Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychiatry, 02 August 2019
Sec. Psychological Therapy and Psychosomatics
This article is part of the Research Topic Placebo and Nocebo Effects in Psychiatry and Beyond View all 39 articles

Placebo and Nocebo Effects in Patients With Takotsubo Cardiomyopathy and Heart-Healthy Controls

Elisabeth Olliges*&#x;Elisabeth Olliges1*†Simon Schneider&#x;Simon Schneider2†Georg SchmidtGeorg Schmidt2Daniel Sinnecker,Daniel Sinnecker2,3Alexander MüllerAlexander Müller2Christof Burgdorf,Christof Burgdorf4,5Siegmund BraunSiegmund Braun6Stefan HoldenriederStefan Holdenrieder6Hansjrg EbellHansjörg Ebell7Karl-Heinz Ladwig,Karl-Heinz Ladwig8,9Karin Meissner,&#x;Karin Meissner1,10‡Joram Ronel,&#x;Joram Ronel8,11‡
  • 1Institute of Medical Psychology, Medical Faculty, LMU Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 2Medizinische Klinik und Poliklinik I, Klinikum rechts der Isar, Technische Universitaet Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 3German Centre for Cardiovascular Research (DZHK), partner site Munich Heart Alliance, Munich, Germany
  • 4Klinik für Herz- und Kreislauferkrankungen, Deutsches Herzzentrum Munich, Technische Universitaet Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 5Department of Cardiology, Heart and Vascular Centre Bad Bevensen, Bad Bevensen, Germany
  • 6Institute of Laboratory Medicine, Deutsches Herzzentrum Munich, Technische Universitaet Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 7Private Practitioner, Munich, Germany
  • 8Department of Psychosomatic Medicine and Psychotherapy, Klinikum rechts der Isar, Technische Universitaet Munich, Munich, Germany
  • 9Department of Epidemiology II, Helmholtz Zentrum, Munich, Germany
  • 10Division of Health Promotion, Coburg University of Applied Sciences, Coburg, Germany
  • 11Department of Psychosomatic Medicine, Klinik Barmelweid AG, Barmelweid, Switzerland

The etiology of takotsubo cardiomyopathy (TTC)—a rare, reversible, and acquired form of cardiac diseases—is not yet fully explained. An exaggerated activation of the sympathetic-nervous-system (SNS) following stressful psychosocial life events is discussed to be of key importance. In this experimental study, we tested whether TTC patients, compared to heart-healthy controls, respond more strongly to supporting placebo interventions and stressful nocebo interventions targeting cardiac function. In a single experimental session, 20 female TTC patients and 20 age matched (mean age 61.5 years, ± 12.89) catheter-confirmed heart-healthy women were examined. Saline solution was administered three times i.v. to all participants, with the verbal suggestion they receive an inert substance with no effects on the heart (neutral condition), a drug that would support cardiac functions (positive condition), and a drug that would burden the heart (negative condition). Systolic and diastolic blood pressure (DBP/SBP), heart rate (HR), endocrine markers cortisol (µg/dl), copeptin (pmol/l), and subjective stress ratings (SUD) were assessed to examine alterations of the SNS and the hypothalamic–pituitary–adrenal axis (HPA). Before and after each intervention SUD was rated. One pre and three post serum cortisol and copeptin samples were assessed, and a long-term electrocardiogram as well as non-invasive, continuous blood pressure was recorded. The study design elucidated a significant increase of SUD levels as a response to the nocebo intervention, while perceived stress remained unaffected during the preceding neutral and positive interventions. Increasing SUD levels were accompanied by higher SBP and an anticipatory increase of HR shortly prior to the nocebo intervention. SBP increased also as a response to positive verbal suggestions (Bonferroni-corrected p-values > .05). Alterations of cortisol and copeptin due to the interventions and significant placebo effects failed to appear. Interestingly no differences between TCC patients and controls could be found.These findings do not support the assumption of an exaggerated activation of the SNS as a discriminatory factor for TTC. Since especially the nocebo intervention revealed negative subjective and objective effects, our results underscore the urgent need to consider carefully the impact of verbal suggestions in the interaction with cardiac patients in daily clinical routine. This study is registered at the Deutsches Register Klinischer Studien (DRKS00009296).

Introduction

Placebo effects are conceptualized as neurobiological phenomena, resulting from the positive psychosocial context, a treatment is embedded in. Correspondingly, a negative psychosocial context may induce negative clinical outcomes, referred to as “nocebo effects.” The current state of research suggests that placebo and nocebo effects are mediated by explicit expectations and shaped by different means; social observational learning (1), classical conditioning (2), and verbal suggestions (3). The doctor’s verbal suggestions inducing positive or negative outcome expectations are an important feature for placebo and nocebo effects (47). Placebo effects on functions linked to the central nervous system (CNS) such as pain or Parkinson’s disease have been extensively investigated and their mechanisms are well understood (6, 8). For example, placebo analgesia is often associated with the release of endogenous opioids, whereas placebo-induced motor improvement in patients with Parkinson’s disease could be connected to the release of dopamine in the dorsal striatum (8, 9). Within several studies, it has been demonstrated that placebo interventions can also affect peripheral organ functions (e.g., pulmonary and cardiovascular functions) controlled by the autonomic nervous system (ANS) (1013), but results in this neglected area of placebo research are often ambiguous. For example, significant effects of verbal suggestions specifically targeting the diameter of coronary arteries could be observed during a coronary angiography. Here participants received intracoronary saline injections, together with the verbal suggestion the “drug” would widen the heart vessels and improve cardiac perfusion. Interestingly, the verbal suggestion led to coronary vasoconstriction accompanied by chest pain reduction. Acute psychological burden, HR and BP did not change significantly. Authors concluded that the coronary vasoconstriction was not caused by increased stress levels but by a reduction of sympathetic outflow and/or increase of parasympathetic outflow to the cardiac vessels (12).

Takotsubo cardiomyopathy (TTC) (also referred to as “stress-induced cardiomyopathy” or “broken heart syndrome”) is considered a very rare, reversible, and acquired form of primary myocardial disorders (1416). TTC is characterized by an acute, functional disturbance in the contraction of the myocardium, primarily affecting mid and apical areas of the left ventricle, accompanied by symptoms and signs rather similar to those of the acute phase of a myocardial infarction (MI) (e.g., chest pain, dyspnea or alterations in the electrocardiogram or cardiac markers such as troponin), while the coronary arteries are mostly unaffected in TTC patients (17). Medeiros and colleagues found a similar impairment of systolic and diastolic function in TTCs and post MI patients, despite of their completely different pathophysiology (18). An increased sympathetic tone as well as a concomitant enhanced myocyte and microvascular catecholamine sensitivity is considered to increase the individual’s vulnerability and may therefore serve as a risk factor for the development of TTC (19).

Approximately 0.07–2.3% of patients, suspected with an acute coronary syndrome (ACS), are diagnosed with TTC after cardiac catheter examination, with almost 90% being postmenopausal women (14, 2024). The etiology of TTC is not yet fully explained. A dysfunctional presentation and processing of external physiological or psychosocial stressors are assumed to initiate an inadequate activation of the sympathetic nervous system, and therefore a pathophysiological cascade of the TTC-patient’s myocardium (23, 25, 26). Triggers are not necessarily negative. A very small percentage of TTC patients (approximately 4%) experience a positive life event (e.g. a birthday party or the child’s wedding), prior to the onset of the disease. It is supposed that, positive as well as negative events are proceeding through analogous signal pathways in the central nervous system (26, 27).

Further, data on the recurrence of TTC varies, but relapses are not infrequent with approximately 1.5% to 2.4% per patient-year and a rate of 5% to 11.4% within the first 4 years (25, 2830). Simultaneously, several studies found a significantly higher mortality rate in TTCs in comparison with a control group of the same age and sex (25, 31, 32). Apart from cardiovascular events, this appears to be due to an increased prevalence of non-cardiac comorbidities, which suggests a persistent pathology, presumably referring to an alteration of the sympathetic system, inherent in TTC patients (28, 3336).

Based on these considerations, we investigated whether the cardiac regulation of TTC patients reacts more sensitively to positive and negative external stimuli than that of heart-healthy individuals. In a case–control study, we examined the cardiovascular response to placebo and nocebo interventions targeting the cardiac functions in 20 TTC patients on average two years after disease onset and 20 matched heart-healthy individuals. We hypothesized that in TTC patients cardiovascular and perceived stress parameters would be stronger regulated as a response to placebo and nocebo interventions compared to healthy individuals.

Material and Methods

Sample

This case–control study (controlled for age) included 20 women, diagnosed with TTC, and 20 volunteers (CG) free of significant coronary artery disease (vessel stenosis ≤30%, confirmed via heart catheterization in the past) (see Table 1). TTC patients were diagnosed regarding Mayo Clinic’s diagnostic criteria for Takotsubo Cardiomyopathy. These are: 1) transient hypokinesis, akinesis, or dyskinesis of the left ventricular mid segments with or without apical involvement; regional wall motion abnormalities extending beyond a single epicardial vascular distribution, with a stressful trigger often, but not always present, 2) absence of obstructive coronary disease or angiographic evidence of acute plaque rupture, 3) new electrocardiographic abnormalities (either ST-segment elevation and/or T-wave inversion) or modest elevation in cardiac troponin, 4) absence of a pheochromocytoma or myocarditis (37). Participants with significantly decreased ejection fraction (<55%) or low German proficiency, were excluded from the study. The mean time interval between the episode of TTC and the participation in the study was 24.61 months (±22.8). A total of 40 eligible women diagnosed at “Deutsches Herzzentrum” and “Medizinische Klinik und Poliklinik I, Klinikum rechts der Isar,” Technical University, Munich, were enrolled in the study and contacted via mail and followed-up by a phone call. The study protocol was approved by the institutional review board. All participants received 50 € compensation, borne by the Deutsches Herzzentrum, Munich.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1 Baseline characteristics.

Endpoints

The following parameter were chosen as primary endpoints in order to indicate alterations of the SNS and the HPA, the main peripheral pathways of the human stress system: Non-invasive continuous systolic (SBP) and diastolic blood pressure (DBP) as well as heart rate (HR) measured with Finapress Nova device (Finapres Medical Systems B.V.), as established indicators for the adaptive response to altered environmental, bio-psycho-social stimuli. Both cardiac functions are self-modifiable to attune the delivery of oxygenated blood by augmenting the beating frequency, respectively the pressure, with which the blood is pumped through the arteries (38). In addition, perceived stress was assessed by the “subjective units of distress scale” (SUD), an 11-point numeric rating scale from 0 (no stress) to 10 (maximal stress). Furthermore, blood samples were taken to measure cortisol (µg/dl) and copeptin (pmol/l). Cortisol has been shown to be proportionate to the degree of stress on a peripheral level. To gain a more direct insight in the stress level on the cerebral level, copeptin was chosen as a second humoral stress marker. Copeptin, a pre-hormone of vasopressin, is considered a relevant marker for acute, endogenous stress, especially associated with cardiological diseases (e.g. myocardial infarctions) (3942).

Procedure

The experiment was performed in the Department of Cardiology at Klinikum rechts der Isar, between 10:00 am and 1:00 pm in a cardiological outpatient lab. Participants were examined at different time points with no contact to each other; therefore an exchange of experiences during the experiment was not possible and no “placebo-by-proxy” effects could emerge (43). After obtaining informed consent, participants received a transthoracic echocardiography to assess standard parameters [e.g., septum thickness (mm) and ejection fraction (%)]. Thereafter, the study coordinator connected the participants to the Finapress Nova device (Finapres Medical Systems B.V.) and activated the continuous measurement of cardiovascular parameters [blood pressure (mmHg), heart rate (bpm)] while the attending physician established vascular access and took the first blood sample [cortisol (µg/dl) and copeptin (pmol/l)] (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1 Procedure. M, Measurement; I, Intervention; HR, heart rate; RR, blood pressure.

At the beginning of the experiment (M0), the participants were asked to rate their perceived stress (SUD). After a baseline measurement of approximately 5 min, during which the cardiological parameters were continuously assessed, the first sham-intervention took place (I1). Here, the physician administered 2 ml of 0.9% physiological saline solution (NaCl) intravenously together with a standardized verbal neutral suggestion that the intravenously administered solution would not cause any bodily changes “similar to taking a sip of water.” Thereafter, the first post-intervention measurement of physiological parameters was performed (approximately 5 min). At the end, patients were asked again to rate their level of distress on an 11-point numeric rating scale (from 0 = no stress to 10 = maximal stress) and blood samples were taken for a second time (M2). Subsequently, the same procedure was performed for the placebo and the nocebo interventions: after a pre-intervention measurement of physiological parameters of approximately 5 min patients were asked to rate perceived stress levels (SUD) (M3). Next, 2 ml NaCl was administered intravenously accompanied by a standardized verbal positive suggestion that the intervention would “strengthen the heart,” “blood pressure and heart rate would decrease,” and “breathing would become easier” as the body would be “better supplied with oxygen” (I3). Then another post-intervention measuring period (approximately 5 min) was obtained with continuous measurement of physiological parameters. At the end of this period, distress levels were assessed and blood samples were taken (M4). Again after a pre-intervention period of approximately 5 min, stress ratings (SUD) were assessed again (M5). Finally, the last 2 ml NaCl was administered analogously to the previous conditions, with the verbal suggestion that this intervention would “burden” the heart, it would need to work “stronger and faster,” and “hot flashes” could occur (I5). Conclusively, the last post-intervention period (approximately 5 min) was performed with continuous measurement of physiological parameters and assessment of distress levels, and the last blood sample was taken (M6). At the end of the examination the study rationale was disclosed to the participants and they were informed about the placebo character of the study with the administered substance being only “water.” Additionally, the individual echocardiography results were reviewed together with the patient.

Statistical Analysis

Analyses were performed by means of IBM SPSS Statistics 25 with a p-value ≤ 0.05 considered as significant. Mean values of HR, SBP, and DBP were calculated for the period from 200 to 20 s prior to the interventions (pre values) and 20 to 200 s after the interventions (post values). Data that did not fit normal distribution were logarithmized. Pre-post changes of HR, SBP, and DBP induced by the neutral, positive, and negative interventions were compared between groups by means of a mixed-design ANOVA with the within-subject factors “time” (pre and post intervention) and “condition” (neutral, positive, and negative), and the between-subject factor “group” (TTC, controls). Subsequently Bonferroni-corrected post hoc tests were performed. Due to the absence of a normal distribution, SUD levels were evaluated by using Bonferroni-corrected Wilcoxon signed-rank tests and Kruskal–Wallis tests, respectively; changes of cortisol as well as copeptin levels were calculated using Wilcoxon signed-rank tests, Mann–Whitney–U, and Friedman tests.

Results

Baseline Characteristics

TTC patients and controls were comparable with regard to age, employment situation, living situation, and quality of life. The time point of evaluation did not differ between groups and the mean time span between the TTC diagnosis and the examination was 24.61 months (±22.8) (Table 1).

Subjective Units of Distress (SUD)

SUD changes from before to after the neutral, positive, and negative intervention were evaluated by using the Wilcoxon signed-rank tests. No significant changes were observed in response to the neutral and positive verbal suggestions (Bonferroni-corrected p = .1 and p = .06, respectively). However, SUD ratings increased in response to the negative verbal suggestion (Bonferroni-corrected p < .001), indicating a nocebo effect on perceived stress. SUD did not differ between patients with a history of TTC and heart-healthy controls at any time point during the experiment (Mann–Whitney–U test, all Bonferroni-corrected p > .05) (Figure 2 and Table 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2 Subjective Units of Distress (means ± SD) before and after the neutral, positive, and negative interventions.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2 Subjective Units of Distress (SUD), systolic blood pressure (mmHg).

Systolic Blood Pressure (SBP)

The mixed-design ANOVA with the within-subject factors “time” (pre, post intervention) and “condition” (neutral, positive, negative) and the between-subject factor “group” (TTC, controls) was used to examine SBP levels. A significant interaction between “time” and “condition” was found (F(2,76) = 14.09; p < .001). Post hoc tests showed higher SBP levels in response to the negative and the positive verbal suggestions as compared to the neutral verbal suggestion (Bonferroni-corrected p-values, p = .045 and p = .002, respectively). There was also a significant main effect for “condition” (F(2,76) = 3.2, p = .047). Bonferroni-corrected post hoc tests, however, revealed no significant difference between conditions. No other main or interaction effects were significant (Figure 3 and Table 2).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3 Changes in systolic blood pressure in response to the neutral, positive, and negative placebo interventions.

Diastolic Blood Pressure (DBP)

The mixed-design ANOVA for DBP levels with the within-subject factors “time” (pre andf post intervention) and “condition” (neutral, positive, and negative) and the between-subject factor “group” (TTC and controls) revealed no significant main or interaction effects (Figure 4 and Table 2).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4 Changes diastolic blood pressure in response to the neutral, positive, and negative placebo interventions.

Heart Rate (HR)

The mixed-design ANOVA with the within-subject factors “time” (pre and post intervention) and “condition” (neutral, positive, and negative) and the between-subject factor “group” (TTC and controls) for HR levels revealed a significant interaction effect between “time” and “condition” (F(2,76) = 5.5; p = .01). Simple effects analyses showed that this interaction was due to higher HR levels before the negative verbal suggestion compared to before the positive verbal suggestion, indicating an anticipatory increase of HR (Bonferroni-corrected p = .02). Furthermore, a significant main effect of “condition” was found (F(2,78) = 5.11, p = .01), with higher HR levels in the nocebo condition compared to the neutral condition (Bonferroni-corrected p = .037). Finally, the main effect of “time” was significant (F(1,39) = 46.8, p < .001), which was due to increasing HR levels from before to after the intervention (estimated means ± SE, before: 56.5 ± 1.2 and after: 57.4 ± 1.2). No other main or interaction effects were significant (Figures 5, 6 and Table 2).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5 Changes in heart rate in response to the neutral, positive, and negative placebo interventions.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6 Heart rate (bpm) during the anticipation phases (pre values) of the three interventions.

Humoral Stress Markers

Cortisol levels at baseline and after the neutral, the positive and the negative verbal suggestions were compared by Friedman tests. Results revealed a significant difference between conditions (x2 = 64.3, p < .001), which was due to a significant decrease of cortisol levels from condition to condition (Wilcoxon tests, all Bonferroni-corrected p < .001). In no condition significant group differences between TTC patients and controls were observed (Mann–Whitney–U test, all Bonferroni-corrected p-values = 1) (Table 2). A Friedman test for copeptin levels at baseline and after the neutral, the positive and the negative verbal suggestions revealed no significant differences between conditions (p = .84). In no condition significant differences between TTC patients and controls were observed (Mann–Whitney–U test, all Bonferroni-corrected p = 1) (Figure 7 and Table 2).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7 Plasma cortisol levels according to interventions.

Discussion

In this study, we investigated cardiac, psychological, and endocrine stress responses to placebo and nocebo interventions targeting the heart in patients with a history of TTC and matched heart-healthy controls. Although the pathophysiology underlying TTC is not yet entirely clear, a dysfunctional, overmodulated stress response with enhanced sympathetic stimulation might be of key importance (19). We expected that physiological and behavioral responses to placebo and nocebo interventions would be more pronounced in patients with a history of TTC compared to controls than in heart-healthy controls. In our study a significant nocebo effect on subjective units of distress was detected for the whole group of 40 participants. Furthermore, HR increased significantly before the nocebo intervention, possibly indicating anticipatory anxiety towards the upcoming negative intervention. In addition, SBP levels increased significantly in response to both, the placebo and nocebo interventions, suggesting a possible nocebo effect on SBP. Significant alterations of DBP, cortisol and copeptin due to the interventions failed to appear. Contrary to our expectations, none of these responses differed between TTC patients and heart-healthy controls.

Evidence regarding placebo effects on end organ functions regulated by the ANS (e.g., cardiovascular or gastric functions) is less clear compared to the accumulating evidence for placebo effects on functions associated with the central nervous system [e.g., pain and itch, e.g. Refs. (4446)]. The ANS is characterized by high functional specificity provided through elaborated afferent and efferent fibers. Hence, it is not surprising that placebo and nocebo interventions targeting end-organ functions controlled by the ANS can display a high target-specificity (10, 47). The present study adds to this field of placebo research in addressing cardiac parameters that are under control of the autonomic nervous system (HR, SPB, and DBP), as well as subjective stress ratings (SUD) and humoral correlates (copeptin and cortisol). To our knowledge this is one of the first experimental studies, and the first placebo study, in patients with a history of TTC.

Our observations of significant effects from placebo and nocebo interventions on SPB and HR but not on DBP are in accordance with previous studies, which investigated placebo and nocebo effects on cardiovascular parameters by means of verbal suggestions (13, 48). Former investigations that aimed to induce BP changes in healthy individuals by means of a placebo-spray in combination with verbal suggestions for instance, assumed that the absence of significant BP alterations could potentially be explained by lacking associations between memories of physiological or mental states with specific autonomic changes in the brain, which might be a necessary condition for verbal suggestions to induce the intended effects (49). This explanation was linked to the central organizational principle of the brain named, the “reuse of neural circuity,” supposing that neural circuits established for a specific purpose, diversify or exploit to new uses, without losing their genuine function (50). This explanatory approach might also give insightful hints for the results of our study. A link between memories of BD and HR decreases and specific autonomic changes in the brain that could be crucial for the targeted physiological changes might not have been available.

Also the disclosure of the fixed order of the interventions, with the negative intervention being at the end, might have prevented the positive verbal suggestions to evoke HR and BP decrease. The increase of HR prior to the beginning of the nocebo intervention might be linked to the disclosure of the chronological order of interventions as well and could indicate anticipatory anxiety towards the nocebo intervention. Lyby and colleagues could show that fear can eliminate placebo effects induced by verbal suggestions (51). In this regard several imaging studies especially from the area of pain indicate that there is altered activity in the cortical nociceptor network already during the anticipation of pain (52, 53). Moreover, the perception of pain is not exclusively depending on the specific noxious stimulus. Attention, expectation and reappraisal seem to play an important role in the cognitive modulation of pain (54). Among other brain regions [e.g., dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) or the periaqueductal gray (PAG)], especially the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC) seems to play an important role in the nociceptive network and reveals complex response patterns provoked by placebo interventions, but also during anticipation phases (5559). An activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis also underlines the impact of negative expectations resulting from past experiences and present information on pain perception, which in turn might lead to higher pain intensity (60). Therefore, the anticipation of the negative intervention might explain the absence of relaxing effects due to the positive verbal suggestion and the increase of HR prior to the negative verbal suggestion. Nocebo effects (especially in the area of pain) have proven to be associated with complex biochemical and neuroendocrine mechanisms that seem to be connected to anticipatory anxiety (44). This suggests the activation of the HPA or SNS, which build the main peripheral pathways of the human stress system. The HPA axis regulates the release of cortisol that has been shown to be proportionate to the degree of stress on a peripheral level. In our study cortisol levels did not change as a response to the interventions, as it could be seen in previous studies on nocebo hyperalgesia but “naturally” decreased during the examination (61, 62). A similar phenomenon could be seen in a study done by Meissner et al. who examined the predictive value of cortisol on motion sickness (63) or Benedetti et al. who showed that placebo and nocebo effects in cortisol secretion could not be induced by verbal suggestions, but were affected by pharmacological conditioning (3). A meta-analysis, again in the area of pain, showed that the combination of verbal suggestions and conditioning induces larger placebo and nocebo effects than verbal suggestions alone (64, 65). Colloca and colleagues concluded that conditioning is less important in nocebo hyperalgesia compared to placebo analgesia (1). Unintended expectations and stimulus pairings could have been developed through the TTC patient’s experiences during their disease history that might have led to a “blending” of expectation- and conditioning-induced effects in our examination (66).

The question of whether TTC is a transient, reversible disease, or is based on an enduring pathology affecting the sympathetic nervous system, is not yet fully clarified. It is widely believed that the suspected, exaggerated sympathetic activation within the acute phase of TCC is triggered by a precedent, mostly unexpected stressful life event [e.g., Ref. (21)]. The assumption that the normalization of the shape of the left ventricle and the systolic LVEF is accompanied by a regulation of the underlying sympathetic activation, would in turn explain the lacking difference between TTCs and heart-healthy controls. Additionally, recent studies indicate that the exposure to repeated stressors (in contrast to a single life event) is associated with the onset of TTC (67, 68), the authors argued that long-term stressful conditions might have led to an increased vulnerability towards strong emotional or physical stressors triggering the development of TTC. Within our study, positive as well as negative interventions were announced far in advance, took place in the “save environment” of the hospital and might therefore not have served as suitable stimuli for an exaggerated activation of the sympathetic nervous system. Another recent study focused on altered β-adrenergic signaling in TTC cardiomyocytes derived from pluripotent stem cells to explore whether genetic susceptibility underlies the pathophysiology of TTC. These findings point at a complex, multifactorial etiology of TTC with genetic predispositions combined with environmental factors such as age, postmenopausal hormonal status and stressful life events (69). At the cellular level, Borchert and colleagues could demonstrate that TTC phenotype was associated with enhanced β-adrenergic signaling and higher sensitivity to catecholamine-induced toxicity (70). These considerations might be further promising regarding distinguishing features between TTC and heart healthy individuals.

Although the sample size of 20 TTC patients is comparably high considering the prevalence of 0.07–2.3% of patients suspected with an ACS, a larger number of participants in our study would have been desirable. As a further issue the participants’ medication intake (e.g., β-blocker) needs to be considered. Although the intake of antihypertensive medication was relatively similar in both groups, this could have led to a dampening effect of sympathetic activation and might therefore have reduced differences between groups. Furthermore, in the light of the explanations above, a combining of classical conditioning and verbal suggestions might have improved especially the placebo response but also the nocebo response. It could have shed new light on the impact of conditioning and verbal suggestions (resp. explicit expectations) on placebo and nocebo effects within the autonomic nervous system. A further limitation might be the variety of time spans between the cardiac event and the investigation that is attributed to the low prevalence of TTC. If we would have included patients within their acute phase only, the recruitment period would have been enormously long, which would have meant that constancy in further parameters, for instance examiner or examination rooms, could not have been guaranteed. If TTC is seen as a reversible disease or a maintaining pathology in stress processing, a predefinition of one or more specific time points (e.g., within the acute phase together with a two-year follow-up) needs to be considered in a further study. Due to standardization resp. generalization reasons (especially considering the relatively small sample size) the chronological order of the three interventions was standardized. Future studies should consider a cross-over design with a randomized order. The observation that the positive verbal suggestion did not reduce perceived stress is most probably due to a floor effect, since stress at baseline was very low (see Figure 2). Finally, the consideration that anticipatory anxiety might have prevented the induction of a placebo effect suggests to additionally collect fear ratings during the course of the intervention.

Summarizing, this study was the first to investigate effects of positive and negative verbal suggestions in combination with the intravenous application of saline solution on cardiac parameters in patients with a history of TTC compared to controls. Only an increase of SBP could be observed as a response to both positive and negative suggestions. Secondly the increase of SBP as a response to the nocebo intervention was congruently accompanied by higher levels of SUD. The increase of HR prior to the beginning of the nocebo intervention is possibly associated to anticipatory anxiety of the nocebo intervention. Our hypothesis that the cardiac response towards placebo and nocebo interventions in patients with a history of TTC would be different from those of heart-healthy controls could not be confirmed with our data, a TTC, on average diagnosed two years ago, does not appear to have an influence on the responsivity to placebo resp. nocebo interventions. This becomes even more important considering the fact that the etiology of TTC is not yet fully explained. The assumption that an altered sympathetic disposition might build the precondition for the pathophysiological cascade of TTC-patient’s myocardium within the acute phase, could not be verified with our placebo resp. nocebo interventions, at least at the time of our examination, on average, two years after the acute phase.

Ethics Statement

This study was carried out in accordance with the recommendations of the Code of Conduct of the Technical University Munich, Germany, with written informed consent from all subjects. All subjects gave written informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. The protocol was approved by the institutional board of the Technical University Munich, Germany.

Author Contributions

EO, KM, SS, GS, DS, AM, CB, HE, and JR designed the experiment. EO, SS, and DS performed the experiment. EO, KM, AM, DS, SB, K-HL and SH analyzed the data. EO drafted the first version of the manuscript. All authors interpreted the data and critically reviewed the manuscript.

Funding

The study was supported by the Deutsches Herzzentrum, Technische Universitaet, Munich, Germany. This work was supported by the German Research Foundation (DFG) and the Technical University of Munich within the funding program Open Access Publishing.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Colloca L, Sigaudo M, Benedetti F. The role of learning in nocebo and placebo effects. Pain (2008) 136(1–2):211–8. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2008.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. De Houwer J. A functional-cognitive perspective on the relation between conditioning and placebo research. Int Rev Neurobiol (2018) 138:95–111. doi: 10.1016/bs.irn.2018.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Benedetti F, Pollo A, Lopiano L, Lanotte M, Vighetti S, Rainero I. Conscious expectation and unconscious conditioning in analgesic, motor, and hormonal placebo/nocebo responses. J Neurosci (2003) 23(10):4315–23. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-10-04315.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Benedetti F. Placebo effects, 2nd ed. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press (2014). doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198705086.001.0001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Aslaksen PM, Bystad M, Vambheim SM, Flaten MA. Gender differences in placebo analgesia: event-related potentials and emotional modulation. Psychosom Med (2011) 73(2):193–9. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e3182080d73

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Colloca L, Klinger R, Flor H, Bingel U. Placebo analgesia: psychological and neurobiological mechanisms. Pain (2013) 154(4):511–4. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2013.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Colloca L, Benedetti F. Nocebo hyperalgesia: how anxiety is turned into pain. Curr Opin Anesthesiology (2007) 20(5):435–9. doi: 10.1097/ACO.0b013e3282b972fb

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. de la Fuente-Fernandez R, Ruth TJ, Sossi V, Schulzer M, Calne DB, Stoessl AJ. Expectation and dopamine release: mechanism of the placebo effect in Parkinson’s disease. Science (2001) 293(5532):1164–6. doi: 10.1126/science.1060937

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Benedetti F, Arduino C, Amanzio M. Somatotopic activation of opioid systems by target-directed expectations of analgesia. J Neurosci (1999) 19(9):3639–48. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-09-03639.1999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Meissner K. The placebo effect and the autonomic nervous system: evidence for an intimate relationship. Philos Trans Royal Society B Biol Sci (2011) 366(1572):1808–17. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0403

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Meissner K, Fässler M, Rücker G, Kleijnen J, Hróbjartsson A, Schneider A, et al. Differential effectiveness of placebo treatments: a systematic review of migraine prophylaxis. JAMA Internal Med (2013) 173(21):1941–51. doi: 10.1001/jamainternmed.2013.10391

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Ronel J, Mehilli J, Ladwig K-H, Blättler H, Oversohl N, Byrne RA, et al. Effects of verbal suggestion on coronary arteries: results of a randomized controlled experimental investigation during coronary angiography. Am Heart J (2011) 162(3):507–11. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2011.06.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Amigo I, Cuesta V, Fernández A, González A. The effect of verbal instructions on blood pressure measurement. J Hypertens (1993) 11(3):293–6. doi: 10.1097/00004872-199303000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. El Mahmoud R, Mansencal N, Pilliere R, Leyer F, Abbou N, Michaud P, et al. Prevalence and characteristics of left ventricular outflow tract obstruction in Tako-Tsubo syndrome. Am Heart J (2008) 156(3):543–8. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2008.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Maron BJ, Towbin JA, Thiene G, Antzelevitch C, Corrado D, Arnett D, et al. Contemporary definitions and classification of the cardiomyopathies: an American heart association scientific statement from the council on clinical cardiology, heart failure and transplantation committee; quality of care and outcomes research and functional genomics and translational biology interdisciplinary working groups; and council on epidemiology and prevention. Circulation (2006) 113(14):1807–16. doi: 10.1161/CIRCULATIONAHA.106.174287

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Kurisu S, Sato H, Kawagoe T, Ishihara M, Shimatani Y, Nishioka K, et al. Tako-tsubo-like left ventricular dysfunction with ST-segment elevation: a novel cardiac syndrome mimicking acute myocardial infarction. Am Heart J (2002) 143(3):448–55. doi: 10.1067/mhj.2002.120403

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Haghi D, Roehm S, Hamm K, Harder N, Suselbeck T, Borggrefe M, et al. Takotsubo cardiomyopathy is not due to plaque rupture: an intravascular ultrasound study. Clin Cardiol (2010) 33(5):307–10. doi: 10.1002/clc.20747

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Medeiros K, O’Connor MJ, Baicu CF, Fitzgibbons TP, Shaw P, Tighe DA, et al. Systolic and diastolic mechanics in stress cardiomyopathy CLINICAL PERSPECTIVE. Circulation (2014) 129(16):1659.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

19. Wittstein IS. The sympathetic nervous system in the pathogenesis of takotsubo syndrome. Heart Failure Clinics (2016) 12(4):485–98. doi: 10.1016/j.hfc.2016.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Deshmukh A, Kumar G, Pant S, Rihal C, Murugiah K, Mehta JL. Prevalence of Takotsubo cardiomyopathy in the United States. Am Heart J (2012) 164(1):66–71 e1. doi: 10.1016/j.ahj.2012.03.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Gianni M, Dentali F, Grandi AM, Sumner G, Hiralal R, Lonn E. Apical ballooning syndrome or takotsubo cardiomyopathy: a systematic review. Eur Heart J (2006) 27(13):1523–9. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehl032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Klinceva M, Widimsky P, Pesl L, Stasek J, Tousek F, Vambera M, et al. Prevalence of stress-induced myocardial stunning (Tako-Tsubo cardiomyopathy) among patients undergoing emergency coronary angiography for suspected acute myocardial infarction. Int J Cardiol (2007) 120(3):411–3. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2006.07.228

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Kurowski V, Kaiser A, von Hof K, Killermann DP, Mayer B, Hartmann F, et al. Apical and midventricular transient left ventricular dysfunction syndrome (tako-tsubo cardiomyopathy): frequency, mechanisms, and prognosis. Chest (2007) 132(3):809–16. doi: 10.1378/chest.07-0608

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Parodi G, Del Pace S, Carrabba N, Salvadori C, Memisha G, Simonetti I, et al. Incidence, clinical findings, and outcome of women with left ventricular apical ballooning syndrome. Am J Cardiol (2007) 99(2):182–5. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2006.07.080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Templin C, Ghadri JR, Diekmann J, Napp LC, Bataiosu DR, Jaguszewski M, et al. Clinical features and outcomes of takotsubo (stress) cardiomyopathy. N Engl J Med (2015) 373(10):929–38.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

26. Ghadri JR, Sarcon A, Diekmann J, Bataiosu DR, Cammann VL, Jurisic S, et al. Happy heart syndrome: role of positive emotional stress in takotsubo syndrome. Eur Heart J (2016) 37(37):2823–9. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehv757

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Katsanos S, Filippatou A, Ruschitzka F, Filippatos G. Positive emotions and Takotsubo syndrome:’happy heart’or ‘Diagoras’ syndrome? Eur Heart J (2016) 37(37):2821–2. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehw197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Sharkey SW, Windenburg DC, Lesser JR, Maron MS, Hauser RG, Lesser JN, et al. Natural history and expansive clinical profile of stress (tako-tsubo) cardiomyopathy. J Am Coll Cardiol (2010) 55(4):333–41. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2009.08.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Elesber AA, Prasad A, Lennon RJ, Wright RS, Lerman A, Rihal CS. Four-year recurrence rate and prognosis of the apical ballooning syndrome. J Am Coll Cardiol (2007) 50(5):448–52. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2007.03.050

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Ghadri J-R, Wittstein IS, Prasad A, Sharkey S, Dote K, Akashi YJ, et al. International expert consensus document on takotsubo syndrome (part I): clinical characteristics, diagnostic criteria, and pathophysiology. Eur Heart J (2018) 39(22):2032–46. doi: 10.1093/eurheartj/ehy076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Redfors B, Vedad R, Angeras O, Ramunddal T, Petursson P, Haraldsson I, et al. Mortality in takotsubo syndrome is similar to mortality in myocardial infarction - A report from the SWEDEHEART registry. Int J Cardiol (2015) 185:282–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2015.03.162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Sobue Y, Watanabe E, Ichikawa T, Koshikawa M, Yamamoto M, Harada M, et al. Physically triggered Takotsubo cardiomyopathy has a higher in-hospital mortality rate. Int J Cardiol (2017) 235:87–93. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2017.02.090

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Burgdorf C, Kurowski V, Bonnemeier H, Schunkert H, Radke PW. Long-term prognosis of the transient left ventricular dysfunction syndrome (Tako-Tsubo cardiomyopathy): focus on malignancies. Eur J Heart Fail (2008) 10(10):1015–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ejheart.2008.07.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Burgdorf C, Nef HM, Haghi D, Kurowski V, Radke PW. Tako-tsubo (stress-induced) cardiomyopathy and cancer. Ann Intern Med (2010) 152(12):830–1. doi: 10.7326/0003-4819-152-12-201006150-00026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Singh K, Carson K, Shah R, Sawhney G, Singh B, Parsaik A, et al. Meta-analysis of clinical correlates of acute mortality in takotsubo cardiomyopathy. Am J Cardiol (2014) 113(8):1420–8. doi: 10.1016/j.amjcard.2014.01.419

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Singh K, Carson K, Usmani Z, Sawhney G, Shah R, Horowitz J. Systematic review and meta-analysis of incidence and correlates of recurrence of takotsubo cardiomyopathy. Int J Cardiol (2014) 174(3):696–701. doi: 10.1016/j.ijcard.2014.04.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Madhavan M, Prasad A. Proposed Mayo Clinic criteria for the diagnosis of Tako-Tsubo cardiomyopathy and long-term prognosis. Herz (2010) 35(4):240–3. doi: 10.1007/s00059-010-3339-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Kresh JY, Izrailtyan I. Evolution in functional complexity of heart rate dynamics: a measure of cardiac allograft adaptability. Am J Physiol (1998) 275(3 Pt 2):R720–7. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1998.275.3.R720

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Neumeister B, Böhm BO, Adler S. Klinikleitfaden Labordiagnostik. Elsevier Health Sciences (2018).

Google Scholar

40. Katan M, Morgenthaler N, Widmer I, Puder JJ, Konig C, Muller B, et al. Copeptin, a stable peptide derived from the vasopressin precursor, correlates with the individual stress level. Neuroendocrinology Letters (2008) 29(3):341–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

41. Struck J, Morgenthaler NG, Bergmann A. Copeptin, a stable peptide derived from the vasopressin precursor, is elevated in serum of sepsis patients. Peptides (2005) 26(12):2500–4. doi: 10.1016/j.peptides.2005.04.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Widmer IE, Puder JJ, Ko¨nig C, Pargger H, Zerkowski HR, Girard Jr, et al. Cortisol response in relation to the severity of stress and illness. J Clin Endocr Metabolism (2005) 90(8):4579–86. doi: 10.1210/jc.2005-0354

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Grelotti DJ, Kaptchuk TJ. Placebo by proxy. BMJ (Clinical research ed.) (2011) 343, d4345. doi:10.1136/bmj.d4345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Benedetti F, Lanotte M, Lopiano L, Colloca L. When words are painful: unraveling the mechanisms of the nocebo effect. Neuroscience (2007) 147(2):260–71. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.02.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. van Laarhoven AI, Vogelaar ML, Wilder-Smith OH, van Riel PL, de Kerkhof PC, Kraaimaat FW, et al. Induction of nocebo and placebo effects on itch and pain by verbal suggestions. PAIN® (2011) 152(7):1486–94. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2011.01.043

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. van Laarhoven AI, van der Sman-Mauriks IM, Donders ART, Pronk MC, de Kerkhof PC, Evers AW. Placebo effects on itch: a meta-analysis of clinical trials of patients with dermatological conditions. J Invest Dermatol (2015) 135(5):1234–43. doi: 10.1038/jid.2014.522

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Meissner K. Placebo responses on cardiovascular, gastrointestinal, and respiratory organ functions. In:Placebo., Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer (2014). p. 183–203. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-44519-8_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Agras SW, Horne M, Taylor BC. Expectation and the blood-pressure-lowering effects of relaxation. Psychosom Med (1982) 44(4):389–95. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198209000-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Zimmermann-Viehoff F, Meissner K, Koch J, Weber CS, Richter S, Deter HC. Autonomic effects of suggestive placebo interventions to increase or decrease blood pressure: a randomized controlled trial in healthy subjects. J Psychosom Res (2013) 75(1):32–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Anderson ML. Neural reuse: a fundamental organizational principle of the brain. Behav Brain Sci (2010) 33(4):245–66. doi: 10.1017/S0140525X10000853

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Lyby PS, Forsberg JT, Åsli O, Flaten MA. Induced fear reduces the effectiveness of a placebo intervention on pain. PAIN® (2012) 153(5):1114–21. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.02.042

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Ploghaus A, Tracey I, Clare S, Gati JS, Rawlins JNP, Matthews PM. Learning about pain: the neural substrate of the prediction error for aversive events. Proc Natl Academy Sci (2000) 97(16):9281–6. doi: 10.1073/pnas.160266497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Porro CA, Baraldi P, Pagnoni G, Serafini M, Facchin P, Maieron M, et al. Does anticipation of pain affect cortical nociceptive systems? J Neurosci (2002) 22(8):3206–14. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-08-03206.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Wiech K, Ploner M, Tracey I. Neurocognitive aspects of pain perception. Trends Cognit Sci (2008) 12(8):306–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2008.05.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ritter C, Hebart MN, Wolbers T, Bingel U. Representation of spatial information in key areas of the descending pain modulatory system. J Neurosci (2014) 34(13):4634–9. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4342-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Petrovic P, Kalso E, Petersson KM, Ingvar M. Placebo and opioid analgesia– imaging a shared neuronal network. Science (2002) 295(5560):1737–40. doi: 10.1126/science.1067176

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Geuter S, Eippert F, Attar CH, Büchel C. Cortical and subcortical responses to high and low effective placebo treatments. Neuroimage (2013) 67:227–36. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Watson A, El-Deredy W, Iannetti GD, Lloyd D, Tracey I, Vogt BA, et al. Placebo conditioning and placebo analgesia modulate a common brain network during pain anticipation and perception. PAIN® (2009) 145(1–2): 24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, et al. Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science (2004) 303(5661):1162–7. doi: 10.1126/science.1093065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Palermo S, Benedetti F, Costa T, Amanzio M. Pain anticipation: an activation likelihood estimation meta-analysis of brain imaging studies. Human brain mapping (2015) 36(5):1648–61. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Johansen O, Brox J, Flaten MA. Placebo and nocebo responses, cortisol, and circulating beta-endorphin. Psychosom Med (2003) 65(5):786–90. doi: 10.1097/01.PSY.0000082626.56217.CF

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Greenberg G, Haraway MM. Comparative psychology: a handbook. New York & London:Taylor & Francis (1998). doi: 10.4324/9780203826492

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Meissner K, Enck P, Muth E, Kellermann S, Klosterhalfen S. Cortisol levels predict motion sickness tolerance in women but not in men. Physiol Behav (2009) 97(1):102–6. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2009.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Petersen GL, Finnerup NB, Colloca L, Amanzio M, Price DD, Jensen TS, et al. The magnitude of nocebo effects in pain: a meta-analysis. PAIN® (2014) 155(8):1426–34. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2014.04.016

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Vase L, Riley JL II, Price DD. A comparison of placebo effects in clinical analgesic trials versus studies of placebo analgesia. Pain (2002) 99(3):443–52. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00205-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Montgomery GH, Kirsch I. Classical conditioning and the placebo effect. Pain (1997) 72(1–2):107–13. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(97)00016-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Wallström S, Ulin K, Määttä S, Omerovic E, Ekman I. Impact of long-term stress in Takotsubo syndrome: Experience of patients. Eur J Cardiovasc Nurs (2016) 15(7):522–8. doi: 10.1177/1474515115618568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Rosman L, Dunsiger S, Salmoirago-Blotcher E. Cumulative impact of stressful life events on the development of Takotsubo cardiomyopathy. Ann Behav Med (2017) 51(6):925–30. doi: 10.1007/s12160-017-9908-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Lyon A. Stress in a dish. Explor Mech Takotsubo Syndrome (2017) 70(8):992–5. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2017.07.716

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Borchert T, Hubscher D, Guessoum CI, Lam TD, Ghadri JR, Schellinger IN, et al. Catecholamine-dependent beta-adrenergic signaling in a pluripotent stem cell model of takotsubo cardiomyopathy. J Am Coll Cardiol. (2017) 70(8):975–91. doi: 10.1016/j.jacc.2017.06.061

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: placebo effects, nocebo effects, takotsubo cardiomyopathy, cardiological response, sympathetic nervous system

Citation: Olliges E, Schneider S, Schmidt G, Sinnecker D, Müller A, Burgdorf C, Braun S, Holdenrieder S, Ebell H, Ladwig K-H, Meissner K and Ronel J (2019) Placebo and Nocebo Effects in Patients With Takotsubo Cardiomyopathy and Heart-Healthy Controls. Front. Psychiatry 10:549. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00549

Received: 14 December 2018; Accepted: 15 July 2019;
Published: 02 August 2019.

Edited by:

Seetal Dodd, Barwon Health, Australia

Reviewed by:

Karl Bechter, University of Ulm, Germany
Victor Chavarria, Parc Sanitari Sant Joan de Déu, Spain

Copyright © 2019 Olliges, Schneider, Schmidt, Sinnecker, Müller, Burgdorf, Braun, Holdenrieder, Ebell, Ladwig, Meissner and Ronel. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Elisabeth Olliges, Elisabeth.Olliges@med.uni-muenchen.de; Joram Ronel, j.ronel@tum.de

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share first authorship.

These authors have contributed equally to this work and share senior authorship.

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.