Skip to main content

BRIEF RESEARCH REPORT article

Front. Psychiatry, 03 March 2020
Sec. Neuroimaging
This article is part of the Research Topic Progress in Translational Neuroimaging: Integrating Pathways, Systems and Phenomenology in Neurology and Psychiatry View all 20 articles

An fMRI Study of Adult Brain Cortical Activation Following Intensive Learning

Ferihan Ahmed-Popova,*Ferihan Ahmed-Popova1,4*Stefan Sivkov,Stefan Sivkov1,4Mariyan TopolovMariyan Topolov2Asen BeshkovAsen Beshkov3
  • 1Department of Anatomy, Histology and Embryology, Faculty of Medicine, Medical University - Plovdiv, Plovdiv, Bulgaria
  • 2Department of Pharmacology and Drug Toxicology, Faculty of Pharmacy, Medical University - Plovdiv, Plovdiv, Bulgaria
  • 3Department of Psychiatry and Medical Psychology, Faculty of Medicine, Medical University - Plovdiv, Plovdiv, Bulgaria
  • 4Research Institute, Medical University - Plovdiv, Plovdiv, Bulgaria

Functional imaging techniques, fMRI in particular, has given the possibility to investigate non-invasively the cognitive processes in healthy populations and different disorders concerning neuro-psychiatry, thus unfolding the concepts guiding diagnosis and patient management. Different brain structures seem to support different types of cognitive functions in particular learning and memory thus the neurobiological explanation of the retrieval of information is associated with knowledge of brain plasticity, memory circuits, synaptic neurotransmission and the modulation of glial cells. Consistent with fMRI investigations of memory systems we tested the dependability of a memory paradigm using heterogeneous memory stimuli in order to find the neurobiological basis that correlates with memory task performance. Our study resulted with statistical significant differences in brain activations across the block design contrasts in both occipital and temporal regions in 29 mentally healthy students during a memory paradigm performance after intensive learning. As functional magnetic resonance imaging has become an important and reliable tool for investigation of brain anatomy and its function in health and disease, it becomes clear that further research of neurobiological basis of cognitive and memory domains can clarify different diagnostic prototypes and thus explain the human brain impairments in neuropsychological patients, since these are characterized by various cognitive dysfunctions.

Introduction

Functional imaging techniques, fMRI in particular, has given the possibility to investigate non-invasively the cognitive processes not only in healthy populations, but in different disorders concerning neuro-psychiatry, thus unfolding the concepts guiding diagnosis and patient management (1, 2). In this sense, the original definition of translational neuroscience suggests that fundamental neurobiological knowledge can explain the brain functioning in healthy and pathological conditions (3).

Human brain seems to be a dynamic structure, which is continuously changing in response to experiences (4, 5). The application of different stimuli to the brain during experimental studies and the evaluation of functional imaging results support the increasing evidence of activity-dependent neuroplastic changes of the human brain across the life-span (6, 7). Studying the cognitive functions and memory systems in particular may uncover the specific processes associated with aging and brain development (8).

The present study aimed to investigate the cortical brain activations in response to a memory paradigm performance during a functional MRI scan after intensive learning in healthy individuals.

Materials and Methods

Subjects

We conducted an fMRI scanning of 29 mentally healthy, right handed students of Bulgarian origin (15 males and 14 females with a mean age of 20.0 ± 2.0). The handedness was determined using the Edinburgh handedness inventory (9). In order to examine the brain functioning after intensive learning, the experimental procedure was applied just after one month summer examination session with a duration of average 8 h intensive studying per day and mean exams final grade of 5.50.

Exclusion criteria included signs or a history of first degree relative with mental retardation, somatic disorders with neurological components; developmental, learning or psychotic disorders (schizophrenia, affective disorders, etc.) under DSM-IV (10); an identifiable neurological disorder (seizure disorder, head injury, multiple sclerosis etc.); a history of drug or alcohol abuse.

The study was carried out under the recommendations and protocol approval by the committee of “Research Complex for Translational Neuroscience” of Medical University of Plovdiv, Bulgaria. We obtained a written informed consent from all participants in accordance with ethical principles of the Declaration of Helsinki.

Experimental Procedure

The experimental procedure included a scan with a 3Т MRI system—GE Discovery 750w with a protocol of structural scan—Sag 3D T1 F-BRAVO FSPGR, slice thickness 1 mm, matrix 256 × 256, flip angle 10° and standard block design functional scan—2D EPI, slice thickness 3 mm, matrix 64 × 64, TR (repetition time)—3,000 msec, TE (echo time)—30, flip angle 90°.

Using a standard block design with total duration of 4 min, we defined four consecutive sets of “on” and “off” blocks. Participants were first presented with four consecutive pictures of the same theme—landscapes, portraits, anatomical images of internal organs and geometric figures denoted by specific mismatching nouns (seasons, personal names, internal organs, figures) (fixation—F part) with a duration of 3 s, followed by three of the pictures with questions (recall—R part) for memory evaluation (What was the word under this picture? Four possible answers.), for each of which the response time was 6 s. Subjects responded by pressing one of four possible buttons (two in each hand). The total duration of the active “on” blocks was 30 s. A rest—”off” block of 30 s followed each of the “on” blocks (Figure 1). The subjects were instructed to read carefully the questions and answer with a button press during the active conditions and to look at the central fixation cross without thinking of anything in particular for the resting conditions.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Graphical scheme of the experimental design of the fMRI memory paradigm.

fMRI Data Analysis

Functional MRI data analysis was done with SPM 12 (Statistical Parametric Mapping) software running on MATLAB R2015 for Windows. The preprocessing included five steps as follows: 1) realignment of the functional data for correction for head motion, 2) co-registration between the high-resolution anatomical image and the functional scans, 3) intra-individual estimation of spatial registration parameters based on the anatomical image, 4) transformation of the co-registered functional data to standardized MNI (Montreal Neurological Institute) space, followed by 5) spatial smoothing with a 6mm full-width-at-half-maximum Gaussian kernel.

We specified the first level analysis model, estimated and defined the parameters and t-contrasts for both active conditions vs. the passive condition (F > off, R > off), the first active condition vs. the second (F > R) and the opposite (R > F). The resulting contrast maps from each contrast and for each subject were then used in a second level random effects analysis for within group and between groups effects (males vs. females). Statistical significant level was set at P <0.05 Family Wise Error (FWE) corrected.

Results

fMRI Results

Our study resulted with statistical significant differences in brain activations across the block design contrasts in both occipital and temporal regions in males and females for two of the contrast maps—F > R and R > F.

Within both genders, the F > R contrast resulted in statistical significance of brain activations. We found significant clusters with peak activations in males in the region of left superior occipital gyrus (MNI coordinates −12, −88, 16) expanding to the right caudate nucleus (20, 16, 12) and left middle temporal gyrus (−54, −32, −2) (Table 1, Figure 2).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Males peak-level in a one-sample t-test of the F > R and R > F contrast maps after intensive learning.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Significant residual activations of the F > R and R > F t-contrasts in male and female subjects after intensive learning.

The clusters of female participants located in the right middle occipital gyrus (34, −80, 12), right putamen (18, 12, −2) and left caudate nucleus (−16, 14, 0) showed significant residual activations as a result of the F > R contrast (Table 2, Figure 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Females peak-level in a one-sample t-test of the F > R and R > F contrast maps after intensive learning.

Significant BOLD-signal differences were found in the R > F contrast with clusters in left postcentral gyrus and left supramarginal gyrus (−58, −20, 30) in males (Table 1, Figure 2). Females showed peak activation of the R > F contrast in the right precuneus (14, −58, 30) and right supramarginal gyrus (58, −24, 28) (Table 2, Figure 2).

Correction for multiple comparisons failed in finding significant residual activations for the F > R and R > F contrasts when applied in between-group analysis of males vs. females.

Discussion

Consistent with similar studies of functional magnetic resonance imaging performance of memory tasks we determined the reliability of a memory paradigm in an experimental investigation of adult brain cortical areas that correlate with memory encoding after intensive learning in healthy individuals from both genders. Despite we did not find significant distinctions in the brain activations between males and females for the F > R and R > F contrasts, the within group results showed statistically significant differences for both the contrast maps.

The occipital residual brain activations of the F > R contrast in both genders is explicable due to the presence of visual stimuli during the fixation part. The same contrast map showed significant differences in the peak activation with clusters in right caudate nucleus and left middle temporal gyrus in males and right putamen and left caudate nucleus in females. Consistent with other findings (11, 12), our results indicate that memory encoding during spontaneous memory tasks including visual stimuli involve both sensory and non-sensory brain areas. Thus, occipital–temporal lobe regions are considered to participate in associative recognition and item recognition (13), and their dynamic connectivity with other cortical areas, such as basal ganglia, is essential in the memory process (14). Several studies highlighted the relevance of the striatal gray matter volume for memory and working memory operations (15, 16). A research report of Bauer et al., 2015 revealed “the relevance of caudate for associative memory decline in the aging brain” suggesting that the “right striatum (putamen and caudate) was correlated with recognition accuracy”, whereas the “bilateral caudate was positively associated with associative learning accuracy” (17).

Furthermore, the significant peak activations of the R > F contrast resulted in clusters at the supramarginal gyrus in both males and females, although in women, due to the small number of clusters, the visualization of brain activity is almost impossible (Figure 2). The association brain cortex at the parietal areas of right and left hemispheres and their inferior regions in particular, including the supramarginal gyrus, mediate somatosensory integration, representation and organization of movements, motor attention and identification of motor gestures, as well as in visual word recognition (18). It is considered that left supramarginal gyrus plays a key role of the short-term memory network, preserving an abstract representation of information from serial orders, regardless of content information (1921). As functional magnetic resonance imaging has become an important and reliable tool for investigation of brain anatomy and its function in health and disease, it becomes clear that further research of neurobiological basis of cognitive and memory domains can clarify different diagnostic prototypes and thus explain the human brain impairments in neuropsychological patients, since these are characterized by various cognitive dysfunctions (22, 23).

However, our study has several limitations. Greater sample size is probably necessary to find statistical significant differences between the genders when comparing the BOLD-activations. We found similar imaging results in the active brain regions in one of our previous studies applying the same memory paradigm and same volunteers during a period of time when the students were not subjected to intensive learning (24). Therefore, further research and statistical analysis is indispensable for evaluation of the specific differences in cortical activation areas between both conditions in order to contribute to the disclosure of the way brain works in health and disease.

Conclusion

The results of the present study support the recent investigations on brain plasticity and memory systems as an experimental approach in the multidisciplinary essence of translational neuroscience. Despite the limitations that should be considered in future research, the present study corroborates the findings that human brain is a dynamic structure, which reacts with complex interactions in both cortical and subcortical regions with regard to memory stimuli and tiny areas with subtle activations as supramarginal gyrus seem to play an important role in short-term memory circuits. Considering the results obtained after intensive learning and previous similar studies of brain activations at rest, we suppose that a further in-depth comparative analysis would enrich the data on how the human brain responds to stress, which in turn would be important in evaluating brain plasticity and function in disease.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets generated for this study are available on request to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by Committee on Scientific Ethics, “Research Complex for Translational Neuroscience”, Medical University of Plovdiv, Bulgaria. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

FA-P and SS designed and performed the experimental study. FA-P and SS processed the results and wrote the manuscript. MT and AB managed the literature searches. All authors have approved the final manuscript after revision.

Funding

This work was made possible as a result from a project of the Medical University of Plovdiv, for “Research Complex for Translational Neuroscience”, established with a grant from Operative Program “Competitiveness of Bulgarian Economy”.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Matthews PM, Hampshire A. Clinical concepts emerging from fMRI functional connectomics. Neuron (2016) 91:511–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.07.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Watanabe T, Sasaki Y, Shibata K, Kawato M. Advances in fMRI real-time neurofeedback. Trends Cognit Sci (2017) 21(12):997–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.09.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Stoyanov DS. Key Developments in translational neuroscience: an update. Balkan Med J (2017) 34(6):485–6. doi: 10.4274/balkanmedj.2017.6.0002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Taubert M, Draganski B, Anwander A, Müller K, Horstmann A, Villringer A, et al. Dynamic properties of human brain structure: learning-related changes in cortical areas and associated fiber connections. J Neurosci (2010) 30(35):11670–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2567-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Draganski B, Gaser C, Kempermann G, Kuhn HG, Winkler J, Büchel C, et al. Temporal and spatial dynamics of brain structure changes during extensive learning. J Neurosci (2006) 26(23):6314–7. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4628-05.2006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Koch K, Reess TJ, Rus OG, Zimmer C. Extensive learning is associated with gray matter changes in the right hippocampus. Neuroimage (2016) 125:627–32. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.056

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Klamer S, Milian M, Erb M, Rona S, Lerche H, Ethofer T. Face-name association task reveals memory networks in patients with left and right hippocampal sclerosis. Neuroimage: Clin (2017) 14:174–82. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2017.01.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Lövdén M, Wenger E, Martensson J, Lindenberger U, Bäckman L. Structural brain plasticity in adult learning and development. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2013) 37:2296–310. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia (1971) 9:97–113. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. ed. 4. Washington, DC, APA: American Psychiatry Press Inc. (1994).

Google Scholar

11. Golby AJ, Poldrack RA, Brewer JB, Spencer D, Desmond JE, Aron AP, et al. Material-specific lateralization in the medial temporal lobe and prefrontal cortex during memory encoding. Brain (2001) 124:1841–54. doi: 10.1093/brain/124.9.1841

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Urner M, Schwarzkopf DS, Friston K, Rees G. Early visual learning induces long-lasting connectivity changes during rest in the human brain. NeuroImage (2013) 77:148–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.03.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Yonelinas AP, Hopfinger JB, Buonocore MH, Kroll NEA, Baynes K. Hippocampal, parahippocampal and occipital-temporal contributions to associative and item recognition memory: an fMRI study. NeuroReport (2001) 12(2):359–63. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200102120-00035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Ding L, Gold JI. Caudate encodes multiple computations for perceptual decisions. J Neurosci (2010) 30:15747–59. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2894-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Jeong W, Chung CK, Kim JS. Episodic memory in aspects of large-scale brain networks. Front Hum Neurosci (2015) 9:454. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Dimond D, Perry R, Iaria G, Bray S. Visuospatial short-term memory and dorsal visual gray matter volume. Cortex (2019) 113:184–90. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.12.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Bauer E, Toepper M, Gebhardt H, Gallhofer B, Sammer G. The significance of caudate volume for age-related associative memory decline. Brain Res (2015) 1622:137–48. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2015.06.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Hedden T, Schultz AP, Rieckmann A, Mormino EC, Johnson KA, Sperling RA, et al. Multiple brainmarkers are linked to age-related variation in cognition. Cereb Cortex (2016) 26(4):1388–400. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhu238

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Russ MO, Mack W, Grama CR, Lanfermann H, Knopf M. Enactment effect in memory: evidence concerning the function of the supramarginal gyrus. Exp Brain Res (2003) 149(4):497–504. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1398-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Stoeckel C, Gough PM, Watkins KE, Devlin JT. Supramarginal gyrus involvement in visual word recognition. Cortex (2009) 45(9):1091–6. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2008.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Guidali G, Pisoni A, Bolognini N, Papagno C. Keeping order in the brain: the supramarginal gyrus and serial order in short-term memory. Cortex (2019) 119:89–99. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2019.04.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Stoyanov D. A linkage of mind and brain: towards translational validity between neurobiology and psychiatry. BioMed Rev (2011) 22:65–76. doi: 10.14748/bmr.v22.36

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Beshkov A, Topolov M, Ahmed-Popova F, Sivkov S. A review of neuroimaging studies on working memory paradigms in patients with bipolar disorder. Curr Top Med Chem (2018) 18(21):1883–92. doi: 10.2174/1568026619666181130135703

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Popova F, Kovacheva A, Garov P, Sivkov S, Kandilarova S, Sirakov N, et al. Adult brain activation during visual learning and memory tasks. Exp approach to Trans Neurosci J Eval Clin Pract (2018) 24(4):864–8. doi: 10.1111/jep.12911

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: functional MRI, memory paradigm, brain activation, intensive learning, cognition

Citation: Ahmed-Popova F, Sivkov S, Topolov M and Beshkov A (2020) An fMRI Study of Adult Brain Cortical Activation Following Intensive Learning. Front. Psychiatry 11:115. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2020.00115

Received: 29 July 2019; Accepted: 10 February 2020;
Published: 03 March 2020.

Edited by:

Paolo Brambilla, University of Milan, Italy

Reviewed by:

Stefan Martin Golaszewski, Paracelsus Medical University, Austria
Dora Zlatareva, Medical University-Sofia, Bulgaria

Copyright © 2020 Ahmed-Popova, Sivkov, Topolov and Beshkov. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ferihan Ahmed-Popova, ferylin@abv.bg

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.