a baseline correction from −200 ms to 0 ms pre-stimulus. The latency and amplitude of MMN were measured. Latency refers to the time from the beginning of stimulation to the maximum negative peak between 100 and 250 ms, and its amplitude is the vertical distance from the baseline to the maximum peak.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical analyses were carried out by using the Statistics Product and Service Solutions (SPSS18.0) software package. Cochran & Cox Approximate t-test was used in the analysis of PANSS-P3 score between the two patient groups. For the resting EEG data, repeated-measures ANOVA with group (AVH patients, Non-AVH patients, healthy controls) as between-subject variable, and frequency band (delta, theta, alpha, beta and gamma) as within-subject variable. For the ERP data, one-way ANOVA was performed. All post-hoc analyses used the Bonferroni adjustment. Statistical analyses were adjusted for variance nonsphericity using the Greenhouse-Geisser correction (37). Statistical significance was considered at p < 0.05. Spearman correlation analysis was applied to explore the relationship between MMN amplitude and properties of resting EEG data in AVH patients.

Results

Demographic Results

The demographic information for all participants is presented in Table 1. The schizophrenia patients and healthy controls were matched with respect to age, gender and education level. The two patient groups had no significant difference in PANSS positive symptom, negative symptom and general psychopathology. The difference in the PANSS-P3 was statistically significant between AVH patients and Non-AVH patients, and AVH patients had a higher PANSS-P3 score (t’ = 15.08, p < 0.001).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Demographic information for participant and trait questionnaires.

Resting EEG

For clustering coefficient, a significant interaction of group × frequency band (F = 2.320, p = 0.037) was also observed. And further simple effect analyses found that on delta and beta bands AVH patients had a higher clustering coefficient than Non-AVH patients (delta band: p = 0.042; beta band: p = 0.038) and healthy controls (delta band: p = 0.001; beta band: p = 0.002), on theta and gamma bands AVH patients showed a higher clustering coefficient than healthy controls (theta band: p = 0.023; gamma band: p = 0.019), and on alpha band, no significant difference was found among three groups. See in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Network properties on delta, theta, alpha, beta, and gamma bands for auditory verbal hallucination (AVH) patients, non-AVH patients, and healthy controls.

For characteristic path length, main group was found (F = 7.754, p = 0.002) and post-hoc test showed that AVH patients had shorter characteristic path length than Non-AVH patients (p = 0.034) and healthy controls (p = 0.002). No interaction of group × frequency band was observed.

MMN

Grand averages of MMN is presented in Figure 2. For the MMN latency, no significant differences were observed (Table 2). For MMN amplitude, a significant effect was found. Post hoc test revealed statistical differences between AVH patients and healthy controls (p = 0.017), and the MMN amplitude of AVH patients was smaller than the healthy controls. Post hoc test also revealed statistical differences between AVH patients and Non-AVH patients (p = 0.033), and the MMN amplitude of AVH patients was smaller than Non-AVH patients. No statistical difference was found between healthy controls and Non-AVH patients.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Grand average mismatch negativity (MMN) waveforms for auditory verbal hallucination (AVH) patients, non-AVH patients, and healthy controls at Fz.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 ANOVA results for the mismatch negativity (MMN) latency and amplitude.

The brain topographies at the Fz electrode are shown in Figure 3. It reflects the EEG changes from 100–248 ms after stimulation. The negative wave appeared gradually at approximately 138 ms and was increased with time. The amplitude reached the maximum during 176–212 ms and then decreased gradually. The differences between groups were obvious from 176–212 ms. The amplitude of the negative wave in AVH patients was lower than that in healthy controls and Non-AVH patients.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Brain topographies at Fz electrode of an interval 100–115 ms for auditory verbal hallucination (AVH) patients, non-AVH patients, and healthy controls.

Correlation Analysis in AVH Patients

Spearman correlation analysis between MMN amplitude and properties of resting EEG data in AVH patients is presented in Table 3. MMN amplitude was positively correlated with clustering coefficient and negatively correlated with characteristic path length in the delta, theta, beta and gamma band. Because MMN is a negative ERP component, positive loadings indicate smaller amplitudes while negative loadings indicate larger amplitude.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Spearman correlation analysis between mismatch negativity (MMN) amplitude and properties of resting network in auditory verbal hallucination (AVH) patients.

Discussion

Due to the complexity and variety of schizophrenia symptoms, as well as the interaction between symptoms, auditory-hallucination-related research is very difficult to implement. The pathogenesis of AVH in schizophrenia has not been fully understood, and no complete neurocognitive theory can explain it yet (3840). In this study, we explored the EEG changes from the resting-state to the task-related sate in schizophrenia patients with AVH by using the ERP method. We processed the resting EEG data by brain network analysis and compared the MMN induced by auditory oddball paradigm among AVH patients, Non-AVH patients, and healthy controls. In the resting-state, we found AVH patients had a higher clustering coefficient on delta and beta band and a shorter characteristic path length on all bands than Non-AVH patients and healthy controls. In the task-related state, AVH patients showed smaller MMN amplitude than Non-AVH patients and healthy controls. What’s more, MMN amplitude was positively correlated with the clustering coefficient, and negatively correlated with characteristic path length.

The clustering coefficient evaluates the number of connections between neighbors, which provides a measure of the local structure of the network (local segregation) and an indicator of the efficiency of information transfer (41, 42). The characteristic path length evaluates the number of steps from one node to another for all possible pairs of nodes and it is usually interpreted as a metric of information integration across the overall network, which refers to the capacity of the network to become interconnected and exchange information (43, 44). The larger baseline clustering coefficient in AVH patients may reflect higher and more segregated cortical activity and the shorter characteristic path length in AVH patients may reflect more active information exchange in the resting-state brain network.

Our results are similar to previous findings. At rest, Lee et al. (30) reported greater amplitude of beta oscillations in schizophrenia patients with treatment-refractory AVH compared those without AVH, with group differences localizing to left frontoparietal regions implicated in speech and language processing. A meta-analysis demonstrated that experiencing AVH is associated with increased activity in fronto-temporal areas involved in speech generation and perception (11). In schizophrenia patients, the bilateral temporal cortex was over-activated during AVH (15). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation could reduce intractable AVH by reducing the degree of activation of the left superior temporal gyrus (45). So, AVH patients may have an activated brain RSN.

In addition to schizophrenia, AVH in non-schizophrenia is also related to the activation of brain regions (46). In healthy individuals, high hallucination-prone participants reported high false alarms (i.e., reported a voice when it was not), while the temporal cortex showed high activation during these false alarms (47). To exclude the effects of delusions, negative symptoms, and antipsychotics, Remko van Lutterveld and coworkers (48) collected non-psychotic individuals with AVH to study AVHs by using resting-state fMRI. They found that in comparison with non-hallucinating controls, nonpsychotic individuals with AVH exhibit increased function in the temporal cortices (48). Therefore, AVH may be related to the activation of brain regions, and the activated brain RSN might be a neurobiological basis for auditory hallucination in schizophrenia.

In comparison with healthy controls and Non-AVH patients, AVH patients showed smaller MMN amplitude. These results are consistent with those of previous studies. AVH patients generally exhibited a lower duration MMN compared with healthy controls, particularly at F3 and Fz (49). And as measures of AVH increase, there is a corresponding decrease in MMN amplitude (50). Schizophrenia patients with clear, persistent AVH exhibited reduced MMN amplitude to duration than healthy controls and Non-AVH patients, while Non-AVH patients were not significantly different than healthy controls (4). AVH may contribute to MMN deficits in schizophrenia patients (49), and the MMN amplitude was correlated to the state and trait measures of AVH (19, 50), which further confirmed that AVH might affect the formation of MMN. MMN reflects a failure in higher-order auditory expectations (1617). So, AVH patients may have impaired higher-order auditory expectations than healthy controls and Non-AVH patients.

MMN was predominantly distributed in the frontal–central area, so in this study, we chose Fz electrode to extract MMN. The EEG topographies show that negative waves reached their maximum from 176–212 ms after stimulation and the largest negative wave appeared in the frontal–central area. This finding was in line with previous magnetoencephalography findings (51) and agree with the characteristic that MMN enhanced the processing in the central and frontal regions (52).

We found that AVH patients showed higher activity level in the resting-state brain network, and when external auditory stimuli occurred, AVH patients showed smaller MMN amplitude. What’s more, correlation analysis found that MMN amplitude positively correlated with clustering coefficient, and negatively correlated with characteristic path length characteristic path length in the delta, theta, beta, and gamma band. So, there may be some relation between anomalies in the two states. We speculated that the formation of AVH might occupy certain brain resources, which leads to an increased brain activation level and compete for the limited brain resources with external auditory stimuli (51, 53). Which then leads to relatively inadequate brain resources in the process of external auditory stimuli and MMN deficits. The activation of the auditory cortex in schizophrenia patients with AVH was reduced when receiving external speech (54), which might be caused by the competition between AVH and normal external speech for resources within the temporal cortex. Hubl et al. found that AVH lowered the N100 amplitude and changed the topography presumably due to a reduced left temporal responsivity (55), which indicates a competition between AVH and the normal stimuli for physiological resources in the primary auditory cortex, and that the abnormal activation of the primary auditory cortex may be a constituent of AVH (55).

This study has some limitations which need to be considered when interpreting the results. The biggest is the relatively small sample size, which limitied our statistical power to detect the smaller between-group differences and reduces the reliability of results. In this study, the values of clustering coefficient, characteristic path length and MMN amplitude in Non-AVH patients were between those in AVH patients and healthy controls. Unfortunately, no statistical difference was found between healthy controls and Non-AVH patients, which was inconsistent with previous studies (17, 56) and may be partly due to the small sample size. Increasing the sample size in future studies would provide opportunities to identify additional differences. In addition, patients collected in this study were untreated patients with serious auditory verbal hallucination. But we did not assess whether AVH actually occurred when the EEG data were recorded. This topic is the first step for us to study the specific symptoms of schizophrenia. In the future, we will further confirm our results by changing stimulus materials, enlarging the sample size, comparing the situation before and after medication, and collecting refractory AVH patients for a better understanding of the mechanism of AVH and schizophrenia.

Conclusion

In summary, our results indicate that AVH patients showed a hyper-activity in resting-state and may have an impaired higher-order auditory expectations in the task-related state than healthy controls and Non-AVH patients. And it seems reasonable to conclude that the formation of AVH may occupy certain brain resources and compete for brain resources with external auditory stimuli.

Data Availability Statement

All datasets presented in this study are included in the article/supplementary material.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Biomedical Ethics Board of the Second Xiangya Hospital. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

LT designed the study and wrote the protocol. YF managed the analyses, and QS wrote the first draft of the manuscript. XP, YS, JC, and LW contributed to conducting the study. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

This study was supported by the Fundamental Research Funds for the Central Universities of Central South University.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors thank all individuals who served as the research participants.

References

1. van Os J, Kapur S. Schizophrenia. Lancet (2009) 374(9690):635–45. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(09)60995-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Wen Y, Nie X, Wu D, Liu H, Zhang P, Lu X. Amusia and cognitive deficits in schizophrenia: is there a relationship? Schizophr Res (2014) 157(1-3):60–2. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.05.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Ford JM, Dierks T, Fisher DJ, Herrmann CS, Hubl D, Kindler J, et al. Neurophysiological studies of auditory verbal hallucinations. Schizophr Bull (2012) 38(4):715–23. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbs009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Fisher DJ, Labelle A, Knott VJ. The right profile: mismatch negativity in schizophrenia with and without auditory hallucinations as measured by a multi-feature paradigm. Clin Neurophysiol (2008) 119(4):909–21. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Shapleske J, Rossell SL, Chitnis XA, Suckling J, Simmons A, Bullmore ET, et al. A computational morphometric MRI study of schizophrenia: effects of hallucinations. Cereb Cortex (2002) 12(12):1331–41. doi: 10.1093/cercor/12.12.1331

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. David AS. Auditory hallucinations: phenomenology, neuropsychology and neuroimaging update. Acta Psychiatr Scand Suppl (1999) 395(s395):95–104. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb05988.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. David AS. The cognitive neuropsychiatry of auditory verbal hallucinations: an overview. Cognit Neuropsychiatry (2004) 9(1-2):107–23. doi: 10.1080/13546800344000183

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Alderson-Day B, McCarthy-Jones S, Fernyhough C. Hearing voices in the resting brain: A review of intrinsic functional connectivity research on auditory verbal hallucinations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2015) 55:78–87. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Thorsen AL, Johansson K, Loberg EM. Neurobiology of cognitive remediation therapy for schizophrenia: a systematic review. Front Psychiatry (2014) 5(5):103. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00103

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Northoff G. Immanuel Kant’s mind and the brain’s resting state. Trends Cognit Sci (2012) 16(7):356–9. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.06.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Jardri R, Pouchet A, Pins D, Thomas P. Cortical activations during auditory verbal hallucinations in schizophrenia: a coordinate-based meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry (2011) 168(1):73–81. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09101522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Dierks T, Linden DE, Jandl M, Formisano E, Goebel R, Lanfermann H, et al. Activation of Heschl’s gyrus during auditory hallucinations. Neuron (1999) 22(3):615–21. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80715-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Allen P, Laroi F, McGuire PK, Aleman A. The hallucinating brain: a review of structural and functional neuroimaging studies of hallucinations. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2008) 32(1):175–91. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Bentaleb LA, Beauregard M, Liddle P, Stip E. Cerebral activity associated with auditory verbal hallucinations: a functional magnetic resonance imaging case study. J Psychiatry Neurosci (2002) 27(2):110–5. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3956(01)00053-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Hoffman RE, Hampson M, Wu K, Anderson AW, Gore JC, Buchanan RJ, et al. Probing the pathophysiology of auditory/verbal hallucinations by combining functional magnetic resonance imaging and transcranial magnetic stimulation. Cereb Cortex (2007) 17(11):2733–43. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhl183

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Naatanen R, Paavilainen P, Rinne T, Alho K. The mismatch negativity (MMN) in basic research of central auditory processing: a review. Clin Neurophysiol (2007) 118(12):2544–90. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.04.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Erickson MA, Ruffle A. Gold JM. A Meta-Analysis of Mismatch Negativity in Schizophrenia: From Clinical Risk to Disease Specificity and Progression. Biol Psychiatry (2016) 79(12):980–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.08.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Rentzsch J, Shen C, Jockers-Scherubl MC, Gallinat J, Neuhaus AH. Auditory mismatch negativity and repetition suppression deficits in schizophrenia explained by irregular computation of prediction error. PloS One (2015) 10(5):e0126775. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0126775

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Fisher DJ, Smith DM, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Attenuation of mismatch negativity (MMN) and novelty P300 in schizophrenia patients with auditory hallucinations experiencing acute exacerbation of illness. Biol Psychol (2014) 100:43–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Todd J, Harms L, Schall U, Michie PT. Mismatch negativity: translating the potential. Front Psychiatry (2013) 4:171. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2013.00171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Rubinov M, Sporns O. Complex network measures of brain connectivity: uses and interpretations. Neuroimage (2010) 52(3):1059–69. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.10.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Honing H, Bouwer FL, Haden GP. Perceiving temporal regularity in music: the role of auditory event-related potentials (ERPs) in probing beat perception. Adv Exp Med Biol (2014) 829:305–23. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4939-1782-2_16

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Jardri R, Thomas P, Delmaire C, Delion P, Pins D. The neurodynamic organization of modality-dependent hallucinations. Cereb Cortex (2013) 23(5):1108–17. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Alonso-Solis A, Vives-Gilabert Y, Grasa E, Portella MJ, Rabella M, Sauras RB, et al. Resting-state functional connectivity alterations in the default network of schizophrenia patients with persistent auditory verbal hallucinations. Schizophr Res (2015) 161(2-3):261–8. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Wolf N. Dysconnectivity of multiple resting-state networks in patients with schizophrenia who have persistent auditory verbal hallucinations. J Psychiatry Neurosci (2011) 36(6):366–74. doi: 10.1503/jpn.110008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Oertel-Knöchel V, Knöchel C, Matura S, Stäblein M, Prvulovic D, Maurer K, et al. Association between symptoms of psychosis and reduced functional connectivity of auditory cortex. Schizophr Res (2014) 160(1-3):35–42. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gavrilescu M, Rossell S, Stuart GW, Shea TL, Innes-Brown H, Henshall K, et al. Reduced connectivity of the auditory cortex in patients with auditory hallucinations: a resting state functional magnetic resonance imaging study. psychol Med (2009) 40(7):1149–58. doi: 10.1017/s0033291709991632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Hoffman RE, Fernandez T, Pittman B, Hampson M. Elevated functional connectivity along a corticostriatal loop and the mechanism of auditory/verbal hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2011) 69(5):407–14. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.09.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Alderson-Day B, Diederen K, Fernyhough C, Ford JM, Horga G, Margulies DS, et al. Auditory Hallucinations and the Brain’s Resting-State Networks: Findings and Methodological Observations. Schizophr Bull (2016) 42(5):1110–23. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw078

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Lee SH, Wynn JK, Green MF, Kim H, Lee KJ, Nam M, et al. Quantitative EEG and low resolution electromagnetic tomography (LORETA) imaging of patients with persistent auditory hallucinations. Schizophr Res (2006) 83(2-3):111–9. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2005.11.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Koutsoukos E, Angelopoulos E, Maillis A, Papadimitriou GN, Stefanis C. Indication of increased phase coupling between theta and gamma EEG rhythms associated with the experience of auditory verbal hallucinations. Neurosci Lett (2013) 534:242–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2012.12.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Fisher DJ, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Auditory hallucinations and the P3a: attention-switching to speech in schizophrenia. Biol Psychol (2010) 85(3):417–23. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Opler LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (1986) 13(2):261. doi: 10.1093/schbul/13.2.261

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Fisher DJ, Grant B, Smith DM, Knott VJ. Effects of deviant probability on the ‘optimal’ multi-feature mismatch negativity (MMN) paradigm. Int J Psychophysiol (2011) 79(2):311–5. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2010.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Kasai K, Nakagome K, Itoh K, Koshida I, Hata A, Iwanami A, et al. Impaired cortical network for preattentive detection of change in speech sounds in schizophrenia: a high-resolution event-related potential study. Am J Psychiatry (2002) 159(4):546–53. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.159.4.546

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Xu P, Tian C, Zhang Y, Jing W, Wang Z, Liu T, et al. Cortical network properties revealed by SSVEP in anesthetized rats. Sci Rep (2013) 3(6148):2496. doi: 10.1038/srep02496

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Jennings JR, Wood CC. Letter: The epsilon-adjustment procedure for repeated-measures analyses of variance. Psychophysiology (1976) 13(3):277–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1976.tb00116.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Graux J, Bidet-Caulet A, Bonnet-Brilhault F, Camus V, Bruneau N. Hallucinations and negative symptoms differentially revealed by frontal and temporal responses to speech in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2014) 155(1-3):39–44. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Waters F, Fernyhough C. Hallucinations: A Systematic Review of Points of Similarity and Difference Across Diagnostic Classes. Schizophr Bull (2017) 43(1):32–43. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw132

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Jardri R, Hugdahl K, Hughes M, Brunelin J, Waters F, Alderson-Day B, et al. Are Hallucinations Due to an Imbalance Between Excitatory and Inhibitory Influences on the Brain? Schizophr Bull (2016) 42(5):1124–34. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbw075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Bullmore E, Sporns O. Complex brain networks: graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10(3):186–98. doi: 10.1038/nrn2575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Stam CJ, Jones BF, Nolte G, Breakspear M, Scheltens P. Small-world networks and functional connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. Cereb Cortex (2007) 17(1):92–9. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhj127

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fogelson N, Li L, Li Y, Fernandez-Del-Olmo M, Santos-Garcia D, Peled A. Functional connectivity abnormalities during contextual processing in schizophrenia and in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cognit (2013) 82(3):243–53. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2013.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Stam CJ, van Straaten EC. The organization of physiological brain networks. Clin Neurophysiol (2012) 123(6):1067–87. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2012.01.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Slotema CW, Blom JD, Sommer IE. [Treatment strategies for auditory verbal hallucinations]. Tijdschr Psychiatr (2014) 56(4):247–56.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

46. Diederen KM, Daalman K, de Weijer AD, Neggers SF, van Gastel W, Blom JD, et al. Auditory hallucinations elicit similar brain activation in psychotic and nonpsychotic individuals. Schizophr Bull (2012) 38(5):1074–82. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Barkus E, Stirling J, Hopkins R, McKie S, Lewis S. Cognitive and neural processes in non-clinical auditory hallucinations. Br J Psychiatry Suppl (2007) 51(51):s76–81. doi: 10.1192/bjp.191.51.s76

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. van Lutterveld R, Diederen KM, Otte WM, Sommer IE. Network analysis of auditory hallucinations in nonpsychotic individuals. Hum Brain Mapp (2014) 35(4):1436–45. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Fisher DJ, Smith DM, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Attenuation of mismatch negativity (MMN) and novelty P300 in schizophrenia patients with auditory hallucinations experiencing acute exacerbation of illness. Schizophr Res (2012) 139(1-3):237–45. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2012.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Fisher DJ, Grant B, Smith DM, Borracci G, Labelle A, Knott VJ. Effects of auditory hallucinations on the mismatch negativity (MMN) in schizophrenia as measured by a modified ‘optimal’ multi-feature paradigm. Int J Psychophysiol (2011) 81(3):245–51. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.06.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Duncan CC, Barry RJ, Connolly JF, Fischer C, Michie PT, Naatanen R, et al. Event-related potentials in clinical research: guidelines for eliciting, recording, and quantifying mismatch negativity, P300, and N400. Clin Neurophysiol (2009) 120(11):1883–908. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2009.07.045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Thonnessen H, Boers F, Dammers J, Chen YH, Norra C, Mathiak K. Early sensory encoding of affective prosody: neuromagnetic tomography of emotional category changes. Neuroimage (2010) 50(1):250–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.11.082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Woodruff PW. Auditory hallucinations: Insights and questions from neuroimaging. Cognit Neuropsychiatry (2004) 9(1-2):73–91. doi: 10.1080/13546800344000165

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Salisbury DF, Kuroki N, Kasai K, Shenton ME, McCarley RW. Progressive and interrelated functional and structural evidence of post-onset brain reduction in schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2007) 64(5):521–9. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.64.5.521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Hubl D, Koenig T, Strik WK, Garcia LM, Dierks T. Competition for neuronal resources: how hallucinations make themselves heard. Br J Psychiatry (2007) 190(1):57–62. doi: 10.1192/bjp.bp.106.022954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Gomez-Pilar J, Lubeiro A, Poza J, Hornero R, Ayuso M, Valcarcel C, et al. Functional EEG network analysis in schizophrenia: Evidence of larger segregation and deficit of modulation. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2017) 76:116–23. doi: 10.1016/j.pnpbp.2017.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizophrenia, auditory verbal hallucination (AVH), event-related potential, mismatch negativity (MMN), resting-state network (RSN)

Citation: Sun Q, Fang Y, Peng X, Shi Y, Chen J, Wang L and Tan L (2020) Hyper-Activated Brain Resting-State Network and Mismatch Negativity Deficit in Schizophrenia With Auditory Verbal Hallucination Revealed by an Event-Related Potential Evidence. Front. Psychiatry 11:765. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00765

Received: 22 March 2020; Accepted: 20 July 2020;
Published: 03 August 2020.

Edited by:

Shaohua Hu, Zhejiang University, China

Reviewed by:

Li Sun, Peking University Sixth Hospital, China
Long-Biao Cui, Chinese PLA General Hospital, China

Copyright © 2020 Sun, Fang, Peng, Shi, Chen, Wang and Tan. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Liwen Tan, tanliwen@csu.edu.cn

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.