Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 08 January 2018
Sec. Schizophrenia
This article is part of the Research Topic Neurological Soft Signs in Neuropsychiatric Conditions View all 7 articles

Neurological Soft Signs in Schizophrenia: An Update on the State- versus Trait-Perspective

  • 1Department of Psychiatry, Psychotherapy, and Psychosomatics, University Hospitals of Halle (Saale), Halle, Germany
  • 2Clienia Littenheid AG, Hospital for Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Littenheid, Switzerland
  • 3Section of Geriatric Psychiatry, Department of General Psychiatry, Center for Psychosocial Medicine, University of Heidelberg, Heidelberg, Germany

Background: Neurological soft signs (NSS) represent minor neurological signs, which indicate non-specific cerebral dysfunction. In schizophrenia, their presence has been documented extensively across all stages of the disease. Until recently, NSS were considered an endophenotype or a trait phenomenon. During the past years, however, researchers report fluctuations of the NSS scores.

Aims: To further clarify the question whether NSS exhibit state or trait components or both, studies that have investigated NSS longitudinally were reviewed.

Method: Studies which have assessed NSS longitudinally in adults suffering from schizophrenia, were searched for. The time frame was January 1966 to June 2017. Studies on teenagers were excluded because of interferences between brain maturation and pathology.

Results: Twenty-nine follow-up studies were identified. They included patients during different stages of their illness and mainly used established instruments for NSS assessment. Patients with a first episode or a remitting course predominantly show a decrease of NSS over time, whereas a worsening of NSS can be found in the chronically ill. It was shown that change of NSS total scores over time is predominantly caused by motor system subscales and to a lesser extent by sensory integration scales. With respect to medication, the majority of studies agree on a relationship between medication response and improvement of NSS while the type of antipsychotic does not seem to play a major role. Moreover, where information on side-effects is given, it does not favor a strong relationship with NSS. However, NSS seem to correlate with negative and cognitive symptoms.

Conclusion: Studies manifest a conformity regarding the presence of NSS in schizophrenia patients on the one hand. On the other hand, fluctuations of NSS scores have been widely described in subgroups. Taken together results strongly support a state-trait dichotomy of NSS. Thus, the usage of NSS as an endophenotype has to be called into question.

Introduction

It is generally accepted that neurological soft signs (NSS) are present in schizophrenia patients. These neurological abnormalities do not reflect hard pathology, i.e., localized, nuclear, or primary tract lesions (13) and are, therefore, labeled “soft.” Overall, in schizophrenia patients, distinctive NSS consistently pertain to motor coordination, motor sequencing, and sensory integration [review by Boks et al. (4)], but also to eye movements and developmental reflexes (3, 5). In first-episode (FE) patients NSS mostly strike motor coordination, motor sequencing, and developmental reflexes [review by Dazzan and Murray (6)].

Yet, the presence of NSS is not specific to schizophrenia. In early studies, they were detected in 5% (7, 8) to 50% (9, 10) of healthy individuals, depending on the instrument used. NSS can be found in patients with mood disorders (11), obsessive–compulsive disorder (12), and personality disorders (13). However, the incidence of NSS is higher in schizophrenia compared to other psychiatric disorders [review by Dazzan and Murray (6)].

Also, compared to healthy controls (HC) NSS occurrence in schizophrenia patients is more pronounced with regard to both quantity and quality (14). Several reviews have summarized the respective research results on schizophrenia patients (4, 6, 1518). These reviews also emphasize that NSS and abnormal movements similar to tardive dyskinesia are not sequelae of neuroleptic medication but an integral part of the disease as has been described by Kraepelin for the first time in the end of the nineteenth century (19).

Predominantly, NSS have been looked at as endophenotypes, i.e., hardcopies of the genetic liability and thus as a trait phenomenon. In fact, much support for this notion derives from the literature on neuroleptic-naïve patients. The review by Wolff and O’Driscoll (20) reports a rate of approximately 25–33% NSS in this group and elevated rates in high-risk subjects with an emphasis on motor abnormalities.

Another strong piece of evidence supporting the trait aspect of NSS stems from FE studies [review by Dazzan and Murray (6)], where the reported prevalence ranges from 20 to 97% of patients. In contrast, NSS can be ascertained in up to 60% of patients treated with antipsychotic medication compared to less than 1% in healthy subjects (14). Yet another argument underpinning the trait hypothesis is provided by the literature on increased NSS in first-degree relatives (20) and more specifically by twin studies (2124) as well as by studies on patients’ off-spring, i.e., high-risk individuals (2527). Interestingly, NSS abnormalities are comparable in patients and their relatives, they affect motor coordination, motor sequencing, and sensory integration.

On the whole, the literature thus offers good evidence in favor of NSS representing a trait phenomenon which again is in line with the neurodevelopmental hypothesis (28).

Despite the aforementioned evidence, a number of studies present arguments in favor of a state feature being present in NSS. First and foremost, follow-up studies report clearly marked fluctuations regarding the expression of NSS during the disease course (2931). These fluctuations concern quality more than quantity (number) of abnormal NSS. Thus, single NSS may reach a higher level during acute phases of the disease and return to a baseline thereafter. However, this baseline does not compare to healthy individuals but rather to healthy first-degree relatives (22).

The documentation of a NSS state component may bear the following impact:

1. it may argue in favor of the existence of neuronal regeneration processes in individuals suffering from schizophrenia.

2. it may represent an argument for the necessity to supplement the neurodevelopmental hypothesis.

3. it may—together with fluctuations in symptomatology—hint at a third underlying factor influencing the disease process.

This paper aims to raise the question whether NSS are constituted by trait properties only or whether there is another property, namely state component, or whether NSS are even Janus-faced. This state-trait discussion can only be led with reference to follow-up studies.

Moreover, this paper represents an update on our meta-analysis (32), where only studies were incorporated which gave sufficient statistical data, so that several meaningful longitudinal studies were not included.

Methods

We performed a literature search of the period between January 1966 and June 2017 in the following databases: PsycNET, PSycINFO, Psyndex, and SCOPUS which includes PubMed. We used the following terms: schizophren* AND neurology* OR soft sign OR motor AND follow-up OR longitudinal OR Verlauf (German word for course, used in the German database Psyndex only). In addition, we examined references from all articles identified. Moreover, we excluded literature on children and adolescents because NSS are present at birth and decrease over time at least until puberty. They are, thus, correlated with brain maturation (33), reaching a “normal” level only in early adulthood.

Discussion

Patients, Instruments, Follow-up Period

We identified 26 studies (see Table 1) which had performed follow-up assessments of patients diagnosed with schizophrenia or related disorders. These studies were presented by 18 groups, 3 of which (29, 31, 3739, 4143, 47, 52, 53) had carried out two to five studies each. Two other groups published two to three papers each on different follow-up examinations of one patient samples (30, 4850, 58). Therefore, the latter data were merged for this review’s purpose.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Longitudinal studies on patients with schizophrenia in alphabetical order.

The number of patients included ranged from 10 to 126, with less than 20 patients in 7 studies; 15 studies dealt with FE patients. Patients suffering from bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, affective disorders, delusional disorder, substance-induced psychotic disorders, and personality disorders accounted for 21 and 36% of participants in two studies (44, 58); all remaining exclusively consisted of subjects suffering from schizophrenia.

Instruments for the assessment of NSS were mostly standardized and widely used scales, namely the Neurological Evaluation Scale [NES (62)], the Cambridge Neurological Inventory [CNI (61)], the Condensed Neurological Examination [CNE (60)], the Heidelberg Scale [HD (29)], and the Brief Motor Scale [BMI (59)]. The following six exceptions were made: Emsley et al. (41, 42) shortened the NES to 13 items, Madsen et al. (44) employed a standard neurological examination complemented by tests of sensory function and complex motor acts, Mittal et al. (46) used the Quitkin scale (2), Tucker and Silberfarb (1) applied 4, Torrey (56) and Wahlheim et al. (57) two single tests respectively.

Apart from adjustments and exceptions, the different NSS scales and scores are not comparable directly since the number of items differs between instruments as does the scoring (0–1 to 0–3 points) of the individual items. However, items and/or subscales are contrastable, especially with respect to motor sequencing, motor coordination, and sensory integration, which are the most relevant items concerning pathology in schizophrenia. These important components of NSS were assessed in almost all studies integrated in this review.

Ratings were performed by 2–3 raters in half of the studies, achieving overall good interrater reliabilities (see Table 1). One single rater applied the tests in the remaining 50%, which was mainly the case in older studies.

Catamnestic periods extended from a short-term of 2–8 weeks to a medium duration of 6 months to 2 years to long-term follow-ups of 5 and 6 years (see Table 1).

NSS during Follow-up

Researchers who included HC in their assessments unanimously reported that patients scored significantly worse than healthy comparison subjects [review by Dazzan and Murray (6)]. Table 1 indicates that 35% of all studies in this review compared to HC patients, all of which showed significant differences in NSS scores throughout.

For reasons of better comparison, we grouped all identified studies according to patients’ illness status and their NSS course (Table 2). Independent of the number of patients included and the instruments used for the assessment of NSS, all studies on chronic schizophrenia showed that NSS remained stable or deteriorated, whereas studies on patients with a remitting course found that NSS decreased over time. The pattern also appears in the studies by Schröder et al. (29, 52, 53), in which both chronic and remitting patients participated. There are two exceptions, namely the studies by Jahn et al. (43) and Torrey (56) where mixed samples were included and the respective NSS results on follow-up are heterogeneous. Presumably the divide follows the above model, which, however, cannot be proven or rebuted.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Follow-up studies in schizophrenia and NSS course (decreasing – stable – increasing).

The picture on FE patients is less clear. Whereas patients’ NSS status improved in several studies (30, 31, 42, 45, 58), the patients’ cohort fell into two subgroups in the majority of studies, mostly with one subgroup exhibiting an improving and another a stable NSS course over time.

Only Boks et al. (35) and Madsen et al. (44) reported a division into two subgroups with stable and deteriorating NSS, and Prikryl et al. (4850) described two subgroups with improving and worsening course, respectively. No change except for worsening of the glabellar reflex was observed in one study (51).

NSS Subscales during Follow-up

Change in total NSS—if present—was mostly accounted for by change in the motor system subscales, i.e., motor coordination and motor sequencing (29, 31, 38, 49, 51, 58). Cuesta et al. (40) reported an improvement in all subscales except for frontal signs.

Emsley et al. (41) reported a decrease of motor signs during the first 3 months of treatment with a subsequent return to initial values. Accordingly, in their second study, the group (42) did not note a drop in motor signs despite of a significant decline in total NSS. Other authors divided their samples into remitters and non-remitters and reported a decrease of motor signs being related to a remitting course (50 plus sensory integration; 29 plus spatial orientation). Likewise, in a FE study (31) subgroups arose after 1 year, which were distinguishable by the subscales motor coordination, motor sequencing, and sensory integration. The sensory integration subscale also added to an overall change in NSS scores in two other studies (51, 38), right-left and spatial orientation did so in one (31). Moreover, one group detected a non-significant decrease of the above-indicated subscale scores (54) and another group described a concordant finding for sensory integration (34).

Among the groups that did not detect any NSS change over time, Smith et al. (55) pointed out the relatively high stability of motor signs as opposed to the low stability of sensory integration signs in chronic patients. In FE individuals two groups found stability of motor signs (39, 47).

Chen et al. (38) reported deterioration of motor coordination, sensory integration, and disinhibition in a complete sample of chronic patients. Unfortunately, Boks et al. (35), Mittal et al. (46), and Whitty et al. (30) did not give any information on subscales when there was an overall NSS deterioration. In the study by Madsen et al. (44), NSS increase was mostly related to corticospinal tract signs.

NSS, Symptoms, and Other Clinical Variables

Medication influences both clinical variables and NSS (see below). Nevertheless, the relationship between NSS and clinical variables will be discussed separately.

Two studies did not assess or indicate psychopathology. The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms, and the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms were used by two groups, completed by the Present State Examination (PSE-9) once, whereas all remaining researchers employed the Positive and Negative Syndrome Scale (PANSS) or the Brief Psychiatric Rating Scale (BPRS). The respective scores at study inception and follow-up are given by 13 authors, whereas, 6 reported incomplete data (see Table 1). Scores are essentially comparable in the sense that (a) scores assessed with the same instrument fall into the same range, (b) the absolute follow-up scores are even similar, (c) acutely ill patients’ score decrease over time, and (d) scores of chronic patients or of those who were assessed in a stable condition remain widely unchanged.

Several authors have reported an improvement of NSS in parallel to a decrease of symptoms or treatment response, respectively (1, 2931, 40, 4244, 4652, 54, 5658). This pattern was even found in patients without symptom change (44) and in a subgroup without negative symptoms (34). Furthermore, associations of remission and motor signs were described (44, 58, 30), whereas remitters in the studies by Prikryl et al. (4850) exhibited lower total, sensory integration, and motor sequencing scores. Also severity of illness and lower social functioning (44) or severity of illness, except for motor sequencing, correlated with NSS (47). Other researchers did not find an association between NSS with symptoms in first-episode (35, 45) or chronic patients (36, 55). In two studies, even a deterioration of NSS in chronically ill (38) and of corticospinal functions in non-remitting FE patients was detected (44).

Positive symptoms were only addressed separately in a few studies. Scheffer (51) reported significant associations of NSS change in motor coordination, sequencing of complex motor acts, and sensory integration with change in the positive subscale (and total BPRS score, as discussed above). This is supported by findings of significant positive correlations between NSS and positive symptoms in two studies (45, 46) and of balance and positive symptoms in another study (41). Results by Chen et al. (38) did not underline the aforementioned findings. Even after a split into high and low positive symptoms no association with NSS emerged.

Researchers reported more extensively on negative symptoms compared to other symptoms present at the catamnestic examination. Many groups found a positive or increasing correlation over time between total NSS scores and negative symptoms (30, 37, 40, 42, 44, 4853, 58), as well as correlations between motor signs and negative symptoms (39, 47). One group described partial correlations of negative symptoms with primitive reflexes (34). Yet others reported an association of NSS with discomfort (46) or with negative and disorganized symptoms (47). Some groups depicted constancy of negative symptoms and NSS over time (34, 54, 55) in diverse patient samples and even in individuals with variations of NSS. Whitty et al. (30) even posit that negative symptoms rather than positive or general psychopathological symptoms may predict NSS.

Some authors considered further symptoms in their reports. They found an association between rapid movement or convergence and depression or anxiety (41), of total NSS with disorganization symptoms (42) on the one hand and a negative correlation of NSS and discomfort (46) on the other hand.

The meta-analysis by Chan et al. (16) already reported a strong relationship between NSS and symptoms in general. This notion is confirmed and extended by longitudinal studies which argue that the association even increases over time, the longer the more, and holds true mostly for negative symptoms.

Results on outcome are thus divergent: authors report (a) either no difference between good and poor outcome patients with respect to motor signs (39) or (b) better outcome measures in patients who complied with treatment and exhibited decreasing NSS scores (31); along these lines, remission was inversely correlated to NSS as described by two groups (29, 48, 49). In terms of prediction, bidirectional relationships are possible: NSS predict symptomatology (46), positive and negative symptoms, as well as other parameters predict increasing NSS (30). From the perspective of outcome, there seems to be a close relationship to NSS and psychopathology, in the sense that both higher NSS and higher psychopathology are related to a worse outcome.

Cognitive functioning was addressed in several studies. Chan et al. (37) did not find any group*time effect with NSS and overall cognitive parameters. However, over time, NSS correlated with poorer performance on the letter–number span test. Their group also reported paralleling trends of NSS and Winsconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST) perseverative errors in both patient groups as well as higher scores in logical memory, delayed logical memory, and WCST category, which authors relate to worse motor coordination and total NSS scores. In another study (42), working memory amelioration was predicted by sensory integration and motor coordination, whereas motor sequencing was predicted by working memory. Jahn et al. (43) even found correlations between NSS and all subscales of the modified WCST. Another study from Germany (52) reported a parallelizing improvement of both NSS and cognitive parameters, namely d2, Tower of Toronto test, SKT: delayed recall (Syndrom-Kurzztest, short assessment of cognitive performance), and SKT: delayed recognition. Further nonsignificant improvements occurred in the Tower of London test and SKT: immediate recall. Along these lines worsening of higher cognitive functioning paralleled higher hard signs (30, 46), and patients with NSS—as opposed to those without—exhibited negative symptoms and cognitive disorganization (57). Respective correlations emerged in cross-sectional research (6468).

Patients included in the reviewed study were of relatively young age. None of the authors discussed age with respect to NSS. The same holds true for gender. The literature does not report any influence of sex on NSS. Neither did we detect any influence. Family history was addressed by Madsen et al. (44) who found an NSS deterioration in a subgroup with first-degree relatives who suffered from a psychiatric disease. The same authors reported a worsening of NSS in patients with a history of obstetric complications. A relationship between level of education and NSS on follow-up was reported by two groups (39, 58, 30).

Handedness was examined where indicated in Table 1. Whereas our group did not detect any influence of handedness on NSS, Whitty et al. (58, 30) found higher NSS in mixed handers. The latter may be related to a common basis of both, mixed-handedness and NSS, consisting of still ill-defined neurological abnormalities.

Several authors addressed the duration of untreated psychosis (DUP) which was positively associated with NSS (39, 56, 58, 30) and negative symptoms or with motor symptoms only (39, 41). Similar associations arose with the overall duration of illness (45, 46, 55). Whitty et al. (58, 30) even posit that the DUP predicts catamnestic levels of NSS.

Alcohol and substance use were exclusion criteria in almost all studies, thus making the study population less representative. When this was not the case, alcohol did not influence NSS (57) or, together with negative symptoms, predicted higher NSS scores (58, 30). The latter finding is supported by the literature (69), stating that drug and alcohol abuse are associated with more neurological abnormalities in schizophrenia.

NSS, Medication, and Side-Effects (SE)

Patients were treated with typical neuroleptics in 10 studies, with atypical compounds in 5 studies, and with any of both or a mixture in the remaining 11 studies, where authors not always gave the respective information or, e.g., just displayed chlorpromazine-equivalents.

Five groups reported on a sample of antipsychotic-naïve or about 90% medication-naïve patients; another two groups included antipsychotic-naïve individuals but to a lesser extent (51, 29). A washout phase prior to starting the study was reported by two researchers (46, 54).

Dosages, where given, are not comparable between studies due to the different ways of reporting. Cuesta et al. (40), who used different antipsychotics or combinations, explicitly stated that there was significant NSS improvement over time independent of the type of medication, whereas Smith et al. (55) observed a trend toward lower NSS scores on atypical antipsychotics.

Whether or not antipsychotic medication exerts an effect on NSS can neither be proven nor disproven because the effect can only be generated indirectly. Although there is no indication regarding different effects of typical versus atypical compounds, evidence from the reviewed studies points toward different medication effects:

(a) a positive influence of medication on NSS in terms of a decrease mostly stems from FE patients (1, 31, 40, 45, 51, 58), but also from those with a longer-standing illness (46, 54, 55). Others found that medication intake was associated with less motor signs as opposed to a non-medicated status (30, 35, 58).

(b) partial positive effects were reported as well in FE and remitting versus non-remitting patients (29, 37, 47, 52, 53, 56, 57).

(c) no relationship arose between NSS daily dose or type of antipsychotic compound (44, 44, 49, 55).

(d) medication non-response and non-remitting NSS scores were related in studies on FE (31, 34, 35, 44, 48, 49) and on chronic patients (36). One group reported that the association pertained to all subscales except sensory integration.

(e) two studies reported a relation between lifetime exposure to medication and less NSS change or even NSS increase in medication non-responders (35, 44).

In summary, medicated patients seem to fall into at least two subgroups, namely one which improves clinically and in terms of NSS, and another which exhibits stable and/or worsening psychopathology as well as NSS. Possibly, the effect of medication may be related to the stage of illness and the overall magnitude of NSS which have been said to be predictors toward response to medication (70). Despite the diverse results, there is thus room for supporting the notion that antipsychotic compounds exert a protective effect on NSS (6). Already, Heinrichs and Buchanan (14) argued similarly by reporting that medicated patients exhibited fewer signs.

Consequentially, the relationship between clinical response to medication and NSS improvement—or non-improvement, respectively—leads to the assumption of a common underlying factor. For example, this may be the effect of antipsychotics on brain structure as reviewed by Scherk and Falkai (71). Ultimately though, the link between medication, brain structure, and NSS has not been located yet.

Side-effects of medication were assessed by 15 groups, using widely accepted instruments. Six authors indicated that no SE were present, five others did not give any results, hereby suggesting that non-reporting is due to a lack of significant pathology. A minority of six groups reported associations between NSS and extrapyramidal side-effects: a positive correlation on follow-up (47), a relationship to motor signs (44), to motor signs on 2-year follow-up (41), to sensory integration (45), or to stable, i.e., higher NSS (57). A significant increase of tardive dyskinesia on follow-up was observed in haloperidole treatment (36).

NSS and Ethnicity

The presented follow-up studies on NSS in schizophrenia were performed in different countries on four continents, namely, (North) America, Europe, Asia, and Africa. So far, study results do not show any pattern in favoring or contradicting an association of NSS and ethnicity, although more studies are warranted. For now, there is reason to assume that NSS may be remarkably similar across countries and ethnicities (72).

Methodology of Studies

Representativeness of the reviewed studies is high for the following reasons: only two research groups included diagnostic groups other than schizophrenias, the majority of researchers followed patients for at least 6 months, 47% of the studies focused on FE patients, and interrater reliability or intraclass correlation was high overall. Different assessment instruments do not seem to exert an influence: although different researchers have used different instruments, these were standardized and featured the same dimensions of NSS. Moreover, there is agreement in the literature on the relative importance of the different groups of NSS: motor signs, i.e., motor sequencing, complex motor tasks, and motor integration are of overriding importance as they usually account for the largest part of the total NSS score. Sensory integration on the other hand plays a minor role.

The Trait versus State Debate

As schizophrenia has a genetic basis in up to 50% of cases (9), NSS have been looked at as endophenotypes, i.e., markers which help to discover the underlying genetic basis. There is an assumption that the amount of NSS and the abnormality of the single sign may even discriminate sporadic and familial schizophrenia with more pronounced abnormalities in familial cases (51). Along these genetic lines, many studies have reported more NSS and higher item scores in patients’ relatives compared to HC; the closer the relative, the more explicit the abnormalities (21). On the other hand, according to Kinney et al. (73) and Rossi et al. (60) NSS have a small to modest share in the liability toward schizophrenia. Gureje Gureje (74) even stated that NSS might not be specific to schizophrenia but related to obstetrical complications in general. Madsen et al. (44) may reconcile the opposite views: in their study differences between patients and healthy subjects regarding frontal, cerebrospinal, and temporo-parietal functions became larger over time—which was more prominent in patients with a positive family history. Similarly, Hyde et al. (75) claim that “frontal release signs” are influenced by both genetic and environmental factors.

Meehl (76) stated that NSS can only be considered to represent genetic markers if:

1. they are specific for schizophrenia, i.e., discriminate patients from HC and patients with other psychiatric diseases;

2. siblings show at least a tendency for differing from healthy subjects;

3. acutely ill and remitted patients are comparable; and

4. schizophrenia patients with varying degrees of severity do not differ regarding NSS.

Meehl’s first requirement for a genetic marker partially holds true in NSS. There was congruence among the reviewed studies that patients exhibit more NSS than HC. This is supported by the literature including FE patients (6, 77). However, NSS can also be detected in patients suffering from a number of other psychiatric diseases including bipolar disorder, obsessive–compulsive disorder, borderline personality disorder (7880).

Meehl’s second postulation is fulfilled. NSS were also detected in siblings, but were qualitatively and quantitatively less distinct, i.e., taking an intermediate position between HC and patients. This represents a strong argument for a trait or hereditary component as backed up by the literature on patients’ first-degree relatives (20), on twins (2124), and on off-spring (2527).

Requirement three demands comparability between acutely ill and remitted patients. Some studies in our review argue in favor of comparability by reporting completely stable NSS over time. This was the case even in four FE studies (35, 39, 41, 51) and in studies on chronic patients (36, 39, 55). The other studies reported differences between subgroups. However, the above-indicated FE patients presented with a long duration of untreated illness and might already have crossed the border toward chronicity, e.g., in the study by Chen et al. (39) where DUP amounted to 474 ± 768 days. Along these lines, some authors suggested an interrelationship between NSS and duration of illness (81, 83, 84). Moreover, there was one report (38) on a clear deterioration of NSS in chronic patients and a further one on patients worsening on a trend level (44), related to a non-remitting course, a family history of psychotic disorder in a first-degree relative, obstetric complications, and male gender.

Further arguments in favor of non-comparability between acutely ill, remitted, and chronically ill patients arise from studies which showed NSS improvement over time in most or all patients, differences in phases of the disease and other varying factors notwithstanding. Several follow-up studies found NSS improvement, especially in FE patients (30, 31, 42, 45, 49, 58) with respect to subscales in FE (16, 34), in patients with a remitting course (1, 29, 44, 49, 56, 57) with significant effects in two of the cited studies, and in patients who were unmedicated prior to study inclusion (46, 54). Moreover, in the study by Sevincok and Topaloglu (54), patients who were unmedicated at first assessment and medicated at the second displayed an absolute decrease of NSS—possibly due to the small number of patients the results did not reach statistical significance. In all probability, compliance plays a role toward improvement (31). As adherence most likely is particularly high in patients who participate in a study, compliance presumably was present in the presented studies. Thus, at least at the onset of the illness and in remitting subtypes an NSS state component is present and expressed by fluctuations of NSS scores. Given the number of studies reporting fluctuating and decreasing NSS in FE and remitting patients, and the fact that only chronic patients experience stability or deterioration of NSS, the state component cannot be ignored. The fact that patients’ samples mostly divide into those with stable NSS and improving NSS at an early stage during their course of illness, is in line with schizophrenia not just representing one homogeneous disease but a group of entities, namely the “group of schizophrenias” as Bleuler put it (82). As the existence of subgroups is not debatable (83, 85)—subtypes may represent another argument for the existence of an NSS state feature in some patients but not others. Unfortunately, subtypes of the disorder and their influence over time were discussed by Schröder et al. (29) only.

The fourth claim by Meehl states that schizophrenia patients with varying degrees of severity should not differ regarding NSS. Severity of patients’ illness in the different studies can either be compared by authors’ classification as FE, remitting, and chronic course or by looking at psychopathology. Whenever scores of well-known scales such as BPRS or PANSS were disclosed, their range was comparable. The latter comparison seems more valid because the former already is based on symptoms. With respect to psychopathology and NSS, differing patterns arise in those studies, which gave all necessary information. Some authors report a paralleling course of NSS and psychopathology in all patients (30, 31, 40, 42, 46, 54, 58) or in subgroups (44, 4850, 52). Others describe decreasing symptoms (except negative symptoms) but stable NSS (34) or the opposite, namely stable symptoms and decreasing (34, 53) respectively increasing NSS (38). Yet, further studies found unchanged symptoms as well as NSS over time (35, 36).

With this diffuse pattern, Meehl’s fourth demand is clearly not fulfilled. However, the pattern speaks in favor of a state component in NSS and the necessity to consider the stage of illness, as Smith et al. (55) reasoned: if a state dimension was predominant, the timing of NSS assessment would be important. This argument also calls into question the predictive value of NSS which was proposed by earlier authors (66, 87).

Thus, only two of four of Meehl’s claims are fulfilled and NSS qualify for both, a stable, and early-acquired trait and a fluctuating state component. The latter may possibly be related to the stage of the disease as well as to treatment.

The Nature of NSS

In accordance with the literature, we argue that the NSS dimensions might be related to a neurointegrative defect in general (86), to dopamine-dependent pathways (87), or even more specifically to certain brain regions and circuits connecting them (11, 14, 88), be it at a subcortical level (9, 89) or in terms of a cortical-subcortical pathway (90). Among the circuits in question, the fronto–striatal–thalamic route emerges as the most relevant toward motor signs which again seem to contribute overridingly toward overall NSS scores. According to our understanding the fronto–striatal–thalamic pathway is being complemented by integration of the cerebellum (9193), thus forming the cortico–cerebellar–thalamo–cortical circuitry suggested by Andreasen et al. (94). Interestingly, Kong and colleagues (95) detected changes of the basal ganglia in ultra-high-risk subjects prior to manifestation of the disease. Changes of the basal ganglia in relation to NSS were already reported in 1998 (53) by applying SPECT. The authors showed that upregulation of dopamine d2 receptors under neuroleptic treatment may be involved.

These findings may partly explain the above cited effect of antipsychotics on brain structure as reviewed by Scherk and Falkai (71): typical and atypical antipsychotics exert varying effects on cortical gray matter and on almost all subcortical structures. The thalamus increases with both types of neuroleptics. This may reflect protective or regenerative processes related to the stabilization of NSS, especially of motor signs which most likely pertain to the cortico–thalamo–cerebellar–cortical circuit.

As far as the remaining NSS are concerned, Carter et al. (96) suggested a disturbed integration of the sensory systems. Dazzan et al. (6, 88) concluded that agreement concerning the neuro-dysfunctional basis of NSS is still missing.

However, similarities between longitudinal studies, in terms of general NSS elevation and change of NSS scores over time, clearly support a combination of state and trait aspects or a Janus-faced nature of NSS in schizophrenia. Unfortunately, the question of what underlies the state component has to remain open. At this point, innate and adaptive immune response may come in to play. As far as innate immune system alterations are concerned, prenatal maternal infections (97, 98) as well as obstetric complications, neonatal hypoxia, and brain injury lead to recruitment of cytokines which mediate inflammatory processes or represent a proinflammatory immune state in itself. Also, early strikes hitting the immune system may lead to a lifelong change in immune response and low-level neuroinflammation. For example, cytokines activate indoleamine 2,3-dioxygenase, which influences tryptophan, kynurenin, and serotonin in the central nervous system. As a consequence, serotonergic, noradrenergic, and glutamatergic neurotransmission may be modified (99). Changes in other inflammatory biomarkers (macrophages/monocytes, reduction in T cell numbers and proliferation, and alternations in T-helper cell 1 balance) may activate microglia, i.e., the intracerebral macrophages (98). Alterations of the immune response via inflammatory processes may also be related to infectious agents, which can cause acute or latent infections, namely, viruses and parasites, such as Human Herpesvirus 2, Borna Virus, Chlamydophila pneumoniae, Chlamydophila psittaci, and Toxoplasma gondii (100). Latent infections and reactivation of latent infections seem to be associated with acute disease phases in schizophrenia (31, 101, 102). Moreover, Human Endogenous Retrovirus may be activated in the genome by microbiological agents, immune mediators as mentioned above, and multiple other activators of the cell and thus be linked to schizophrenia in some individuals (103106).

All of the above-indicated factors may cause dysregulation of dopamine and further neurotransmitters, either directly or indirectly, and lead to impairments in cortico-subcortical networks.

These findings on infection and inflammation very much support a two-hit model of schizophrenia with a genetic or early acquired as well as a second-adapted factor forming the base of NSS and their trait and state aspects.

Above and beyond open questions on the nature of NSS, an integrative view of the different motor symptoms is warranted (107), as well as a taxonomy of symptoms in schizophrenia encompassing the sensory–motor dimensions.

Conclusion

The literature on NSS in schizophrenia presents at least partially diverging results. On the one hand, research speaks in favor of NSS as being a stable entity, i.e., a trait: NSS are more pronounced in patients compared to their relatives; they are present at disease onset; there is a relationship with the more stable symptoms, namely negative and cognitive ones. Duration and chronicity of illness have also been linked with stability of NSS. On the other hand, there are arguments supporting a fluctuating aspect, especially of motor signs. Follow-up studies—as summarized in this paper—have reported modulations of NSS scores over time, especially in correlation with clinical improvement, in FE patients, and in the relapsing–remitting subtype. Taken together, the evidence on NSS reconciles the dispute by suggesting that both components are involved, e.g., a trait or baseline component as well as a state component associated with psychotic exacerbations.

This underlying state-trait dichotomy of NSS is hardly influenced by the type of medication, supposedly apart from the lifetime load. Furthermore, in a state-trait feature, only the presence of the trait can be used as an endophenotype or a predictor, but not the quantitative expression, i.e., the state. The latter however, is called into question by studies which detected a paralleling course of symptoms and NSS, meaning that the volatility in terms of NSS decrease may serve as an outcome predictor.

Future research should raise the question of whether or not testing of NSS can be restricted to motor and sensory signs because these are the major subscales which account mostly for fluctuations in NSS scores. Moreover, apart from identifying the circuits which underlie the different NSS, researchers should study the mechanisms involved in the state component, i.e., undulations of NSS scores.

Author Contributions

SB wrote and edited the manuscript. JS critically revised the different versions of the manuscript.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Dipl. Psych. Colette Guillaumier, who proofread the manuscript, for her very valuable help.

References

1. Tucker GJ, Silberfarb PM. Neurologic dysfunction in schizophrenia: significance for diagnostic practice. In: Akiskal H, Webb W, editors. Psychiatric Diagnosis: Exploration of Biological Predictors. New York: Spektrum (1978). p. 453–62.

Google Scholar

2. Quitkin F, Rifkin A, Klein DF. Neurologic soft signs in schizophrenia and character disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1976) 33:845–53. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1976.01770070075008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Woods BT, Kinney DK, Yurgelun-Todd D. Neurologic abnormalities in schizophrenic patients and their families. I. comparison of schizophrenic, bipolar, and substance abuse patients and normal controls. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1986) 43:657–63. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800070043006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Boks MPM, Russo S, Knegtering R, van den Bosch RJ. The specificity of neurological signs in schizophrenia: a review. Schizophr Res (2000) 43:109–16. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00145-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Stevens JR, Livermore A. Telemetered EEG in schizophrenia: spectral analysis during abnormal behaviour episodes. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (1982) 45:385–95. doi:10.1136/jnnp.45.5.385

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Dazzan P, Murray RM. Neurological soft signs in first-episode psychosis: a systematic review. Br J Psychiatry (2002) 181:S50–7. doi:10.1192/bjp.181.43.s50

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Hertzig M, Birch H. Neurologic organization in psychiatrically disturbed adolescents. A comparative consideration of sex differences. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1968) 19:528–37. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1968.01740110016003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Rochford JM, Detre T, Tucker GJ, Harrow M. Neuropsychological impairments in functional psychiatric diseases. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1970) 22:114–9. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1970.01740260018003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Kennard MA. Value ofequivocal signs in neurologic diagnosis. Neurology (1960) 10:753–64. doi:10.1212/WNL.10.8.753

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Cox SM, Ludwig AM. Neurological soft signs and psychopathology: incidence in diagnostic groups. Can J Psychiatry (1979) 24:668–73. doi:10.1177/070674377902400711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Boks MPM, Liddle PF, Burgerhof JGM, Knegtering R, van den Bosch RJ. Neurological soft signs discriminating mood disorders from first episode schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2004) 110:29–35. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.2004.00298.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Mergl R, Hegerl U. Neurological soft signs in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder. Fortschr Neurol Psychiatr (2005) 73:504–16. doi:10.1055/s-2004-830173

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. De la Fuente JM, Bobes J, Vizuete C, Bascaran MT, Morlán I, Mendlewicz J. Neurologic soft signs in borderline personality disorder. J Clin Psychiatry (2006) 67:541–6. doi:10.4088/JCP.v67n0404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Heinrichs DW, Buchanan RW. Significance and meaning of neurological signs in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (1988) 145:11–8. doi:10.1176/ajp.145.1.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Bombin I, Arango C, Buchanan RW. Significance and meaning of neurological signs in schizophrenia: two decades later. Schizophr Bull (2005) 31:962–77. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbi028

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Chan RCK, Xu T, Heinrichs RW, Yu Y, Wang Y. Neurological soft signs in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Bull (2010) 36:1089–104. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbp011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Krebs MO, Fayand AG, Bourdel MC, Dischamp J, Olie JP. Validity and factorial structure of a standardized examination for neurological soft-signs in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2000) 45:245–60. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(99)00206-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Tosato S, Dazzan P. The psychopathology of schizophrenia and the presence of neurological soft signs: a review. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2005) 18:285–8. doi:10.1097/01.yco.0000165599.90928.c7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Kraepelin E. Psychiatrie. Ein Lehrbuch für Studierende und Ärzte. 8. Auflage, III. Band. Klinische Psychiatrie II. Teil, Leipzig: Johann Ambrosiums Barth (1913). 751 p.

Google Scholar

20. Wolff AL, O’Driscoll GA. Motor deficits and schizophrenia: the evidence from neuroleptic-naïve patients and populations at risk. J Psychiatry Neurosci (1999) 24:304–14.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

21. Cantor Graae E, McNeil TF, Rickler KC, Sjöström K, Rawlings R, Higgins ES, et al. Are neurological abnormalities in well discordant monozygotic cotwins of schizophrenic subjects the result of perinatal trauma? Am J Psychiatry (1994) 151:1194–9. doi:10.1176/ajp.151.8.1194

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Niethammer R, Weisbrod M, Schiesser S, Grothe J, Maier S, Peter U, et al. Genetic influence on laterality in schizophrenia? A twin study of neurological soft signs. Am J Psychiatry (2000) 157:272–4. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.2.272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Kelly BD, Cotter D, Denihan C, Larkin D, Murphy P, Kinsella A, et al. Neurological soft signs and dermatoglyphic anomalies in twins with schizophrenia. Eur Psychiatry (2004) 19:159–63. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2003.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Picchioni MM, Toulopoulou T, Landau S, Davies N, Ribchester T, Murray RM. Neurological abnormalities in schizophrenic twins. Biol Psychiatry (2006) 59:341–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.07.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Fish B, Hagin R. Visual-motor disorders in infants at risk for schizophrenia. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1973) 28:900–4. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1973.01750360110017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Marcus J, Hans SL, Mednick SA, Schulsinger F, Michelsen N. Neurological dysfunctioning in offspring of schizophrenics in Israel and Denmark. A replication analysis. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1985) 42:753–61. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1985.01790310015002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lawrie SM, Byrne M, Miller P, Hodges A, Clafferty RA, Cunningham Owens DG, et al. Neurodevelopmental indices and the development of psychotic symptoms in subjects at high risk of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry (2001) 178:524–30. doi:10.1192/bjp.178.6.524

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Weinberger DR. From neuropathology to neurodevelopment. Lancet (1995) 346:552–7. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(95)91386-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Schröder J, Niethammer R, Geider FJ, Reitz C, Binkert M, Jauss M, et al. Neurological soft signs in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (1992) 6:25–30. doi:10.1016/0920-9964(91)90017-L

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Whitty P, Clarke M, Browne S, McTique O, Kamali M, Feeney L, et al. Prospective evaluation of neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia in relation to psychopathology: state versus trait phenomena. Psychol Med (2003) 33:1479–84. doi:10.1017/S0033291703008225

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Bachmann S, Bottmer C, Schröder S. Neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia: a follow-up study. Am J Psychiatry (2005) 162:2337–43. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.162.12.2337

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Bachmann S, Degen C, Geider FJ, Schröder J. Neurological soft signs in the clinical course of schizophrenia: results of a meta-analysis. Front Psychiatry (2014) 5:185. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Fellick J, Thompson APJ, Sills J, Hart CA. Neurological soft signs in mainstream pupils. Arch Dis Child (2001) 85:371–4. doi:10.1136/adc.85.5.371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Behere RV. Dorsolateral prefrontal lobe volume and neurological soft signs as predictors of clinical social and functional outcome in schizophrenia: a longitudinal study. Indian J Psychiatry (2013) 55:111–6. doi:10.4103/0019-5545.111445

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Boks MPM, Selten JP, Leask S, Van den Bosch RJ. The 2-year stability of neurological soft signs after a first episode of non-affective psychosis. Eur Psychiatry (2006) 21:288–90. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2005.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Buchanan RW, Koeppl P, Breier A. Stability of neurological signs with clozapine treatment. Biol Psychiatry (1994) 36:198–200. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(94)91225-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Chan RCK, Geng F, Lui SSY, Wang Y, Ho KKY, Hung KSY, et al. Course of neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia: relationsship with negative symptoms and cognitive performances. Sci Rep (2015) 5:11053. doi:10.1038/srep11053

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Chen EYH, Kwok CL, Au JWY, Chen RYL, Lau BST. Progressive deterioration of soft neurological signs in chronic schizophrenic patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2000) 102:342–9. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0447.2000.102005342.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Chen EYH, Hui CLM, Chan RCK, Dunn ELW, Miao MYK, Yeung WS, et al. A 3-year prospective study of neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2005) 75:45–54. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Cuesta MJ, Jalon EG, Campos MS, Peralta V. Cognitive effectiveness of olanzapine and risperidone in first episode psychosis. Br J Psychiatry (2009) 194:439–45. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.108.055137

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Emsley R, Turner HJ, Oosthuizen PP, Carr J. Neurological abnormalities in first-episode schizophrenia: temporal stability and clinical and outcome correlates. Schizophr Res (2005) 75:35–44. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2004.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Emsley R, Chiliza B, Asmal L, Kilian S, Olivier MR, Phahladira L, et al. Neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia: state- and trait-related relationships to psychopathology, cognition and antipsychotic medication effects. Schizophr Res (2017) 188:144–50. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2017.01.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Jahn T, Hubmann W, Karr M, Mohr F, Schlenker R, Heidenreich T, et al. Motoric neurological soft signs and psychopathological symptoms in schizophrenic psychoses. Psychiatry Res (2006) 142:191–9. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2002.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Madsen AL, Vorstrup S, Rubin P, Larsen JK, Hemmingsen R. Neurological abnormalities in schizophrenic patients: a prospective follow-up study 5 years after first admission. Acta Psychiatr Scand (1999) 100:119–25. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1999.tb10832.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Mangot AG, Sawant NS. First episode schizophrenia: neurological abnormalities and prognosis. German J Psychiatry (2013) 16:75–80.

Google Scholar

46. Mittal VA, Hasenkamp W, Sanfilipo M, Wieland S, Angrist B, Rotrosen J, et al. Relation of neurological soft signs to psychiatric symptoms in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2007) 94:37–44. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.04.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Ojagbemi A, Esan O, Emsley R, Gureje O. Motor sequencing abnormalities are the trait marking neurological soft signs of schizophrenia. Neurosci Lett (2015) 600:226–31. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2015.06.028

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Prikryl R, Ceskova E, Kasparek T, Kucerova H. Neurological soft signs, clinical symptoms and treatment reactivity in patients suffering from first episode schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res (2006) 40:141–6. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Prikryl R, Ceskova E, Kasparek T, Kucerova H. Neurological soft signs and their relationship to 1-year outcome in first-episode schizophrenia. Eur Psychiatry (2007) 22:449–504. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2007.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Prikryl R, Ceskova E, Tronerova S, Kasparek T, Kucerova HP, Ustohal L, et al. Dynamics of neurological soft signs and its relationship to clinical course in patients with first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (2012) 200:67–72. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2012.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Scheffer RE. Abnormal neurological signs at the onset of psychosis. Schizophr Res (2004) 70:19–26. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2003.10.007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Schröder J, Tittel A, Stockert A, Karr M. Memory deficits in subsyndromes of chronic schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (1996) 21:19–26. doi:10.1016/0920-9964(96)00027-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Schröder J, Silvestri S, Bubeck B, Karr M, Demisch S, Scherrer S, et al. D2 Dopamine receptor up-regulation, treatment response, neurological soft signs, and extrapyramidal side effects in schizophrenia: a follow-up study with 123I-iodobenzamide single photon emission computed tomography in the drug-naïve state and after neuroleptic treatment. Biol Psychiatry (1998) 43:660–5.

Google Scholar

54. Sevincok L, Topaloglu B. Neurological soft signs and positive treatment response to olanzapine in chronic schizophrenia. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2006) 30:141–3. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Smith RC, Hussain MI, Chowdhury SA, Stearns A. Stability of neurological soft signs in chronically hospitalized schizophrenic patients. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (1999) 11:91–6. doi:10.1176/jnp.11.1.91

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Torrey EF. Neurological abnormalities in schizophrenic patients. Biol Psychiatry (1980) 15:381–9.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

57. Wahlheim C, Hubmann W, Mohr F, Cohen R, Schlenker R, Werther P, et al. Prävalenz und Stabilität neurologischer Soft Signs bei schizophrenen Patienten. In: Bräunig P, editor. Motorische Störungen bei schizophrenen Psychosen. Stuttgart, New York: Schattauer (1999). p. 93–106.

Google Scholar

58. Whitty P, Clarke M, McTique O, Browne S, Gervin M, Kamali M, et al. Diagnostic specificity and predictors of neurological soft signs in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder and other psychoses over the first 4 years of illness. Schizophr Res (2006) 86:110–7. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2006.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Jahn T, Cohen R, Hubmann W, Mohr F, Köhler I, Schlenker R, et al. The Brief Motor Scale (BMS) for the assessment of motor soft signs in schizophrenic psychoses and other psychiatric disorders. Psychiatry Res (2006) 142:177–89. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2002.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Rossi A, De Cataldo S, Di Michele V, Manna V, Ceccoli S, Stratta P, et al. Neurological soft signs in schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry (1990) 157:735–9. doi:10.1192/bjp.157.5.735

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Chen EYH, Shapleske J, Luque R, McKenna PJ, Hodges JR, Calloway SP, et al. The Cambridge Neurological Inventory: a clinical instrument for assessment of soft neurological signs in psychiatric patients. Psychiatry Res (1995) 56:183–204. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(95)02535-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Buchanan RW, Heinrichs DW. The Neurological Evaluation Scale (NES): a structured instrument for the assessment of neurological signs in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (1989) 27:335–50. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(89)90148-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Astrachan BM, Harrow M, Adler D, Brauer L, Schwartz A, Schwartz C, et al. A checklist for the diagnosis of schizophrenia. Br J Psychiatry (1972) 121:529–39. doi:10.1192/bjp.121.5.529

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Arango C, Kirkpatrick B, Buchanan RW. Neurological signs and the heterogeneity of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2000) 157:560–5. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.157.4.560

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Bersani G, Clemente R, Gherardelli S, Pancheri P. Deficit of executive functions in schizophrenia: relationship to neurological soft signs and psychopathology. Psychopathology (2004) 37:118–23. doi:10.1159/000078610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Kolakowska T, Williams AO, Jambor K, Ardern M. Schizophrenia with good and poor outcome. III: neurological ‘soft’ signs, cognitive impairment and their clinical significance. Br J Psychiatry (1985) 146:348–57. doi:10.1192/bjp.146.4.348

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Liddle PF. Schizophrenic syndromes, cognitive performance and neurological dysfunction. Psychol Med (1987) 17:49–57. doi:10.1017/S0033291700012976

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Wong AH, Voruganti LN, Heslegrave RJ, Awad AG. Neurocognitive deficits and neurological signs in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (1997) 23:139–46. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(96)00095-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Keenan E, O’Donnell C, Sinanan K, O’Callaghan E. Severity of alcohol dependence and it’s relationship to neurological soft signs, neuropsychological impairment and family history. Acta Psychiatr Scand (1997) 95:272–6. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1997.tb09631.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Awad AG. Drug therapy in schizophrenia – variability of outcome and prediction of response. Can J Psychiatry (1989) 34:711–20. doi:10.1177/070674378903400716

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Scherk H, Falkai P. Effects of antipsychotics on brain structure. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2006) 19:145–50. doi:10.1097/01.yco.0000214339.06507.d8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Emsley RA, Niehaus DJ, Mbanga N, Oosthuizen PP, Stein DJ, Maritz JS, et al. The factor structue for positiv and negative symptoms in South African Shoas patients with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2001) 47:149–57. doi:10.1016/S0920-9964(00)00010-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

73. Kinney DK, Woods BT, Yurgelun-Todd D. Neurologic abnormalities in schizophrenic patients and their families. II. Neurologic and psychiatric findings in relatives. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1986) 43:665–8. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1986.01800070051007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Gureje Gureje O. Tardive dyskinesia in schizophrenics: prevalence, distribution and relationship to neurological ‘‘soft’’ signs in Nigerian patients. Acta Psychiatr Scand (1987) 76:523–8. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1987.tb02913.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Hyde TM, Goldberg TE, Egan MF, Lener MC, Weinberger DR. Frontal release signs and cognition in people with schizophrenia, their siblings and healthy controls. Br J Psychiatry (2007) 191:120–5. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.106.026773

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Meehl P. Toward an integrated theory of schizotaxia, schizotypy, and schizophrenia. J Pers Disord (1990) 4:1–99. doi:10.1521/pedi.1990.4.1.1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Neelam K, Garg D, Marshall M. A systematic review and meta-analysis of neurological soft signs in relatives of people with schizophrenia. BMC Psychiatry (2011) 11:139. doi:10.1186/1471-244X-11-139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Arbabi M, Paast N, Karim HR, Fgahfori S, Memari AH. Mild neurological impairment may indicate a psychomotor endophenotype in patients with borderline personality disorder. Psychiatry Res (2016) 245:22–6. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2016.08.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Mrad A, Wassim Krir M, Ajmi I, Gaha L, Mechri A. Neurological soft signs in euthymic bipolar I patients: a comparative study with healthy siblings and controls. Psychiatry Res (2016) 236:173–8. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2015.11.047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Ozcan H, Ozer S, Yagcioglu S. Neuropsychological, electrophysiological and neurological impairments in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder, their healthy siblings and healthy controls: identifying potential endophenotype(s). Psychiatry Res (2016) 240:110–7. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2016.04.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Lane A, Colgana K, Moynihana F, Burke T, Waddington JH, Larkin D, et al. Schizophrenia and neurological soft signs: gender differences in clinical correlates and antecedent factors. Psychiatry Res (1996) 64:105–14. doi:10.1016/0165-1781(96)02602-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Bleuler E. Dementia praecox oder die Gruppe der Schizophrenien. Leipzig, Wien: Franz Deuticke (1911). p. 187–200.

Google Scholar

83. Malla AK, Norman RM, Aguilar O, Cortese L. Relationship between neurological ‘‘soft signs’’ and syndromes of schizophrenia. Acta Psychiatr Scand (1997) 96:274–80. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1997.tb10163.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Peralta V, Moreno-Izco L, Sanchez-Torres A, García de Jalón E, Campos MS, Cuesta MJ. Characterization of the deficit syndrome in drug-naive schizophrenia patients: the role of spontaneous movement disorders and neurological soft signs. Schizophr Bull (2014) 40:214–24. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbs152

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Nasrallah HA, McCalley-Whitters M, Kuperman S. Neurological differences between paranoid and nonparanoid schizophrenia: part I. sensory-motor lateralization. J Clin Psychiatry (1982) 43:305–6.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

86. Fish B, Marcus J, Hans SL, Auerbach JG, Perdue S. Infants at risk for schizophrenia: sequelae of a genetic neurointegrative defect. A review and replication analysis of pandysmaturation in the Jerusalem Infant Development Study. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1992) 49:221–35. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1992.01820030053007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

87. Johnstone EC, Macmillan JF, Frith CD, Benn DK, Crow TJ. Further investigation of the predictors of outcome following first schizophrenic episodes. Br J Psychiatry (1990) 157:182–9. doi:10.1192/bjp.157.2.182

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Dazzan P, Morgan KD, Orr KG, Hutchinson G, Chitnis X, Suckling J, et al. The structural brain correlates of neurological soft signs in AESOP first-episode psychoses study. Brain (2004) 127:143–53. doi:10.1093/brain/awh015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Mosher LR, Pollin W, Stabenau JR. Identical twins discordant for schizophrenia. Neurologic findings. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1971) 24:422–30. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1971.01750110034006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Friston KJ, Frith CD. Schizophrenia: a disconnection syndrome? Clin Neurosci (1995) 3:89–97.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

91. Bottmer C, Bachmann S, Pantel J, Essig M, Amann M, Schad LR, et al. Reduced cerebellar volume and neurological soft signs in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychiatry Res (2005) 140:239–50. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2005.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Thomann PA, Wüstenberg T, Santos VD, Bachmann S, Essig M, Schröder J. Neurological soft signs and brain morphology in first-episode schizophrenia. Psychol Med (2009) 39:371–9. doi:10.1017/S0033291708003656

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Andreasen NC, Pierson R. The role of the cerebellum in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2008) 64:81–8. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.01.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Andreasen NC, O’Leary DS, Cizadlo T, Arndt S, Rezai K, Ponto LL, et al. Schizophrenia and cognitive dysmetria: a positive emission tomography study of dysfunctional prefrontal-thalamic-cerebellar circuitry. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1996) 93:9985–90. doi:10.1073/pnas.93.18.9985

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Kong L, Cui H, Zhang T, Zhu Y, Tang Y, Herold CJ, et al. Neurological soft signs related grey matter abnormalities in prodromal psychosis. (in press).

Google Scholar

96. Carter O, Bennett D, Nash T, Arnold S, Brown L, Cai RY, et al. Sensory integration deficits support a dimensional view of psychosis and are not limited to schizophrenia. Transl Psychiatry (2017) 7:e1118. doi:10.1038/tp.2017.69

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Bechter K. Virus infection as a cause of inflammation in psychiatric disorders. Mod Trends Pharmacopsychiatry (2013) 28:49–60. doi:10.1159/000343967

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Brown AS. Epidemiologic studies of exposure to prenatal infection and risk of schizophrenia and autism. Dev Neurobiol (2012) 72:1272–6. doi:10.1002/dneu.22024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Müller N. Immunological aspects of the treatment of depression and schizophrenia. Dialogues Clinl Neurosci (2017) 19:55–63.

Google Scholar

100. Arias I, Sorlozano A, Villegas E, de Dios Luna J, McKenney K, Cervilla J, et al. Infectious agents associated with schizophrenia: a meta-analysis. Schizophr Res (2012) 136:128–36. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2011.10.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Khandaker GM, Cousins L, Deakin J, Lennox BR, Yolken R, Jones PB. Inflammation and immunity in schizophrenia: implications for pathophysiology and treatment. Lancet Psychiatry (2015) 2:258–70. doi:10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00122-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Sutterland AL, Fond G, Kuin A, Koeter MW, Lutter R, van Gool T, et al. Beyond the association. Toxoplasma gondii in schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, and addiction: systematic review and meta-analysis. Acta Psychiatr Scand (2015) 132:161–79. doi:10.1111/acps.12423

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Karlsson H, Bachmann S, Schröder J, McArthur J, Torrey EF, Yolken RH. Retroviral RNA identified in the cerebrospinal fluids and brains of individuals with schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2001) 98:4634–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.061021998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Karlsson H, Schröder J, Bachmann S, Bottmer C, Yolken RH. HERV-W-related RNA detected in plasma from individuals with recent-onset schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder. Mol Psychiatry (2004) 9:12–3. doi:10.1038/sj.mp.4001439

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Yao Y, Schröder J, Nellåker C, Bottmer C, Bachmann S, Yolken RH, et al. Elevated levels of human endogenous retrovirus-W transcripts in blood cells from patients with first episode schizophrenia. Genes Brain Behav (2008) 7:103–12.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

106. Li F, Karlsson H. Expression and regulation of human endogenous retrovirus W elements. APMIS (2016) 124:52–66. doi:10.1111/apm.12478

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Morrens M, Docx L, Walther S. Beyond boundaries: in search of an integrative view on motor symptoms in schizophrenia. Front Psychiatry (2014) 5:145. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00145

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: schizophrenia, neurological soft signs, longitudinal, follow-up, state, trait, review

Citation: Bachmann S and Schröder J (2018) Neurological Soft Signs in Schizophrenia: An Update on the State- versus Trait-Perspective. Front. Psychiatry 8:272. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00272

Received: 27 September 2017; Accepted: 21 November 2017;
Published: 08 January 2018

Edited by:

Philip D. Harvey, Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, United States

Reviewed by:

Assen Veniaminov Jablensky, University of Western Australia, Australia
Roberto Cavallaro, Vita-Salute San Raffaele University, Italy

Copyright: © 2018 Bachmann and Schröder. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Silke Bachmann, silke.bachmann@clienia.ch

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.