Skip to main content

CLINICAL CASE STUDY article

Front. Psychiatry, 07 April 2015
Sec. Neuropharmacology

Musical hallucinations treated with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors

  • 1Parnassia Psychiatric Institute, The Hague, Netherlands
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, Groningen, Netherlands
  • 3Department of Neurology, Ashford and St. Peter’s Hospitals, Chertsey, UK
  • 4Department of Neurology, St. George’s Hospital, London, UK
  • 5Department of Psychiatry, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, Netherlands
  • 6Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, Utrecht, Netherlands

Musical hallucinations are relatively rare auditory percepts which, due to their intrusive nature and the accompanying fear of impending mental decline, tend to cause significant distress and impairment. Although their etiology and pathophysiology appear to be heterogeneous and no evidence-based treatment methods are available, case reports indicate that acetylcholinesterase inhibitors may yield positive results in patients with comorbid hearing loss. We present two female patients (aged 76 and 78 years) both of whom suffered from hearing impairment and practically incessant musical hallucinations. Both patients were successfully treated with the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor rivastigmine. Based on these two case descriptions and an overview of studies describing the use of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors in similar patients, we discuss possible mechanisms and propose further research on the use of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors for musical hallucinations experienced in concordance with hearing loss.

Introduction

Musical hallucinations (also known as musical hallucinosis, auditory Charles Bonnet syndrome, and Oliver Sacks’ syndrome) are characterized by hallucinated songs, tunes, melodies, harmonics, rhythms, and/or timbres (1). They can be perceived within the head, or as though emanating from the environment. However, by definition, they are perceptual in nature and thus different from the “earworms” or “tunes in the head” that we all experience at times (2). When first perceiving musical hallucinations, people tend to attribute them to an external source but, within a few days, most realize that the music originates from “within” their head. Insight is often intact and, apart from hearing loss or tinnitus, most patients display no additional comorbidity. Therefore, the term “idiopathic musical hallucination” is used to describe such cases, in contrast to those which are attributed to demonstrable underlying pathology, i.e., symptomatic musical hallucinations.

The prevalence of musical hallucinations seems to be higher than traditionally suspected, even when taking into account a review by Cope and Baguley (3) who reported their presence in almost 1% of individuals in a population with acquired hearing loss. Experienced clinicians report relatively frequent encounters with people experiencing them (4), and a survey among patients referred for audiometric testing found musical hallucinations in 3.6% of the cases (5). The pathophysiology of such hallucinations is probably diverse and certainly needs further elucidation. A magnetoencephalography (MEG) study in one individual with musical hallucinations and hearing loss indicates involvement of right temporoparietal areas (6), whereas a more recent MEG study in a similar patient indicates involvement of the left anterior superior temporal gyrus, motor cortex, posteromedial cortex, and left lateral orbitofrontal cortex at the onset of hallucinations after a residual inhibition paradigm (7). However, apart from those specific areas, the vast brain network involved in their mediation seems to comprise auditory areas, basal ganglia, brainstem, pons, tegmentum, cerebellum, hippocampi, amygdala, visual areas and, in some cases, perhaps also the peripheral auditory system (4). The risk factors for musical hallucinations are also complex and diverse (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Known risk factors for musical hallucinations: after Sacks and Blom (4).

The main risk factors for musical hallucinations are impaired hearing, tinnitus, advanced age and, perhaps, also female sex; however, the latter finding may be due to an overrepresentation of females in the literature (4). It remains uncertain whether psychosis, schizotypal or schizoid personality, and other psychiatric disorders increase the risk for musical hallucinations (811).

Evidence-based treatment protocols are lacking. However, case reports and small case series indicate that some people can be treated non-pharmacologically through psychoeducation, use of a hearing aid, and/or attention-diverting activities, whereas others can be treated pharmacologically with anticonvulsants, antidepressants, or antipsychotics; however, in many cases, the hallucinations prove refractory to treatment (4, 12). Here, we present two patients who derived benefit from the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor rivastigmine. Based on these two cases and a discussion of similar earlier cases, we explore possible mechanisms of action for acetylcholinesterase inhibitors in the treatment of musical hallucinations.

Materials and Methods

We describe two patients, the first of whom is a 76-year-old female who was treated at the outpatient clinic of Parnassia Psychiatric Institute (The Hague, the Netherlands). As this patient died at age 80 years, written consent to publish was obtained from her son. The second patient is a 78-year-old female who was treated at Ashford/St. Peter’s Hospital (Chertsey, UK). Due to her sudden death no consent to publish could be obtained. For the present review, we conducted a systematic search in Pubmed and the Ovid database, which included EMBASE (1980 through November 2014), Ovid Medline (1948 through November 2014), and PsycINFO (1806 through November 2014). In each database, the search terms musical hallucination, musical hallucinosis, and auditory Charles Bonnet syndrome, were used separately. Each of the terms was then combined separately with cholinesterase inhibitor, acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, rivastigmine, and donepezil.

Results

Case Reports

Patient 1

In 2009, a 76-year-old woman with impaired hearing was referred because of musical hallucinations, which she had experienced since her husband’s death 6 years earlier. On the day of his death, she had suddenly heard hymns, lullabies, pop songs, and classical tunes, which repeated themselves indefinitely before changing into different pieces of music. Although she perceived them inside the head, they had a definite perceptual quality. They were present from the moment she awoke until late at night, and she used a radio to drown them out when trying to sleep. During the daytime, they receded into the background only when she carried out tasks that required her full attention. Because she and her husband had shared a fondness for the same music, she initially considered the hallucinations to be a sign of ongoing interconnectedness between the two of them. This conviction was strengthened by various hypnagogic episodes during which she once saw her husband surrounded by a bright light, and on other occasions felt a presence nearby or a hand through her hair. After 6 months, the musical hallucinations had exhausted her to such an extent that she consulted a psychologist, who interpreted them as part of a normal grieving process and offered her counseling – which she quit after a few sessions. When she consulted our group 5 years later, she was not receiving any psychiatric treatment. Her somatic history comprised tension headaches, asthma, arthritis, and a myocardial infarction. Her medication consisted of acetylsalicylate, dipyridamole, pantoprazole, isosorbide mononitrate, simvastatin, salbutamol, and tiotropium bromide. We found no evidence for any of these medications to be a potential cause of her hallucinations, except for the acetylsalicylate (13). The psychiatric examination yielded no abnormalities other than the grief-related hypnagogic phenomena described above. Notably, there were no signs of other hallucinations, depression, psychosis, schizotypy, or cognitive decline (although the latter was not formally tested). The neurological examination and the electroencephalogram (EEG) were also unremarkable. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the brain showed multiple white-matter lesions, especially in the right inferior frontal area and the left parietal lobe, but nothing unusual for her age. To rule out the possibility that the hallucinations were caused by the use of acetylsalicylate, the latter drug was changed to clopidogrel, but without any effect. Despite the negative EEG findings, we then prescribed 150 mg of valproic acid; with this treatment, the musical hallucinations diminished substantially and our patient experienced a range of symptom-free hours every day. We also advised her to use her hearing aid, to optimize the use of her radio, and to engage in social events; however, these latter activities had no effect. Meanwhile, because a higher dose of valproic acid was not well tolerated, in an attempt to improve her condition we tapered it off and proposed rivastigmine instead. As in dementia, we titrated the rivastigmine dose upwards; however, because our patient reported severe dizziness at a dosage of 4.5 mg, we advised her to reduce the dose to 1.5 mg/day. Within a few days, the musical hallucinations had vanished completely. At this low dose, there were no side-effects and, during a follow-up period of 4 years until her death in 2013 due to a cerebral hemorrhage, she remained symptom-free, even though she occasionally missed a dose of rivastigmine.

Patient 2

In 2014, a 78-year-old woman with hearing impairment was referred because of musical hallucinations, which had been present for 1 year. Initially, she was convinced that the people next door kept playing Christmas carols, even at night and when they had left the house. Later, the experienced music was unrecognizable to her. She felt she wanted to harm the neighbors, and also became estranged from her sister who maintained that she did not hear the music. Despite also hearing it when she stayed at her daughter’s home, she continued to believe that the neighbors were causing the noise. Adjusting the hearing aids that she had worn for 1 year brought no relief, but watching TV reduced the volume of the hallucinated music. She scored the usual level of loudness on a visual analog scale (VAS) as 8–9/10 and stated that the noise interfered with her falling asleep. Our patient had lived alone for 20 years and on most days did not speak to anyone. Although there was no psychiatric history, due to the hallucinations she developed a depressed mood, which did not improve significantly with mirtazapine. Her other medications comprised metformin, gliclazide, sitagliptin, co-codamol, simvastatin, and atenolol, neither of which could be held responsible for her hallucinations. There were no abnormalities on neurological and psychiatric examination. Notably, we found no evidence for any other types of hallucination. In the consulting room, the musical hallucinations commenced after 30 s of silence. The Addenbrooke’s Cognitive Examination-Revised (ACE-R) score was 79/100, suggesting mild cognitive impairment (MCI). An MRI scan of the brain showed unremarkable white-matter changes. Within days of starting rivastigmine 1.5 mg, the music became less frequent and less loud (VAS 5/10) and her anger and depressed mood disappeared. We did not repeat ACE-R, but insight into the origin of the music improved significantly even though at times she still suspected the neighbors. Three months after starting the rivastigmine, in the consulting room the music started after 1 min, rather than after 30 s, of silence. The dose was increased to 4.5 mg/day in an attempt to treat both the hallucinations and MCI, but 6 months later she died suddenly due to pneumonia.

Literature Search

Our search yielded 210 papers on musical hallucinations and 72,999 papers on acetylcholinesterase inhibitors. Of those papers, three described four original cases involving the successful treatment of musical hallucinations with the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor donepezil (1416). A fourth paper (6), which apparently described one of those four cases (14) at an earlier stage, was omitted. Table 2 provides a summary of the four original cases plus a summary of our two present cases.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Overview of reported cases of musical hallucinations treated with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors.

Discussion

All six cases published so far are consistent with the definition of idiopathic musical hallucinations, even though one of the patients described by Strauss and Gertz (15) experienced an intensification of preexisting musical hallucinations after a stroke. Regarding their pathophysiology, such hallucinations are conceptualized as deafferentiation phenomena, i.e., release hallucinations evoked by the relative absence of normal auditory input (4, 14). As noted by Ukai et al. (14), age-dependent dysfunction of cholinergic neurons may be a predisposing condition for the mediation of such release phenomena.

The brain’s cholinergic system is mainly involved not only in attention, learning, and memory but also in visual perception and visual attention (the latter being modulated by gamma oscillations and possibly also by low-frequency alpha/beta oscillations under the influence of ACh in visual cortex) (18). Thus, there is substantial evidence for the efficacy of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors in the treatment of visual hallucinations experienced in the context of Parkinson’s disease (1921). In addition, there is evidence for their efficacy in instances of isolated visual hallucinations. For example, Ukai et al. (22) described a patient with isolated visual hallucinations in the absence of cognitive decline who was successfully treated with donepezil; also, our group reported the successful treatment of visual disorders of perception with rivastigmine (17).

Animal studies suggest that cortical cholinergic input is also required for normal auditory perception (23). In humans, Irimajiri et al. (24) found that acetylcholinesterase inhibitors alter auditory cortical somatosensory potentials (N20, P50) in treated versus untreated MCI patients, while a study among Alzheimer patients indicates that they also alter resting-state EEG rhythms (25). Our knowledge of ACh in the perception, storage, production, and reproduction of music is still in its infancy, even though its involvement in auditory learning and memory has been established in animal studies and implications for neuromusic research are considered imminent (26). The role of ACh in the processing of music is supported by case reports on potent anticholinergic antidepressants (such as amitriptyline) and their ability to evoke musical hallucinations (9, 27), and on studies of musical hallucinations in association with degenerative brain diseases characterized by cholinergic deficits, such as Alzheimer’s disease and Lewy body disease (19, 28).

The receptor sites responsible for the mechanism of action of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors in cases of musical hallucination are as yet unknown. Postsynaptically, ACh acts upon nicotinic and muscarinic acetylcholine receptors. Acetylcholinesterase inhibitors increase the presynaptic availability, as well as the duration of action of ACh, by preventing the acetylcholinesterase enzyme from breaking this neuromodulator down. Rivastigmine, a brain-selective “pseudo-irreversible” acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, prevents the breakdown of ACh for a duration of 10 h (29). Animal studies have shown that rivastigmine has a specific effect on cortex and hippocampus (30). Because musical hallucinations often present in the form of familiar songs and other pieces remembered from the past (31), the hippocampus with its central role in memory and emotion seems a likely candidate. That hypothesis is further supported by the deactivation of the hippocampus in functional MRI studies of verbal auditory hallucinations (32). In addition, posterior medial cortex (with its role in memory and perception) may be involved in the retrieval of musical melody from memory (7). Other candidate mechanisms include those found by Kumar et al. (7), i.e., an increase in the gamma band in left anterior superior temporal gyrus with stronger hallucinations (which is normally involved in melody perception), in the beta band in motor cortex and posteromedial cortex, and in the theta/alpha bands in left lateral orbitofrontal cortex (associated with unpleasant music).

Conclusion and Limitations

Although our knowledge of ACh and its role in the processing of music needs further elucidation, there are reasons to consider acetylcholinesterase inhibitors for pharmacoclinical research in musical hallucinations associated with hearing loss. First of all, they seem to be safer and to yield fewer side-effects than antipsychotics, antidepressants, and anticonvulsants (33). Second, they may even yield beneficial side-effects when administered to patients suffering from comorbid cognitive decline. Furthermore, a beneficial effect may be obtained relatively quickly. For example, in the six patients described above, acetylcholinesterase inhibitors were effective within a few days (i.e., much faster than the antipsychotics used in patients experiencing verbal auditory hallucinations), at a low dose (rivastigmine 1.5 mg is much lower than the 9 mg dose used for dementia), and with substantial effect. Nevertheless, most of the evidence for the involvement of ACh in the mediation of musical hallucinations is indirect and based on observations among very small numbers of patients. Moreover, there may be some publication bias as musical hallucinations refractory to treatment with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors may have remained unreported. For other types of musical hallucination, especially those not associated with hearing loss and/or deafferentiation, the efficacy of these drugs has not yet been established.

Author Contributions

JB designed the manuscript, acquired data for the first case description, drafted the manuscript, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. JC contributed substantially to the design of the manuscript, acquired data for the second case description, revised the manuscript, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. RL acquired data for the first case description, revised the manuscript, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved. IS contributed substantially to the design of the manuscript, revised the manuscript, gave final approval of the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Berrios GE. Musical hallucinations. A historical and clinical study. Br J Psychiatry (1990) 156:188–94. doi:10.1192/bjp.156.2.188

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Sacks O. The power of music. Brain (2006) 129:2528–32. doi:10.1093/brain/awl234

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Cope TE, Baguley DM. Is musical hallucination an otological phenomenon? A review of the literature. Clin Otolaryngol (2009) 34:423–30. doi:10.1111/j.1749-4486.2009.02013.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Sacks O, Blom JD, Musical hallucinations. In: Blom JD, Sommer IEC, editors. Hallucinations. Research and Practice. New York, NY: Springer (2012). p. 133–42.

Google Scholar

5. Teunisse RJ, Olde Rikkert MG. Prevalence of musical hallucinations in patients referred for audiometric testing. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry (2012) 20:1075–7. doi:10.1097/JGP.0b013e31823e31c4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Shinosaki K, Yamamoto M, Ukai S, Kawaguchi S, Ogawa A, Ishii R, et al. Desynchronization in the right auditory cortex during musical hallucinations: a MEG study. Psychogeriatrics (2003) 3:88–92. doi:10.1046/j.1479-8301.2003.00009.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Kumar A, Sedley W, Barnes GR, Teki S, Friston KJ, Griffiths TD. A brain basis for musical hallucinations. Cortex (2014) 52:86–97. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2013.12.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Baba A, Hamada H. Musical hallucinations in schizophrenia. Psychopathology (1999) 32:242–51. doi:10.1159/000029096

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Evers S, Ellger T. The clinical spectrum of musical hallucinations. J Neurol Sci (2004) 227:55–65. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2004.08.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Hermesh H, Konas S, Shiloh R, Dar R, Marom S, Weizman A, et al. Musical hallucinations: prevalence in psychotic and nonpsychotic outpatients. J Clin Psychiatry (2004) 65:191–7. doi:10.4088/JCP.v65n0208

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Evers S. Musical hallucinations. Curr Psychiatry Rep (2006) 8:205–10. doi:10.1007/s11920-006-0024-0

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Colon-Rivera HA, Oldham MA. The mind with a radio of its own: a case report and review of the literature on the treatment of musical hallucinations. Gen Hosp Psychiatry (2014) 36:220–4. doi:10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2013.10.021

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Allen JR. Salicylate-induced musical perceptions. N Engl J Med (1985) 313:642–3. doi:10.1056/NEJM198509053131020

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Ukai S, Yamamoto M, Tanaka M, Shinosaki K, Takeda M. Donezepil in the treatment of musical hallucinations. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2007) 61:190–2. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2007.01636.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Strauss M, Gertz HJ. Treatment of musical hallucinations with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2009) 80:1298–9. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.160978

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Zilles D, Zerr I, Wedekind D. Successful treatment of musical hallucinations with the acetylcholinesterase inhibitor donezepil. J Clin Psychopharmacol (2012) 32:422–4. doi:10.1097/JCP.0b013e318253a086

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Blom JD, Sommer IEC, Koops S, Sacks O. Prosopometamorphopsia and facial hallucinations. Lancet (2014) 384:1998. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(14)61690-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Bauer M, Kluge C, Bach D, Bradbury D, Heinze HJ, Dolan RJ, et al. Cholinergic enhancement of visual attention and neural oscillations in the human brain. Curr Biol (2012) 22:397–402. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2012.01.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Figiel G, Sadowsky C. A systematic review of the effectiveness of rivastigmine for the treatment of behavioral disturbances in dementia and other neurological disorders. Curr Med Res Opin (2008) 24:157–66. doi:10.1185/030079908X260961

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Poewe W. What to do… When a Parkinson’s disease patient starts to hallucinate. Pract Neurol (2008) 8:238–41. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2008.152579

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Zahodne LB, Fernandez HH. Pathophysiology and treatment of psychosis in Parkinson’s disease. Drugs Aging (2008) 25:665–82. doi:10.2165/00002512-200825080-00004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Ukai S, Yamamoto M, Tanaka M, Takeda M. Treatment of typical Charles Bonnet syndrome with donezepil. Int Clin Psychopharmacol (2004) 19:355–7. doi:10.1097/00004850-200411000-00008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Leach ND, Nodal FR, Cordery PM, King AJ, Bajo VM. Cortical cholinergic input is required for normal auditory perception and experience-dependent plasticity in adult ferrets. J Neurosci (2013) 33:6659–71. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5039-12.2013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Irimajiri R, Michalewski HJ, Golob EJ, Starr A. Cholinesterase inhibitors affect brain potentials in amnestic mild cognitive impairment. Brain Res (2007) 1145:108–16. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2007.01.120

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Babiloni C, Del Percio C, Bordet R, Bourriez JL, Bentivoglio M, Payoux P, et al. Effects of acetylcholinesterase inhibitors and memantine on resting-state electroencephalographic rhythms in Alzheimer’s disease patients. Clin Neurophysiol (2013) 124:837–50. doi:10.1016/j.clinph.2012.09.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Weinberger NM. Neuromusic research: some benefits of incorporating basic research on the neurobiology of auditory learning and memory. Front Syst Neurosci (2013) 7:128. doi:10.3389/fnsys.2013.00128

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Gordon AG. Tricyclic-induced musical hallucinations. Biol Psychiatry (1996) 40:309–10. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(96)00066-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Mori T, Ikeda M, Fukuhara R, Suqawara Y, Nakata S, Matsumoto N, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow change in a case of Alzheimer’s disease with musical hallucinations. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2006) 256:236–9. doi:10.1007/s00406-005-0631-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Anand R, Gharabawi G, Enz A. Efficacy and safety results of the early phase studies with Exelon (ENA 713) in Alzheimer’s disease: an overview. J Drug Dev Clin Pract (1996) 8:109–16.

Google Scholar

30. Enz A, Amstutz R, Boddeke H. Brain selective inhibition of acetylcholinesterase: a novel approach to therapy for Alzheimer’s disease. Prog Brain Res (1993) 98:431–8. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62429-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Warner N, Aziz V. Hymns and arias: musical hallucinations in older people in Wales. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (2005) 20:658–60. doi:10.1002/gps.1338

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Diederen KMJ, Neggers SFW, Daalman K, Blom JD, Goekoop R, Kahn RS, et al. Deactivation of the parahippocampal gyrus preceding auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry (2010) 167:427–35. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09040456

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Rossignol DA, Frye RE. The use of medications approved for Alzheimer’s disease in autism spectrum disorder: a systematic review. Front Pediatr (2014) 2:87. doi:10.3389/fped.2014.00087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: auditory Charles Bonnet syndrome, cholinergic system, deafferentiation, donepezil, hearing loss, Oliver Sacks’ syndrome, release hallucination, rivastigmine

Citation: Blom JD, Coebergh JAF, Lauw R and Sommer IEC (2015) Musical hallucinations treated with acetylcholinesterase inhibitors. Front. Psychiatry 6:46. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00046

Received: 27 January 2015; Accepted: 18 March 2015;
Published online: 07 April 2015.

Edited by:

Adrian Preda, University of California Irvine, USA

Reviewed by:

Ciaran J. Faherty, Mallinckrodt Pharmaceuticals, USA
Laura Gray, Deakin University, Australia

Copyright: © 2015 Blom, Coebergh, Lauw and Sommer. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Jan Dirk Blom, Parnassia Psychiatric Institute, Kiwistraat 43, The Hague 2552 DH, Netherlands e-mail: jd.blom@parnassia.nl

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.