Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 17 July 2020
Sec. Social Neuroscience

Disguised Emotion in Alexithymia: Subjective Difficulties in Emotion Processing and Increased Empathic Distress

  • 1Clinical Neuro-Psychology Lab, Department of Psychology, Chung-Ang University, Seoul, South Korea
  • 2Department of Psychology, Chung-Ang University, Seoul, South Korea
  • 3Department of Psychology, Korea University, Seoul, South Korea

Despite decades of speculation, many causal aspects that contribute to the heterogeneity of alexithymia still must be clarified. This study examined the extent of the alexithymia phenotype and its contribution to social function in the general population. In total, 200 participants (females = 111) completed the Toronto Alexithymia Scale-20 (TAS-20), multiple self-reporting questionnaires measuring emotion intelligence, empathy, hostility and impulsivity, and the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET). In the multivariate analysis, highly alexithymic individuals appeared to report subjective deficits in emotion recognition and regulation as well as increased impulsivity; however, their empathy skills were intact, and even the proneness to experiencing empathic distress with others’ suffering was increased among alexithymic individuals. We also compared the clinical and behavioral manifestations of highly alexithymic male and female subjects to those of each gender control group. As a result, in contrast to their subjective self-reports of emotion processing impairment, the RMET performance appeared to be preserved in alexithymic females; however, highly alexithymic males showed actual deficits in the emotion identification task. Future research needs to further refine the constructs of alexithymia to incorporate the phenotypic changes in affected individuals in relation to measuring instruments, the extent of empathic distress, and gender.

Introduction

Alexithymia (from the Greek stems, a = lack, lexis = word, and thymos = emotion, literally “lack of words for emotion”) was coined by Sifneos (1) to denote a stable, dimensional psychological construct that includes difficulties in identifying, describing, and distinguishing emotions and an externally oriented thought style (2, 3). Previous studies have shown that alexithymia exists on a continuum in the general population (4), and from 5 to 19% of the population has recently been found to have alexithymia (5, 6). Individuals with a high degree of alexithymia are reported to have more dysfunction in coping with daily stressors and to be more vulnerable to mental illnesses (710).

In the early years of research, these mental problems were known to be related to a reduced ability to recognize others’ emotions as well as to experience empathy. However, newly accumulated findings, to which various research methods have been applied, have begun to neutralize the hypothesis of social dysfunction in alexithymia (9). For instance, behavioral evidence has demonstrated that social cognition in individuals with alexithymia is quite intact (1114). In accordance with this finding, emotion perception deficits in alexithymia, once considered relatively reliable features, have become controversial issues (15, 16).

In particular, the works of Freyberger (17) and McDougall (18) raised the possibility that alexithymia could be a consequence of an inefficient strategy against overwhelming affects rather than a primary deficit of emotional experience. These psychodynamic approaches provided a more complex perspective that alexithymia may be related to immature defense mechanisms such as repression and/or avoidance of intense emotional experiences (1921). In addition, empirical evidence obtained using emotional or visceral stimuli have suggested relations between alexithymia and physiological hyperarousal (2224). For instance, the most recent research using odor stimuli (25) and a case study (26) have provided evidence that highly alexithymic individuals who seem to have limited emotional experience might, in fact, have an improperly elevated physiological response (i.e., arousal) to experimental stimuli.

An equally incoherent picture has emerged from research exploring the relationships of alexithymia with problematic emotional expressions, including impulsivity, aggression, or hostility (2732). For example, while gender has been reported as a significant variable influencing impulse control problems such as drinking, gambling, and suicide (33), little is known about whether gender contributes to alexithymia-related externalizing problems.

Indeed, gender is another issue of particular importance in alexithymia. Early research about this subclinical phenomenon was originally intended to illuminate the male-specific altered emotional profile: Levant (34) suggested the normative male alexithymia hypothesis. Levant believed that males under the pressure of gender socialization were discouraged from expressing feelings, namely, weakness or attachment feelings and that they consequently displayed alexithymia (34, 35). As research has continued, however, this undifferentiated emotional experience was found to not be exclusive to males (3638). The early findings that alexithymia was more prevalent in males (34, 39, 40) have not been consistently replicated in subsequent studies (6, 4144).

On the one hand, the mixed results in alexithymia research may partially stem from the research methods such as the nature of the clinical populations involved in the study or the self-questionnaire assessments (40). Indeed, many studies have been performed in such a way that self-reporting questionnaires are conducted for alexithymic subjects with various mental disorders such as depression, substance abuse, psychosomatic illnesses, or chronic pain (29, 30, 32). Therefore, there has been a need for general population-based research including behavioral experiments to generalize the clinical implications of alexithymia (45, 46).

In short, this study was designed to clarify the phenotypes of alexithymia. Considering the major methodological limitations in previous research, the aim of the present study was to examine whether highly alexithymic individuals in the general population have emotion processing deficits, including problems with (a) emotion recognition, (b) emotion regulation, and (c) empathy as well as to explore the salient gender-specific features of alexithymia. Multivariate analysis of covariance (MANCOVA) was also used to compare the different clinical features within each gender group. In particular, we assumed that considering gender in alexithymia research may shed light on understanding alexithymia-related emotions and externalizing behaviors (47). Therefore, we compared the behavioral and clinical manifestations of alexithymic subjects to gender control subjects in order to identify unique clinical phenotypes in highly alexithymic male or female subjects. In addition, regarding the limitations of previous research using self-report questionnaires (48), we also used a four-alternative forced-choice paradigm, the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET) (49, 50) to objectively assess the ability to decode others’ emotional experiences in individuals with alexithymia. We expect that the current research will help to comprehend the extent of the alexithymia phenotype and its implications that have previously been contradictory.

Method

Participants

The participants included 200 individuals aged between 19 and 32 years who agreed to participate in the study. Korean was the first language of participants, and none of them had a problem communicating or responding to the questionnaires. All participants had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity. We placed advertisements in an Internet community and on a Social Network System to recruit research participants with diverse ages and educational levels. This study was approved by the University Ethics Committee (1041078-201707-BRSB-148-01). All participants provided informed consent.

Materials

Five questionnaires and one experimental task were used in this study to measure the participants’ alexithymic tendency, emotional intelligence, and multiple dimensions of empathy, impulsivity, hostility, and behavioral empathy ability. The entire procedure took 50 min on average.

Korean Version of the 20-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20K)

The alexithymic traits were measured by the Toronto Alexithymia Scale (TAS-20K) (5153). This scale consists of 20 items rated on a 5-point Likert scale according to the symptom severity. TAS-20K consists of three factors: (1) difficulty identifying feelings (DIF). Seven items are used to identify emotions and the emotional distinction between emotional and physical senses. (2) Difficulty describing feelings (DDF). This factor indicates the ability to express subjective emotions to others and consists of five items. (3) Externally oriented thinking (EOT). This factor consists of eight items that assess the ability to express externally oriented thinking. The internal consistency for the original scale was reported to be 0.81 (53). Cronbach’s alpha for this study was 0.82.

Emotional Quotient

The individual ability to process emotional experience was assessed by the emotional quotient (EQ) (54, 55). This tool consists of five subscales: perception and expression of emotion, empathy, integrate emotion to facilitate thought, use of emotions, and regulation of emotions. A total of 50 items (10 items per subscale) are rated on a 5-point Likert scale, and a higher score indicates that an individual has higher emotional intelligence. Cronbach’s alpha of the adult emotional intelligence test was 0.96 (55). In this study, Cronbach’s alpha of EQ was 0.84.

Korean Version of the Interpersonal Reactivity Index

Multiple dimensions of empathy were measured by the Interpersonal Reactivity Index (IRI) (56, 57) which consist of four separated dimensions of empathy. This measure is a self-report questionnaire using a 5-point Likert scale consisting of 28 items and two factors. The first factor, cognitive empathy, consists of perspective taking (7 items: the tendency to immediately adopt the viewpoint of others) and fantasy (7 items: the tendency to imaginatively transpose oneself into the feelings and actions of fictitious characters). The second factor, emotional empathy, consists of empathic concern (7 items: the tendency to experience feelings of sympathy and concern for others’ misfortune and for their welfare) and personal distress (7 items: self-directed feelings of anxiety, discomfort, and unpleasantness in response to tense interpersonal situations). The internal consistency of the scale was 0.80, and the test-retest reliability was 0.76 (57). In this study, Cronbach’s alpha of IRI was 0.77.

Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory

Hostility was assessed by the Buss-Durkee Hostility Inventory (BDHI). The subscales measuring active aggressiveness were extracted from the original Hostility Inventory (58, 59) to determine externalized expression in alexithymic individuals. A total of 21 items were rated with a 4-point Likert scale. The internal consistency of this scale was 0.78, and the test-retest reliability was 0.79 in undergraduate students (59). In the present study, the internal consistency was 0.85.

Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11-Revised

Impulsivity was assessed by the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11-Revised (BIS). This scale was first developed by Barratt and has since been re-established as the BIS-11 (6062). The BIS-11 consists of 30 self-report questionnaires, including 11 revised scoring items, and each question is rated with a 4-point Likert scale. The score is calculated by summing the scores of each item considering the negatively scored items. A higher total score indicates greater impulsivity. The internal consistency coefficient (Cronbach’s alpha) of the Korean version was 0.78 (61). Cronbach alpha of BIS in the current sample was 0.81.

Korean Version of the Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test (RMET)

Based on a photograph of a person’s eyes, the participant must choose the most plausible answer to a question regarding the person’s feelings and intentions (49, 50). This task is used to assess the individual’s ability to perceive emotions as a basic function of social cognitive function. The RMET consists of 36 photographs, each of which show a photograph of a face with a rectangle cut out to display only the eye. The photographs are 5 x 2 inches in size and are displayed in monochrome. Four target words are presented (one correct word and three foils) with a picture of the eye. Each of the four words is presented at the four corners of the rectangle, with a number immediately below the word, allowing participants to select the word by pressing a number. There was no time limit for choosing words. The presentation and experimental stimulation were carried out using the PsychoPy program (63), which is free software program that can create and execute psychological experiments for the production and implementation of RMET tasks.

Procedure

When participants arrived at the laboratory, they first received an explanation of the overall study and agreed to study participation. Participants who signed the agreement to participate were given a brief explanation of the experimental computer operation by listening to the explanation of the research process. Then, self-report questionnaires, including demographic data questionnaires, were conducted through computer-generated screens. The RMET task was conducted after an explanation of how to perform the task was provided by the researcher.

When the RMET task started, the first page displayed a brief description of the assignment, along with the explanations on the page. After participants were instructed to choose only one word that best matched the stimulus presented, they proceeded to the next page and performed a practice item and then the full-scale experiment. The experiment was completed by conducting a debriefing of the experiment. The total duration of the RMET experiment was approximately 20 min, and no break was given during the test.

Statistical Analysis

We compared the demographic characteristics of alexithymic participants and non-alexithymic participants. In this study, we used a binary cutoff and divided participants as alexithymic (TAS-20K total score ≥ 52) and non-alexithymic (TAS-20K total score ≤ 51) according to previous studies that suggested that a TAS-20K total score ≥ 52 indicates the presence of moderate alexithymia (6466). Frequency analyses and descriptive statistics were used to examine demographic characteristics such as the age and education level of participants. The primary comparisons of RMET scores and clinical measures were conducted using MANOVA.

The data were also divided by gender in order to assess potential gender differences. Thus, a MANCOVA test with age as a covariate as well as correlation analyses were used to assess gender-specific features of alexithymia. Then, multivariate tests of the simple effects of gender group differences were conducted to identify the behavioral and clinical manifestations of highly alexithymic individuals within each gender group. Regarding previous research (65, 67), the highest 25th percentile (alexithymic males ≥ 52; alexithymic females ≥ 58 total TAS-20K score) was defined as the alexithymia group, and the lowest 25th percentile was defined as the non-alexithymic control group (non-alexithymic males ≤ 38; non-alexithymic females ≤ 42 total TAS-20K score). The statistical analysis was conducted using SPSS 25.0 for Windows (SPSS Inc.; Chicago, IL, USA).

Results

Alexithymia vs. Control Groups for All the 200 Participants

Demographic Characteristics

Among our participants, 89 (44.5%) were male, and 111 (55.5%) were female, resulting in a higher percentage of female participants. The mean age of the all participants was 23.07 years (SD = 2.68), and male participants were significantly older than females (t(198) = 3.78, P <.001). There were no significant group differences in education level (χ²(3) = 5.73, P = .13) (Table 3).

Of the 200 participants, 71 (14.2%) were classified as having alexithymia according to the previously recommended cutoff score. There were no significant differences in age and education level (t(198) = 1.49, P = .14 and χ²(3) = 0.90, P = .83, respectively) between the alexithymia (≥52 on the TAS-20K determined at screening) and non-alexithymia groups (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Group comparison of demographic characteristics by alexithymic tendency.

Behavioral and Clinical Characteristics

MANOVA was used to determine whether there was a significant difference in dependent variables (RMET, EQ, IRI, BDHI, BIS scores) between alexithymic and non-alexithymic participants (Wilks’ lambda = 0.78, F(12, 187) = 4.51, P <.001, η2p = .22) (Table 2). In this analysis, we demonstrated that there was a significant decrease in most of the EQ subscales (EQ perception and expression of emotion: F(1,198) = 31.64, P <.001, η2p = .14; EQ integration of emotion to facilitate thought: F(1,198) = 5.61, P = .02, η2p = .03; EQ use of emotions: F(1,198) = 5.53, P = .02, η2p = .03; EQ regulation of emotions: F(1,198) = 12.74, P <.001, η2p = .06) and IRI subscales (IRI perspective taking, F(1,198) = 4.16, P = .04, η2p = .02; and IRI higher personal distress, F(1,198) = 21.84, P <.001, η2p = .10) for the alexithymia groups. Alexithymic individuals also showed increased impulsivity (BIS, F(1,198) = 10.42, P = .00, η2p = .05). However, there were no significant group differences in RMET performance (F(1,198) = 2.26, P = .14, η2p = .01) or hostility (BDHI, F(1,198) = 0.18, P = .68, η2p = .00).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Comparison of measurements in alexithymic and non-alexithymic participants.

Gender-Related Behavioral and Clinical Features in Alexithymia

Differences in Alexithymia by Gender

There were no significant gender differences in education level, but age differences were found (males 23.8 ± 2.5 years vs. females 22.5 ± 2.7, t(198) = 3.78, P <.001). MANCOVA was used to assess the TAS-20K of the two gender groups adjusted by subject age (as a covariate) (Wilk’s lambda = 0.95, F(3,195) = 3.78, P = .01, η2p = .05).

As a result, the TAS-20K total score was significantly higher for female participants than for males (F(1,197) = 5.09, P = .03, η2p = .03). We found that there were significantly elevated scores in the TAS-20K DIF factor (F(1,197) = 9.34, P = .00, η2p = .05) in female subjects, and the TAS-20K DDF factor (F(1,197) = 3.54, P = .06, η2p = .02) among female subjects compared to males was marginally increased. However, no significant gender difference was observed for the TAS-20K EOT factor (F(1,197) = 0.39, P = .54, η2p = .00) (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3 Group comparison of demographic characteristics by gender.

Correlation Analysis in Each Gender Group

The results of correlation analysis with the TAS-20K score and other measurements in male and female subjects are shown in Tables 4 and 5. Male participants showed a negative correlation in the TAS-20K total score, TAS-20K DIF and TAS-20K EOT with behavioral tasks and RMET (r = −.25, P <.05; r = −.29, P <.01; r = −.21, P <.05). However, in female subjects, there were no significant correlations in TAS-20K scores and RMET performance.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4 Correlation analysis results with TAS-20K and other measurements in male participants.

TABLE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Table 5 Correlation analysis results with TAS-20K and other measurements in female participants.

Alexithymic Male Group vs. Non-Alexithymic Male Group

Since we found significant gender differences in the patterns of alexithymia, potential differences between the alexithymic and non-alexithymic group were tested within each gender group by calculating MANOVAs using the RMET, EQ, IRI, BDHI, and BIS scores as the dependent measures. Male subjects who scored ≥ 52 on the TAS-20K, which was the highest 25th percentile of TAS-20K scores for male participants, were assigned to the alexithymic group (n = 22). We assigned participants with the lowest 25th percentile of TAS-20K scores (≤ 38 on the TAS-20K; n = 23) to the non-alexithymic group. We excluded the remaining participants with subthreshold symptoms of alexithymia from the multivariate analyses to rule out their possible effects on the dependent variables (Table 6).

TABLE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Table 6 Comparison of measurements in alexithymic and non-alexithymic male participants.

The MANOVA results indicated significant group differences in alexithymic male and non-alexithymic male groups (Wilks’ lambda = 0.35, F(12,32) = 4.85, P <.001, η2p = .65). Alexithymic male subjects showed poorer performance than male controls in the RMET (F(1,43) = 4.55, P = .04, η2p = .10). Regarding self-questionnaires, there was a significant decrease in EQ perception and expression of emotion (F(1,43) = 18.55, P <.001, η2p = .30), EQ integration of emotion to facilitate thought (F(1,43) = 8.53, P = .01 η2p = .17), and EQ regulation of emotions (F(1,43) = 9.78, P = .00, η2p = .19) for highly alexithymic males. They also reported both higher IRI personal distress (F(1,43) = 21.68, P <.001, η2p = .34) and impulsivity than male controls (BIS score, F(1,43) = 10.60, P = .00, η2p = .20).

Alexithymic Female Group vs. Non-Alexithymic Female Group

To obtain a statistical distribution similar to the male group within the female group, we contrasted the behavioral and clinical measures of only those participants who scored in the highest 25th percentile (≥ 58 total TAS-20K score, n = 35) versus the lowest 25th percentile (≤ 42 total TAS-20K score, n = 29) on the TAS-20K within the female group by conducting a MANOVA (dependent variables: RMET, EQ, IRI, BDHI, and BIS scores) (Table 7).

TABLE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Table 7 Comparison of measurements in alexithymic and non-alexithymic female participants.

There were significant group differences in alexithymic and non-alexithymic females according to the MANOVA test (Wilks’ lambda = 0.46, F(12,51) = 4.90, P <.001, η2p = .54). We found that, within females, highly alexithymic subjects reported attenuated emotional intelligence (EQ perception and expression of emotion: F(1,62) = 28.96, P <.001, η2p = .32; EQ integration of emotion to facilitate thought: F(1,62) = 6.87, P = .01, η2p = .10; EQ use of emotions: F(1,62) = 10.71, P = .00, η2p = .15; and EQ regulation of emotions: F(1,62) = 8.81, P = .00, η2p = .12). In contrast, scores on the IRI personal distress (F(1,62) = 22.85, P <.001, η2p = .27) and BIS (F(1,62) = 4.32, P = .04, η2p = .07) were increased in the alexithymic group compared with the female control group. Intriguingly, there was no evidence of poor performance on the RMET in alexithymic female subjects (F(1,62) = 0.39, P = .54, η2p = .01).

Discussion

In the past decade, alexithymia, a multifaceted construct, has been an attractive topic in clinical research (68). However, previous findings were not valid enough to ensure a reliable exploration of the adverse effects of alexithymia because of methodological issues and sample heterogeneity (16, 69, 70). In the present work, we recruited participants from the general population to perform representative alexithymia assessments and the RMET to measure emotion recognition ability and then examined not only the clinical and behavioral features observed in alexithymia but also the gender-specific alexithymic phenotypes.

First, we found that self-reported difficulties in emotion recognition, integration, and regulation were salient for alexithymic participants compared to non-alexithymic controls, regardless of gender variance. Intriguingly, there were no significant reductions in most empathy measures for alexithymia. Rather, IRI personal distress scores measuring the level of emotional distress or reactivity in stressful situations were significantly increased in the alexithymia groups compared to the control groups.

We also completed multivariate analyses to identify the discriminative gender-specific features of alexithymia. As a result, symptom severity of alexithymia was shown to be greater in females than in males, which is consistent with previous findings using general populations (e.g., university students) (6, 42, 43). Additionally, even though the highly alexithymic females reported themselves as having difficulties in emotion processing, the RMET performance was preserved according to the within-gender analyses. The male participants with high alexithymia scores, who self-reported functional impairments in most areas of emotional intelligence except emotion utilization, revealed increased impulsivity as well as poor RMET performance. All findings were replicated via nonparametric statistical analyses (Kruskal-Wallis H test), and the following subsections provide more detailed descriptions.

Emotion Recognition (TAS-Difficulty Identifying Feeling, EQ-Emotion Perception and Expression, RMET)

Compared to the participants with little alexithymia, highly alexithymic individuals revealed increased subjective difficulties in emotion processing in accordance with previous research (48, 69). However, the behavioral experiment using the RMET showed different patterns according to gender. Alexithymic female subjects were found to maintain the ability of emotion recognition, while compared to non-alexithymic males, alexithymic males exhibited poor performance on the RMET compared to non-alexithymic males. Given previous reports on the preserved ability to decode facial expression in alexithymia (22, 23) as well as the female superiority over males on the RMET (71, 72), this gender difference seems to be a reasonable finding.

In particular, the finding of intact RMET performance in females is consistent with the results of a recent alexithymia study showing a normal performance of judgment for facial emotion blends (23). However, even with intact performance on average, emotional stimuli seemed to be challenging for alexithymic individuals; the alexithymic participants had a reduced viewing preference for facial eye regions. Looking at the eyes of the blended faces even increased performance errors. According to the authors, their findings reflected intolerance of intense emotion or attempts to avoid emotional stimuli (22) in alexithymic individuals.

The fragility of RMET performance related to alexithymia in males is revealed in the additional correlation analyses. In the case of male subjects, consistent with previous studies (11, 73, 74), alexithymia measured by TAS-20K appeared to be negatively correlated with an impaired ability to decode emotional experience as assessed by the RMET (TAS-20K total score r = −.25, P <.05; TAS-20K DIF factor r = −.29, P <.001; see Table 4). These correlations were not observed in the female subjects (Table 5). There has been a report that individuals with the most pronounced subjective difficulties in identifying feelings are more likely to have psychiatric symptoms (75). In addition to subjective discomfort, declined social cognition may pose further distress to social relationships among alexithymic individuals.

Emotion Regulation (EQ-Emotion Regulation, BDHI, and BIS)

One factor that makes the daily functioning of individuals with high alexithymia scores even more difficult is their inefficient manner of regulating negative emotion (76). Generally, subjective difficulties in emotion regulation are related to undifferentiated and unidentifiable negative moods (77). Along these lines, prior research has suggested possible links among alexithymia, emotion dysregulation, and impulsivity (32).

In our study, impulsivity was found to be prominent in highly alexithymic individuals. In this study, subjective difficulties in toning down emotional arousal were also markedly reported in the current alexithymic participants. The larger effect size in the male group (ηp2 = .20) than in the female group (ηp2 = .07) seems to reflect gender-related emotion processing. For instance, it has been suggested that female genders are more likely to ruminate over a variety of issues. In contrast, males may take action explicitly concerning these issues (78). In particular, alexithymic males who show unstable ability to regulate their emotions may easily engage in impulsive aggression (79) as a way to inappropriately vent their emotions (80). Of note, the impulsivity of male subjects was found to be closely related to high scores on the TAS-20K EOT factor (r = .49, P <.001; see Table 4), which has a negative correlation with EQ regulation of emotions (r = −.38, P <.001). The above results were not prominent in the female subjects. This result is in line with the findings of a previous study carried out by Lander, Lutz-Zois, Rye, and Goodnight (81). They showed that psychopathy was correlated with the TAS-20K EOT factor, representing a cognitive tendency to focus on one’s external rather than internal world. Indeed, an EOT style is related to social detachment and impaired cognitive processing (82). Thus, further research regarding the finely dimensioned profiling of alexithymia was necessary to uncover the underlying mechanisms responsible for alexithymia-related outcomes.

However, unexpectedly, there was no significant group difference in hostility, even though there was a positive relationship between BDHI and TAS-20K scores in male subjects. This seems to be because the scale used in this study reflects only direct and active hostility (59). Given that previous studies have reported a link between hostility and alexithymia (28), further study needs to identify the dynamics of alexithymia using a tool that can measure passive aggression, which involves the use of implicitly hostile words or behaviors as well as active forms.

Empathy (EQ Empathy, IRI Subscales)

Although many empirical findings have attempted to define a link between alexithymia and an empathy deficit (73, 80), the evidence for this association is quite obscure (11).

In the current general population, alexithymic male and female subjects appeared to show no deficits in most empathy measures, except for the personal distress scale of the IRI, relative to their respective gender control subjects. Rather, the scores on the personal distress scale were increased in both gender groups with highly alexithymic features. Correlation analyses also supported the following relationship between alexithymia and empathic over-arousal (83, 84): a higher tendency to suffer emotional distress in a stressful environment was associated with a higher level of alexithymia (males, r = .52, P <.001; females, r = .46, P <.001; see Tables 4 and 5).

Indeed, there have been reports on the strong evidence of coupling between alexithymia and greater emotion distress (8587), which is correlated with high emotionality (56). Beadle and colleagues (85) noted that observing others’ sufferings may lead to high levels of negative arousal in those who try to control their emotions in the absence of proper emotion regulation ability. Thus, it has been suggested that alexithymic individuals should disguise their negative feelings in order to protect themselves from aversive and overwhelming emotional experiences (20, 21).

This finding reminds of the concept of “secondary” alexithymia depicted by McDougall (18) as a defense mechanism. Some authors were interested in exploring the existence of two types of alexithymia (8890): type I, associated with little emotional arousal (affective alexithymia dimension, i.e., schizoid personality trait), and type II, in which emotion lability is present but disconnected from effective emotion regulation at the higher cognitive level (cognitive alexithymia dimension, i.e., borderline personality trait) (90, 91). According to this perspective, the observed features of alexithymia in our general population may be classified as type II, cognitive alexithymia. Indeed, since the TAS-20, which is used in this study, is currently regarded as reflecting the cognitive alexithymia dimension (90), this pattern of results seems plausible. Further empirical investigations adopting various screening tools are needed to illuminate the dominant type in the general population.

Limitations

Even though the current samples seem appropriate to reveal the characteristics of alexithymia [mean TAS-20K score of 59.30 ± 5.82 regarding the observed level of alexithymia in previous studies (e.g., mean TAS-20 score of 54.10 ± 1.49 by Berenson et al. (92) and 58.60 ± 12.84 by Pluta et al. (93))], this study has several drawbacks. First, the participants were relatively young. A larger sample that includes older adults should be considered for further research. Future studies may also benefit from increasing the clinical measures considering depression and anxiety, which are common in the general population, to provide abundant clues to the phenotypes of alexithymia. Additionally, because this study had a cross-sectional design, causality could not be established. Longitudinal studies with larger samples will be helpful for clarifying the associations among alexithymia, empathic distress, and externalizations. Lastly, as the results of the present analyses do not allow us to propose the underlying mechanisms of alexithymia-related features; further research is thus needed to construct an experimental design regarding latent profile analysis, thought suppression and defense mechanisms (94, 95).

Conclusions

The definition of alexithymia, which originally meant a deficiency in the ability to identify one’s own emotional state, was gradually extended to a subclinical state with a weakened ability to perceive others’ emotions or experience empathy. The purpose of this study was to examine the dominant features of alexithymia, focusing on emotion processing, empathic ability, hostility, impulsivity, and gender. In conclusion, alexithymic individuals demonstrated subjective difficulties in both emotion recognition and emotion regulation. We also found that alexithymia-related emotional frustration and personal distress may result in dysfunctional externalized expression such as impulsivity against unidentifiable and unpleasant emotion experiences. However, highly alexithymic females appear to underestimate their own ability to perceive the emotional status of others relative to their actual performance on an emotion recognition task. An interesting finding was that alexithymic individuals in this study revealed preserved empathy skills and even exhibited an increased proneness to experience empathic over-arousal with others’ suffering. Although empathy generally enhances our social connectivity, it is also true that a higher level of empathic distress has detrimental effects on efficient emotion processing for those who lack the ability to manage their own highly aversive feelings in response to others’ suffering. As a whole, these results suggest that those who are highly susceptible to negative emotional stimuli may avoid engaging with emotional experiences that they find disturbing and, especially in females, may underestimate their ability for emotion processing. Further research efforts should focus on the development of new nosological strategies to elucidate the underlying mechanism of alexithymia regarding actual emotion recognition ability, empathic distress, as well as gender factors.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/supplementary material; further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Chung-Ang University Ethics Committee. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

GN: conceptualization, formal analysis, methodology, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing. J-WH: conceptualization, formal analysis, methodology, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing, funding acquisition, project administration. HL: data curation, investigation. J-HL: resources, supervision.

Funding

This research was supported by the Brain Research Program of the National Research Foundation (NRF) funded by the Korean government (MSIT) (NRF-2017M3C7A1048040).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Sifneos PE. The prevalence of ‘alexithymic’ characteristics in psychosomatic patients. Psychother Psychosom (1973) 22(2):255–62. doi: 10.1159/000286529

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Sifneos PE. Affect deficit and alexithymia. New Trends Exp Clin Psychiat (1994) 10(4):193–5.

Google Scholar

3. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JDA. The alexithymia construct: A potential paradigm for psychosomatic medicine. Psychosomatics (1991) 32(2):153–64. doi: 10.1016/S0033-3182(91)72086-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Parker JDA, Keefer KV, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. Latent structure of the alexithymia construct: A taxometric investigation. Psychol Assess (2008) 20(4):385–96. doi: 10.1037/a0014262

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Lyvers M, Holloway N, Needham K, Thorberg FA. Resilience, alexithymia, and university stress in relation to anxiety and problematic alcohol use among female university students. Aust J Psychol (2019) 72(1):59–67. doi: 10.1111/ajpy.12258

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Mason O, Tyson M, Jones C, Potts S. Alexithymia: Its prevalence and correlates in a British undergraduate sample. Psychol Psychother (2005) 78(1):113–25. doi: 10.1348/147608304x21374

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Bernhardt BC, Singer T. The neural basis of empathy. Annu Rev Neurosci (2012) 35(1):1–23. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150536

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Berthoz S, Pouga L, Wessa M. “Alexithymia from the social neuroscience perspective" in The Oxford Handbook of Social Neuroscience. Decety J, Cacioppo J, eds. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2011). p. 906–34.

Google Scholar

9. Grynberg D, Luminet O, Corneille O, Grèzes J, Berthoz S. Alexithymia in the interpersonal domain: A general deficit of empathy? Pers Individ Differ (2010) 49(8):845–50. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2010.07.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Xu P, Opmeer EM, van Tol MJ, Goerlich KS, Aleman A. Structure of the alexithymic brain: A parametric coordinate-based meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2018) 87:50–5. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Gökçen E, Frederickson N, Petrides KV. Theory of mind and executive control deficits in typically developing adults and adolescents with high levels of autism traits. J Autism Dev Disord (2016) 46:2072–87. doi: 10.1007/s10803-016-2735-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Lockwood PL, Bird G, Bridge M, Viding E. Dissecting empathy: High levels of psychopathic and autistic traits are characterized by difficulties in different social information processing domains. Front Hum Neurosci (2013) 7:760. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Pandey R, Mandal MK. Processing of facial expressions of emotion and alexithymia. Br J Clin Psychol (1997) 36(4):631–3. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8260.1997.tb01269.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Vermeulen N, Luminet O, Cordovil de Sousa M. Campanella S. Impaired processing of emotional faces in alexithymia: Evidence from Event-Related Potentials (ERPs). Clin EEG Neurosci (2005) 36:226. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31817e41e6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bird G, Cook R. Mixed emotions: The contribution of alexithymia to the emotional symptoms of autism. Transl Psychiatry (2013) 3:e285. doi: 10.1038/tp.2013.61

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Grynberg D, Chang B, Corneille O, Maurage P, Vermeulen N, Berthoz S, et al. Alexithymia and the processing of Emotional Facial Expressions (EFEs): Systematic review, unanswered questions and further perspectives. PloS One (2012) 7(8):e42429. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0042429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Freyberger H. Supportive psychotherapeutic techniques in primary and secondary alexithymia. Psychother Psychosom (1977) 28(1/4):337–42. doi: 10.1159/000287080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. McDougall J. Theatres of the Body: A Psychoanalytic Approach to Psychosomatic Illness. London: Free Association Books (1989).

Google Scholar

19. Helmes E, McNeill PD, Holden RR, Jackson C. The construct of alexithymia: Associations with defense mechanisms. J Clin Psychol (2008) 64(3):318–31. doi: 10.1002/jclp.20461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JDA. What’s in the name ‘alexithymia’? A commentary on “Affective agnosia: Expansion of the alexithymia construct and a new opportunity to integrate and extend Freud’s legacy.” Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2016) 68:1006–20. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.05.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ziadni MS, Jasinski MJ, Labouvie-Vief G, Lumley MA. Alexithymia, defenses, and ego strength: Cross-sectional and longitudinal relationships with psychological well-being and depression. J Happiness Stud (2017) 18(6):1799–813. doi: 10.1007/s10902-016-9800-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Constantinou E, Panayiotou G, Theodorou M. Emotion processing deficits in alexithymia and response to a depth of processing intervention. Biol Psychol (2014) 103:212–22. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.09.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Fujiwara E. Looking at the eyes interferes with facial emotion recognition in alexithymia. J Abnorm Psychol (2018) 127(6):571–7. doi: 10.1037/abn0000361

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Kano M, Hamaguchi T, Itoh M, Yanai K, Fukudo S. Correlation between alexithymia and hypersensitivity to visceral stimulation in human. Pain (2007) 132(3):252–63. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2007.01.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Cecchetto C, Rumiati RI, Aiello M. Alexithymia and emotional reactions to odors. Sci Rep (2017) 7(1):14097. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-14404-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Smith R, Kaszniak AW, Katsanis J, Lane RD, Nielsen L. The importance of identifying underlying process abnormalities in alexithymia: Implications of the three-process model and a single case study illustration. Conscious Cognit (2019) 68:33–46. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2018.12.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Beresnevaité M, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. Assessing alexithymia and type A behavior in coronary heart disease patients: A multimethod approach. Psychother Psychosom (2007) 76(3):186–92. doi: 10.1159/000099846

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. de Schutter MAM, Kramer HJMT, Franken EJF, Lodewijkx HFM, Kleinepier T. The influence of dysfunctional impulsivity and alexithymia on aggressive behavior of psychiatric patients. Psychiatr Res (2016) 243:128–34. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.06.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Gerra G, Baldaro B, Zaimovic A, Moi G, Bussandri M, Raggi MA, et al. Neuroendocrine responses to experimentally-induced emotions among abstinent opioid-dependent subjects. Drug Alcohol Depend (2003) 71(1):25–35. doi: 10.1016/S0376-8716(03)00065-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Handelsman L, Stein JA, Bernstein DP, Oppenheim SE, Rosenblum A, Magura S. A latent variable analysis of coexisting emotional deficits in substance abusers: Alexithymia, hostility, and PTSD. Addict Behav (2000) 25(3):423–8. doi: 10.1016/S0306-4603(99)00010-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Honkalampi K, Saarinen P, Hintikka J, Virtanen V, Viinamaki H. Factors associated with alexithymia in patients suffering from depression. Psychother Psychosom (1999) 68(5):270–5. doi: 10.1159/000012343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Velotti P, Garofalo C, Petrocchi C, Cavallo F, Popolo R, Dimaggio G. Alexithymia, emotion dysregulation, impulsivity and aggression: A multiple mediation model. Psychiatry Res (2016) 237:296–303. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.01.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Rosenfield S, Mouzon D. Gender and Mental Health. In: Aneshensel CS, Phelan JC, Bierman A, editors. Handbooks of Sociology of Mental Health. Dordrecht: Springer Netherlands (2013). p. 227–96. doi: 10.1007/978-94-007-4276-5_14

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Levant RF. Toward the reconstruction of masculinity. J Fam Psychol (1992) 5(3-4):379–402. doi: 10.1037/0893-3200.5.3-4.379

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Levant RF, Hall RJ, Williams CM, Hasan NT. Gender differences in alexithymia. Psychol Men Masc (2009) 10(3):190–203. doi: 10.1037/a0015652

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Abrams G. Gender Roles, Alexithymia, Stigma, and Men’s Attitudes Towards Help-Seeking for Depression. Master’s thesis. Edwardsville (IL): Southern Illinois University at Edwardsville (2016).

Google Scholar

37. Fischer AR, Good GE. Men and psychotherapy: An investigation of alexithymia, intimacy, and masculine gender roles. Psychother Theor Res Pract Train (1997) 34(2):160–70. doi: 10.1037/h0087646

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Joukamaa M, Saarijärvi S, Muuriaisniemi ML, Salokangas RKR. Alexithymia in a normal elderly population. Compr Psychiatry (1996) 37(2):144–7. doi: 10.1016/S0010-440X(96)90576-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Kokkonen P, Karvonen JT, Veijola J, Laksy K, Jokelainen J, Jarvelin MR, et al. Prevalence and sociodemographic correlates of alexithymia in a population sample of young adults. Compr Psychiatry (2001) 42(6):471–6. doi: 10.1053/comp.2001.27892

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Salminen JK, Saarijärvi S, Äärelä E, Toikka T, Kauhanen J. Prevalence of alexithymia and its association with sociodemographic variables in the general population of Finland. J Psychosom Res (1999) 46(1):75–82. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(98)00053-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Brown S, Fite PJ, Stone K, Richey A, Bortolato M. Associations between emotional abuse and neglect and dimensions of alexithymia: The moderating role of sex. Psychol Trauma (2018) 10(3):300–8. doi: 10.1037/tra0000279

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Hamaideh SH. Alexithymia among Jordanian university students: Its prevalence and correlates with depression, anxiety, stress, and demographics. Perspect Psychiatr C (2018) 54(2):274–80. doi: 10.1111/ppc.12234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Pasini A, Chiaie RD, Seripa S, Ciani N. Alexithymia as related to sex, age, and educational level: Results of the Toronto Alexithymia Scale in 417 normal subjects. Compr Psychiatry (1992) 33(1):42–6. doi: 10.1016/0010-440X(92)90078-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Taycan O, Özdemir A, Erdoğan TS. Alexithymia and somatization in depressed patients: The role of the type of somatic symptom attribution. Arch Neuropsychiatr (2017) 54(2):99–104. doi: 10.5152/npa.2016.12385

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Franz M, Popp K, Schaefer R, Sitte W, Schneider C, Hardt J, et al. Alexithymia in the German general population. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol (2008) 43(1):54–62. doi: 10.1007/s00127-007-0265-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Loas G, Corcos M, Stephan P, Pellet J, Bizouard P, Venisse JL, et al. Factorial structure of the 20-item Toronto Alexithymia Scale: Confirmatory factorial analyses in nonclinical and clinical samples. J Psychosom Res (2001) 50(5):255–61. doi: 10.1016/S0022-3999(01)00197-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Chen P, Coccaro EF, Jacobson KC. Hostile attributional bias, negative emotional responding, and aggression in adults: Moderating effects of gender and impulsivity. Aggress Behav (2012) 38(1):47–63. doi: 10.1002/ab.21407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Parker JDA, Taylor GJ, Bagby RM. The relationship between emotional intelligence and alexithymia. Pers Individ Differ (2001) 30(1):107–15. doi: 10.1016/S0191-8869(00)00014-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Adams RB Jr., Rule NO, Franklin RG Jr., Wang E, Stevenson MT, Yoshikawa S, et al. Cross-cultural reading the mind in the eyes: An fMRI investigation. J Cognit Neurosci (2010) 22(1):97–108. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21187

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Baron-Cohen S, Wheelwright S, Hill J, Raste Y, Plumb I. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test revised version: A study with normal adults, and adults with Asperger syndrome or high-functioning autism. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2001) 42(2):241–51. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00715

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Bagby RM, Parker JDA, Taylor GJ. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia scale—I. Item selection and cross-validation of the factor structure. J Psychosom Res (1994) 38(1):23–32. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90005-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Bagby RM, Taylor GJ, Parker JDA. The twenty-item Toronto Alexithymia scale—II. Convergent, discriminant, and concurrent validity. J Psychosom Res (1994) 38(1):33–40. doi: 10.1016/0022-3999(94)90006-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Chung US, Rim HD, Lee YH, Kim SH. Comparison of reliability and validity of three Korean versions of the 20-Item Toronto Alexithymia Scale. Korean J Psychosom Med (2003) 11(1):77–88.

Google Scholar

54. Mayer JD, Geher G. Emotional intelligence and the identification of emotion. Intelligence (1996) 22(2):89–113. doi: 10.1016/S0160-2896(96)90011-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Moon YR. Emotional Quotient test for adults. Seoul: Moral Psychology LAB, Seoul National University (1999).

Google Scholar

56. Davis MH. Measuring individual differences in empathy: Evidence for a multidimensional approach. J Pers Soc Psychol (1983) 44(1):113–26. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.44.1.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Kang I, Kee S, Kim SE, Jeong B, Hwang JH, Song JE, et al. Reliability and validity of the Korean-version of Interpersonal Reactivity Index. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc (2009) 48:352–8.

Google Scholar

58. Buss AH, Durkee A. An inventory for assessing different kinds of hostility. J Cons Psychol (1957) 21(4):343–9. doi: 10.1037/h0046900

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Ko YI. The college students" anger expression modes in relation to depression and aggression. Doctoral dissertation. Busan: Pusan National University (1994).

Google Scholar

60. Go HJ, Kim DJ, Lee HP. A validation study of the suicide probability scale for adolescents (SPS-A). J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc (2000) 39(4):680–90.

Google Scholar

61. Lee SR, Lee WH, Park JS, Kim SM, Kim JW, Shim JH. The study on reliability and validity of Korean version of the Barratt Impulsiveness Scale-11-Revised in nonclinical adult subjects. J Korean Neuropsychiatr Assoc (2012) 51:378–86. doi: 10.4306/jknpa.2012.51.6.378

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Reise SP, Moore TM, Sabb FW, Brown AK, London ED. The Barratt Impulsiveness Scale–11: Reassessment of its structure in a community sample. Psychol Assess (2013) 25(2):631–42. doi: 10.1037/a0032161

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Peirce JW. PsychoPy—psychophysics software in Python. J Neurosci Methods (2007) 162(1-2):8–13. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2006.11.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Coriale G, Bilotta E, Leone L, Cosimi F, Porrari R, De Rosa F, et al. Avoidance coping strategies, alexithymia and alcohol abuse: A mediation analysis. Addict Behav (2012) 37(11):1224–9. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2012.05.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Milosavljevic B, Leno VC, Simonoff E, Baird G, Pickles A, Jones CRG, et al. Alexithymia in adolescents with autism spectrum disorder: Its relationship to internalizing difficulties, sensory modulation and social cognition. J Autism Dev Disord (2016) 46:1354–67. doi: 10.1007/s10803-015-2670-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Taylor GJ, Bagby RM, Parker JDA. Disorders of affect regulation: Alexithymia in medical and psychiatric illness. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (1999).

Google Scholar

67. Neumann D, Malec JF, Hammond FM. Reductions in alexithymia and emotion dysregulation after training emotional self-awareness following traumatic brain injury: A phase 1 trial. J Head Trauma Rehab (2017) 32(5):286–95. doi: 10.1097/HTR.0000000000000277

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Murphy J, Catmur C, Bird G. Alexithymia is associated with a multidomain, multidimensional failure of interoception: Evidence from novel tests. J Exp Psychol Gen (2018) 147(3):398–408. doi: 10.1037/xge0000366

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Cook R, Brewer R, Shah P, Bird G. Alexithymia, not autism, predicts poor recognition of emotional facial expressions. Psychol Sci (2013) 24(5):723–32. doi: 10.1177/0956797612463582

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Hiirola A, Pirkola S, Karukivi M, Markkula N, Bagby RM, Joukamaa M, et al. An evaluation of the absolute and relative stability of alexithymia over 11 years in a Finnish general population. J Psychosom Res (2017) 95:81–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Kirkland RA, Peterson E, Baker CA, Miller S, Pulos S. Meta-analysis reveals adult female superiority in “Reading the Mind in the Eyes Test”. N Ame J Psychol (2013) 15(1):121–46.

Google Scholar

72. Baron-Cohen S, Bowen DC, Holt RJ, Allison C, Auyeung B, Lombardo MV, et al. The “Reading the Mind in the Eyes” Test: Complete absence of typical sex difference in ~400 men and women with autism. PloS One (2015) 10(8):e0136521. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136521

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Moriguchi Y, Ohnishi T, Decety J, Hirakata M, Motonari M, Hiroshi M, et al. The human mirror neuron system in a population with deficient self-awareness: An fMRI study in alexithymia. Hum Brain Mapp (2009) 30(7):2063–76. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Samur D, Luminet O, Koole SL. Alexithymia predicts lower reading frequency: The mediating roles of mentalising ability and reading attitude. Poetics (2017) 65:1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.poetic.2017.09.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Kajanoja J, Scheinin NM, Karlsson L, Karlsson H, Karukivi M. Illuminating the clinical significance of alexithymia subtypes: A cluster analysis of alexithymic traits and psychiatric symptoms. J Psychosom Res (2017) 97:111–7. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2017.04.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Laloyaux J, Fantini C, Lemaire M, Luminet O, Larøi F. Evidence of contrasting patterns for suppression and reappraisal emotion regulation strategies in alexithymia. J Nerv Ment Dis (2015) 203(9):709–17. doi: 10.1097/nmd.0000000000000353

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Lyvers M, Kohlsdorf SM, Edwards MS, Thorberg FA. Alexithymia and mood: Recognition of emotion in self and others. Am J Psychol (2017) 130(1):83–92. doi: 10.5406/amerjpsy.130.1.0083

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Nolen-Hoeksema S, Girgus JS. The emergence of gender differences in depression during adolescence. Psychol Bull (1994) 115(3):424–43. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.115.3.424

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Teten AL, Miller LA, Bailey SD, Dunn NJ, Kent TA. Empathic deficits and alexithymia in trauma-related impulsive aggression. Behav Sci Law (2008) 26(6):823–32. doi: 10.1002/bsl.843

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. FeldmanHall O, Dalgleish T, Mobbs D. Alexithymia decreases altruism in real social decisions. Cortex (2013) 49(3):899–904. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2012.10.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Lander GC, Lutz-Zois CJ, Rye MS, Goodnight JA. The differential association between alexithymia and primary versus secondary psychopathy. Pers Individ Differ (2012) 52(1):45–50. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2011.08.027

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Vanheule S, Meganck R, Desmet M. Alexithymia, social detachment and cognitive processing. Psychiatry Res (2011) 190(1):49–51. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Eisenberg N, Strayer J. Empathy: A Social Psychological Approach. Dubuque, Iowa: Brown & Benchmark (1987).

Google Scholar

84. Cheetham M, Pedroni A, Antley A, Slater M, Jäncke L. Virtual milgram: Empathic concern or personal distress? Evidence from functional MRI and dispositional measures. Front Hum Neurosci (2009) 3:29. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.029.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Beadle JN, Paradiso S, Salerno A, McCormick LM. Alexithymia, emotional empathy, and self-regulation in Anorexia Nervosa. Ann Clin Psychiatry (2013) 25(2):107–20.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

86. Guttman H, Laporte L. Alexithymia, empathy, and psychological symptoms in a family context. Compr Psychiatry (2002) 43(6):448–55. doi: 10.1053/comp.2002.35905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Moriguchi Y, Ohnishi T, Lane RD, Maeda M, Mori T, Nemoto K, et al. Impaired self-awareness and theory of mind: an fMRI study of mentalizing in alexithymia. NeuroImage (2006) 32(3):1472–82. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.04.186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Bailey PE, Henry JD. Alexithymia, somatization and negative affect in a community sample. Psychiatr Res (2007) 150(1):13–20. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2006.05.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Bermond B, Bierman DJ, Cladder MA, Moormann PP, Vorst HCM. The cognitive and affective alexithymia dimensions in the regulation of sympathetic responses. Int J Psychophysiol (2010) 75(3):227–33. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2009.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Goerlich-Dobre KS, Bruce L, Martens S, Aleman A, Hooker CI. Distinct associations of insula and cingulate volume with the cognitive and affective dimensions of alexithymia. Neuropsychologia (2014) 53:284–92. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.12.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Bermond B, Clayton K, Liberova A, Luminet O, Maruszewski T, Bitti PER, et al. A cognitive and an affective dimension of alexithymia in six languages and seven populations. Cognit Emot (2007) 21(5):1125–36. doi: 10.1080/02699930601056989

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Berenson KR, Dochat C, Martin CG, Yang X, Rafaeli E, Downey G. Identification of mental states and interpersonal functioning in borderline personality disorder. Pers Disord: Theory Res Treat (2018) 9(2):172–81. doi: 10.1037/per0000228

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Pluta A, Kulesza M, Grzegorzewski P, Kucharska K. Assessing advanced theory of mind and alexithymia in patients suffering from enduring borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res (2018) 261:436–41. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Alkan Härtwig E, Crayen C, Heuser I, Eid M. It’s in the mix: Psychological distress differs between combinations of alexithymic facets. Front Psychol (2014) 5:1259. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Kang JI, Namkoong K, Yoo SW, Jhung K, Kim SJ. Abnormalities of emotional awareness and perception in patients with obsessive–compulsive disorder. J Affect Disord (2012) 141(2):286–93. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2012.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: alexithymia, emotion processing, empathy, personal distress, gender

Citation: Nam G, Lee H, Lee J-H and Hur J-W (2020) Disguised Emotion in Alexithymia: Subjective Difficulties in Emotion Processing and Increased Empathic Distress. Front. Psychiatry 11:698. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00698

Received: 22 April 2020; Accepted: 02 July 2020;
Published: 17 July 2020.

Edited by:

Giorgia Silani, University of Vienna, Austria

Reviewed by:

Cinzia Cecchetto, University of Padua, Italy
Max Karukivi, University of Turku, Finland

Copyright © 2020 Nam, Lee, Lee and Hur. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ji-Won Hur, j_hur@korea.ac.kr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.