Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 28 March 2019
Sec. Mood Disorders
This article is part of the Research Topic Early Life Stress and Depression View all 26 articles

Review of Abnormal Self-Knowledge in Major Depressive Disorder

\r\nYixue Lou,,Yixue Lou1,2,3Yi Lei,*Yi Lei1,3*Ying Mei,Ying Mei1,3Paavo H. T. LeppnenPaavo H. T. Leppänen2Hong Li,Hong Li1,3
  • 1College of Psychology and Sociology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, China
  • 2Faculty of Education and Psychology, University of Jyväskylä, Jyväskylä, Finland
  • 3Center for Neurogenetics, Shenzhen Institute of Neuroscience, Shenzhen, China

Background: Major depressive disorder (MDD) is an affective disorder that is harmful to both physical and mental health. Abnormal self-knowledge, which refers to abnormal judgments about oneself, is a core symptom of depression. However, little research has summarized how and why patients with MDD differ from healthy individuals in terms of self-knowledge.

Objective: To gain a better understanding of MDD, we reviewed previous studies that focused on the behavioral and neurological changes of self-knowledge in this illness.

Main Findings: On the behavioral level, depressed individuals exhibited negative self-knowledge in an explicit way, while more heterogeneous patterns were reported in implicit results. On the neurological level, depressed individuals, as compared with non-depressed controls, showed abnormal self-referential processing in both early perception and higher cognitive processing phases during the Self-Referential Encoding Task. Furthermore, fMRI studies have reported aberrant activity in the medial prefrontal cortex area for negative self-related items in depression. These results revealed several behavioral features and brain mechanisms underlying abnormal self-knowledge in depression.

Future Studies: The neural mechanism of implicit self-knowledge in MDD remains unclear. Future research should examine the importance of others' attitudes on the self-concept of individuals with MDD, and whether abnormal self-views may be modified through cognitive or pharmacological approaches. In addition, differences in abnormal self-knowledge due to genetic variation between depressed and non-depressed populations remain unconfirmed. Importantly, it remains unknown whether abnormal self-knowledge could be used as a specific marker to distinguish healthy individuals from those with MDD.

Conclusion: This review extends our understanding of the relationship between self-knowledge and depression by indicating several abnormalities among individuals with MDD and those who are at risk for this illness.

Introduction

Major depressive disorder (MDD) is a complicated affective disease characterized by abnormal clinical symptoms, including neurovegetative dysfunction (appetite or sleep disturbances), cognitive dissonance (inappropriate guilt, feelings of worthlessness), aberrant psychomotor activities (agitation or retardation) (1), and elevated suicide risk (2, 3). According to the World Health Organization, there are approximately 350 million people suffering from depression worldwide (4). In a recent survey, the proportion of years lived with disability (YLDs) caused by MDD was 4.2%, approximately 34.1 million of the total YLDs (5). Thus, MDD is thought to be a major global cause of disease burden and human suffering (57).

Abnormal perception and understanding of the self is a core symptom of MDD (1). This includes abnormal processes and/or representations involved in being aware of the self, abnormal knowledge about the self, and/or abnormal judgments about the self (National Institute of Mental Health; NIMH). As a sub-construct of perception and understanding of the self, self-knowledge, which refers to the ability to make judgments about one's current cognitive or emotional internal states, traits, and/or abilities (NIMH), is also impaired in individuals with MDD (811). For instance, individuals with MDD, unlike non-depressed healthy individuals, often exhibit negative self-evaluation, inappropriate self-blame, and excessive self-criticism (8, 12).

Although researchers have increasingly begun exploring abnormal self-knowledge in depression, few have compared existing findings in a single study. To enable a better understanding of how and why patients with MDD differ from healthy individuals in terms of self-knowledge, the current review focused on previous studies that examined behavioral patterns and brain mechanisms underlying abnormal self-knowledge in depression. Both explicit and implicit self-knowledge, which reflect conscious and unconscious self-views respectively, were discussed. Various abnormalities such as abnormal brain responses and aberrant neural circuits were illustrated. Furthermore, the present review pointed out some possible directions for future clinical studies (see Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Framework of the current review.

Literature

Literature Review

A search of previous studies published between January 1960 and August 2018 was conducted using the databases Web of Science and PubMed. Self-knowledge is defined as a construct that includes self-evaluation, self-esteem, and self-reference. Thus, the search terms were designed as follows: “depression AND self-evaluation,” OR “depression AND self-esteem,” OR “depression AND self-reference.” Search filters were set for publications written in English. Empirical research and reviews that examined the role of self-evaluation, self-attitude, self-view, self-reference, and/or self-esteem in MDD were found.

Eligibility Criteria

We screened for inclusion based on titles and abstracts, and again using full text. To be included, previous studies had to focus on behavioral and neurological changes of self-knowledge in MDD. All publications had to be reported on clinical populations currently or previously diagnosed with MDD, or populations who were currently in a depressive episode, regardless of gender and age. Conference abstracts were excluded if they were not published in a scientific journal. Publications were also excluded if they were published in a language other than English (see Supplementary Figure 1).

Paradigms

The majority of the research conformed to one of two methods. Specifically, these were explicit and implicit research paradigms.

Explicit Paradigms

Explicit methods are used to assess individuals' self-attitudes by using self-reported measures such as direct self-evaluation. The most commonly used explicit methods are the Self-Referential Encoding Task (SRET) (13) and self-reported questionnaires (14, 15).

Self-Referential Encoding Task, SRET

The self-referential encoding task (SRET) was designed to examine one's self-attitude (13, 16). Theoretically, individuals are more sensitive to information that is encoded as strongly related to oneself (17). Thus, self-related stimuli commonly display better recall and recognition performance, when compared to other-related stimuli (18). In the SRET, researchers present participants with positive and negative personality trait words, and ask them to decide whether each trait describes themselves (self-related condition), a familiar other (other-related condition) (1922), or a socially desirable trait (semantic encoding condition; see Figure 2) (10, 23). After the judgment, the participants were asked to recall or recognize all the trait words that had been presented to them.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Illustration of the self-referential encoding task (SRET).

Individuals with positive self-attitudes, such as feelings of self-value, commonly endorse more positive traits relative to negative traits as self-describing, and show better recall and recognition rates of these words (18). Conversely, negative self-attitudes, such as feelings of worthlessness in individuals with MDD, often lead to more endorsement of negative traits and, in turn, better memory performance of these words (18, 24).

Other Explicit Approaches

Direct self-report questionnaires are often used in studies of depressive self-knowledge. For instance, researchers have used the Rosenberg Self-Esteem Scale (RSES) to measure explicit self-esteem in depression (15). In addition, the self-worth subscale of the World Assumption Scale (25) and the self-acceptance subscale of the Scales of Psychological Well-Being (26) are used to assess explicit self-attitude in depression. Moreover, the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI), which is commonly used to measure the depressive state, also contains self-evaluation factors, such as the self-blame factor, in its items (14).

Implicit Paradigms

The efficacy of explicit methods is debated by some researchers for the following reasons. First, according to cognitive theory, the self-concept involves automatic processes that occur without reflection and/or logical reasoning accessible within the conscious mind (27). Second, direct self-appraisal might be affected by social desirability and cultural differences (28, 29). In brief, explicit methods may not accurately reflect a person's real attitude about him/herself (30, 31). Thus, implicit paradigms were introduced into self-knowledge studies (3234). The most commonly used implicit paradigms are the Implicit Association Test (IAT) (35, 36) and the Name-Letter Test (NLT) (37, 38).

Implicit Association Task, IAT

The self-evaluation IAT (sIAT) is a paradigm that has been commonly used to examine implicit self-attitudes of depression (39, 40). In the sIAT, it is assumed that information that is compatible with one's implicit attitude would be better processed as compared to that which is incompatible (36). Thus, participants are asked to complete two types of categorization (compatible and incompatible) by using a two key-press system. In the compatible condition, self-related stimulus words (e.g., one's own name or date of birth) shared the same key with “valuable” personality trait words (e.g., competent), while self-unrelated words (e.g., other's name or non-meaningful date) shared another key with “worthless” personality trait words (e.g., unsuccessful). The incompatible condition was reverse coded (34, 41, 42) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Illustration of the self-evaluation Implicit Association Task (sIAT).

Differences in reaction times (RTs) and accuracy (ACC) between compatible and incompatible conditions were analyzed. Typically, the condition that is congruent with one's implicit self-attitude should show better performance when compared to the incongruent one. For instance, individuals with positive self-bias should demonstrate a faster and more accurate response in the compatible condition, relative to the incompatible condition (43, 44), while the negative self-attitude found in depression should lead to an opposite pattern.

Name-Letter Test, NLT

The name-letter test (NLT) has also been used in previous studies to measure implicit self-attitudes (38, 4547). In the NLT, researchers presented participants with the 26 letters of the alphabet one-by-one, and asked them to judge the attractiveness or likability of each letter, relying on their first, intuitive reaction (48). According to the name letter effect, one's initial is thought to be highly associated with the self (49, 50). Thus, under the influence of positive self-bias, non-depressed individuals should show a rational preference toward their initials relative to other letters, even though they are generally unaware of this effect (38). However, an opposite pattern may be true for individuals with MDD (33).

The name letter effect has been shown to be a cross-cultural phenomenon, since it has also been reported in Thai, Japanese, and Korean studies (5153). Thus, the NLT qualifies as an indirect assessment of self-attitude in depression (33).

Main findings

By using the aforementioned paradigms, researchers have found abnormal behavioral patterns and brain responses in individuals with MDD, when compared to non-depressed, healthy controls. Evaluation of the quality of included studies was listed in Supplementary Table 1.

Behavioral Abnormalities

Behavioral abnormalities include explicit/conscious and implicit/unconscious behaviors that have been observed in depression.

Explicit: Negative Self-View

At the explicit level, previous behavioral research has revealed a negative self-view in depression, as compared with a non-depressed healthy population. For instance, healthy individuals typically exhibit positive attitudes about themselves (5457). For instance, they often attribute themselves with more positive, rather than negative, personality traits (54, 58), so that their self-esteem may be protected (18, 59). However, individuals with depression typically demonstrate an abnormally negative self-view (1, 60, 61).

For instance, under the influence of negative self-knowledge, individuals with MDD show less positive self-bias, less self-confidence, and lower self-esteem (6265), as well as excessive self-criticism, negative self-evaluation, inappropriate self-blame, and shame (8, 12, 6668). This negative self-representation has been associated with greater self-reported depression (69, 70), poor and slower recovery from a major depressive episode (71, 72), and higher probability of suicide attempt (73, 74). In addition, individuals with higher self-esteem may exhibit sudden improvements in depressive symptomatology even without treatment (75), while lower self-esteem is thought to be a prospective risk factor for depressive symptoms from young adulthood to old age (7678).

In the SRET, individuals with depression, relative to healthy controls, endorsed more negative trait words as self-described, and showed faster response, better recall performance, and increased recognition rate for these words (9, 23, 79, 80). In a longitudinal study Derry and Kuiper (13), found that such negative self-bias might be a specific symptom in currently depressed patients, since the recall rate of negative self-related words decreased after recovery from the illness.

Implicit: Discrepancy Between Explicit and Implicit Self-Esteem

Although a large number of studies have indicated a lower self-attitude in MDD, relative to healthy individuals, at an explicit level (810, 20), more heterogeneous patterns have been reported in implicit studies (34, 36, 41, 42, 81).

For instance, when using the IAT and/or NLT to measure implicit self-esteem (ISE) and RSES to assess explicit self-esteem (ESE), some researchers have observed both low ESE and ISE in currently depressed individuals (39, 40, 42) and chronically depressed individuals with early onset (33), relative to never depressed healthy controls. However, more researchers have reported a normal ISE combined with a lower ESE in individuals with current depression (41, 42, 8285), previous depression (41), remitted depression (11, 39, 86), and chronic depression with late onset (33), when compared to non-depressed individuals. Some researchers have even observed higher ISE and lower ESE in current depression (83, 85, 87) and previous depression (34, 82).

The discrepancy between explicit and implicit self-esteem, especially the combination of low ESE and high ISE, is thought to be associated with internalizing problems such as affective disorders (8892). For major depression, such a discrepancy seems to be more severe in depressed individuals with suicidal ideation relative to those without such ideation (42). Moreover, depressed patients with congruent self-esteem, compared to those with incongruent self-esteem, exhibited better recovery from the illness throughout antidepressant treatment (93).

Neurological Abnormalities

Neurological abnormalities include abnormal electrophysiological responses and aberrant functional neural activities. These abnormalities were all detected using the SRET.

Abnormal Electrophysiological Response

To explore the brain mechanism of negative self-knowledge in depression, electroencephalography (EEG) technology was used in conjunction with the SRET. By collecting the event-related potentials (ERPs) during the SRET, researchers attempted to identify the key ERP components that are involved in negative self-referent processing in MDD.

For instance, Shestyuk and Deldin (62) observed increased P2 component, which was quantified as a positive peak in the 200- to 300-ms time window poststimulus, in individuals with depression while processing negative, relative to positive, self-referential items. The opposite, however, was true for the non-depressed healthy controls. A recent study reported decreased N400 amplitude, which was measured as mean voltage of the ERP average between 350–500 ms, in individuals with depression, as compared with healthy controls, in negative self-referent processing (9). Regarding the latter component, Poulsen et al. (94) found an attenuated or absent MFN response between 260 ms and 480 ms in depression, relative to non-depressed controls, when specifically endorsing negative trait descriptors. However, in a recent study, depressed individuals were found to exhibit enhanced MFN for both positive and negative endorsement (95). Consistently, an attenuated P300 response from 300- to 600- ms was observed in both of these two studies (94, 95). Concerning the more delayed late positive potential (LPP), larger LPP amplitudes were detected following negative vs. positive endorsement in depressed adults (62, 96), depressed adolescents (8), and young girls who were vulnerable to depression (97), when compared to healthy controls.

In these studies, the P2 component is thought to be related to automatic semantic processes (98). Thus, an increased P2 reflects a stronger automatic attentional capture and orientation in patients with depression under the negative, relative to positive, self-related condition (62). The N400 component was interpreted to be influenced by semantic memories about the self, and could be reduced by greater association of the stimuli with a preceding self-related context (99, 100). Therefore, this result indexed a congruent pattern between negative semantic memories and the self-concept in individuals with depression (9). In addition, the MFN is thought to be associated with early cognitive evaluation during self-referential processing (95). The altered MFN response may reflect abnormal self-evaluation among clinically depressed individuals. The greater P300, which is evoked by a saliency effect of self-referential information and positive affect (101), was attenuated in depression. One possible interpretation is that it was possibly associated with a chronically negative self-view in this population (95). Last, an increased LPP amplitude, which is associated with effortful encoding (102), indicates that individuals with depression engage more cognitive effort in processing self-related negative, relative to positive, items (62).

In all, in the time domain, abnormal self-knowledge in depression could be reflected in early phases of self-related processing, such as automatic attention and orientation toward negative self-descriptive items (62). Retrieval of negative memories about the self could also be involved (9). For later phases of self-referential processing, an attenuated bonding between positive affect and the self may be associated with negative self-view in depression (95). Furthermore, depressed individuals seem to engage more cognitive effort in negative, instead of positive, self-reference (62).

Abnormal Functional Neural Activities

The high spatial resolution of functional MRI technology makes it possible for researchers to determine abnormal brain activities in depression during the SRET. Several fMRI studies, thus, have suggested that the prefrontal cortex and its sub-regions might be abnormal in individuals with MDD (103). The prefrontal cortex is thought to play an important role in self-referential processing (104). In particular, dysfunction within the medial prefrontal cortex (mPFC) and in the circuits that connect the mPFC to other cortical and limbic structures is responsible for the cognitive dissonance found in depression (103).

For instance, the cortical midline structures (CMS), such as the mPFC, are critical for self-referential processing in healthy individuals (17, 105), adult patients (106108), and adolescent patients with MDD (109). However, aberrant activity in the mPFC was reported in depression when compared to healthy controls (17, 23, 106). Additionally, Yoshimura et al. (108) found that individuals with depression, relative to healthy controls, exhibited hyperactivity in the mPFC and the rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rostral ACC) during self-referential processing of negative personality traits; such activity was shown to be associated with depressive symptoms (108).

Furthermore, abnormal activities of other sub-regions of the prefrontal cortex were also observed during the processing of self-related negative stimuli in depression (10, 23). For instance, by using the SRET, researchers found significantly higher activation of the central mPFC and significantly lower activation of the dorsal mPFC in depression, relative to healthy controls, during the self-referential condition (10). Local connectivity of the dorsal mPFC was also reduced during self-reflection in depressed adolescents (109). The activity of the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (dlPFC) was also involved in self-referential processing in depression, but was absent in healthy controls (23). In addition, a meta-analysis revealed hyperactivation in the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) within major depression during resting state, which was discussed as a neural reflection of self-referential processing (110).

Therefore, aberrant activity of the prefrontal cortex and its sub-regions could index the abnormal brain activity that is a hallmark of depression, specifically during the processing of self-referential stimuli. In particular, hyperactivity in the mPFC during negative self-referential processing could possibly even be associated with the severity of depressive symptoms.

Discussion

According to previous studies, abnormal self-knowledge, which is commonly found in MDD, is mainly reflected in abnormal behaviors and abnormal neurological responses during self-evaluation, self-esteem, and/or self-referential processing.

At the behavioral level, abnormal self-knowledge could be indexed by a negative explicit self-view (13, 80) and discrepant self-esteem, which involves relatively higher implicit self-esteem and lower explicit self-esteem (11, 33, 34, 111). Furthermore, a greater discrepancy between implicit and explicit self-esteem is related to more severe MDD, or a higher possibility of being affected by the illness (42, 111).

At the neurological level, several abnormalities have been found during abnormal self-referential processing, by using electrophysiological technology (8, 9, 62) and fMRI technology (10, 108, 112, 113). For instance, for abnormal electrophysiological processing, enhanced P2 and LPP and decreased N400 amplitudes were all detected in depression, relative to non-depressed controls, in the SRET. For aberrant brain activities, higher activation of the central mPFC, lower activation of the dorsal mPFC (10), and aberrant activity of the dlPFC (23) during self-referential processing can also distinguish MDD, as well as indicate the severity of symptoms.

By using the indexes above, researchers and clinicians could distinguish patients with MDD and non-depressed individuals more objectively and effectively. However, caution should be exercised for several reasons. First, some of the studies involved limited samples and poor replications. For instance, abnormalities in P2 and LPP amplitude in MDD were reported in a study with 17 patients with current depression, 17 patients with remitted depression, and 18 controls, and abnormalities of N400s were reported in a study including 16 patients with MDD and 16 controls. Considering this issue, larger samples are needed to confirm changes of electrophysiological response during depressive self-referential processing.

Second, abnormal self-knowledge is only one component of MDD, despite being a core feature. Behavioral abnormalities may not be sensitive and specific for MDD, since they are affected by non-clinical factors such as personality traits (114117). Thus, more evidence is needed to confirm the behavioral abnormalities identified in the current review.

Third, although we reviewed various investigations that focused on abnormal self-knowledge in depression, a classical review is relatively less objective compared with a systematic meta-analysis.

Future studies

In the exploration of self-knowledge in depression, there are still many unanswered questions. First, although the discrepancy between explicit and implicit self-esteem in depression has been confirmed by several previous studies (11), and the neural mechanism of explicit self-esteem has been richly explored (8, 10, 23, 108), little is known about the neural basis of implicit self-esteem in depression, suggesting the need for further research.

Second, it remains unclear whether the pattern of self-knowledge in patients with depression would be different in a cross-cultural context. For instance, collectivism of eastern Asia, relative to individualism in Western culture, allows individuals to view themselves as dynamic entities that are continually defined by their social context and relationships (118). Thus, in Eastern cultures, judgment by important others about oneself, which is currently ignored in self-related studies, plays a critical role in the quality of one's self-view (119). Indeed, the development of self-knowledge relies not only on one's reflection of the self, but also on how important others evaluate the individual (22, 58, 119121).

Third, some previous neurogenetic research explored the association between different gene types and abnormal self-knowledge in depression, and found that the serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5-HTTLPR) played a crucial role in susceptibility to developing depression (122). In a recent study, Ma et al. (21) reported a modulation effect of the 5-HTTLPR polymorphism in brain activities associated with negative self-knowledge in depression. It was suggested that the s allele of 5-HTTLPR could possibly be a risk factor for individuals vulnerable to depression (21). However, differences in abnormal self-knowledge due to genetic variation between healthy and depressed populations remains unconfirmed, calling for further research.

Fourth, to repair discrepant self-esteem found in depression, which involves low explicit and high implicit self-esteem, the development of cognitive and/or medical approaches is needed to enhance explicit self-attitudes. A previous study indicated that depression can be prevented or reduced by interventions that improve explicit self-esteem (123126). For example, researchers have utilized positive self-images (127) and mindfulness (128, 129) to realize an improvement of both explicit and implicit self-esteem. It is possible that these methods can also be used to diminish the discrepancy of self-esteem found in depression. Furthermore, since the s allele of 5-HTTLPR may elevate the risk of developing depression (21), it is reasonable to consider whether the use of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) could enhance self-satisfaction (130132).

Finally, to conquer complex diseases such as MDD, the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) has raised the importance of identifying clinically useful biomarkers and behavioral indicators that predict change across the trajectory of illnesses (19). However, the most fundamental challenge is to identify these diseases effectively. In the diagnosis of MDD, the most commonly used measurements are structured interviews and/or depression inventories (133), which are relatively subjective and require researchers to be professionally trained. To facilitate the identification of objective criteria for MDD diagnosis, it must be determined whether abnormal self-knowledge can be used as an objective and specific marker for identifying MDD. For this purpose, patterns of abnormal self-knowledge should be compared between MDD and other mental disorders, such as bipolar disorder.

Conclusion

MDD is a main cause of disease burden worldwide (6, 7), and abnormal self-knowledge is one of the cardinal symptoms of this disorder. Through a review of previous studies that measured abnormal self-knowledge in individuals with clinical MDD, several abnormalities that distinguish patients with MDD as well as those at risk of the illness were revealed. We also pointed out several possible directions for future clinical studies based on previous findings. Overall, this review extends our understanding of the relationship between self-knowledge and depression.

Author Contributions

YLo wrote the paper. YLe supervised the review and assisted in paper revision. YM assisted in paper revision. PL assisted in paper revision. HL assisted in paper writing and funding supports. All authors were involved in revising the manuscript critically for important intellectual content and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Shenzhen Peacock Plan [Grant No. KQTD2015033016104926] and the Guangdong Pearl River Talents Plan Innovative and Entrepreneurial Team [Grant No. 2016ZT06S220].

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00130/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Fava M, Kenneth KS. Major depressive disorder. Neuron. (2000) 28:335. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(00)00112-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Chesney E, Goodwin GM, Fazel S. Risks of all-cause and suicide mortality in mental disorders: a meta-review. World Psychiatry Offic J World Psychiatr Assoc. (2014) 13:153. doi: 10.1002/wps.20128

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Klonsky ED, May AM, Saffer BY. Suicide, suicide attempts, and suicidal ideation. Ann Rev Clin Psychol. (2016) 12:307. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-021815-093204

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Smith K. Mental health: a world of depression. Nature. (2014) 515:181. doi: 10.1038/515180a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Vos T, Abajobir AA, Abbafati C, Abbas KM, Abate KH, Abd-Allah F, et al. Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 328 diseases and injuries for 195 countries, 1990-2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet. (2017) 390:1211–59. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)32154-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Lozano R, Naghavi M, Foreman K, Lim S, Shibuya K, Aboyans V, et al. Years lived with disability (YLDs) for 1160 sequelae of 289 diseases and injuries 1990–2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet. (2012) 380:2163–96. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(12)61729-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Murray CJ, Vos T, Lozano R, Naghavi M, Flaxman AD, Michaud C, et al. Disability-adjusted life years (DALYs) for 291 diseases and injuries in 21 regions, 1990-2010: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet. (2012) 380:2197–223. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(12)61689-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Auerbach RP, Stanton CH, Proudfit GH, Pizzagalli DA. Self-referential processing in depressed adolescents: a high-density event-related potential study. J Abnorm Psychol. (2015) 124:233–45. doi: 10.1037/abn0000023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kiang M, Farzan F, Blumberger DM, Kutas M, McKinnon MC, Kansal V, et al. Abnormal self-schema in semantic memory in major depressive disorder: evidence from event-related brain potentials. Biol Psychol. (2017) 126:41–7. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Li Y, Li M, Wei D, Kong X, Du X, Hou X, et al. Self-referential processing in unipolar depression: distinct roles of subregions of the medial prefrontal cortex. Psychiatry Res. (2017) 263:8–14. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2017.02.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Smeijers D, Vrijsen JN, van Oostrom I, Isaac L, Speckens A, Becker ES, et al. Implicit and explicit self-esteem in remitted depressed patients. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2017) 54:301–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2016.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Auerbach RP, Ho M, Kim JC. Identifying cognitive and interpersonal predictors of adolescent depression. J Abnorm Child Psychol. (2014) 42:913–24. doi: 10.1007/s10802-013-9845-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Derry PA, Kuiper NA. Schematic processing and self-reference in clinical depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (1981) 90:286. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.90.4.286

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Beck AT, Steer RA, Brown GK. Beck depression inventory - second edition manual. Psihologijski Mjerni Instrumenti. (1996) 93:88.

Google Scholar

15. Rosenberg M. Society and the Adolescent Self-Image (rev. ed.). Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press (1965).

Google Scholar

16. Ramel W, Goldin PR, Eyler LT, Brown GG, Gotlib IH, McQuaid JR. Amygdala reactivity and mood-congruent memory in individuals at risk for depressive relapse. Biol Psychiatry. (2007) 61:231–9. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2006.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Northoff G, Heinzel A, De GM, Bermpohl F, Dobrowolny H, Panksepp J. Self-referential processing in our brain–a meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. Neuroimage. (2006) 31:440–57. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Caudek C. Individual differences in cognitive control on self-referenced and other-referenced memory. Conscious Cogn. (2014) 30:169–83. doi: 10.1016/j.concog.2014.08.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Auerbach RP, Bondy E, Stanton CH, Webb CA, Shankman SA, Pizzagalli DA. Self-referential processing in adolescents: Stability of behavioral and ERP markers. Psychophysiology. (2016) 53:1398–406. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12686

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Ji JL, Grafton B, MacLeod C. Referential focus moderates depression-linked attentional avoidance of positive information. Behav Res Ther. (2017) 93:47–54. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2017.03.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Ma Y, Li B, Wang C, Shi Z, Sun Y, Sheng F, et al. 5-HTTLPR polymorphism modulates neural mechanisms of negative self-reflection. Cereb Cortex. (2014) 24:2421–9. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bht099

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Yang J, Xu X, Chen Y, Shi Z, Han S. Trait self-esteem and neural activities related to self-evaluation and social feedback. Sci Rep. (2016) 6:20274. doi: 10.1038/srep20274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Lemogne C, le Bastard G, Mayberg H, Volle E, Bergouignan L, Lehericy S, et al. In search of the depressive self: extended medial prefrontal network during self-referential processing in major depression. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2009) 4:305–12. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Caudek C, Monni A. Do you remember your sad face? Roles Negat Cogn Style Sad Mood Memory. (2013) 21:891. doi: 10.1080/09658211.2013.765893

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Janoffbulman R. Assumptive worlds and the stress of traumatic events: applications of the schema construct. Soc Cogn. (1989) 7:113–36. doi: 10.1521/soco.1989.7.2.113

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Ryff CD, Keyes C. The structure of psychological well-being revisited. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1995) 69:719–27. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.69.4.719

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Franck E, De Raedt R, Dereu M, Van den Abbeele D. Implicit and explicit self-esteem in currently depressed individuals with and without suicidal ideation. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2007) 38:75–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2006.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Kitayama S, Uchida Y. Explicit self-criticism and implicit self-regard: Evaluating self and friend in two cultures. J Exp Soc Psychology. (2003) 39:476–82. doi: 10.1016/S0022-1031(03)00026-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Yamaguchi S, Greenwald AG, Banaji MR, Murakami F, Chen D, Shiomura K, et al. Apparent universality of positive implicit self-esteem. Psychol Sci. (2007) 18:498–500. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01928.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Kim YH, Chiu CY, Peng S, Cai H, Tov W. Explaining east-west differences in the likelihood of making favorable self-evaluations: the role of evaluation apprehension and directness of expression. J Cross Cult Psychol. (2010) 41:62–75. doi: 10.1177/0022022109348921

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Kudo E, Numazaki M. Explicit and direct self-serving bias in Japan. J Cross Cult Psychol. (2003) 34:511–21. doi: 10.1177/0022022103256475

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Grundy JG, Benarroch MF, Lebarr AN, Shedden JM. Electrophysiological correlates of implicit valenced self-processing in high vs. Low Self Esteem Individ Soc Neurosci. (2015) 10:100–12. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2014.965339

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Randenborgh AV, Pawelzik M, Quirin M, Kuhl J. Bad roots to grow: deficient implicit self-evaluations in chronic depression with an early onset. J Clin Psychol. (2016) 72:580–90. doi: 10.1002/jclp.22275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Roberts JE, Porter A, Vergara-Lopez C. Implicit and explicit self-esteem in previously and never depressed individuals: Baseline differences and reactivity to rumination. Cogn Ther Res. (2015) 40:164–72. doi: 10.1007/s10608-015-9732-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Greenwald AG, Mcghee DE, Schwartz JL. Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: the implicit association test. J Personal Soc Psychol. (1998) 74:1464. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Greenwald AG, Nosek BA, Banaji MR. Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm J Personal Soc Psychol. (2003) 85:197. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Greenwald AG, Banaji MR. Implicit social cognition: attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes. Psychol Rev. (1995) 102:4–27. doi: 10.1037/0033-295X.102.1.4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Nuttin JM. Narcissism beyond Gestalt and awareness: the name letter effect. Eur J Soc Psychol. (1985) 15:353–61. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420150309

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Risch AK, Buba A, Birk U, Morina N, Steffens MC, Stangier U. Implicit self-esteem in recurrently depressed patients. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2010) 41:199–206. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2010.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Romero N, Sanchez A, Vázquez C, Valiente C. Explicit self-esteem mediates the relationship between implicit self-esteem and memory biases in major depression. Psychiatry Res. (2016) 242:336–44. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2016.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Franck E, De Raedt R, De Houwer J. Activation of latent self-schemas as a cognitive vulnerability factor for depression: the potential role of implicit self-esteem. Cogn Emot. (2008) 22:1588–99. doi: 10.1080/02699930801921271

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Franck E, De Raedt R, De HJ. Implicit but not explicit self-esteem predicts future depressive symptomatology. Behav Res Ther. (2007) 45:2448. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2007.01.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Fleischhauer M, Strobel A, Diers K, Enge S. Electrophysiological evidence for early perceptual facilitation and efficient categorization of self-related stimuli during an Implicit Association Test measuring neuroticism. Psychophysiology. (2014) 51:142–51. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Wu L, Gu R, Cai H, Zhang J. Electrophysiological evidence for executive control and efficient categorization involved in implicit self-evaluation. Soc Neurosci. (2016) 11:153–63. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2015.1044673

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Hoorens V, Takano K, Franck E, Roberts JE, Raes F. Initial and noninitial name-letter preferences as obtained through repeated letter rating tasks continue to reflect (different aspects of) self-esteem. Psychol Assess. (2015) 27:905. doi: 10.1037/pas0000092

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Jones JT, Pelham BW, Mirenberg MC, Hetts JJ. Name letter preferences are not merely mere exposure: implicit egotism as self-regulation. J Exp Soc Psychol. (2002) 38:170–7. doi: 10.1006/jesp.2001.1497

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Stieger S, Burger C. More complex than previously thought: new insights into the optimal administration of the initial preference task. Self Ident. (2013) 12:201–16. doi: 10.1080/15298868.2012.655897

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Koole SL, Dijksterhuis A, Van KA. What's in a name: implicit self-esteem and the automatic self. J Personal Soc Psychol. (2001) 80:669. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.80.4.669

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Fischer C, Dailler F, Morlet D. Novelty P3 elicited by the subject's own name in comatose patients. Clin Neurophysiol Offic J Int Feder Clin Neurophysiol. (2008) 119:2224. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.03.035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Koole SL, Pelham BW. On the nature of implicit self-esteem: the case of the Name Letter Effect. In: Spencer S, Fein S, Zanna MP, editors. Motivated Social Perception: The Ontario Symposium. Hillsdale: NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum (2003), p. 93–116.

Google Scholar

51. Fujii T. Does the name-letter effect emerge in Korea? Japan J Res Emot. (2015) 23:32–7. doi: 10.4092/jsre.23.32

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Hoorens V, Nuttin JM, Herman IE, Pavakanun U. Mastery pleasure versus mere ownership: a quasi-experimental cross-cultural and cross-alphabetical test of the name letter effect. Eur J Soc Psychol. (1990) 20:181–205. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.2420200302

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Kitayama S, Karasawa M. Implicit self-esteem in Japan: name letters and birthday numbers. Personal Soc Psychol Bull. (1997) 23:736–42. doi: 10.1177/0146167297237006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Chen Y, Zhong Y, Zhou H, Zhang S, Tan Q, Fan W. Evidence for implicit self-positivity bias: an event-related brain potential study. Exp Brain Res. (2014) 232:985–94. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3810-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Mezulis AH, Abramson LY, Hyde JS, Hankin BL. Is there a universal positivity bias in attributions? A meta-analytic review of individual, developmental, and cultural differences in the self-serving attributional bias. Psychol Bull. (2004) 130:711–47. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Pahl S, Eiser JR. Valence, comparison focus and self-positivity biases: does it matter whether people judge positive or negative traits? Exp Psychol. (2005) 52:303. doi: 10.1027/1618-3169.52.4.303

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Watson LA, Dritschel B, Obonsawin MC, Jentzsch I. Seeing yourself in a positive light: brain correlates of the self-positivity bias. Brain Res. (2007) 1152:106. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2007.03.049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Zhou A, Li S, Herbert C, Xia R, Xu K, Xu Q, et al. Perspective taking modulates positivity bias in self-appraisals: behavioral and event-related potential evidence. Soc Neurosci. (2013) 8:326. doi: 10.1080/17470919.2013.807873

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Leary MR. Motivational and emotional aspects of the self. Ann Rev Psychol. (2007) 58:317. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.58.110405.085658

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Brand-Gothelf A, Yoeli-Bligh N, Gilboa-Schechtman E, Benaroya-Milshtein N, Apter A. Perceptions of self, mother and family and behavior of prepubertal depressed children. Eur Psychiatry. (2015) 30:69–74. doi: 10.1016/j.eurpsy.2014.05.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Ruehlman LS, West SG, Pasahow RJ. Depression and evaluative schemata. J Personal. (2010) 53:46–92. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.1985.tb00888.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Shestyuk AY, Deldin PJ. Automatic and strategic representation of the self in major depression: trait and state abnormalities. Am J Psychiatry. (2010) 167:536–44. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.06091444

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Shestyuk AY, Deldin PJ, Brand JE, Deveney CM. Reduced sustained brain activity during processing of positive emotional stimuli in major depression. Biol Psychiatry. (2005) 57:1089–96. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.02.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Tripković I, Roje R, Krnić S, Nazor M, Karin Ž, Capkun V. (2015). Depression and self-esteem in farly adolescence. Centr Eur J Public Health. 23:166–9. doi: 10.21101/cejph.a4017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Watson LA, Dritschel B, Jentzsch I, Obonsawin MC. Changes in the relationship between self-reference and emotional valence as a function of dysphoria. Br J Psychol. (2008) 99:143–52. doi: 10.1348/000712607X248689

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Alexander B, Brewin CR, Vearnals S, Wolff G, Leff J. An investigation of shame and guilt in a depressed sample. Psychol Psychother Theor Res Pract. (2011) 72:323–38. doi: 10.1348/000711299160031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Braet C, Wante L, Van Beveren ML, Theuwis L. Is the cognitive triad a clear marker of depressive symptoms in youngsters? Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. (2015) 24:1261. doi: 10.1007/s00787-015-0674-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Thew GR, Gregory JD, Roberts K, Rimes KA. The phenomenology of self-critical thinking in people with depression, eating disorders, and in healthy individuals. Psychol Psychother. (2017) 90:751–69. doi: 10.1111/papt.12137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Gara MA, Woolfolk RL, Allen LA. Social cognitive complexity and depression: cognitive complexity moderates the correlation between depression self-ratings and global self-evaluation. J Nerv Mental Dis. (2002) 190:670. doi: 10.1097/00005053-200210000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Orchard F, Reynolds S. The combined influence of cognitions in adolescent depression: biases of interpretation, self-evaluation, and memory. Br J Clin Psychol. (2018) 57:420–35. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Gildengers AG, Houck PR, Mulsant BH, Dew MA, Aizenstein HJ, Jones BL, et al. Trajectories of treatment response in late-life depression: psychosocial and clinical correlates. J Clin Psychopharmacol. (2005) 25(4 Suppl. 1):S8. doi: 10.1097/01.jcp.0000161498.81137.12

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Woolfolk RL, Gara MA, Ambrose TK, Williams JE, Allen LA, Irvin SL, et al. Self-complexity and the persistence of depression. J Nerv Ment Dis. (1999) 187:393–9. doi: 10.1097/00005053-199907000-00001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Hur JW, Kim WJ, Kim YK. The mediating effect of psychosocial factors on suicidal probability among adolescents. Arch Suicide Res. (2011) 15:327–36. doi: 10.1080/13811118.2011.615701

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Wetzel RD, Reich T. The cognitive triad and suicide intent in depressed in-patients. Psychol Rep. (1989) 65:1027–32. doi: 10.2466/pr0.1989.65.3.1027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Kelly MA, Roberts JE, Bottonari KA. Non-treatment-related sudden gains in depression: the role of self-evaluation. Behav Res Ther. (2007) 45:737–47. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2006.06.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Orth U, Robins RW, Roberts BW. Low self-esteem prospectively predicts depression in adolescence and young adulthood. J Personal Soc Psychol. (2008) 95:695–708. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.95.3.695

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Orth U, Robins RW, Widaman KF, Conger RD. Is low self-esteem a risk factor for depression? Findings from a longitudinal study of Mexican-origin youth. Dev Psychol. (2014) 50:622–33. doi: 10.1037/a0033817

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Steiger AE, Allemand M, Robins RW, Fend HA. Low and decreasing self-esteem during adolescence predict adult depression two decades later. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2014) 106:325–38. doi: 10.1037/a0035133

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Disner SG, Shumake JD, Beevers CG. Self-referential schemas and attentional bias predict severity and naturalistic course of depression symptoms. Cogn Emot. (2017) 31:632–44. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2016.1146123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Dobson KS, Shaw BF. Specificity and stability of self-referent encoding in clinical depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (1987) 96:34. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.96.1.34

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. De Houwer J. What are implicit measures and why are we using them? Rwwiers. (2006) 11–28. doi: 10.4135/9781412976237.n2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Gemar MC, Segal ZV, Sagrati S, Kennedy SJ. Mood-induced changes on the implicit association test in recovered depressed patients. J Abnorm Psychol. (2001) 110:282. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.110.2.282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Kesting ML, Mehl S, Rief W, Lindenmeyer J, Lincoln TM. When paranoia fails to enhance self-esteem: explicit and implicit self-esteem and its discrepancy in patients with persecutory delusions compared to depressed and healthy controls. Psychiatry Res. (2011) 186:197–202. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2010.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Lemmens LH, Roefs A, Arntz A, van Teeseling HC, Peeters F, Huibers MJ. The value of an implicit self-associative measure specific to core beliefs of depression. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry. (2014) 45:196–202. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Raedt RD, Schacht R, Franck E, Houwer JD. Self-esteem and depression revisited: implicit positive self-esteem in depressed patients? Behav Res Ther. (2006) 44:1017–28. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2005.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. van Tuijl LA, Glashouwer KA, Bockting CL, Tendeiro JN, Penninx BW, de Jong PJ. Implicit and explicit self-esteem in current, remitted, recovered, and comorbid depression and anxiety Disorders: the NESDA study. PLoS ONE. (2016) 11:e0166116. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Jabben N, de Jong PJ, Kupka RW, Glashouwer KA, Nolen WA, Penninx BW. Implicit and explicit self-associations in bipolar disorder: a comparison with healthy controls and unipolar depressive disorder. Psychiatry Res. (2014) 215:329–34. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2013.11.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Creemers DHM, Scholte RHJ, Engels RCME, Prinstein MJ, Wiers RW. Damaged self-esteem is associated with internalizing problems. Front Psychol. (2013) 4:152. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Dimaro LV, Roberts NA, Moghaddam NG, Dawson DL, Brown I, Reuber M. Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancies in people with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures. Epilepsy Behav. (2015) 46:109–17. doi: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2015.03.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Leeuwis FH, Koot HM, Creemers DH, van Lier PA. Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancies, victimization and the development of late childhood internalizing problems. J Abnor Child Psychol. (2015) 43:909–19. doi: 10.1007/s10802-014-9959-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Schröder-Abé M, Rudolph A, Schütz A. High implicit self-esteem is not necessarily advantageous: discrepancies between explicit and implicit self-esteem and their relationship with anger expression and psychological health. Eur J Pers. (2007) 21:319–39. doi: 10.1002/per.626

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Valiente C, Cantero D, Vazquez C, Sanchez A, Provencio M, Espinosa R. Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancies in paranoia and depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (2011) 120:691–9. doi: 10.1037/a0022856

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Eberl C, Winkler I, Pawelczack S, Tröbitz E, Rinck M, Becker ES, et al. Self-esteem consistency predicts the course of therapy in depressed patients. PLoS ONE. (2018) 13:e0199957. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0199957

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Poulsen C, Luu P, Crane SM, Quiring J, Tucker DM. Frontolimbic activity and cognitive bias in major depression. J Abnorm Psychol. (2009) 118:494–506. doi: 10.1037/a0015920

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Waters AC, Tucker DM. Principal components of electrocortical activity during self-evaluation indicate depressive symptom severity. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2016) 11:1335–43. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsw046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Dainer-best J, Trujillo LT, Schnyer DM, Beevers CG. Sustained engagement of attention is associated with increased negative self-referent processing in major depressive disorder. Biol Psychol. (2017) 129:231–41. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2017.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Speed BC, Nelson BD, Auerbach RP, Klein DN, Hajcak G. Depression risk and electrocortical reactivity during self-referential emotional processing in 8 to 14 year-old girls. J Abnorm Psychol. (2016) 125:607–19. doi: 10.1037/abn0000173

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. West WC, Holcomb PJ. Imaginal, Semantic, and Surface-Level Processing of Concrete and Abstract Words: An Electrophysiological Investigation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. (2000) 12:1024–37. doi: 10.1162/08989290051137558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Kutas M, Federmeier KD. Thirty years and counting: finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Ann Rev Psychol. (2011) 62:621. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. White KR, Crites SL, Taylor JH, Corral G. Wait, what? Assessing stereotype incongruities using the N400 ERP component. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. (2009) 4:191. doi: 10.1093/scan/nsp004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Yang J, Hong L, Jiang Q, Qinglin Z. Effects of self evaluation on late ERP components. yes Natural in progress. (2007) B07:106–9.

Google Scholar

102. Naumann E, Bartussek D, Diedrich O, Laufer ME. Assessing cognitive and affective information processing functions of the brain by means of the late positive complex of the event-related potential. J Psychophysiol. (1992) 63:285–98.

Google Scholar

103. Drevets WC, Price JL, Furey ML. Brain structural and functional abnormalities in mood disorders: implications for neurocircuitry models of depression. Brain Struct Funct. (2008) 213:93–118. doi: 10.1007/s00429-008-0189-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Morel N, Villain N, Rauchs G, Gaubert M, Piolino P, Landeau B, et al. Brain activity and functional coupling changes associated with self-reference effect during both encoding and retrieval. PLoS ONE. (2014) 9:e90488. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0090488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Gillihan SJ, Farah MJ. Is self special? A critical review of evidence from experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Psychol Bull. (2005) 131:76–97. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.76

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Grimm S, Ernst J, Boesiger P, Schuepbach D, Hell D, Boeker H, et al. Increased self-focus in major depressive disorder is related to neural abnormalities in subcortical-cortical midline structures. Hum Brain Mapp. (2009) 30:2617–27. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20693

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Mor N, Winquist J. Self-focused attention and negative affect: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. (2002) 128:638. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.128.4.638

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Yoshimura S, Okamoto Y, Onoda K, Matsunaga M, Ueda K, Suzuki S, et al. Rostral anterior cingulate cortex activity mediates the relationship between the depressive symptoms and the medial prefrontal cortex activity. J Affect Disord. (2010) 122:76–85. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2009.06.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Bradley KA, Colcombe S, Henderson SE, Alonso CM, Milham MP, Gabbay V. Neural correlates of self-perceptions in adolescents with major depressive disorder. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2016) 19:87–97. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Kuhn S, Gallinat J. Resting-state brain activity in schizophrenia and major depression: a quantitative meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull. (2013) 39:358–65. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbr151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

111. Pavlickova H, Turnbull OH, Bentall RP. Discrepancies between explicit and implicit self-esteem and their relationship to symptoms of depression and mania. Psychol Psychother Theor Res Prac. (2014) 87:311–23. doi: 10.1111/papt.12015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

112. Grimm S, Boesiger P, Beck J, Schuepbach D, Bermpohl F, Walter M, et al. Altered negative BOLD responses in the default-mode network during emotion processing in depressed subjects. Neuropsychopharmacology. (2009) 34:932. doi: 10.1038/npp.2008.81

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Masten CL, Eisenberger NI, Borofsky LA, Mcnealy K, Pfeifer JH, Dapretto M. Subgenual anterior cingulate responses to peer rejection: a marker of adolescents' risk for depression. Dev Psychopathol. (2011) 23:283. doi: 10.1017/s0954579410000799

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Moritz D, Roberts JE. Self-other agreement and metaperception accuracy across the Big Five: examining the roles of depression and self-esteem. J Personal. (2017) 86:296–307. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12313

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Ranjith G, Farmer A, Mcguffin P, Cleare AJ. Personality as a determinant of social functioning in depression. J Affect Disord. (2005) 84:73–6. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2004.10.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

116. Roberts SB, Kendler KS. Neuroticism and self-esteem as indices of the vulnerability to major depression in women. Psychol Med. (1999) 29:1101–9. doi: 10.1017/S0033291799008739

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Schmitz N, Kugler J, Rollnik J. On the relation between neuroticism, self-esteem, and depression: results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Comprehens Psychiatry. (2003) 44:169–76. doi: 10.1016/S0010-440X(03)00008-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

118. Chiao JY, Harada T, Komeda H, Li Z, Mano Y, Saito D, et al. Neural basis of individualistic and collectivistic views of self. Hum Brain Mapp. (2009) 30:2813–20. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20707

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Bosson JK, Brown RP, Zeigler-Hill V, Swann WB. Self-enhancement tendencies among people with high explicit self-esteem: the moderating role of implicit self-esteem. Self Identity. (2003) 2:169–87. doi: 10.1080/15298860309029

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Leary MR, Tambor ES, Terdal SK, Downs DL. Self-esteem as an interpersonal monitor: the sociometer hypothesis. J Pers Soc Psychol. (1995) 68:270–4. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.68.3.518

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Pfeifer JH, Lieberman MD, Dapretto M. “I Know You Are But What Am I?!”: neural bases of self- and social knowledge retrieval in children and adults. J Cogn Neurosci. (2007) 19:1323–37. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2007.19.8.1323

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Hauser J, Leszczynska A, Samochowiec J, Czerski PM, Ostapowicz A, Chlopocka M, et al. Association analysis of the insertion/deletion polymorphism in serotonin transporter gene in patients with affective disorder. Eur Psychiatry J Assoc Eur Psychiatr. (2003) 18:129. doi: 10.1016/S0924-9338(03)00026-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Dinger U, Ehrenthal JC, Nikendei C, Schauenburg H. Change in self-esteem predicts depressive symptoms at follow-up after intensive multimodal psychotherapy for major depression. Clin Psychol Psychother. (2017) 24:1040–6. doi: 10.1002/cpp.2067

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

124. Orth U, Robins RW. Understanding the Link between low self-esteem and depression. Curr Direct Psychol Sci. (2013) 22:455–60. doi: 10.1177/0963721413492763

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

125. Sowislo JF, Orth U. Does low self-esteem predict depression and anxiety? A meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Psychol Bull. (2013) 139:213–40. doi: 10.1037/a0028931

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

126. Wegener I, Geiser F, Alfter S, Mierke J, Imbierowicz K, Kleiman A, et al. Changes of explicitly and implicitly measured self-esteem in the treatment of major depression: evidence for implicit self-esteem compensation. Comprehensive Psychiatry. (2015) 58:57–67. doi: 10.1016/j.comppsych.2014.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

127. Hulme N, Hirsch C, Stopa L. Images of the self and self-esteem: do positive self-images improve self-esteem in social anxiety? Cogn Behav Ther. (2012) 41:163. doi: 10.1080/16506073.2012.664557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

128. Keng SL, Seah STH, Tong EMW, Smoski M. Effects of brief mindful acceptance induction on implicit dysfunctional attitudes and concordance between implicit and explicit dysfunctional attitudes. Behav Res Ther. (2016) 83:1–10. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2016.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

129. Remmers C, Topolinski S, Koole SL. Why being mindful may have more benefits than you realize: Mindfulness improves both explicit and implicit mood regulation. Mindfulness. (2016) 7:829–37. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0520-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

130. Di SM, Norbury R, Harmer CJ. Short-term antidepressant administration reduces negative self-referential processing in the medial prefrontal cortex in subjects at risk for depression. Mol Psychiatry. (2012) 17:503–10. doi: 10.1038/mp.2011.16

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

131. Dubini A, Bosc M, Polin V. Noradrenaline-selective versus serotonin-selective antidepressant therapy: differential effects on social functioning. J Psychopharmacol. (1997) 11:17–23.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

132. Matthews SC, Simmons AN, Strigo IA, Arce E, Stein MB, Paulus MP. Escitalopram attenuates posterior cingulate activity during self-evaluation in healthy volunteers. Psychiatry Res. (2010) 182:81–7. doi: 10.1016/j.pscychresns.2010.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

133. Beck AT, Ward CH, Mendelson M, Mock J, Erbaugh J. An inventory for measuring depression. Arch General Psychiatry. (1961) 4:561–71. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1961.01710120031004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: major depressive disorder, self-knowledge, abnormality, behavioral abnormality, neurological abnormality

Citation: Lou YX, Lei Y, Mei Y, Leppänen PHT and Li H (2019) Review of Abnormal Self-Knowledge in Major Depressive Disorder. Front. Psychiatry 10:130. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00130

Received: 30 May 2018; Accepted: 22 February 2019;
Published: 28 March 2019.

Edited by:

Fushun Wang, Nanjing University of Chinese Medicine, China

Reviewed by:

Chiara Fabbri, King's College London, United Kingdom
Juan Yang, Southwest University, China

Copyright © 2019 Lou, Lei, Mei, Leppänen and Li. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Yi Lei, yutian@szu.edu.cn

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.