Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 09 November 2017
Sec. Psychopathology
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive Enhancement in Psychiatric Disorders View all 29 articles

Drugs Interfering with Muscarinic Acetylcholine Receptors and Their Effects on Place Navigation

  • Institute of Physiology of the Czech Academy of Sciences, Prague, Czechia

Muscarinic acetylcholine receptors (mAChRs) have been found to regulate many diverse functions, ranging from motivation and feeding to spatial navigation, an important and widely studied type of cognitive behavior. Systemic administration of non-selective antagonists of mAChRs, such as scopolamine or atropine, have been found to have adverse effects on a vast majority of place navigation tasks. However, many of these results may be potentially confounded by disruptions of functions other than spatial learning and memory. Although studies with selective antimuscarinics point to mutually opposite effects of M1 and M2 receptors, their particular contribution to spatial cognition is still poorly understood, partly due to a lack of truly selective agents. Furthermore, constitutive knock-outs do not always support results from selective antagonists. For modeling impaired spatial cognition, the scopolamine-induced amnesia model still maintains some limited validity, but there is an apparent need for more targeted approaches such as local intracerebral administration of antagonists, as well as novel techniques such as optogenetics focused on cholinergic neurons and chemogenetics aimed at cells expressing metabotropic mAChRs.

Introduction

Acetylcholine (ACh) is one of the major neurotransmitters and modulators of the nervous system. Its receptors are abundantly expressed in a wide variety of tissues, from neuromuscular junctions and parasympathetic system to cortical regions involved in cognitive functions such as learning and memory (1). The cholinergic system has been shown to play an important role in processes such as circadian rhythmicity (2), addiction (3), motivation, pain, and reward (1), as well as cognitive flexibility (4), perceptual memory (5), spatial learning (6), and many more. It comes as no surprise that abnormalities in the function of the cholinergic system and its components underlie a multitude of pathologies, such as Parkinson’s disease (7), Alzheimer’s disease (AD) (8), schizophrenia, bipolar disorder (9, 10), and depression (11). For these reasons, the cholinergic system has been extensively studied in recent years; however, many mechanisms of its function still remain unclear.

ACh System in the Brain

There are two main types of ACh receptors, named historically after their naturally occurring alkaloid agonists: (1) nicotinic receptors (nAChRs), a family of ionotropic receptors which act as ligand-gated cation channels and (2) muscarinic Ach receptors (mAChRs), a metabotropic G-protein-coupled receptor (GPCRs) family whose activation may trigger various responses depending on the specific subtype and context of the signal (8). Nicotinic ACh receptors are named after nicotine, their prototypical agonist. Probably the most famous antagonist of the nAChRs is D-tubocurarine, a compound isolated from the curare poisons (12, 13). mAChRs are named after muscarine, a toxic alkaloid synthesized in the toadstool Amanita muscaria. Possibly the best known antagonist of the mAChRs is atropine, found in the deadly nightshade (Atropa belladonna) (14).

Nicotinic Receptors

Despite being best known for their involvement in signal transduction at neuromuscular junctions, these receptors are also expressed throughout the central nervous system (CNS). As mentioned above, nicotinic ACh receptors are ionotropic, i.e., ligand-gated cation channels, whose activation by an agonist evokes a flux of K+, Ca2+, and Na+ ions (however not all subtypes of nAChRs are permeable for Na+), which in turn triggers mechanisms of Ca2+ signaling (1). These receptors typically comprise five subunits: either a homomeric combination of α subunits (for example α7) or a heteromeric combination of α(1–10), β(1–4), δ, and ε subunits. The specific combination of these subunits results in different pharmacological properties of the individual subtypes, such as ion selectivity and ligand affinity (14). The most common nAChR subtypes found in the brain are α7 and α4β2 receptors. Located at both pre- and postsynaptic sites, they play a pivotal role in various processes, such as learning and cognition (15), decision-making (9), and regulation of the postnatal development of the visual cortex (16). Thus, nicotinic ACh receptors constituted of specific subunits appear to be suitable pharmacological target for cognitive enhancement.

Muscarinic Receptors

Muscarinic receptors are abundantly expressed throughout the brain; however, they are also found in various other tissues in the body, such as the heart (17, 18), the bladder and pulmonary system (19), and the intestine (20). As mentioned above, mAChRs do not serve as cation channels like nicotinic receptors, but instead are coupled with G-proteins, which transmit signals into the cell by affecting the activity of certain enzymes (such as the adenylyl cyclase, phospholipase C, etc.) (14, 21).

Five subtypes of mAChRs have been described, M1–M5. They differ in their level of expression in various parts of the body and the signal cascades they trigger after binding an agonist. Located mostly postsynaptically, the M1, M3, and M5 receptors (sometimes referred to as “M1-like” receptors) activate phospholipase C via Gq/11 protein, thus inducing a calcium influx into the cell. M2 and M4 on the other hand (the “M2-like” group), when activated lower the level of cyclic adenosine mono-phosphate in the cell by Go/i protein-mediated inhibition of adenylyl cyclase. They are found both pre- and postsynaptically (8, 21, 22).

The outputs of signaling through specific cholinergic receptor subtypes may vary tremendously depending on the subtype of the receptors and their pre- or postsynaptic localization. The specific tissue and the type of the cell that expresses the receptors is also of major importance, as well as the metabolic state of the neuron at the precise time of receiving the signal, i.e., a cell with high intracellular levels of calcium may react differently to a signal than one with low intracellular concentrations of calcium. To further complicate any predictions of outcomes of cholinergic signaling and behavioral analysis, many neurons corelease ACh and glutamate, or ACh and gamma-aminobutyric acid (4, 21).

One of the major characteristics of the molecular structure of mAChRs is the evolutionarily highly conserved orthosteric ACh binding site, with a key asparagine residue (Asn105). This results in great difficulty when developing direct agonists and antagonists selective for a specific receptor subtype, and non-selective agents such as scopolamine, an antiemetic drug, and 3-iodothyronamine are widely used in research on memory impairment (2325). Researchers have rather focused on developing compounds acting as allosteric ant/agonists and positive allosteric modulators (8, 26).

M1 mAChR

The M1 receptor is considered to be the most abundant subtype (50–60% of all mAChRs) of mAChRs in the brain. It plays an essential role in many cognitive functions such as learning and memory, and thus has become a target of research focusing on developing therapeutics for neurodegenerative diseases (8, 10, 27). For example, Ragozzino et al. (28) reported an enhancing effect of CDD-0102A, a partial M1 agonist, on working memory and strategy changing in rats. The compound improved the rats’ performance in a spontaneous alteration task (designed to test working memory) and, under changed circumstances, their ability to deem a previously useful strategy irrelevant and to find and retain a new one. They demonstrated the involvement of M1 receptors in these processes, and further suggested the use of CDD-0102A as a potential therapeutic agent for disorders such as AD and schizophrenia, emphasizing its enhancing influence and the lack of observed adverse effects (28). The M1 receptor is also expressed in other tissues than the brain; for example it has been shown to participate in the regulation of non-quantal ACh release in neuromuscular junctions (29).

Antimuscarinic Drugs

Due to the diverse expression and functions of AChR in the brain, compounds affecting the cholinergic neurotransmission are employed in the treatment of a wide range of conditions and diseases. They are generally used for antiparkinsonian treatments, specifically targeting extrapyramidal symptoms such as rigidity, tremors, and bradykinesia. For example, it is generally accepted that an imbalance of cholinergic and dopaminergic transmission in the brain is one of the mechanisms underlying or accompanying schizophrenia, particularly in the negative symptoms and cognitive impairment. Anticholinergic drugs are often prescribed along with antipsychotics to alleviate their unwanted side effects. However, their usage has often been questioned as they themselves cause a range of side-effects, such as cognitive impairment, tardive dyskinesia, blurred vision, dry mouth, problems with urinary retention, psychosis, addiction, and many more (3032). To give an example, Veselinović et al. (33) investigated the effect of the administration of anticholinergics on cognition in untreated patients with schizophrenia and healthy control subjects. Their results showed a marked impairment in both groups, which was however more pronounced in the schizophrenia patients, thus again casting doubt on the suitability of these drugs in the treatment of schizophrenia (33).

Interestingly, some antimuscarinic agents (namely scopolamine) also appear to possess antidepressant qualities, especially in treatment of those patients who are unresponsive to the standard therapy. Witkin et al. (11) reported that these antidepressant effects might be mediated specifically by the blockage of the M1 and M2 receptors (11).

The general consensus is that anticholinergics disrupt acquisition learning and long-term memory processing. As such, these compounds are often employed for inducing memory and cognitive impairments in laboratory animals in order to model pathological states observed in human diseases such as schizophrenia, AD and other dementias (5). Despite its popularity, such an approach has received a lot of criticism. For example, antimuscarinic agents provide only a limited predictive and face axes of validity, but low construct validity in AD research. Furthermore, it is sometimes very difficult to tease apart effects on memory and attention, or procedural deficits in general, that are separable from the cognitive deficits in many navigational tasks (34).

Mechanisms of Action

As mentioned above, the ACh binding site is evolutionarily highly conserved across all five mAChR subtypes, which in turn complicates the search for subtype-selective ligands. However, there is an abundance of allosteric sites that facilitate receptor activity modulation and are specific for each receptor subtype. These have enabled the development of highly selective compounds (8).

Orthosteric subtype-selective agents are scarce, though some may be found; for example, a recent study reported a novel compound PCS1055 that exhibits high selectivity for the M4 receptor (35). Also, some ligands have been shown to bind at the orthosteric site as well as one of the allosteric sites, thus achieving relatively high selectivity for a specific mAChR subtype. An example may be seen in the work of Jakubík et al. (36), where the mechanism of action of the M2-selective antagonist methoctramine was put under scrutiny. The authors reported that methoctramine binds with high affinity to the orthosteric site and at the same time interacts with lower affinity with an allosteric site at the second and third extracellular loops. Interestingly, in the presence of another orthosteric-binding ligand (such as N-methyl-scopolamine), methoctramine may still bind to the allosteric site, thus preventing the other ligand from dissociating from the receptor. This antagonist occasionally binds the M3 receptor as well, but with a much lower affinity due to the lack of the allosteric site found on M2 (36). Also, the time that antagonists take to bind to the receptor has been shown to be of crucial importance for the efficacy of receptor blockage. For example, due to its relatively slow binding, tiotropium seems less effective at blocking the M3AChR (37).

As to the effects of antimuscarinic drugs on the organism, these naturally depend on the means and site of administration (which determines where the agent exerts its influence, such as the brain following an intracerebroventricular injection or the heart after a systemic application of a drug unable to cross the blood–brain barrier). Thus, as the M1 and M4 receptors are abundantly expressed in parts of the brain affected in neurodegenerative diseases such as AD, it seems probable—and has been repeatedly reported—that stimulating cholinergic transmission via these receptors should enhance cognitive abilities, learning and memory, whereas blocking it would result in cognitive impairment (26).

Clinical Potential of Antimuscarinic Drugs

In spite of the risk of various undesirable side-effects such as cognitive impairment, dry mouth, or even psychosis and addiction, if dosed with care, antimuscarinic drugs provide therapeutic effects in a number of conditions. For illustration, aclidinium and tiotropium are often prescribed in the treatment of chronic pulmonary disease, as well as asthma, overactive bladder, and irritable bowel syndrome (3841).

Quite recently, scopolamine, a non-selective antagonist capable of crossing the blood–brain barrier, has been found to exhibit antidepressant properties (mediated probably by its binding to M1 and M2 receptors), even in patients unresponsive to standard therapy (11). This has proven beneficial not only to patients with major depressive disorder but also to those suffering from bipolar disorder (42). In addition, scopolamine is also used as an antiemetic, for example in treating postoperational nausea (23).

As mentioned previously, mAChR antagonists (e.g., biperiden, trihexyphenidyl) are also employed as prophylaxis and/or for the treatment of side-effects of antipsychotics prescribed in diseases such as schizophrenia. However, this method is currently on the decline due to the multitude of unwanted side-effects of the anticholinergic treatment (30, 33).

Biperiden, amongst other antimuscarinics, also acts as an antiparkinsonian agent and is thus sometimes prescribed to patients with Parkinson’s disease, as well as other diseases manifesting with parkinsonian symptoms. However, even here the risks of addiction and detrimental side-effects still remain (43, 44).

Quite surprisingly, given the amount of criticism regarding the cognitive side-effects of muscarinic antagonists, a recent study investigating the properties of a new potential treatment for AD reported M1-antagonism for these agents. The tested drug candidate was developed using a newly proposed approach to treating multifactorial diseases such as AD, which aims to hit multiple therapeutic targets with a single drug comprising a series of compounds, in this case combining 7-methoxytacrine and memantine. As the results of other tests (such as successful prevention of β-amyloid fibrillization, AChE inhibition, etc.) looked rather promising, the authors recommended the novel compound as a potential treatment, claiming that the observed M1-antagonism did not seem to exhibit noticeable effects (45). It is conceivable that muscarinic antagonism can act beneficially when it is a part of a broader spectrum of mechanisms of action.

Biperiden As a Prototype Drug

Biperiden hydrochloride (or lactate) is an established M1-receptor selective antagonist. Approved for human usage and sold under the brand name of Akineton, it is prescribed for Parkinsonism (to improve motor abilities such as gait and tremor) and occasionally to suppress the side-effects of neuroleptics.

Apart from clinical practice, biperiden is also used in research as a cognitive impairer (46, 47). Biperiden has been shown to cross the blood–brain barrier without difficulties, thus enabling a simple administration of the drug, such as using intraperitoneal or subcutaneous injections (s.c.). The tissue distribution (Vd) for biperiden has been reported to be relatively high: with a brain to plasma ratio of up to 7–12 (44). The uptake of the drug by the tissues is quite rapid, possibly also due to its substantial transport into lysosomes (48). This makes biperiden a useful candidate as a specific drug, contrarily to scopolamine or atropine.

Place Navigation

To increase their chances of survival, including successful foraging for food and other resources, as well as finding their nest or burrow, animals employ a variety of spatial navigation strategies. In principle, such strategies can be based on idiothesis or allothesis (or a combination of both). In the first case, an individual finds its way based on the information from vestibular receptors, muscle proprioceptors and tendon receptors complemented with efference copies of motor commands and/or optic and haptic flow, whereas in the second case, the spatial representation is established upon external cues (49). Three navigation strategies may be used to reach a goal:

(1) a praxis strategy, when an animal follows a set of learned, usually stereotypic movements that lead to a known goal,

(2) a taxon strategy, when the goal is clearly visible from a distance or marked by other cues,

(3) a spatial strategy or mapping, when long-distance external cues become the spatial reference points, as the goal cannot be located otherwise (by sight or smell) (5052).

To illustrate, a man waking up at night and finding his way to the bathroom in the dark employs a praxis strategy; he knows it takes approximately four steps to the door of the room and then he has to turn right in the hallway and walk five more steps. A taxon strategy is used for example by a man approaching a bank—a large conspicuous building bearing an easy-to-see “Bank” sign. Finally, the mapping strategy focuses on finding the correct configuration of distal external cues, such as a man searching for a buried treasure (after his unsuccessful trip to the bank): e.g., he has to stand at a place with the big pine tree to his left, the strangely shaped mountain on the horizon behind him, and the lake a short distance in front of him.

Spatial navigation is based on the so-called place coding (53). The key structure of the brain involved in these processes is generally thought to be the hippocampus (more specifically its dorsal part); however, other parts of the brain play important roles as well. The neuronal substrate consists of place cells, large hippocampal pyramidal neurons with characteristic complex spikes that fire only in a specific part (or parts) of a given environment [the so-called firing fields or place fields (54); for review see Ref. (55)]. Interestingly, their structural organization in the brain is not topological, i.e., it does not reflect the outside world. Groups of these cells constitute ensembles, which serve as representations of the environment (56). Apart from these, there are grid cells, located in the entorhinal cortex (57). The spatial pattern of their firing fields resembles a hexagonal grid. And the final type is represented by head direction cells, found in the Papez circuit, and whose activity is dependent on the inclination or direction of an individual’s head (49, 53, 5860). The specific roles and mechanisms of function of these cells are not yet fully understood. A recent study has proposed a model for spatial navigation based on cooperation between place cells and grid cells, in which place cells are responsible mainly for locating a goal, whereas grid cells are in charge of directing an individual toward that goal (60).

Other important aspects of effective spatial navigation are sets of spatial stimuli that yield so-called frames of reference. An individual often needs to distinguish and correctly assess conflicting information from several of these frames to solve a task. An example of a behavioral test specifically assessing this ability is active place avoidance (see Active Place Avoidance Tasks). The hippocampus has been shown to be the structure responsible for organizing this spatial information into representations correctly corresponding to the outside world (6164). Behavioral tests based on spatial navigation are largely used by researchers in studying certain types of memory.

Antimuscarinic Agents in Spatial Tasks

Morris Water Maze (MWM)

Non-Specific Antagonists

Scopolamine is possibly one of the most frequently used antimuscarinic agents in the MWM. In spite of becoming something of a “gold standard” in research of cognitive impairment, its validity as a model has often been questioned because of its considerable side effects. As it lacks selectivity for any of the subtypes of mAChRs, apart from memory and cognition it also affects the sensorimotor functions of the treated subjects, thus sometimes compromising the results of the behavioral tests (65). However, Robinson et al. (66) reported impaired performance in the MWM in both rats and mice following scopolamine administration at a dose that exhibited no effect on visual acuity. This was studied in a variant of the MWM task specially adjusted to test for compromised visual perception, in which the animals were required to discriminate between two marginally differing cards in order to successfully find the hidden platform (66). A lack of effect on performance in a mainly vision-reliant task (the visible platform variant of the MWM) was also reported by Entlerova et al. (67) in their study focusing on a comparison of two commonly used rat strains (Wistar and Long-Evans) and their performance and sensitivity to anticholinergic blockade in the MWM and active place avoidance. Following scopolamine treatment, they found no marked differences in the MWM between the two strains, whereas in active place avoidance the Wistar rats exhibited significantly worse performance than the Long-Evans group, suggesting a higher sensitivity to scopolamine in the Wistar strain (67).

Furthermore, von Linstow Roloff et al. (68) set out to investigate whether the poor performance of scopolamine-treated rats in the MWM is in any part due to an effect on memory processes, or whether it is just the result of compromised sensorimotor abilities. In a series of experiments consisting of acquisition tasks combined with both spatial and non-spatial pretraining, as well as delayed-match-to-position (DMP) and a variant of the DMP with an on-demand platform [also called the Atlantis platform (69, 70)], they were able to show that although scopolamine undoubtedly causes side-effects leading to altered swimming speeds and higher levels of thigmotaxis, these can be eliminated by extensive spatial pretraining. In such a case however, scopolamine-treated animals still perform more poorly than controls, thus confirming that scopolamine does indeed affect spatial memory. In the Atlantis platform paradigm, the researchers were able to discriminate between the effects on procedural and spatial memory: scopolamine was found to impair the latter (68).

Navigating to a submerged platform requires a mapping strategy. As reviewed in Ref. (6), scopolamine disrupts forming a memory for platform location that is held constant across days (reference memory) or changes daily (working memory). When directly compared, working memory seems to be affected more than reference memory (71). Compromised navigation in the water maze can be explained in terms of the inaccurate positional information of place cells. Intraventricular or intrahippocampal infusions of scopolamine increase the firing of place cells outside of the usual place cell firing field of the neuron, leading to lesser place specificity (72, 73). Scopolamine seems to also affect other correlates of spatial memory. Its systemic administration flattens the typically robust positive correlation between running speed and theta frequency (74) and reduces spatial tuning of the grid cells (75). However, at least in the entorhinal cortex, scopolamine does not alter the tuning of head direction cells (75).

Water maze studies are able to provide some evidence regarding how scopolamine specifically affects particular stages of memory processing. There is general agreement on its effects on memory encoding [reviewed in Ref. (76)], while reports on consolidation or recall are mixed. Most studies report no or little effect on consolidation or recall (6, 77, 78) but a recent investigation demonstrated that systemic scopolamine administration in mice had a detrimental effect on the retrieval of platform location (79).

Scopolamine-induced cognitive impairment has also been shown to possess good validity as a translational model in research. Laczó et al. (80) compared the effects of scopolamine administration (as well as its coadministration with donepezil, an AChE inhibitor) in rats and humans in the MWM and the Hidden Goal Task, an analog of the water maze fit for use in humans. The authors reported successful validation of the tasks and scopolamine, as no significant differences were found between the human volunteers and the animals. Donepezil was shown to exhibit some ameliorative effect; however, this was not clear in all cases (80).

Although mostly of an older date, studies examining the effects of other antimuscarinic agents may also be found. In one such report by Sutherland et al. (52) focused on atropine, atropine sulfate-treated rats were found to lack the ability to employ spatial mapping as means of learning the location of the hidden platform, thus turning to a combination of taxon and praxis strategies (i.e., not remembering the position of the platform but instead rather a way of finding it). No such deficit was observed in control animals and a group treated with atropine methylnitrate (a substance acting solely in the periphery as it is unable to cross the blood–brain barrier), hence confirming the hypothesis that the central cholinergic system underlies spatial mapping strategies (52). It has also been proposed that atropine may interfere with the ability to inhibit non-efficient spatial strategies that appear initially during water maze acquisition (81).

The use of the MWM also occurred in a report assessing the properties of 3-quinuclidinyl benzilate (QNB), a non-selective muscarinic antagonist that has been proposed as a potential agent for modeling cognitive deficits in rats. The study showed a significant detrimental effect of QNB on acquisition in the MWM, whereas no impairment was found in memory consolidation and retrieval. Apart from hyperlocomotion leading to higher swimming speeds, the authors observed no adverse side effects of QNB on vision and sensorimotor functions (82). A study on oxybutynin, an antagonist of M1, M2, and M3 receptors, further confirmed that non-selective antagonists exert detrimental effects on acquisition in the MWM (83).

M1-Like Family mAChR Antagonists

Due to their abundance, it has been suggested that the effects of non-selective antagonists may be exerted primarily through M1 receptors. However, it turned out that attempts to silence M1 receptors functioning have provided mixed results. Pirenzepine, a selective M1 antagonist, was evaluated in the studies of Hagan et al. (84) and Hunter and Roberts (85). Although less potent than scopolamine, it was nevertheless shown to impair spatial navigation in the MWM while preserving the taxon strategy (navigation to a visible platform). However, one of the major drawbacks of this drug is its inability to cross the blood–brain barrier, thus requiring intraventricular administration (84, 85). In contrast to that line of evidence, mice lacking M1 receptors display unimpaired performance in a water maze in spite of general hyperactivity (86). Furthermore, systemic administration of imidafecin, a selective M1 and M3 antagonist, appeared to have no significant effect on navigation in a water maze (83). These results therefore questioned the exclusive role of M1 receptors in scopolamine-induced deficits in water maze navigation. In an attempt to explain this discrepancy, Bubser et al. concluded that M1 receptors seem to play a more significant role in mPFC-mediated tasks than in hippocampus-dependent tasks (87).

M2-Like Family Antagonists

An exception to the “rule” of muscarinic antagonists having detrimental effects on learning and memory are compounds selective for receptors expressed presynaptically (such as M2), which by blocking the presynaptically mediated inhibition of ACh release actually help to increase the levels of ACh in the synapse, and thus also cholinergic transmission (88, 89). For example, BIBN-99, a selective M2 antagonist, has been shown to improve the performance of aged rats in the MWM (88). Involvement of the M4 receptor in a water maze was assessed using M4 receptor knock-out mice. Despite elevated locomotion observed in the open field, knock-out mice displayed both unaltered acquisition and preference to a target location in probe trials in the water maze (90). It can be generally concluded that M2-like family muscarinic antagonists have weaker and sometimes even positive effects on place navigation tasks due to the different neuronal localization of respective receptors and the de facto different mechanistic mode of action, resulting in specific behavioral outcomes.

Results obtained with the MWM generally support the conclusion that antimuscarinic drugs adversely affect place navigation. On the other hand, this task also points to a number of non-cognitive confounding variables in the effects of antimuscarinic agents in place learning and memory. Importantly, muscarinic antagonists specific for particular receptor subtypes have been found to have only partial advantages over non-specific ligands, stressing the need for highly targeted approaches into the physiology of mAChR system with selective opto- and chemogenetic methods.

Radial Arm Maze

The Radial arm maze presents another task used to test spatial cognition, namely working and reference memory, but the procedure may also be adjusted to assess acquisition and memory retrieval (91, 92). This task was used for example in the study of Kay et al. (93), which showed that scopolamine elicits a stronger effect on working memory, while 3,4-methylendioxy-metamphtamine administration affects reference memory more prominently (93). Similar results regarding scopolamine administration had also been reported by Pilcher et al. (91), who compared the effects of scopolamine on working memory, acquisition and memory retrieval, concluding that there was stronger impairment in working memory relative to the other types (91).

This task may also be used for investigating differences in the consequences of acute vs. chronic drug administration, as shown for example by Ortega-Alvaro et al. (94). In their study, the authors found a significant impairment in rats’ performance in the radial arm maze following an acute injection of atypical antipsychotics (olanzapine and clozapine, used in the treatment of schizophrenia) and scopolamine, marked among others by a lower speed of movement. However, when following a chronic drug treatment, the observed deficits were absent, hence hinting at the ability to build a tolerance. The authors also concluded that chronic muscarinic antagonism may exert little or no influence over working memory (94).

One possible drawback of this task was raised in a study of Hodges et al. (95). The authors pointed out that the peripheral effects of scopolamine administration include “dry mouth,” which can lead to disruption of a rat’s ability to eat multiple food pellets and thus decrease their reward value.

Spatial Alternation Tasks

The natural tendency of rodents to alternate between two choices in successive trials is exploited in a variety of simple T-shaped or Y-shaped mazes. Due to the simplicity of the task, alternation has been employed in the bulk of pharmacological studies using the scopolamine-induced amnesia model. Numerous studies [reviewed in Ref. (96)] have consistently shown that scopolamine treatment disrupts working memory both in discrete (97) and continuous versions of the alternation paradigm (98, 99). A article by Givens and Olton (100) demonstrated that intraseptal injections of scopolamine mimicked the detrimental dose-dependent effect of systemic scopolamine injections, indicating a critical contribution of the medial septal area. Further studies supported the central position of the septohippocampal pathway and revealed a more distributed network including a few other limbic and non-limbic structures (101).

Intraventricular administration of the M1 antagonist pirenzepine exerts similar effects as scopolamine, suggesting that M1 receptors may dominate in mediating spontaneous spatial alternation (102). On the other hand, M2 knockout mice were found to perform worse only under longer (20 s) but not short (5 s) delays in reinforced alternation in a T-maze compared to wild-type controls (103), suggesting a more complex contribution of particular mAChR types. M5 receptors seem to play a role in alternation as well, but the mechanism of action is likely indirect. As M5 receptors are expressed by endothelial cells and control cerebral vasodilatation, M5R−/− mice were found to exhibit a significantly reduced cerebral blood flow in the cerebral cortex, hippocampus, basal ganglia, and thalamus. In consequence, the low blood supply led to impaired long-term potentiation and consequently to a deterioration of spatial alternation (104).

Despite being almost ubiquitous in pharmacological research, the spatial alternation paradigm has some drawbacks. Investigators do not usually configure the maze to enforce animals to use praxis, taxon, or mapping strategies, or any combination of these. Therefore authors cannot report, in contrast to the MWM, whether effects are due to impairment of a particular mode of place navigation. Furthermore, the variability and consistency of results have been disputed, particularly in the spontaneous alternation paradigm. However, this drawback can be counterbalanced by the fact that under some circumstances, spatial alternation has been found to be superb at detecting hippocampal dysfunction (105).

Active Place Avoidance Tasks

Active place avoidance [(106117), for review see Ref. (110, 111)] is a behavioral test specifically focusing on a rat’s ability to coordinate two conflicting frames of reference. An animal is placed into a slowly rotating arena where it needs to learn to locate a “to-be-avoided sector,” upon which stepping into it receives a foot-shock. The position of this sector does not change relative to the room frame; i.e., the animal has to actively move to another place in the arena so as not to be carried into the sector. The arena’s surroundings ought to contain distinct extramaze cues for the rats to navigate (110, 112116).

The first study with scopolamine in this task (117) showed that a deficit induced by scopolamine at doses 1 and 2 mg/kg was not alleviated by intact spatial pretraining. A follow-up study (67) compared the performance of two rat strains obtained from the breeding colony of Institute of Physiology, CAS, Prague (Long-Evans and Wistar) in the MWM and active place avoidance following scopolamine treatment. As already mentioned, whereas in the MWM the disruption in learning and memory was similar, in active place avoidance the Wistar rats exhibited a higher sensitivity to scopolamine than the Long-Evans group (67). In general, active place avoidance tasks are sensitive to antimuscarinic action elicited by scopolamine, yet the effects are strain-specific and also present at relatively higher doses that can also affect procedural aspects. Unfortunately, no active place avoidance results on more selective antagonists, mAChR knockouts or other specific manipulations with the mAChR system are available, indicating the need for future research.

Barnes Maze

In the Barnes maze, a rat is placed in the center of a circular platform with holes at the edges. An escape cylinder is placed under one of these holes; the animals are trained to locate the position of this cylinder based on distal external cues. The use of odor trails is eliminated by rotating the platform in between trials, and animals presumably use a mapping strategy to locate the target (118).

Evaluations of antimuscarinic agents employing this paradigm are scarce. Consistent with other cognitive mapping taxing tasks, scopolamine was found to impair performance (119). Seeger et al. (103) used this task for investigating changes in cognition and behavior in M2 knock-out mice, reporting a severe impairment in learning, accompanied with decreased short-term and long-term potentiation (103). Another example of the usage of this test is the study by Gawel et al. (120), in which the authors examined the potential of cholinesterase inhibitors (donepezil and rivastigmine) to alleviate ethanol-induced cognitive impairment. The results showed an improvement in both memory retention and cognitive flexibility, the latter being more pronounced for rivastigmine (120).

Cone-Field Test

The cone-field task represents another experimental paradigm for testing spatial learning and memory. It consists of a dodecagonal field with a number of cones topped with un/baited food cups in the middle and four starting boxes on the borders, from which the animal is released into the field. The ability of the rat to learn and remember the position of the baited cones is assessed. A suggested advantage of this test over tasks like the MWM is that it is based on positive reward learning (whereas the MWM relies on aversive learning). This task was used for example by Van der Staay et al. (121) to investigate the effects of AChE inhibitors (donepezil and metrifonate) on scopolamine-induced learning deficits in rats. The results showed that metrifonate, but not donepezil, was able to alleviate the working memory disruption produced by scopolamine (121). Specific conclusions on the role of mAChRs in this task are impossible due to the limited data.

Hole-Board Task

In the hole-board task, an animal is placed in a rectangular box with a number of holes in the floor. Some of these are baited with a food reward. An animal is evaluated in its ability to learn and remember the position (using a mapping strategy) of the baited holes as well as the holes it has already visited. Different variations and adaptions of this task have been used. For example, Post et al. (122) published a article on a hole-board paradigm specially designed for mice (COGITAT) and presented its validation as a tool for testing spatial learning and memory via a scopolamine-induced performance deficit and its alleviation by metrifonate (122). Regarding the involvement of particular types of receptors, M1 receptors were shown to be important for reference memory (for non-baited holes) in a study evaluating biperiden in pigs (47). On the other hand, M2 receptors were shown to be important for working memory (memory for already-visited holes) in a study using transgenic mice (123).

General Discussion and Concluding Remarks

The muscarinic system of the brain plays a pivotal role in advanced cognitive processes such as spatial navigation and learning, an extensively studied ability, not only to gain insight into the way humans and animals orient themselves in both familiar and unfamiliar environments, but because spatial memory represents a rodent model of human perceptual memory. Research in this field provides new findings regarding the neurophysiology of higher cognitive processes, as well as pathologies such as those seen in AD and other neurodegenerative diseases, and indicates potential pathways for the therapy and treatment of these conditions.

However, as the muscarinic system is important not only for learning, memory and cognition but also takes parts in other processes such as attention, motivation, sensory perception, and other non-cognitive aspects of behavior, it is no surprise that the blockage of mAChRs also yields a wide range of non-cognitive effects, thus hindering cognition-focused research and complicating interpretations of the effects observed in rodent behavioral experiments. There have been attempts to isolate the purely cognitive effects of muscarinic antagonism from the procedural and motivational aspects, and some have been relatively successful.

One of the more promising ways to study the effects of mAChRs in place navigation lies in the exploitation of local intracerebral administration of antagonists, which ensures no peripheral effects, or the use of specific conditional mutations. Moreover, despite attempts to use more specific muscarininc ligands to eliminate the procedural adverse effect of non-selective antagonists such as scopolamine and atropine, they have often provided ambiguous results. However, Sambeth et al. (24) recently showed that biperiden elicits cognitive deficits extending to the spatial memory domain in humans. It seems that with some caution, a general recommendation of using either non-specific or highly specific antagonists can be provided in conditions with defined place learning strategies having known involvement of the mAChR system.

Nonetheless, the ultimate need and relevance lies in the exploitation of novel techniques such as optogenetics focused on cholinergic neurons, and chemogenetics aimed at cells expressing metabotropic mAChRs. As these methods provide a more precise way to target the mAChR in the CNS, it is conceivable that relatively soon the systemic or even focal application of non-specific antimuscarinic drugs may become a rather obsolete tool for this research. However, the pharmacological development of more specific ligands for mAChRs may yet bring a revival of this traditional neuropharmacology approach. Furthermore, the need for the development of new therapeutics acting on mAChRs will result in an ongoing requirement for testing place navigation as a “prototype” of cognitive functions under the influence of these drugs.

It should also be noted that the choice of a specific behavioral test plays an essential role in the research of cognition, as various tasks examine different aspects of learning and memory (e.g., praxis vs. spatial mapping) and may possess higher or lower sensitivity toward the observed phenomenon. Furthermore, not all tasks are hippocampus-dependent, and even among those which are not all employ M1 as a crucial part (Table 1). Careful attention should also be paid to the rodent strain used; for example, albino rats such as the Wistar strain have difficulty learning vision-reliant tasks. Well-planned rodent behavioral studies with carefully thought-out experimental designs will continue to provide a useful tool for research on the muscarinic system and its role in learning and memory.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Summary of the overall effects (positive, negative, none) of particular groups of antimuscarinergic agents or transgenic manipulations on spatial performance.

Author Contributions

JS, AP, and AS wrote major parts of the manuscript. JS and AS contributed to revisions of the manuscript. AS provided scientific leadership and student supervision. The article is based on the thesis of AP.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We thank laboratory technicians for their support and David W. Hardekopf for proofreading. This work was supported by GACR grant 17-04047S and AZV grant 17-30833A. Institutional support for IPHYS was provided by RVO: 67985823. It was also supported also by Academic CZ-PL bilateral mobility project PAN-17-07. Additional support came from ERDF project, OPPK Mikroskopický systém CZ.2.16/3.1.00/28.034, ERDF OPPK BrainView CZ.2.16/3.1.00/21544, and MEYS (LM2015062) Czech-BioImaging. All rights reserved.

Abbreviations

ACh, acetylcholine; AChE, acetylcholine esterase; AChR, acetylcholine receptors; AD, Alzheimer’s disease; Asn, asparagine; CNS, central nervous system; DMP, delayed matching to position; GPCRs, G-protein-coupled receptors; mAChR, muscarinic acetylcholine receptors; MDMA, 3,4-methylendioxy-metamphtamine; MWM, Morris water maze; nAChR, nicotinic acetylcholine receptors; QNB, quinuclidinyl benzilate.

References

1. VanPatten S, Al-Abed Y. The challenges of modulating the ‘rest and digest’ system: acetylcholine receptors as drug targets. Drug Discov Today (2016) 22(1):97–104. doi:10.1016/j.drudis.2016.09.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Hut RA, Van der Zee EA. The cholinergic system, circadian rhythmicity, and time memory. Behav Brain Res (2011) 221:466–80. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Leslie FM, Mojica CY, Reynaga DD. Nicotinic receptors in addiction pathways. Mol Pharmacol (2013) 83:753–8. doi:10.1124/mol.112.083659

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Prado VF, Janickova H, Al-Onaizi MA, Prado MAM. Cholinergic circuits in cognitive flexibility. Neuroscience (2017) 345:130–41. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2016.09.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Robinson L, Platt B, Riedel G. Involvement of the cholinergic system in conditioning and perceptual memory. Behav Brain Res (2011) 221:443–65. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.01.055

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Deiana S, Platt B, Riedel G. The cholinergic system and spatial learning. Behav Brain Res (2011) 221:389–411. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.036

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Schliebs R, Arendt T. The cholinergic system in aging and neuronal degeneration. Behav Brain Res (2011) 221:555–63. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Jiang S, Li Y, Zhang C, Zhao Y, Bu G, Xu H, et al. M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor in Alzheimer’s disease. Neurosci Bull (2014) 30:295–307. doi:10.1007/s12264-013-1406-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Pittaras E, Faure A, Leray X, Moraitopoulou E, Cressant A, Rabat A, et al. Neuronal nicotinic receptors are crucial for tuning of E/I balance in prelimbic cortex and for decision-making processes. Front Psychiatry (2016) 7:171. doi:10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Carruthers SP, Gurvich CT, Rossell SL. The muscarinic system, cognition and schizophrenia. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2015) 55:393–402. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2015.05.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Witkin JM, Overshiner C, Li X, Catlow JT, Wishart GN, Schober DA, et al. M1 and m2 muscarinic receptor subtypes regulate antidepressant-like effects of the rapidly acting antidepressant scopolamine. J Pharmacol Exp Ther (2014) 351:448–56. doi:10.1124/jpet.114.216804

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Malca Garcia GR, Hennig L, Shelukhina IV, Kudryavtsev DS, Bussmann RW, Tsetlin VI, et al. Curare alkaloids: constituents of a Matis dart poison. J Nat Prod (2015) 78:2537–44. doi:10.1021/acs.jnatprod.5b00457

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Role LW, Berg DK. Nicotinic receptors in the development and modulation of CNS synapses. Neuron (1996) 16:1077–85. doi:10.1016/S0896-6273(00)80134-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Albuquerque EX, Pereira EFR, Alkondon M, Rogers SW. Mammalian nicotinic acetylcholine receptors: from structure to function. Physiol Rev (2009) 89:73–120. doi:10.1152/physrev.00015.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. He Q, Johnston J, Zeitlinger J, City K, City K. Heteromeric α7β2 nicotinic acetylcholine receptors in the brain. Trends Pharmacol Sci (2015) 33:395–401. doi:10.1038/nbt.3121.ChIP-nexus

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Sadahiro M, Sajo M, Morishita H. Nicotinic regulation of experience-dependent plasticity in visual cortex. J Physiol Paris (2016) 110:29–36. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2016.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Tomankova H, Valuskova P, Varejkova E, Rotkova J, Benes J, Myslivecek J. The M 2 muscarinic receptors are essential for signaling in the heart left ventricle during restraint stress in mice. Stress (2015) 3890:208–20. doi:10.3109/10253890.2015.1007345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. De Sarno P, Shestopal SA, King TD, Zmijewska A, Song L, Jope RS. Muscarinic receptor activation protects cells from apoptotic effects of DNA damage, oxidative stress, and mitochondrial inhibition. J Biol Chem (2003) 278:11086–93. doi:10.1074/jbc.M212157200

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Dale PR, Cernecka H, Schmidt M, Dowling MR, Charlton SJ, Pieper MP, et al. The pharmacological rationale for combining muscarinic receptor antagonists and beta-adrenoceptor agonists in the treatment of airway and bladder disease. Curr Opin Pharmacol (2014) 16:31–42. doi:10.1016/j.coph.2014.03.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Muise ED, Gandotra N, Tackett JJ, Bamdad MC, Cowles RA. Distribution of muscarinic acetylcholine receptor subtypes in the murine small intestine. Life Sci (2017) 169:6–10. doi:10.1016/j.lfs.2016.10.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Picciotto MR, Higley MJ, Mineur YS. Acetylcholine as a neuromodulator: cholinergic signaling shapes nervous system function and behavior. Neuron (2012) 76:116–29. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2012.08.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Zhang W, Basile AS, Gomeza J, Volpicelli LA, Levey AI, Wess J. Characterization of central inhibitory muscarinic autoreceptors by the use of muscarinic acetylcholine receptor knock-out mice. J Neurosci (2002) 22:1709–17.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

23. Pergolizzi JV, Philip BK, Leslie JB, Taylor R, Raffa RB. Perspectives on transdermal scopolamine for the treatment of postoperative nausea and vomiting. J Clin Anesth (2012) 24:334–45. doi:10.1016/j.jclinane.2011.07.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Sambeth A, Riedel WJ, Klinkenberg I, Kähkönen S, Blokland A. Biperiden selectively induces memory impairment in healthy volunteers: no interaction with citalopram. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2015) 232:1887–97. doi:10.1007/s00213-014-3822-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Laurino A, Matucci R, Vistoli G, Raimondi L. 3-iodothyronamine (T1AM), a novel antagonist of muscarinic receptors. Eur J Pharmacol (2016) 793:35–42. doi:10.1016/j.ejphar.2016.10.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Digby GJ, Shirey JK, Conn PJ. Allosteric activators of muscarinic receptors as novel approaches for treatment of CNS disorders. Mol Biosyst (2010) 6:1345–54. doi:10.1039/c002938f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Foster DJ, Choi DL, Jeffrey Conn P, Rook JM. Activation of M1 and M4 muscarinic receptors as potential treatments for Alzheimer’s disease and schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat (2014) 10:183–91. doi:10.2147/NDT.S55104

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Ragozzino ME, Artis S, Singh A, Twose TM, Beck JE, Messer WS. The selective M1 muscarinic cholinergic agonist CDD-0102A enhances working memory and cognitive flexibility. J Pharmacol Exp Ther (2012) 340:588–94. doi:10.1124/jpet.111.187625

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Malomouzh AI, Mukhtarov MR, Nikolsky EE, Vyskočil F. Muscarinic M1 acetylcholine receptors regulate the non-quantal release of acetylcholine in the rat neuromuscular junction via NO-dependent mechanism. J Neurochem (2007) 102:2110–7. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.2007.04696.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Desmarais JE, Beauclair L, Margolese HC. Anticholinergics in the era of atypical antipsychotics: short-term or long-term treatment? J Psychopharmacol (2012) 26:1167–74. doi:10.1177/0269881112447988

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Ogino S, Miyamoto S, Miyake N, Yamaguchi N. Benefits and limits of anticholinergic use in schizophrenia: focusing on its effect on cognitive function. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2014) 68:37–49. doi:10.1111/pcn.12088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Vinogradov S, Fisher M, Warm H, Holland C, Kirshner MA, Pollock BG. The cognitive cost of anticholinergic burden: decreased response to cognitive training in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2009) 166:1055–62. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09010017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Veselinović T, Vernaleken I, Janouschek H, Kellermann T, Paulzen M, Cumming P, et al. Effects of anticholinergic challenge on psychopathology and cognition in drug-free patients with schizophrenia and healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2015) 232:1607–17. doi:10.1007/s00213-014-3794-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Terry AV Jr. Muscarinic receptor antagonists in rats. In: Levin ED, Buccafusco JJ, editors. Animal Models of Cognitive Impairment. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis (2017). Available from: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK2525/

Google Scholar

35. Croy CH, Chan WY, Castetter AM, Watt ML, Quets AT, Felder CC. Characterization of PCS1055, a novel muscarinic M4 receptor antagonist. Eur J Pharmacol (2016) 782:70–6. doi:10.1016/j.ejphar.2016.04.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Jakubík J, Zimčík P, Randáková A, Fuksová K, El-Fakahany EE, Doležal V. Molecular mechanisms of methoctramine binding and selectivity at muscarinic acetylcholine receptors. Mol Pharmacol (2014) 86:180–92. doi:10.1124/mol.114.093310

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Deng H, Wang C, Su M, Fang Y. Probing biochemical mechanisms of action of muscarinic M3 receptor antagonists with label-free whole cell assays. Anal Chem (2012) 84:8232–9. doi:10.1021/ac301495n

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Zhong J, Roth M. Clinical potential of aclidinium bromide in chronic obstructive pulmonary disease. Ther Clin Risk Manag (2014) 10:449–53. doi:10.2147/TCRM.S39710

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Busse WW, Dahl R, Jenkins C, Cruz AA. Long-acting muscarinic antagonists: a potential add-on therapy in the treatment of asthma? Eur Respir Rev (2016) 25:54–64. doi:10.1183/16000617.0052-2015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Callegari E, Malhotra B, Bungay PJ, Webster R, Fenner KS, Kempshall S, et al. A comprehensive non-clinical evaluation of the CNS penetration potential of antimuscarinic agents for the treatment of overactive bladder. Br J Clin Pharmacol (2011) 72:235–46. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2125.2011.03961.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Peretto I, Petrillo P, Imbimbo BP. Medicinal chemistry and therapeutic potential of muscarinic M3 antagonists. Med Res Rev (2009) 29:1292–327. doi:10.1002/med

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Jeon WJ, Dean B, Scarr E, Gibbons A. The role of muscarinic receptors in the pathophysiology of mood disorders: a potential novel treatment? Curr Neuropharmacol (2015) 13:739–49. doi:10.2174/1570159X13666150612230045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Espi Martinez F, Espi Forcen F, Shapov A, Martinez Moya A. Biperiden dependence: case report and literature review. Case Rep Psychiatry (2012) 2012:949256. doi:10.1155/2012/949256

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Brocks DR. Anticholinergic drugs used in Parkinson’s disease: an overlooked class of drugs from a pharmacokinetic perspective. J Pharm Pharm Sci (1999) 2:39–46.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

45. Gazova Z, Soukup O, Sepsova V, Siposova K, Drtinova L, Jost P, et al. Multi-target-directed therapeutic potential of 7-methoxytacrine-adamantylamine heterodimers in the Alzheimer’s disease treatment. Biochim Biophys Acta (2016) 1863:607–19. doi:10.1016/j.bbadis.2016.11.020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Asth L, Lobão-Soares B, André E, Soares Vde P, Gavioli EC. The elevated T-maze task as an animal model to simultaneously investigate the effects of drugs on long-term memory and anxiety in mice. Brain Res Bull (2012) 87:526–33. doi:10.1016/j.brainresbull.2012.02.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Gieling E, Wehkamp W, Willigenburg R, Nordquist RE, Ganderup N-C, van der Staay FJ. Performance of conventional pigs and Göttingen miniature pigs in a spatial holeboard task: effects of the putative muscarinic cognition impairer Biperiden. Behav Brain Funct (2013) 9:4. doi:10.1186/1744-9081-9-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Ishizaki J, Yokogawa K, Nakashima E, Ohkuma S, Ichimura F. Influence of ammonium chloride on the tissue distribution of anticholinergic drugs in rats. J Pharm Pharmacol (1998) 50:761–6. doi:10.1111/j.2042-7158.1998.tb07137.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Bures J, Fenton AA, Kaminsky Y, Zinyuk L. Place cells and place navigation. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1997) 94:343–50. doi:10.1073/pnas.94.1.343

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. D’Hooge R, De Deyn PP. Applications of the Morris water maze in the study of learning and memory. Brain Res Brain Res Rev (2001) 36(1):60–90. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(01)00067-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Morris RGM. Spatial localization does not require the presence of local cues. Learn Motiv (1981) 12:239–60. doi:10.1016/0023-9690(81)90020-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Sutherland RJ, Whishaw IQ, Regehr JC. Cholinergic receptor blockade impairs spatial localization by use of distal cues in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol (1982) 96:563–73. doi:10.1037/h0077914

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Kitanishi T, Ito HT, Hayashi Y, Shinohara Y, Mizuseki K, Hikida T. Network mechanisms of hippocampal laterality, place coding, and goal-directed navigation. J Physiol Sci (2017) 67:247–58. doi:10.1007/s12576-016-0502-z

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J. The hippocampus as a spatial map. Preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely-moving rat. Brain Res (1971) 34:171–5. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(71)90358-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. McNaughton BL, Battaglia FP, Jensen O, Moser EI, Moser M-B. Path integration and the neural basis of the “cognitive map”. Nat Rev Neurosci (2006) 7:663–78. doi:10.1038/nrn1932

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Nicolelis MAL, Lebedev MA. Principles of neural ensemble physiology underlying the operation of brain-machine interfaces. Nat Rev Neurosci (2009) 10:530–40. doi:10.1038/nrn2653

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Hafting T, Fyhn M, Molden S, Moser M-B, Moser EI. Microstructure of a spatial map in the entorhinal cortex. Nature (2005) 436:801–6. doi:10.1038/nature03721

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Taube JS, Muller RU, Ranck JB Jr. Head-direction cells recorded from the postsubiculum in freely moving rats. II. Effects of environmental manipulations. J Neurosci (1990) 10:436–47.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

59. Burgess N. Spatial memory: how egocentric and allocentric combine. Trends Cogn Sci (2006) 10:551–7. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.10.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Yan C, Wang R, Qu J, Chen G. Locating and navigation mechanism based on place-cell and grid-cell models. Cogn Neurodyn (2016) 10:353–60. doi:10.1007/s11571-016-9384-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Fenton AA, Wesierska M, Kaminsky Y, Bures J. Both here and there: simultaneous expression of autonomous spatial memories in rats. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1998) 95:11493–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.19.11493

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Cimadevilla JM, Wesierska M, Fenton AA, Bures J. Inactivating one hippocampus impairs avoidance of a stable room-defined place during dissociation of arena cues from room cues by rotation of the arena. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2001) 98:3531–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.051628398

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Wesierska M, Dockery C, Fenton AA. Beyond memory, navigation, and inhibition: behavioral evidence for hippocampus-dependent cognitive coordination in the rat. J Neurosci (2005) 25:2413–9. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3962-04.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kubík S, Fenton AA. Behavioral evidence that segregation and representation are dissociable hippocampal functions. J Neurosci (2005) 25:9205–12. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1707-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Klinkenberg I, Blokland A. A comparison of scopolamine and biperiden as a rodent model for cholinergic cognitive impairment. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2011) 215:549–66. doi:10.1007/s00213-011-2171-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Robinson L, Harbaran D, Riedel G. Visual acuity in the water maze: sensitivity to muscarinic receptor blockade in rats and mice. Behav Brain Res (2004) 151:277–86. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2003.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Entlerova M, Lobellova V, Hatalova H, Zemanova A, Vales K, Stuchlik A. Comparison of Long-Evans and Wistar rats in sensitivity to central cholinergic blockade with scopolamine in two spatial tasks: an active place avoidance and the Morris water maze. Physiol Behav (2013) 120:11–8. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2013.06.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. von Linstow Roloff E, Harbaran D, Micheau J, Platt B, Riedel G. Dissociation of cholinergic function in spatial and procedural learning in rats. Neuroscience (2007) 146:875–89. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2007.02.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Buresova O, Krekule I, Zahalka A, Bures J. On-demand platform improves accuracy of the Morris water maze procedure. J Neurosci Methods (1985) 15:63–72. doi:10.1016/0165-0270(85)90062-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Spooner RI, Thomson A, Hall J, Morris RG, Salter SH. The Atlantis platform: a new design and further developments of Buresova’s on-demand platform for the water maze. Learn Mem (1994) 1:203–11.

Google Scholar

71. Bertrand F, Lehmann O, Galani R, Lazarus C, Jeltsch H, Cassel JC. Effects of MDL 73005 on water-maze performances and locomotor activity in scopolamine-treated rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (2001) 68:647–60. doi:10.1016/S0091-3057(01)00448-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Brazhnik ES, Muller RU, Fox SE. Muscarinic blockade slows and degrades the location-specific firing of hippocampal pyramidal cells. J Neurosci (2003) 23:611–21.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

73. Brazhnik E, Borgnis R, Muller RU, Fox SE. The effects on place cells of local scopolamine dialysis are mimicked by a mixture of two specific muscarinic antagonists. J Neurosci (2004) 24:9313–23. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1618-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

74. Newman EL, Gillet SN, Climer JR, Hasselmo ME. Cholinergic blockade reduces theta-gamma phase amplitude coupling and speed modulation of theta frequency consistent with behavioral effects on encoding. J Neurosci (2013) 33:19635–46. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2586-13.2013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

75. Newman EL, Climer JR, Hasselmo ME. Grid cell spatial tuning reduced following systemic muscarinic receptor blockade. Hippocampus (2014) 24:643–55. doi:10.1002/hipo.22253

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

76. Hasselmo ME. The role of acetylcholine in learning and memory. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2006) 16:710–5. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2006.09.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

77. Cozzolino R, Guaraldi D, Giuliani A, Ghirardi O, Ramacci MT, Angelucci L. Effects of concomitant nicotinic and muscarinic blockade on spatial memory disturbance in rats are purely additive: evidence from the Morris water task. Physiol Behav (1994) 56:111–4. doi:10.1016/0031-9384(94)90267-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

78. Riekkinen M, Riekkinen P. Dorsal hippocampal muscarinic acetylcholine and NMDA receptors disrupt water maze navigation. Neuroreport (1997) 8:645–8. doi:10.1097/00001756-199702100-00013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

79. Huang Z-B, Wang H, Rao X-R, Zhong G-F, Hu W-H, Sheng G-Q. Different effects of scopolamine on the retrieval of spatial memory and fear memory. Behav Brain Res (2011) 221:604–9. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2010.05.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

80. Laczó J, Markova H, Lobellova V, Gazova I, Parizkova M, Cerman J, et al. Scopolamine disrupts place navigation in rats and humans: a translational validation of the Hidden Goal Task in the Morris water maze and a real maze for humans. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2016) 234(4):535–47. doi:10.1007/s00213-016-4488-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

81. Day LB, Schallert T. Anticholinergic effects on acquisition of place learning in the Morris water task: spatial mapping deficit or inability to inhibit nonplace strategies? Behav Neurosci (1996) 110:998–1005. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.110.5.998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

82. Misik J, Vanek J, Musilek K, Kassa J. Cholinergic antagonist 3-quinuclidinyl benzilate – impact on learning and memory in Wistar rats. Behav Brain Res (2014) 266:193–200. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2014.03.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

83. Kobayashi F, Yageta Y, Yamazaki T, Wakabayashi E, Inoue M, Segawa M, et al. Pharmacological effects of imidafenacin (KRP-197/ONO-8025), a new bladder selective anti-cholinergic agent, in rats. Comparison of effects on urinary bladder capacity and contraction, salivary secretion and performance in the Morris water maze task. Arzneimittelforschung (2007) 57:147–54. doi:10.1055/s-0031-1296598

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

84. Hagan JJ, Jansen JHM, Broekkamp CLE. Blockade of spatial learning by the M1 muscarinic antagonist pirenzepine. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (1987) 93:470–6. doi:10.1007/BF00207237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

85. Hunter AJ, Roberts FF. The effect of pirenzepine on spatial learning in the Morris Water Maze. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (1988) 30:519–23. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(88)90490-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

86. Miyakawa T, Yamada M, Duttaroy A, Wess J. Hyperactivity and intact hippocampus-dependent learning in mice lacking the M1 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor. J Neurosci (2001) 21:5239–50.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

87. Bubser M, Byun N, Wood MR, Jones CK. Muscarinic receptor pharmacology and circuitry for the modulation of cognition. Handb Exp Pharmacol (2012) 208:121–66. doi:10.1007/978-3-642-23274-9_7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

88. Rowe WB, O’Donnell J-P, Pearson D, Rose GM, Meaney MJ, Quirion R. Long-term effects of BIBN-99, a selective muscarinic M2 receptor antagonist, on improving spatial memory performance in aged cognitively impaired rats. Behav Brain Res (2003) 145:171–8. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(03)00116-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

89. Greenlee W, Clader J, Asberom T, McCombie S, Ford J, Guzik H, et al. Muscarinic agonists and antagonists in the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease. Farmaco (2001) 56:247–50. doi:10.1016/S0014-827X(01)01102-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

90. Koshimizu H, Leiter LM, Miyakawa T. M4 muscarinic receptor knockout mice display abnormal social behavior and decreased prepulse inhibition. Mol Brain (2012) 5:10. doi:10.1186/1756-6606-5-10

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

91. Pilcher JJ, Sessions GR, McBride SA. Scopolamine impairs spatial working memory in the radial maze: an analysis by error type and arm choice. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (1997) 58:449–59. doi:10.1016/S0091-3057(97)00297-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

92. Myhrer T. Neurotransmitter systems involved in learning and memory in the rat: a meta-analysis based on studies of four behavioral tasks. Brain Res Brain Res Rev (2003) 41:268–87. doi:10.1016/S0165-0173(02)00268-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

93. Kay C, Harper DN, Hunt M. Differential effects of MDMA and scopolamine on working versus reference memory in the radial arm maze task. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2010) 93:151–6. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2009.09.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

94. Ortega-Alvaro A, Gibert-Rahola J, Micó JA. Influence of chronic treatment with olanzapine, clozapine and scopolamine on performance of a learned 8-arm radial maze task in rats. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry (2006) 30:104–11. doi:10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.08.020

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

95. Hodges DB, Lindner MD, Hogan JB, Jones KM, Markus EJ. Scopolamine induced deficits in a battery of rat cognitive tests: comparisons of sensitivity and specificity. Behav Pharmacol (2009) 20:237–51. doi:10.1097/FBP.0b013e32832c70f5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

96. Klinkenberg I, Blokland A. The validity of scopolamine as a pharmacological model for cognitive impairment: a review of animal behavioral studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 34:1307–50. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

97. Moran PM. Differential effects of scopolamine and mecamylamine on working and reference memory in the rat. Pharmacol Biochem Behav (1993) 45:533–8. doi:10.1016/0091-3057(93)90502-K

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

98. Newman LA, Gold PE. Attenuation in rats of impairments of memory by scopolamine, a muscarinic receptor antagonist, by mecamylamine, a nicotinic receptor antagonist. Psychopharmacology (Berl) (2016) 233:925–32. doi:10.1007/s00213-015-4174-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

99. Spowart-Manning L, van der Staay FJ. The T-maze continuous alternation task for assessing the effects of putative cognition enhancers in the mouse. Behav Brain Res (2004) 151:37–46. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2003.08.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

100. Givens B, Olton DS. Bidirectional modulation of scopolamine-induced working memory impairments by muscarinic activation of the medial septal area. Neurobiol Learn Mem (1995) 63:269–76. doi:10.1006/nlme.1995.1031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

101. Lalonde R. The neurobiological basis of spontaneous alternation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2002) 26:91–104. doi:10.1016/S0149-7634(01)00041-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

102. Ukai M, Shinkai N, Kameyama T. Cholinergic receptor agonists inhibit pirenzepine-induced dysfunction of spontaneous alternation performance in the mouse. Gen Pharmacol (1995) 26:1529–32. doi:10.1016/0306-3623(95)00038-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

103. Seeger T, Fedorova I, Zheng F, Miyakawa T, Koustova E, Gomeza J, et al. M2 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor knock-out mice show deficits in behavioral flexibility, working memory, and hippocampal plasticity. J Neurosci (2004) 24:10117–27. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3581-04.2004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

104. Araya R, Noguchi T, Yuhki M, Kitamura N, Higuchi M, Saido TC, et al. Loss of M5 muscarinic acetylcholine receptors leads to cerebrovascular and neuronal abnormalities and cognitive deficits in mice. Neurobiol Dis (2006) 24:334–44. doi:10.1016/j.nbd.2006.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

105. Deacon RMJ, Rawlins JNP. T-maze alternation in the rodent. Nat Protoc (2006) 1:7–12. doi:10.1038/nprot.2006.2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

106. Hatalova H, Radostova D, Pistikova A, Vales K, Stuchlik A. Spatial reversal learning in chronically sensitized rats and in undrugged sensitized rats with dopamine d2-like receptor agonist quinpirole. Front Behav Neurosci (2014) 8:122. doi:10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00122

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

107. Stuchlik A, Petrasek T, Vales K. Dopamine D2 receptors and alpha1-adrenoceptors synergistically modulate locomotion and behavior of rats in a place avoidance task. Behav Brain Res (2008) 189:139–44. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2007.12.025

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

108. Stuchlik A, Vales K. Role of alpha1- and alpha2-adrenoceptors in the regulation of locomotion and spatial behavior in the active place avoidance task: a dose-response study. Neurosci Lett (2008) 433:235–40. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2008.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

109. Stuchlik A, Vales K. Systemic administration of MK-801, a non-competitive NMDA-receptor antagonist, elicits a behavioural deficit of rats in the Active Allothetic Place Avoidance (AAPA) task irrespectively of their intact spatial pretraining. Behav Brain Res (2005) 159:163–71. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2004.10.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

110. Stuchlík A, Petrásek T, Prokopová I, Holubová K, Hatalová H, Valeš K, et al. Place avoidance tasks as tools in the behavioral neuroscience of learning and memory. Physiol Res (2013) 62(Suppl 1):S1–19.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

111. Stuchlik A, Kubik S, Vlcek K, Vales K. Spatial navigation: implications for animal models, drug development and human studies. Physiol Res (2014) 63(Suppl 1):S237–49.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

112. Bubenikova-Valesova V, Stuchlik A, Svoboda J, Bures J, Vales K. Risperidone and ritanserin but not haloperidol block effect of dizocilpine on the active allothetic place avoidance task. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2008) 105:1061–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.0711273105

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

113. Bures J, Fenton AA, Kaminsky YU, Wesierska M, Zahalka A. Rodent navigation after dissociation of the allocentric and idiothetic representations of space. Neuropharmacology (1998) 37:689–99. doi:10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00031-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

114. Czeh B, Stuchlik A, Wesierska M, Cimadevilla JM, Pokorny J, Seress L, et al. Effect of neonatal dentate gyrus lesion on allothetic and idiothetic navigation in rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2001) 75:190–213. doi:10.1006/nlme.2000.3975

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

115. Kubik S, Stuchlik A, Fenton AA. Evidence for hippocampal role in place avoidance other than merely memory storage. Physiol Res (2006) 55:445–52.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

116. Stuchlik A, Bures J. Relative contribution of allothetic and idiothetic navigation to place avoidance on stable and rotating arenas in darkness. Behav Brain Res (2002) 128:179–88. doi:10.1016/S0166-4328(01)00314-X

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

117. Vales K, Stuchlik A. Central muscarinic blockade interferes with retrieval and reacquisition of active allothetic place avoidance despite spatial pretraining. Behav Brain Res (2005) 161(2):238–44.

Google Scholar

118. Barnes CA. Memory deficits associated with senescence: a neurophysiological and behavioral study in the rat. J Comp Physiol Psychol (1979) 93:74–104. doi:10.1037/h0077579

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

119. Komater VA, Buckley MJ, Browman KE, Pan JB, Hancock AA, Decker MW, et al. Effects of histamine H3 receptor antagonists in two models of spatial learning. Behav Brain Res (2005) 159:295–300. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2004.11.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

120. Gawel K, Labuz K, Gibula-Bruzda E, Jenda M, Marszalek-Grabska M, Filarowska J, et al. Cholinesterase inhibitors, donepezil and rivastigmine, attenuate spatial memory and cognitive flexibility impairment induced by acute ethanol in the Barnes maze task in rats. Naunyn Schmiedebergs Arch Pharmacol (2016) 389:1059–71. doi:10.1007/s00210-016-1269-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

121. Van Der Staay FJ, Bouger PC. Effects of the cholinesterase inhibitors donepezil and metrifonate on scopolamine-induced impairments in the spatial cone field orientation task in rats. Behav Brain Res (2005) 156:1–10. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2004.05.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

122. Post AM, Wultsch T, Popp S, Painsipp E, Wetzstein H, Kittel-Schneider S, et al. The COGITAT holeboard system as a valuable tool to assess learning, memory and activity in mice. Behav Brain Res (2011) 220:152–8. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2011.01.054

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

123. Bainbridge NK, Koselke LR, Jeon J, Bailey KR, Wess J, Crawley JN, et al. Learning and memory impairments in a congenic C57BL/6 strain of mice that lacks the M2 muscarinic acetylcholine receptor subtype. Behav Brain Res (2008) 190:50–8. doi:10.1016/j.bbr.2008.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: scopolamine, biperiden, acetylcholine, receptor, behavior, learning, memory, rodents

Citation: Svoboda J, Popelikova A and Stuchlik A (2017) Drugs Interfering with Muscarinic Acetylcholine Receptors and Their Effects on Place Navigation. Front. Psychiatry 8:215. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2017.00215

Received: 30 May 2017; Accepted: 16 October 2017;
Published: 09 November 2017

Edited by:

Tomiki Sumiyoshi, National Center of Neurology and Psychiatry, Japan

Reviewed by:

Istvan Hernadi, University of Pécs, Hungary
Takashi Uehara, Kanazawa Medical University, Japan
Otakar Raska, Charles University, Czechia

Copyright: © 2017 Svoboda, Popelikova and Stuchlik. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Ales Stuchlik, ales.stuchlik@fgu.cas.cz

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.