Skip to main content

CLINICAL TRIAL article

Front. Psychiatry, 26 February 2018
Sec. Schizophrenia
This article is part of the Research Topic Neurobiology, Clinical Course, and Therapeutic Approaches of Treatment Resistant Schizophrenia: Toward an Integrated View View all 12 articles

Randomized Clinical Trial with e-MotionalTraining® 1.0 for Social Cognition Rehabilitation in Schizophrenia

  • 1Department of Psychobiology and Clinical Psychology, Psychology School, University of Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Instituto Biomédico Galicia Sur, Centro de Investigación Biomédica en Red de Salud Mental (CIBERSAM), Complexo Hospitalario Universitario de Ourense, Ourense, Spain
  • 3Complexo Hospitalario Universitario de Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain
  • 4Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Santiago de Compostela, Santiago de Compostela, Spain

Background: Schizophrenia patients present deficits in social cognition (SC), emotion and social perception, theory of mind (ToM), and attributional style. This study tested the efficacy, in real clinical conditions, of a online self-training program in SC, e-Motional Training®, in comparison with treatment as usual.

Method: A randomized single-blinded multicenter clinical trial was conducted with 60 schizophrenia stable outpatients. All patients (control and intervention) were treated with drug therapy, case management, and individual and group psychotherapy (not focused on SC). Intervention group was treated with e-Motional Training®, an online program devised for SC rehabilitation.

Statistical analysis: A descriptive analysis and parametric/non-parametric tests were used to compare both groups at baseline. Analysis of covariance was used to compared post–pre changes in SC between the two interventions. If the group effect was significant, follow-up univariate test (t-test for dependent samples) was carried out in each group to verify whether the effect was due to improvement in the intervention group or deterioration in the control group. We considered statistically significant differences with P < 0.05.

Results: Significant improvements were obtained in the intervention group in emotion recognition and most ToM variables in comparison with the control group.

Discussion: e-Motional Training® seems to be a promising online training tool for SC deficits in schizophrenia, covering the lack of similar intervention instruments in our community.

Background

Social cognition (SC) is defined as the mental operations that underpin perceiving, interpreting, and generating responses during social interactions, including the intentions, dispositions, and behaviors of others (1). The Social Cognition Psychometric Evaluation study identified four core domains of SC, namely emotion recognition (ER), social perception, theory of mind (ToM)/mental state attribution, and attributional style (AS)/bias (2).

In schizophrenia, negative symptoms have been associated with poor performance in SC (3). In particular, individuals with schizophrenia show deficiencies in ER compared with non-clinical participants (4, 5), and these difficulties are significantly associated with symptom severity (6). These limitations are primarily manifested in the identification of negative valence emotions, especially the emotion of fear (710). Longitudinal studies have shown that these difficulties are stable over the course of the disease (11, 12) although there is evidence that individuals in the remission phase perform better on ER tests than individuals who are in the acute phase of the disorder (6). These difficulties are also considered to have a moderate association with social functioning of hospitalized patients (13) in comparison with outpatients (14).

Moreover, difficulties in ToM have been associated with negative symptoms, passivity, behavioral disorders, and paranoid symptoms (3, 15, 16). Studies have found that greater hostile attributions (e.g., increased tendency to report guilt/hostility/aggression in response to ambiguous social situations) correlate with higher levels of positive symptoms, anxiety, depression, and general emotional discomfort (17, 18).

In schizophrenia, these difficulties are associated with poorer social functioning (19), fewer social relationships, and poorer quality of life (5, 20). Various research studies have found that SC serves as a mediator between neurocognition and functional results (21) and determines the quality of interpersonal interactions, which facilitates the enjoyment of recreational activities for individuals with schizophrenia (2225). SC is considered a predictor of social functioning even more relevant than neurocognition (19). However, these difficulties are not restricted to schizophrenia but are also observed in other severe mental disorders (2628).

Patients with schizophrenia often report these difficulties. Therefore, there is a urgent need to find new treatment strategies to enable individuals with schizophrenia to improve these skills (29), given that drug treatments (typical and atypical antipsychotics) generally only have a marginal impact on the domains that constitute SC and social functioning (30). Conversely, there is evidence that SC in schizophrenia can be improved through psychosocial intervention (29, 3134).

In view of the significant impact of social cognitive deficits on daily functioning, many interventions have been developed over the past decade to ameliorate social cognitive deficits. Some interventions using virtual reality, cognitive behavioral techniques, and errorless learning in social skills training show positive results in social functioning, but without specifically targeting SC (3538). Targeted interventions hold much promise for improving SC, particularly ER and ToM. Improvement in ER has been reported, particularly in facial affect recognition. Most of these targeted interventions, such as Training of Affect Recognition (39), Attention Shaping, or MicroExpression Training Tool (40), focus primarily on training affect recognition with good outcomes. ToM is the second most commonly targeted domain, with Mental-State Reasoning Training for Social Cognitive Impairment (SoCog-MSRT) (41), Mary Eddie Bill (MEB) (42), Emotion and ToM Imitation (43), and Theory of Mind Intervention (44) developed to provide effective in-depth training, but with contradictory results in this domain (45). AS is only specifically targeted in SoCog-MSRT and MEB. Social perception and AS appear to be more difficult to measure and train, as evidenced by a meta-analysis that showed no significant effects on these two domains after social cognitive training (46).

Besides video clips, cartoon comic strips, and photographs, computerized online social cognitive games and virtual reality have recently been utilized with high patient satisfaction (36, 37, 47). Specifically, virtual reality has been used for social skills training, but its application has not been yet oriented toward SC training.

e-Motional Training® 1.0 (ET) allows online self-training and stores the data of each individual session. ET is designed following the basic principles of neuropsychological rehabilitation in this domain (4850). The program aims to deliver realistic and natural but attractive exercises of short duration without irrelevant stimuli or distractions, while offering continuous feedback. ER tasks are designed with increasing difficulty, starting with tutorials, following with eyes and mouths recognition and finally scaling to microexpression training. An animated short film with 33 scenes is the vehicle for ToM, social perception, and AS stories. After each scene, a series of questions including ToM, AS, and control questions are posed. When the answer is incorrect, the patient receives metacognitive suggestions, which lead the user to think about the situation from a different perspective or prompts the user to pay attention to specific aspects of the film.

The program was composed of 12 1-h sessions (the minimum number of face-to-face sessions reported in previous studies).

Our hypothesis was that intervention with treatment as usual (TAU) + ET results in greater improvements in the main domains of SC and the measures of social functioning compared with TAU.

The aim of this study is therefore to assess the possible effects of a new SC training program, e-Motional Training® 1.0 (ET), in ER, ToM, AS, and social functioning.

Method

A randomized, multicenter, single-blind clinical trial was performed. Sixty patients with schizophrenia were recruited in Psychiatric Day Hospitals at Ourense, Coruña and Vigo and in Associations of Persons and Families with Mental Illness at Vigo, Santiago de Compostela, Coruña and Ourense. After recruitment, the sample was randomized in each center into two balanced groups.

Inclusion and Exclusion Criteria

We included patients who voluntarily agreed to participate in the study, aged 18–50 years with a diagnosis of schizophrenia (DSM-IV TR), who were clinically stable (no acute psychotic symptoms and not hospitalized during the last 3 months), and who had no comorbidity with other psychiatric or neurological diseases (International Neuropsychiatric Interview-MINI) and excluding current substance abuse (except nicotine).

Treatment Conditions

Control Group (TAU)

All patients received drug therapy, case management, and individual and group psychotherapy not focused on social cognitive rehabilitation.

Intervention Group (TAU + ET)

The intervention group received the same intervention of control group plus 12 sessions (1 h per week) with ET®. All participants in the intervention group completed the same number of sessions. To start the intervention, the patient accessed the website www.e-motionaltraining.com (version 1.0) and registered with a username and password. The first four meetings (1 h each session) were dedicated to recognizing facial emotions. This section included a pretest and posttest, tutorials, and scaling minigames starting with eyes and mouths and finally microexpression (<250 ms) training. The next eight sessions (1 h each) include watching a short, interactive animated cartoon in which a couple invites their friends to their home for a party. As the story unfolds, instances of miscommunication occur among the actors, causing various emotions and mental conditions such as anger, affection, appreciation, and jealousy. After each scene, the user is queried about what happened, with questions about ToM (interpreting irony, insinuations, faux pas, second-order false beliefs, etc.), social perception (interpretation and analysis of the social situation through the visual content of each scene), and AS (the individuals’ attributions to the events, and questions such as, “What kind of thinking would result in Cristina getting better results in this situation?”), as well as control questions. The game provides user feedback and, in the event of errors, can display a hyperlink with information and metacognitive strategies, whose objective is to help users understand the scene that they just watched.

Supervision of the ET group was conducted by the center’s staff as a routine activity, and evaluators were blind to the assignment. No help or guidance regarding social cognitive issues was given, and only advice regarding computer use was provided.

Measurements

Symptoms and Cognitive Ability

Positive and Negative Symptom Scale (PANSS)

Positive and Negative Symptom Scale assesses positive and negative symptom severity (51). The scale consists of 30 items (symptoms) that are scored from 1 (absent) to 7 (extreme). The scale has three subscales: positive (PANSS-P), negative (PANSS-N), and general psychopathology (PANSS-GP).

Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (K-BIT)

Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test provides a verbal intelligence quotient (IQ), a non-verbal IQ, and a compound IQ that summarizes the total performance on the test (52).

Social Cognition

Ekman 60 Faces Test

The test contains 60 photographs of faces with expressions of the 6 basic emotions: anger, disgust, sadness, fear, surprise, and happiness (53). An overall score of 60 indicates the best possible performance, and each basic emotion also has a maximum score of 10 points.

Hinting Task

Ten stories are presented to the patient who must infer the characters actual intention when using indirect speech (54). The total score on the test ranges from 0 to 20 (55).

Recognition of Faux Pas

The participant must recognize the embarrassing situations in the 10 faux pas’ stories, while correctly rejecting misinterpretation of the 10 control situations (56). The test provides scores for five variables: faux pas detection, understanding inappropriateness, intentions, and belief and empathy (57).

F. Happé’s Strange Stories

F. Happé’s Strange Stories include stories containing irony and white lies utterances (58). In each of the stories, the character says something that should not be interpreted literally. The participant is asked to explain why the characters said what they said.

Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC)

A short film is shown to the participant who must answer a series of questions regarding the ToM and emotional content depicted in social interactions (59).

Ambiguous Intentions Hostility Questionnaire (AIHQ)

The AIHQ is an AS questionnaire to measure the biases of hostility perception, composite blame, and aggressive response (60). The AIHQ is composed of 15 hypothetical negative situations. Each situation was varied in intentionality: five scenarios are accidental (e.g., “You’re dancing at a club and someone bumps into you from behind.”), five scenarios are ambiguous (e.g., “You walk past a bunch of teenagers at a mall and your hear them start to laugh.”), and five scenarios are intentional (e.g., “Your neighbors are playing loud music. You knock on the door and ask them to turn it down. Fifteen minutes later, the music is loud again.”). First, participants are prompted to imagine the scenario happening to them. Then, they are asked to write down what is the reason they think that other person (or persons) acted that way. The AIHQ yielded hostility perception and aggressive response bias scores and a composite blame bias score. The scales for the hostility perception and aggressive response indices were rated by rater from 1 (“not at all hostile”) to 5 (“very hostile”) and 1 (“not at all aggressive”) to 5 (“very aggressive”), respectively. The composite blame score (range, 1–5.3) is an average score of subjects’ ratings of intent (range, 1–6; rating about the degree to which the other person committed the act on purpose), anger (range, 1–5; rating about how angry the situation would make subject feel), and blame (range, 1–5; rating about how much subjects blame the other person for the outcome).

Emotional Intelligence

Mayer-Salovey-Caruso Emotional Intelligence Test (MSCEIT)

This test is composed of 141 items and provides a score for emotional intelligence (EIQ), which in turn can be divided into two domains: experiential (EEIQ) and strategic emotional intelligence (SEIQ) (61). The test also provides scores for four areas of emotional intelligence: the ability to perceive emotions accurately (PEIQ), using emotions to facilitate thought (emotional facilitation, FEIQ), understanding emotions (UEIQ), and managing emotions (MEIQ).

Social Functioning

Social Functioning Scale (SFS)

This scale is specifically designed to assess the social functioning of individuals with schizophrenia (62). The scale consists of seven subscales: social isolation/interaction, interpersonal communication, independence-execution, independence-competence, free time, prosocial activities, and employment/occupation. We applied the self-reported version (SFS-SR).

Sample Size

In our pilot study (63), the measure with most reduced differences pre–post intervention was Happé’s Strange Stories with an initial mean (±SD) of 8.20 (±3.58) that increased to 11.20 (±4.68) after intervention. By using these measures for a power of 80% and a confidence level of 95%, the required sample size, assuming 5% of losses, was 30 patients in each group.

Ethical Aspects

This study has been carried out in accordance with national and European legislation on clinical research, following international ethical recommendations, the Declaration of Helsinki, and the Council of Europe with regard to the Convention on Human Rights and Biomedicine. The study has complied at all times with the requirements established in the Spanish legislation in the field of biomedical research, personal data protection, and bioethics. This study was approved by the local ethics committee (Comité de Etica e Investigación Clínica de Galicia) (Registration code: 2014/459) and registered in an international RCT database (BioMed Center: ISRCTN83459317).

Statistical Analysis

Quantitative Gaussian variables were described by mean, SD, and not Gaussian variables as median (range). The qualitative variables were described by frequencies and percentages (%). Parametric/non-parametric tests (Chi square for categorical variables and Student’s t-test and Mann–Whitney U test for continuous variables) were used to compare both groups at baseline.

We compared post–pre changes in SC between the two interventions (TAU + e-Motional Training® vs. TAU) with an analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), entering the change scores on each test (Ekman, Faux Pas, Happé, Hinting, MASC, MSCEIT, and AIHQ) as the dependent variable, treatment as the fixed group effect, and K-BIT score as the covariate.

If the group effect was significant, follow-up univariate test (t-test for dependent samples) was carried out in each group to verify whether the effect was due to improvement in the intervention group or deterioration in the control group.

We considered statistically significant differences with P < 0.05. The sample size was calculated using the Epidat 4.1, and the analyses were performed with SPSS 22.0 and R (http://www.r-project.org).

Results

A total of 77 participants were selected, 15 patients did not meet inclusion criteria, and 1 suffered a relapse prior to randomization. Finally, 61 patients were assigned to the control group (TAU) or to the intervention group (TAU + ET) between January and November 2015 (Figure 1). Prior to retest one patient in the control group abandoned the study and was excluded for further analysis.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flowchart.

Most of the patients recruited were men 47 (78.3%), with a mean (±SD) global age of 39.17 years (±7.03).

There were no significant differences between the two groups at baseline in sociodemographic variables (age, gender, and education) compound IQ (P = 0.385) and non-verbal IQ (P = 0.143) measured with K-BIT. However, significant differences were observed in the verbal IQ (P = 0.042) being the scores in both groups within the normality range (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Demographic and clinical characteristics of sample (N = 60).

All participants were treated with antipsychotics, with a mean chlorpromazine dose 634.82 (±513.01) in control group and 564.74 (±340.19) in the intervention group. There were no significant differences between them (P = 0.807).

To demonstrate the existence of differences in SC variables after treatment, we compared post–pre changes between the two interventions (TAU + e-Motional Training® vs. TAU) with an ANCOVA, entering the change scores on each test (Ekman, Faux Pas, Happé, Hinting, MASC, MSCEIT, and AIHQ) as the dependent variable, treatment as the fixed group effect, and K-Bit score as the covariate. ANCOVA results are displayed in Table 2. These results indicate that there were statistically significant differences in change scores between e-Motional Training® and TAU group in Ekman’s (F = 48.805, P < 0.001) with a large effect size (ηp2=0.461), Faux Pas (F = 9.728; P = 0.003) with a large size effect (ηp2=0.146), Happé ToM (F = 9.447; P = 0.003) with a large effect size (ηp2=0.142), Hinting (F = 14.286; P < 0.001) with a large effect size (ηp2=0.200), MASC change score (F = 12.466; P = 0.001) with a large size effect (ηp2=0.179), and PANSS negative change score (F = 5.169; P = 0.027) with a moderate size effect (ηp2=0.083). No differences were found in Faux Pas and Happé control stories change scores nor in MSCEIT or PANSS positive change scores. Finally, regarding the Ambiguous Stories of AIHQ, only differences in aggressive bias (F = 4.405; P = 0.04) were significant with a moderate size effect (ηp2=0.072).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Results in social cognition variables.

Subsequently follow-up univariate tests (t-test for dependent samples) were carried out in ANCOVA’s significant variables confirming that the effect was due to improvement in the intervention group and not to deterioration in the control group (P < 0.001). Changes in PANSS negative (P = 0.001) and AIHQ aggressive bias (P = 0.018) were also due to improvement in the intervention group.

There were no differences in the seven variables of the SFS-SR.

Discussion

One of the main objectives of cognitive therapy in schizophrenia is to improve social functioning. In this regard, SC programs seem more promising than those directed at neurocognition (29). However, during the last decade, SC rehabilitation has been delivered in group format requiring a significant number of sessions and specialized training for the therapists, therefore limiting its accessibility (64). Bearing these questions in mind, our team designed ET showing its feasibility in a pilot study (32). This study is the first randomized controlled trial conducted with this program.

After treatment, the intervention group showed a significant improvement in ER (Table 2) reaching scores posttreatment within the normal range (65), this result is consistent with other interventions (40, 6668).

Regarding ToM, the intervention group showed significant improvements at Faux Pas, Happé’s Strange Stories, Hinting Task, and MASC (Table 2). However, even with this improvement, our intervention group did not achieve the level of competence of the healthy population, as was found in other studies (32, 33, 69). Nevertheless, our study indicates that online rehabilitation of complex domains of ToM is possible and also that our training strategies are in the correct path.

Unfortunately, the ANCOVA results in AS only show changes in the Aggressive bias of ambiguous scenes with a reduced effect size. However, this is no surprising because the metacognitive instructions delivered with our ToM short film are not focused on AS and should perhaps deserve a specific module. Nevertheless, the absence of positive results in this domain is consistent with other studies (28, 70).

Furthermore, there were no differences in terms of emotional intelligence assessed with MSCEIT after the intervention, as we can see in Table 2, the pretests in both groups were in the normal range [on the MSCEIT’s IQ-like scale with a mean of 100 and a SD of 15, a respondent would have to get a score higher than 116 or lower than 84 to be statistically significantly (P < 0.05) above or below average]; therefore, the instrument seemed unable to detect impairments in ER or ToM nor changes after treatment, a finding also consistent with previous studies (33, 47, 70). For a more detailed review on the concerns over the MSCEIT’s validity, see Maul (2012) (71).

Finally, there was a reduction in PANSS-negative change score (Table 2) in the intervention group, suggesting an eventual effect of the intervention in reducing negative symptoms (3).

In conclusion, e-Motional Training® is one of the first online programs that has shown its usefulness in the training of the most studied SC domains. Compared with other available programs (28, 72), this program allows online self-training and follow-up by therapists, thus filling the lack of similar intervention instruments in our community.

Our study has a number of limitations, including the fact that most participants in the sample underwent drug treatment; therefore, we do not know whether the relationships found in this study can be replicated in other populations, including individuals who refuse to undergo treatment. Most of the participants in our sample had a diagnosis of chronic stable schizophrenia; therefore, we ignore the performance and feasibility of ET in first episodes or in individuals at high risk for psychosis. Moreover, the majority of the study participants were men, and therefore, the generalizability of the results must be regarded with caution. However, it is a well-known fact that schizophrenia is more severe in men than in women, and therefore, day hospitals and day centers are more frequented by men than women (73).

Regarding participation remarkably, attendance in our sample was perfect. Although this fact could be surprising, it is worth noting that research studies in schizophrenia in our community are scarce, and therefore, it is easier to raise the interest of patients as well as therapists and evaluators, especially if the active treatment is a computerized online program with an attractive interface, cognitively not demanding and allowing self-training, factors that should be taken into account to explain the adherence of patients during the study.

Regarding our results on social functioning, measured with the SFS-SR, the lack of significance of our findings should be considered in the light of the following facts: given that chronic patients have insight and metacognitive deficits, using a self-evaluated scale to measure social functioning was not the best idea. Moreover, it seems to us that social functioning has to be the goal but probably a standard too high for computerized interventions. This is a common place in other clinical domains, for instance in Alzheimer’s, where generalizability of computerized interventions to daily living is currently absent (74). Our aim is to create an online tool for helping patients to practice ER and ToM interactions but by no means to substitute group therapy or social skills training. In our opinion, computerized tools give the patients the opportunity to drill and practice skills hardly rehearsable outside the virtual realm, but at least in chronic cases, these skills should be trained in vivo in protected environments before aspiring to show generalization in the real world.

Finally, the study was conducted vs. TAU and not vs. another active condition. This is obviously not the best design, but our inspiration was based in recent studies in SC rehabilitation both in group therapy and with computerized tools (32, 35, 7579). However, it must be taken into account that there is a scarcity of data regarding efficacy of computerized programs for SC and that comparing at this point a computerized tool with group strategies seems at least to us unfair.

In terms of the program’s future, version 2.0 is now available, including version 1.0 games and ER tasks devised to improve processing speed, mimicry abilities, and prosodic recognition. Regarding ToM, a short film with real actors and a 2.5 h gameplay graphic adventure with puzzles on ToM and moral dilemmas have been included, and their aim is to offer a gradual and longer training maintaining the attention of patients and their will to improve. The environment has been created with game mechanics, and it has metacognitive hyperlinks designed for self-training.

Ethics Statement

The study was approved by the Clinical Research Ethics Committee of Galicia (EC) and met all applicable ethical and legal standards (registration code 2014/459) and it has been registered in BioMed Center an international RCT database (ISRCTN83459317).

Author Contributions

AGC (principal investigator), YMS, and MVC are the creators of e-Motional Training. YMS, MVC, and AGC designed the study, selected participants, applied the intervention, extracted data, and supervised the study. FDL participated in patient selection and in obtaining and extracting data, and MRA and RM reviewed the manuscript. Moreover, RM contributed to perform statistical analysis and writing of the final version. The manuscript was authored by YMS, MVC, and AGC. All the authors have had full access to data in the study, have personally reviewed the manuscript, and gave final approval of the version attached.

Conflict of Interest Statement

AC, YS, and MC are the intellectual authors of e-Motional Training® 1.0, a program funded by the Galician Department through the PRIS program, whose objective is to generate a spin-off.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank the following patient’s associations: APEM, DOA, Virxe da Cerca and MOREA for their collaboration. María Teresa Alves (MSci) for her statistical support and Dr. Díaz del Valle, Dr. Serrano, Dr. Paramo, and Dr. Olivares for their help. The project was 80% funded by the Galician Department of Health with European Regional Development Funds through the PRIS program (Program for the precommercial development of Research results of the public health System of Galicia). It has also received the Siota Grant VIII Edition of the College of Psychologists of Galicia, the 2013 Biannual Grant of the Medical- Surgical Academy of Ourense, the II Spanish National award Innova-eVia 2015, and the National Award for the Best Videogame of Health at e-Health Spanish Congress in Madrid 2016. Finally the authors would like to express their gratitute to the referees for their comments that help to improve the manuscript.

References

1. Green MF, Penn DL, Bentall R, Carpenter WT, Gaebel W, Gur RC, et al. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr Bull (2008) 34(6):1211–20. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Pinkham AE, Harvey PD, Penn DL. Social cognition psychometric evaluation: results of the Final Validation Study. Schizophr Bull (2017). doi:10.1093/schbul/sbx117

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Green MF, Olivier B, Crawley JN, Penn DL, Silverstein S. Social cognition in schizophrenia: recommendations from the measurement and treatment research to improve cognition in schizophrenia new approaches conference. Schizophr Bull (2005) 31(4):882–7. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbi049

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Lysaker PH, Gumley A, Luedtke B, Buck KD, Ringer JM, Olesek K, et al. Social cognition and metacognition in schizophrenia: evidence of their independence and linkage with outcomes. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2013) 127(3):239247. doi:10.1111/acps.12012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Pinkham AE, Penn DL, Perkins DO, Lieberman J. Implications for the neural basis of social cognition for the study of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2003) 160(5):815–24. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.815

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hooker C, Park S. Emotion processing and its relationship to social functioning in schizophrenia patients. Psychiatry Res (2002) 112(1):41–50. doi:10.1016/S01651781(02)00177-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Maat A, van Montfort SJ, de Nijs J, Derks EM, Kahn RS, Linszen DH, et al. Emotion processing in schizophrenia is state and trait dependent. Schizophr Res (2015) 161(2–3):392–8. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2014.11.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Edwards J, Pattison PE, Jackson HJ, Wales RJ. Facial affect and affective prosody recognition in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2001) 48(2–3):235–53. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00099-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Evangeli M, Broks P. Face processing in schizophrenia: parallels with the effects of amygdala damage. Cogn Neuropsychiatry (2000) 5(2):81–104. doi:10.1080/135468000395754

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Kohler CG, Bilker W, Hagendoorn M, Gur RE, Gur RC. Emotion recognition deficit in schizophrenia: association with symptomatology and cognition. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 48:127–36. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00847-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Erol A, Putgul G, Kosger F, Ersoy B. Facial emotion recognition in schizophrenia: the impact of gender. Psychiatry Investig (2013) 10(1):69–74. doi:10.4306/pi.2013.10.1.69

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Amminger GP, Schäfer MR, Klier CM, Schlögelhofer M, Mossaheb N, Thompson A, et al. Facial and vocal affect perception in people at ultra-high risk of psychosis, first-episode schizophrenia and healthy controls. Early Interv Psychiatry (2012) 6(4):450–4. doi:10.1111/j.1751-7893.2012.00362.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Vohs JL, Lysaker PH, Francis MM, Hamm J, Buck KD, Olesek K, et al. Metacognition, social cognition, and symptoms in patients with first episode and prolonged psychoses. Schizophr Res (2014) 153(1–3):54–9. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2014.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Penn DL, Spaulding W, Reed D, Sullivan M. The relationship of social cognition to ward behavior in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (1996) 20(3):327–35. doi:10.1016/0920-9964(96)00010-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Ihnen G, Penn DL, Corrigan PW, Martin J. Social perception and social skill in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (1998) 80:275–86. doi:10.1016/S0165-1781(98)00079-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Frith C, Corcoran R. Exploring “theory of mind” in people with schizophrenia. Psychol Med (1996) 26(3):521–30. doi:10.1017/S0033291700035601

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Horan WP, Green MF. Treatment social cognition in schizophrenia: current status and future directions. Schizophr Res (2017). doi:10.1016/j.schres.2017.07.013

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Buck BE, Healey KM, Gagen EC, Roberts DL, Penn DL. Social cognition in schizophrenia: factor structure, clinical and functional correlates. J Ment Health (2016) 8237:1–8. doi:10.3109/09638237.2015.1124397

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Flood AM, Julian Hare D, Wallis P. An investigation into social information processing in young people with Asperger syndrome. Autism (2011) 15(5):601–24. doi:10.1177/1362361310387803

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Fett AKJ, Viechtbauer W, Dominguez MD, Penn DL, van Os J, Krabbendam L. The relationship between neurocognition and social cognition with functional outcomes in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2011) 35(3):573–88. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Couture SM, Penn DL, Roberts DL. The functional significance of social cognition in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Bull (2006) 32(Suppl 1):S44–63. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbl029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Schmidt SJ, Mueller DR, Roder V. Social cognition as a mediator variable between neurocognition and functional outcome in schizophrenia: empirical review and new results by structural equation modeling. Schizophr Bull (2011) 37(Suppl 2):S41–54. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr079

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Couture S, Granholm E, Fish S. A path model investigation of neurocognition, theory of mind, social competence, negative symptoms and real-world functioning in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2011) 125(2–3):152–60. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.09.020.A

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Addington J, Saeedi H, Addington D. Influence of social perception and social knowledge on cognitive and social functioning in early psychosis. Br J Psychiatry (2006) 189:373–8. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.105.021022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Vauth R, Rüsch N, Wirtz M, Corrigan PW. Does social cognition influence the relation between neurocognitive deficits and vocational functioning in schizophrenia? Psychiatry Res (2004) 128(2):155–65. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2004.05.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Berry K, Bucci S, Kinderman P, Emsley R, Corcoran R. An investigation of attributional style, theory of mind and executive functioning in acute paranoie and remission. Psychiatry Res (2015) 226(1):84–90. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2014.12.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Fretland RA, Andersson S, Sundet K, Andreassen OA, Melle I, Vaskinn A. Theory of mind in schizophrenia: error types and associations with symptoms. Schizophr Res (2015) 162(3):42–6. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2015.01.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lahera G, Benito A, Montes JM, Fernández-Liria A, Olbert CM, Penn DL. Social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for outpatients with bipolar disorder. J Affect Disord (2013) 146(1):132–6. doi:10.1016/j.jad.2012.06.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Sparks A, McDonald S, Lino B, O’Donnell M, Green MJ. Social cognition, empathy and functional outcome in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2010) 122(1–3):172–8. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2010.06.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Wölwer W, Frommann N. Social-cognitive remediation in schizophrenia: generalization of effects of the training of affect recognition (TAR). Schizophr Bull (2011) 37(Suppl 2):63–70. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr071

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Hempel RJ, Dekker JA, van Beveren NJ, Tulen JH, Hengeveld MW. The effect of antipsychotic medication on facial affect recognition in schizophrenia: a review. Psychiatry Res (2010) 178(1):1–9. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.07.025

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Roberts DL, Combs DR, Willoughby M, Mintz J, Gibson C, Rupp B, et al. A randomized, controlled trial of Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for outpatients with schizophrenia spectrum disorders. Br J Clin Psychol (2014) 53(3):281–98. doi:10.1111/bjc.12044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fernandez-Gonzalo S, Turon M, Jodar M, Pousa E, Hernandez Rambla C, Palao D. A new computerized cognitive and social cognition training specifically designed for patients with schizophrenia/schizoaffective disorder in early stages of illness: a pilot study. Psychiatry Res (2015) 228(3):501–9. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Lindenmayer JP, McGurk SR, Khan A, Kaushik S, Thanju A, Hoffman L, et al. Improving social cognition in schizophrenia: a pilot intervention combining computerized social cognition training with cognitive remediation. Schizophr Bull (2013) 39(3):507–17. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs120

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Granholm E, McQuaid JR, McClure FS, Link PC, Perivoliotis D, Gottlieb JD, et al. Randomized controlled trial of cognitive behavioral social skills training for older people with schizophrenia: 12-month follow-up. J Clin Psychiatry (2007) 68(5):730–7. doi:10.4088/JCP.v68n0510

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Park KM, Ku J, Choi SH, Jang HJ, Park JY, Kim SI, et al. A virtual reality application in role plays of social skills training for schizophrenia: a randomized, controlled trial. Psychiatry Res (2011) 189(2):166–72. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.04.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Rus-Calafell M, Gutiérrez-Maldonado J, Ribas-Sabaté J. A virtual reality-integrated program for improving social skills in patients with schizophrenia: a pilot study. J Behav Ther Exp Psychiatry (2014) 45(1):81–9. doi:10.1016/j.jbtep.2013.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Seo JM, Ahn S, Byun EK, Kim CK. Social skills training as nursing intervention to improve the social skills and self-esteem of inpatients with chronic schizophrenia. Arch Psychiatr Nurs (2007) 21(6):317–26. doi:10.1016/j.apnu.2006.09.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Drusch K, Stroth S, Kamp D, Frommann N, Wölwer W. Effects of Training of Affect Recognition on the recognition and visual exploration of emotional faces in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2014) 159(2–3):485–90. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2014.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Russell TA, Chu E, Phillips ML. A pilot study to investigate the effectiveness of emotion recognition remediation in schizophrenia using the micro-expression training tool. Br J Clin Psychol (2006) 45(Pt 4):579–83. doi:10.1348/014466505X90866

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Marsh P, Langdon R, McGuire J, Harris A, Polito V, Coltheart M. An open clinical trial assessing a novel training program for social cognitive impairment in schizophrenia. Australas Psychiatry (2013) 21(2):122–6. doi:10.1177/1039856213475683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Roberts DL, Kleinlein P, Stevens B. An alternative to generating alternative interpretations in social cognitive therapy for psychosis. Behav Cogn Psychother (2012) 40(4):491–5. doi:10.1017/S1352465812000082

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Mazza M, Lucci G, Pacitti F, Pino MC, Mariano M, Casacchia M, et al. Could schizophrenic subjects improve their social cognition abilities only with observation and imitation of social situations? Neuropsychol Rehabil (2010) 20(5):675–703. doi:10.1080/09602011.2010.486284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Bechi M, Spangaro M, Bosia M, Zanoletti A, Fresi F, Buonocore M, et al. Theory of Mind intervention for outpatients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychol Rehabil (2013) 23(3):383–400. doi:10.1080/09602011.2012.762751

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Tan BL, Lee SA, Lee J. Social cognitive interventions for people with schizophrenia: a systematic review. Asian J Psychiatr (2016). doi:10.1016/j.ajp.2016.06.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Kurtz MM, Richardson CL. Social cognitive training for schizophrenia: a meta analytic investigation of controlled research. Schizophr Bull (2012) 38(5):1092–104. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Nahum M, Fisher M, Loewy R, Poelke G, Ventura J, Nuechterlein KH, et al. A novel, online social cognitive training program for young adults with schizophrenia: a pilot study. Schizophr Res Cogn (2014) 1(1):e11–9. doi:10.1016/j.scog.2014.01.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Brenner HD, Hodel B, Kube G, Roder V. [Cognitive therapy of schizophrenic patients:problem analysis and empirical results]. Nervenarzt (1987) 58(2):72–83.

Google Scholar

49. Ochsner KN. The social-emotional processing stream: five core constructs and their translational potential for schizophrenia and beyond. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 64(1):48–61. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.04.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Zubin J, Spring B. Vulnerability – a new view of schizophrenia. J Abnorm Psychol (1977) 86(2):103–26. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.86.2.103

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Kay SR, Fiszbein A, Qpjer LA. The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull (1987) 13(2):261–76.

Google Scholar

52. Cordero A, Calonge I. K-BIT, Test Breve de Inteligencia de Kaufman Adaptación española. 2nd ed. Madrid: TEA Ediciones (1996).

Google Scholar

53. Young A, Perrett D, Calde A, Sprengelmeyer R, Ekman P. Facial Expressions of Emotion: Stimuli and Tests (FEEST). Bury St Edmunds: Thames Valley Test Company (2002).

Google Scholar

54. Gil D, Fernández-Modamio M, Bengochea R, Arrieta M. Adaptación al español de la prueba de teoría de la mente Hinting Task. Rev Psiquiatr Salud Ment (2012) 5(2):79–88. doi:10.1016/j.rpsm.2011.11.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Janssen I, Krabbendam L, Jolles J, van Os J. Alterations in theory of mind in patients with schizophrenia and non-psychotic relatives. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2003) 108(2):110117. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0447.2003.00092.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Baron-Cohen S, Jolliffe T, Mortimore C, Robertson M. Another advanced test of theory of mind: evidence from very high functioning adults with autism or asperger syndrome. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (1997) 38(7):813–22. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1997.tb01599.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Stone V. Faux Pas Recognition Test (Adult Version) Created by Valerie Stone & Simon Baron Cohen Correct Citations for Use of This Test. (2002). p. 407–18. Available from: http://docs.autismresearchcentre.com/tests/FauxPas_Adult.pdf

Google Scholar

58. Pousa E. Measurement of Theory of Mind in Healthy Adolescents: Translation and Cultural Adaptation of F. Happé’s Theory of Mind Stories. Barcelona: Tesis Doctoral, Universidad Autónoma de Barcelona (1999).

Google Scholar

59. Lahera G, Boada L, Pousa E, Mirapeix I, Morón-Nozaleda G, Marinas L, et al. Movie for the Assessment of Social Cognition (MASC): Spanish validation. J Autism Dev Disord (2014) 44(8):1886–96. doi:10.1007/s10803-014-2061-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Combs DR, Penn DL, Wicher M, Waldheter E. The Ambiguous Intentions Hostility Questionnaire (AIHQ): a new measure for evaluating hostile social-cognitive biases in paranoia. Cogn Neuropsychiatry (2007) 12(2):128–43. doi:10.1080/13546800600787854

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Extremera N, Fernández-Berrocal P. Adaptación Española Del Test de Inteligencia Emocional de Mayer-Salovey-Caruso (MSCEIT): Manual Y Cuadernillo. Madrid: TEA (2009).

Google Scholar

62. Torres A, Olivares J. Validación en castellano de la Social Functioning Scales (Escala de Funcionamiento Social). Actas Esp Psiquiatr (2005) 33:216–20.

Google Scholar

63. Vázquez-Campo M, Maroño Y, Lahera G, Mateos R, García-Caballero A. e-motional training®: pilot study on a novel online training program on social cognition for patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res Cogn (2016) 4:10–7. doi:10.1016/j.scog.2015.11.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Roberts DL, Penn DL, Labate D, Margolis SA, Sterne A. Transportability and feasibility of Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) in community settings. Behav Cogn Psychother (2010) 38(1):35–47. doi:10.1017/S1352465809990464

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Dodich A, Cerami C, Canessa N, Crespi C, Marcone A, Arpone M, et al. Emotion recognition from facial expressions: a normative study of the Ekman 60-Faces Test in the Italian population. Neurol Sci (2014) 35(7):10151021. doi:10.1007/s10072-014-1631-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Marsh PJ, Luckett G, Russell T, Coltheart M, Green MJ. Effects of facial emotion recognition remediation on visual scanning of novel face stimuli. Schizophr Res (2012) 141(2–3):234–40. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Koelkebeck K, Kohl W, Luettgenau J, Triantafillou S, Ohrmann P, Satoh S, et al. Benefits of using culturally unfamiliar stimuli in ambiguous emotion identification: a cross-cultural study. Psychiatry Res (2015) 228:39–45. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.04.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Gohar S, Hamdi E, El Ray L, Horan W, Green M. Adapting and evaluating a social cognitive remediation program for schizophrenia in Arabic. Schizophr Res (2013) 148(13):12–7. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.05.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Peyroux E, Franck N. Improving social cognition in people with schizophrenia with RC2S: two single-case studies. Front Psychiatry (2016) 7:66. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Rocha N, Queirós C. Metacognitive and social cognition training (MSCT) in schizophrenia: a preliminary efficacy study. Schizophr Res (2013) 150(1):64–8. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2013.07.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Maul A. Examining the structure of emotional intelligence at the item level: new perspectives, new conclusions. Cogn Emot (2012) 26(3):503–20. doi:10.1080/02699931.2011.588690

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Kleinlein P. Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for individuals with schizophrenia spectrum disorders in outpatient treatment settings. (2010). Available from: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/psychdiss/18/

Google Scholar

73. Aleman A, Kahn RS, Selten JP. Sex differences in the risk of schizophrenia: evidence from metaanalysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2003) 60(6):565–71. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.60.6.565

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Hill NT, Mowszowski L, Naismith SL, Chadwick VL, Valenzuela M, Lampit A. Computerized cognitive training in older adults with mild cognitive impairment or dementia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Am J Psychiatry (2017) 174(4):329–40. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2016.16030360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Mueller DR, Schmidt SJ, Roder V. One-year randomized controlled trial and follow up of integrated neurocognitive therapy for schizophrenia outpatients. Schizophr Bull (2015) 41(3):604–16. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbu223

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Sachs G, Winklbaur B, Jagsch R, Lasser I, Kryspin-Exner I, Frommann N, et al. Training of affect recognition (TAR) in schizophrenia – impact on functional outcome. Schizophr Res (2012) 138(2–3):262–7. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2012.03.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Wang Y, Roberts DL, Xu B, Cao R, Yan M, Jiang Q. Social cognition and interaction training for patients with stable schizophrenia in Chinese community settings. Psychiatry Res (2013) 210(3):751–5. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2013.08.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Combs DR, Elerson K, Penn DL, Tiegreen JA, Nelson A, Ledet SN, et al. Stability and generalization of Social Cognition and Interaction Training (SCIT) for schizophrenia: six-month follow-up results. Schizophr Res (2009) 112(1–3):196–7. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.04.010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Roberts DL, Penn DL. Social cognition and interaction training (SCIT) for outpatients with schizophrenia: a preliminary study. Psychiatry Res (2009) 166(2–3):141–7. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2008.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: cognition, emotional adjustment, theory of mind, schizophrenia, emocional perception

Citation: Maroño Souto Y, Vázquez Campo M, Díaz Llenderrozas F, Rodríguez Álvarez M, Mateos R and García Caballero A (2018) Randomized Clinical Trial with e-MotionalTraining® 1.0 for Social Cognition Rehabilitation in Schizophrenia. Front. Psychiatry 9:40. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2018.00040

Received: 29 August 2017; Accepted: 30 January 2018;
Published: 26 February 2018

Edited by:

Felice Iasevoli, University of Naples Federico II, Italy

Reviewed by:

William Horan, University of California, Los Angeles, United States
Philip D. Harvey, Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, United States

Copyright: © 2018 Maroño Souto, Vázquez Campo, Díaz Llenderrozas, Rodríguez Álvarez, Mateos and García Caballero. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Alejandro García Caballero, alejandro.alberto.garcia.caballero@sergas.es

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.