Skip to main content

MINI REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 16 July 2014
Sec. Addictive Disorders

Cognitive impairments in alcohol-dependent subjects

\r\n      Florent Bernardin,Florent Bernardin1,2Anne Maheut-BosserAnne Maheut-Bosser1Franois Paille,*François Paille1,2*
  • 1Service d’Addictologie, CHU Nancy, Vandoeuvre, France
  • 2Faculté de Médecine, Université de Lorraine, Nancy, France

Chronic excessive alcohol consumption induces cognitive impairments mainly affecting executive functions, episodic memory, and visuospatial capacities related to multiple brain lesions. These cognitive impairments not only determine everyday management of these patients, but also impact on the efficacy of management and may compromise the abstinence prognosis. Maintenance of lasting abstinence is associated with cognitive recovery in these patients, but some impairments may persist and interfere with the good conduct and the efficacy of management. It therefore appears essential to clearly define neuropsychological management designed to identify and evaluate the type and severity of alcohol-related cognitive impairments. It is also essential to develop cognitive remediation therapy so that the patient can fully benefit from the management proposed in addiction medicine units.

Alcoholism causes a multitude of social and health problems with negative impact on quality of life and secondary costs to society (13). Evidence suggests that numerous determinants such as environmental, individual, and genetic factors could favor evolution toward alcohol-dependence. These factors may also interact with each other. Among environmental factors, quality of the neighborhood (4) or socio-economic factors (e.g., lower educational level, employment status) (5, 6) for example may increase risks of alcohol abuse. Individual and psychological characteristics including comorbid psychiatric disorders (7, 8), early life stress exposure (9), or impulsivity (10) are also risk-factors associated with chronic alcohol consumption. In addition, family, twin, and adoption studies have highlighted that genetic factors play an important role in the pathogenesis of alcohol-dependence (1113). Heritability of alcohol-dependence is estimated between 50 and 80% (14) and is considered as a complex polygenic phenotype. In the same way, recent studies have examined cognitive endophenotype in alcoholism. They have shown that non-alcoholic relatives of alcohol-dependent individuals performed worse on cognitive tasks (specifically executive functions) and presented greater impulsiveness compared to control [e.g., Ref. (15)].

From a neurobiological perspective, alcohol-dependence is a chronic disorder, which implies the dopaminergic system. As seen in other drugs abuses, alcohol consumption acutely stimulates dopamine (DA) release from the major terminal area of the mesolimbic DA system, nucleus accumbens (NAC). Enhanced DA transmission in the NAC plays a critical role in the positive rewarding aspects of drugs abuses and the initiation of addictive process. Chronic administration is associated with functional alterations of this important part of the brain reward system. Globally, dysregulation of the dopaminergic system caused by chronic alcohol consumption produces drug dependence reinforcement and is most likely involved in the development of drug addiction (1618).

The harmful effects of chronic alcohol consumption on the brain and cognitive functioning have been well described in the literature over recent decades (19). Cognitive impairments observed in alcohol-dependent patients not presenting any other neurological complications are increasingly becoming the focus of attention of addiction medicine professionals due to their impact on management, as, according to various studies, between 50 and 80% of these patients present impaired cognitive function (20, 21). These impairments are moderate to severe but usually remain undiagnosed when they are not specifically investigated. However, detailed neuropsychological assessment or screening of these cognitive impairments appears to be fundamental to optimally adapt patient management strategies.

Alcohol-Related Cognitive Impairments

Neuroanatomical alterations can account for cognitive impairments affecting various functions, primarily executive functions (22). Fifty to 80% of patients present alterations of cognitive functions that probably impact on their management. However, there is a marked interindividual variability of the nature and severity of these impairments.

Ihara et al. (23) defined four profiles in these patients: (1) no cognitive impairment, (2) isolated executive deficits with no impairment of memory and global cognitive efficiency, (3) modified dysexecutive syndrome with memory impairments and preservation of global cognitive efficiency, and (4) global impairment (executive, memory, and global cognitive efficiency). More specifically, the dysexecutive syndrome can affect various processes such as working memory (24), mental flexibility (25), divided attention (26), decision-making (27, 28), or problem-solving (29). Disorders of prepotent response inhibition play a predominant role in addiction medicine to the point that alcohol-dependence has been described as a “disinhibitory disorder” (28).

The characteristic profile of alteration of episodic memory in alcohol-dependent patients comprises limited learning capacities, impairments of encoding, and recollection processes, difficulties recalling the temporospatial context and deficits of autonoetic consciousness, while information storage is preserved (25, 30). Alteration of executive functions, particularly disorders of inhibition, flexibility, or dual-task coordination also constitute predictive factors of memory impairment (25, 30). In contrast, apart from obvious deficits (i.e., related to dysexecutive syndrome), there is also probably a genuine impairment of episodic memory likely due to the hippocampal atrophy observed in these patients (25).

Finally, visuospatial functions are also predominantly affected, as several studies have demonstrated impaired performances on visuospatial processing, memory and visual learning, visuospatial organization, and visuoconstruction tasks (31, 32).

Three main hypotheses have been proposed in the literature to account for the characteristic cognitive profile observed in alcohol-dependent patients [see Ref. (31) for a detailed review of these hypotheses].

The first hypothesis is based on the pervasiveness and impaired recovery after withdrawal of visuospatial cognitive functions that are attributed to the non-dominant hemisphere. The right hemisphere would therefore be more susceptible to the neurotoxic effects of alcohol (32). However, this postulate has been questioned by contradictory results (31). The second hypothesis proposes that the increased susceptibility of frontal structures would account for the cognitive profile dominated by executive deficits (33). However, this hypothesis also appears to be too restrictive, as other cerebral structures are also involved (34). The third hypothesis, based on neuroanatomical and neuropsychological data, reconciles the previous two hypotheses by postulating the existence of global brain damage (35). In their meta-analysis of neuroanatomical data, the authors emphasized not only the existence of right hemisphere and frontal lobe lesions, but also lesions affecting other cerebral regions (medial temporal, subcortical, and cerebellar atrophy). The neuropsychological functional deficits observed in these patients also concern several cognitive functions in both verbal and visual modalities, which constitutes an additional argument in favor of the global brain damage hypothesis.

Anatomical Lesions

Chronic excessive alcohol consumption induces global brain atrophy characterized by reduction of brain volume and enlargement of the ventricles and sulci (36). The severity of brain damage depends on various factors such as the extent of alcohol consumption, age, gender, and neurological or psychiatric comorbidities (31). The most susceptible brain structures are the neocortex in the frontal lobes, the limbic system, and the cerebellum (37). Reduction of gray matter preferentially involves frontoparietal regions, while reduction of white matter tends to be more diffuse (22).

More precisely, Kril et al. (38) estimated that chronic alcohol consumption induced about 15–23% loss of neuronal density in the frontal regions. This result is concordant with those of a more recent study, which demonstrated a 20% reduction of the gray matter volume in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (22). Pfefferbaum et al. (39) showed that aging brain is more sensitive to the deleterious effects of excessive alcohol consumption. This study indicates that the profile of brain damage in the frontal lobes varies according to the patient’s age even after accounting for the effects of normal aging and regardless of amounts of lifetime consumption of alcohol or duration of illness. Therefore, these results indicate an increased susceptibility of the brain in the elderly according to the model of alcohol-related premature aging of the brain (40). Furthermore, many studies have also demonstrated functional changes with a reduction of glucose metabolism or cerebral blood flow in prefrontal regions, particularly in the medial temporal region (37).

Alcohol-related brain damage also concerns the limbic system and particularly the hippocampus, regions involved in episodic memory (4144). The hypothalamus and mammillary bodies are also particularly susceptible to chronic excessive alcohol consumption, especially when it is accompanied by vitamin deficiencies as in Wernicke’s encephalopathy or Korsakoff’s syndrome.

Finally, the cerebellum is also affected in these patients, with a reduction of the white matter volume in the vermis and cerebellar hemispheres (40). A study of the connections between the cerebellum and frontal regions via the pons and thalamus also demonstrated alteration of the frontocerebellar circuit (34).

Detection and Diagnosis

In this context, the detection of cognitive impairments in alcohol-dependent patients is therefore essential and should be systematic. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) Test appears to be the most appropriate screening test for detection of cognitive impairments in these patients (45), as this tool is more sensitive than the Mini Mental State Examination (MMSE) for mild-to-moderate cognitive impairments (46). The MoCA Test can be performed by medical students, medical doctors, or certified neuropsychologist. Detection of cognitive impairments can then lead to referral of the patient for neuropsychological diagnostic assessment performed by a clinical neuropsychologist. Neuropsychological assessments can last 2–3 h and are designed to demonstrate preservation or impairment of the most susceptible cognitive functions in this population. Clinical neuropsychologists have at their disposal a battery of tests to evaluate several cognitive domains such as memory (e.g., California Verbal Learning test and Doors and People test), executive functions (e.g., Trail Making Test part B and Wisconsin Card Sorting Test, the Stroop Color Word test, and the Letter Fluency Test), working memory (Digit Span and Letter-Number Sequencing test), or processing speed (Digit Symbol-Coding).

In the alcohol-dependent population, the most susceptible executive processes to be evaluated are working memory, mental flexibility, inhibition, processing speed, concept formation, planning, and problem-solving capacities. Evaluation of verbal and visual memory must focus on encoding, recall, storage, learning, and recognition capacities, while assessment of visuospatial functions must focus on visuospatial organization and visuoconstruction capacities.

Alcohol Consumption and Implicit Cognitive Processes

The study of the implicit mechanisms involved in addictive behavior has been considerably developed over recent years. The addictive problem of alcohol-dependence results from a conflict between an urge to drink and the desire to limit alcohol intake. Dual-process models can explain this conflict by the fact that drinking behavior involves two cognitive systems: an impulsive system and a reflexive system (47). The impulsive system is a “bottom-up” system, based on implicit cognitive processes that play a role in automatic behavior via strong associative memory between alcohol-related cues, outcome, and behaviors. This system therefore reinforces the incentive effects between stimuli related to the addiction (odors, places of consumption, or advertisements related to alcohol, for example) and the addictive behavior. It mobilizes the dopaminergic system of the amygdala–striatal circuit. The reflective system is a “top-down” system based on controlled cognitive processes, in which executive functions regulate the impulsive system to ensure adapted behavior. The underlying network involves various regions of the frontal lobe (lateral inferior prefrontal, dorsolateral, ventromedial, orbitofrontal, and frontoparietal) and the striatum. Finally, the insula plays a decisive role in the articulation of these two systems by translating unconscious interoceptive signals (or somatic states) into conscious subjective experiences (desires or needs) involved in the decision-making process. This system would therefore play a conflict management role between a stimulus related to addiction and a potentially associated somatic state (for example withdrawal symptoms) in order to guide decision-making.

The interaction between the two systems has been clearly documented in the field of alcohol-dependence (48). According to this theory, drinking behavior is activated by automatic processes (impulsive system) unless the subject is able to ensure control by mobilizing executive functions (reflective system). The impairment of executive functions observed in alcohol-dependent patients would therefore predispose to drinking behavior dictated by the impulsive system. More precisely, disorders of inhibition capacities and working memory play a predominant role in this dysregulation of the impulsive system by the reflective system (28, 47, 4954), thereby resulting in a vicious circle, as chronic excessive alcohol consumption induces working memory and inhibition disorders that are then responsible for dysfunction of the reflective system. Finally, alcohol consumption results exclusively from mobilization of the impulsive system that perpetuates the addictive behavior resulting in continuing deterioration of executive functions (53).

Cognitive and Brain Recovery

The study of alcohol-dependent patients also constitutes a model of brain plasticity, as an increase of brain volume characterized by increased white matter and gray matter volumes and a reduction of the size of sulci and ventricles is observed right from the first months of abstinence (5560). The cognitive effects of this recovery consist of improvement of executive functions and verbal episodic memory (21, 58, 6065).

In parallel, it has been shown that new brain regions can be recruited by recently weaned alcohol-dependent patients to compensate for alcohol-related brain damage (66, 67). Neuroadaptation mechanisms therefore enable patients to maintain a similar level of performance on cognitive tasks to that of control subjects. For example, alcohol-dependent patients recruit neuronal networks parallel to the frontocerebellar circuit normally used by control subjects to perform executive tasks (68).

However, although abstinence allows an improvement of cognitive functions, this is only achieved after a period of several months. A recent meta-analysis showed that, despite studies showing early cognitive recovery, a global deficit was still present several months after installation of abstinence and the cognitive profile tended to become normal only after 1 year of abstinence, while certain residual cognitive impairments may persist. For example, the presence of visuospatial function deficits may be observed after several years of abstinence, related to the decreased volume of the right parietal cortex (35).

Cognitive Impairments and Management of Alcohol Abuse

Appropriate management of alcohol withdrawal is mandatory to prevent severe complications like delirium tremens or epileptic seizure. Prevention of Wernicke’s encephalopathy relies on thiamine prescription. If benzodiazepines usage and appropriate rehydration are codified, the dose and duration of thiamine treatment remains unclear (69). Nevertheless long-term abstinence is the main goal and cognitive behavioral therapy (CBT) and psychosocial programs are necessary. Adjuvant treatments may include: glutamate antagonist (acamprosate) or opioid antagonist (naltrexone).

The presence of cognitive impairments therefore requires adaptation of the management of alcohol-dependent patients. CBT has been demonstrated to be effective in the management of alcohol-dependence (70), but it is somewhat paradoxical to propose management that directly involves cerebral structures and cognitive functions altered by chronic alcohol consumption. This management approach may therefore be inappropriate or at least insufficient for a certain number of patients.

The efficacy of CBT would therefore depend on the integrity of certain brain regions of interest. For example, it has been shown in schizophrenic patients that the volume of gray matter in the frontal, temporal (including hippocampus), parietal, and cerebellar regions, brain regions that are also damaged in alcohol-dependent patients, is predictive of the efficacy of management (71). Similarly, the integrity of the frontocerebellar network, a site of predilection for brain damage in alcohol-dependent patients, would play an essential role in the efficacy of CBT due to its role in executive functioning (72).

Cognitive behavioral therapy in addiction medicine also requires elaborate cognitive capacities such as episodic, semantic and procedural memories, and executive functions (23, 7377). This type of management could therefore be unsuitable for patients with cognitive impairments (25, 7779). Various studies have shown that alcohol-dependent patients with the most severe cognitive impairment also have the least favorable prognosis (8082).

Cognitive impairment can also influence the expression of individual and environmental factors involved in management, such as self-efficacy, readiness to change, active participation in group therapy, or treatment compliance, as the initial cognitive impairment is predictive of poorer treatment compliance and a decreased self-efficacy. Patients with severe cognitive impairments are also less able to use their own resources during management, in which case the prognosis depends more on the role of external factors such as group therapy or the family support network (83).

Finally, Le Berre et al. (80) demonstrated the role of cognitive functions in the motivation process of patients to change their addictive behavior. In their study, the authors used the motivational model described by Prochaska and DiClemente (84), which defines three stages of change as the key to the patient’s commitment to the management process: the precontemplation stage (the subject has no intention to change his/her behavior), the contemplation stage (the subject considers changing his/her behavior but remains ambivalent), and the action stage (cessation of consumption and setting-up of strategies to change behavior). Based on this model, the authors showed that episodic memory plays a role in the subject’s awareness of the addictive behavior and the need for follow-up. The integrity of this function actually determines the subject’s passage from the precontemplation stage to the contemplation stage. Similarly, the integrity of executive functions enables patients to weigh up their decisions to reach the action stage, which can only be implemented when decision-making capacities are preserved. Cognitive impairment therefore influences the degree of motivation of alcohol-dependent patients, an essential prerequisite to the success of management.

The presence of cognitive impairments on admission and during the first months of abstinence therefore influences management at various levels by determining the efficacy of treatment and the prognosis for lasting abstinence. Addiction medicine management must therefore be adapted to alcohol-dependent patients with cognitive impairments.

Cognitive Remediation Therapy

In the light of these findings, it appears essential to propose management based on programs ranging from cognitive remediation to optimal use of the remaining capacities. However, very few addiction medicine units propose cognitive remediation therapy and very few studies have investigated this problem in alcohol-dependent patients. The majority of studies in the field are now relatively old (77, 79) and no longer correspond to current methodological requirements. However, the results of studies conducted in this field are encouraging. A recent study demonstrated that a cognitive remediation program was able to improve divided attention, alert capacities, working memory, and episodic memory. In addition to cognitive improvement, cognitive remediation therapy also improves other non-cognitive domains, especially psychological aspects (well-being, self-esteem) and craving (85).

Studies of the implicit mechanisms involved in addictive behavior also constitute a field of predilection for cognitive remediation. A series of such studies has shown that training working memory and inhibition can lessen the impact of these implicit process on drinking behavior (49, 51, 52, 86, 87). However, the methodology of these studies has been the subject of criticism (87) and the observed modification of drinking behavior also does not appear to be related to improvement of inhibition capacities but rather to the formation of new implicit associations promoting an impulsive drinking avoidance response (50). Finally, only one of these studies has proposed a cognitive remediation therapy that can be transposed to clinical practice (52) and that complies with the clinical criteria defined in the field of cognitive remediation (88). The results of these studies therefore need to be confirmed by further study protocols satisfying the methodological requirements in the field of cognitive remediation therapy in neuropsychologia.

Conclusion

Data published in the literature suggest that it is essential to take into account the cognitive dimension of alcohol-dependent patients in order to adapt their treatment and to palliate their difficulties in activities of daily living. The brain changes and the profile of cognitive impairments presented by patients with chronic excessive alcohol consumption have now been very extensively documented in the literature. The role of these changes on drinking behavior, especially via the cognitive processes involved in the mechanisms of addiction, also constitutes a rapidly growing new field of research. Finally, the impact of these impairments on the modalities and efficacy of the proposed management is a clinical problem systematically raised in research. The last domain to be developed in the field of management of alcohol-dependent patients is therefore that of cognitive remediation, which can establish the link between the various problems related to cognitive deficits in the clinical management of these patients in order to propose specific targeted follow-up in a remediation therapy program devoted to these impairments.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Laramée P, Kusel J, Leonard S, Aubin H-J, François C, Daeppen J-B. The economic burden of alcohol dependence in Europe. Alcohol Alcohol (2013) 48(3):259–69. doi:10.1093/alcalc/agt004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

2. Rehm J. The risks associated with alcohol use and alcoholism. Alcohol Res Health (2011) 34(2):135–43.

3. Whiteford HA, Degenhardt L, Rehm J, Baxter AJ, Ferrari AJ, Erskine HE, et al. Global burden of disease attributable to mental and substance use disorders: findings from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2010. Lancet (2013) 382(9904):1575–86. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(13)61611-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

4. Bernstein KT, Galea S, Ahern J, Tracy M, Vlahov D. The built environment and alcohol consumption in urban neighborhoods. Drug Alcohol Depend (2007) 91(2–3):244–52. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.06.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

5. Galvan FH, Caetano R. Alcohol use and related problems among ethnic minorities in the United States. Alcohol Res Health (2003) 27(1):87–94.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

6. Bryden A, Roberts B, Petticrew M, McKee M. A systematic review of the influence of community level social factors on alcohol use. Health Place (2013) 21:70–85. doi:10.1016/j.healthplace.2013.01.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

7. Dawson DA, Grant BF, Stinson FS, Chou PS. Psychopathology associated with drinking and alcohol use disorders in the college and general adult populations. Drug Alcohol Depend (2005) 77(2):139–50. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.07.012

CrossRef Full Text

8. Grant BF, Hasin DS, Stinson FS, Dawson DA, June Ruan W, Goldstein RB, et al. Prevalence, correlates, co-morbidity, and comparative disability of DSM-IV generalized anxiety disorder in the USA: results from the National Epidemiologic Survey on Alcohol and Related Conditions. Psychol Med (2005) 35(12):1747–59. doi:10.1017/S0033291705006069

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

9. Enoch M-A. The role of early life stress as a predictor for alcohol and drug dependence. Psychopharmacology (2011) 214(1):17–31. doi:10.1007/s00213-010-1916-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

10. Jentsch JD, Ashenhurst JR, Cervantes MC, Groman SM, James AS, Pennington ZT. Dissecting impulsivity and its relationships to drug addictions. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2014). doi:10.1111/nyas.12388

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

11. Wetherill L, Agrawal A, Kapoor M, Bertelsen S, Bierut LJ, Brooks A, et al. Association of substance dependence phenotypes in the COGA sample. Addict Biol (2014). doi:10.1111/adb.12153

CrossRef Full Text

12. Schuckit MA. An overview of genetic influences in alcoholism. J Subst Abuse Treat (2009) 36(1):S5–14.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

13. Kimura M, Higuchi S. Genetics of alcohol dependence. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci (2011) 65(3):213–25. doi:10.1111/j.1440-1819.2011.02190.x

CrossRef Full Text

14. Knopik VS, Heath AC, Madden PAF, Bucholz KK, Slutske WS, Nelson EC, et al. Genetic effects on alcohol dependence risk: re-evaluating the importance of psychiatric and other heritable risk factors. Psychol Med (2004) 34(8):1519–30. doi:10.1017/S0033291704002922

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

15. Gierski F, Hubsch B, Stefaniak N, Benzerouk F, Cuervo-Lombard C, Bera-Potelle C, et al. Executive functions in adult offspring of alcohol-dependent probands: toward a cognitive endophenotype? Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2013) 37(Suppl 1):E356–63. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2012.01903.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

16. Gonzales RA, Job MO, Doyon WM. The role of mesolimbic dopamine in the development and maintenance of ethanol reinforcement. Pharmacol Ther (2004) 103(2):121–46. doi:10.1016/j.pharmthera.2004.06.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

17. Pierce RC, Kumaresan V. The mesolimbic dopamine system: the final common pathway for the reinforcing effect of drugs of abuse? Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2006) 30(2):215–38. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2005.04.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

18. Söderpalm B, Ericson M. Neurocircuitry involved in the development of alcohol addiction: the dopamine system and its access points. Curr Top Behav Neurosci (2013) 13:127–61. doi:10.1007/7854_2011_170

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

19. Fitzhugh LC, Fitzhugh KB, Reitan RM. Adaptive abilities and intellectual functioning in hospitalized alcoholics. Q J Stud Alcohol (1960) 21:414–23.

20. Fein G, Bachman L, Fisher S, Davenport L. Cognitive impairments in abstinent alcoholics. West J Med (1990) 152:531–7.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

21. Bates ME, Voelbel GT, Buckman JF, Labouvie EW, Barry D. Short-term neuropsychological recovery in clients with substance use disorders. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2005) 29:367–77. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000156131.88125.2A

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

22. Chanraud S, Martelli C, Delain F, Kostogianni N, Douaud G, Aubin H-J, et al. Brain morphometry and cognitive performance in detoxified alcohol-dependents with preserved psychosocial functioning. Neuropsychopharmacology (2007) 32:429–38. doi:10.1038/sj.npp.1301219

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

23. Ihara H, Berrios GE, London M. Group and case study of the dysexecutive syndrome in alcoholism without amnesia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2000) 68:731–7. doi:10.1136/jnnp.68.6.731

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

24. Noël X, Paternot J, Van der Linden M, Sferrazza R, Verhas M, Hanak C, et al. Correlation between inhibition, working memory and delimited frontal area blood flow measure by 99mTc-Bicisate SPECT in alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol Alcohol (2001) 36:556–63. doi:10.1093/alcalc/36.6.556

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

25. Pitel AL, Beaunieux H, Witkowski T, Vabret F, Guillery-Girard B, Quinette P, et al. Genuine episodic memory deficits and executive dysfunctions in alcoholic subjects early in abstinence. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2007) 31:1169–78. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2007.00418.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

26. Tedstone D, Coyle K. Cognitive impairments in sober alcoholics: performance on selective and divided attention tasks. Drug Alcohol Depend (2004) 75:277–86. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.03.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

27. Le Berre A-P, Rauchs G, La Joie R, Mézenge F, Boudehent C, Vabret F, et al. Impaired decision-making and brain shrinkage in alcoholism. Eur Psychiatry (2012) 29:125–33. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2012.10.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

28. Noël X, Van der Linden M, d’ Acremont M, Bechara A, Dan B, Hanak C, et al. Alcohol cues increase cognitive impulsivity in individuals with alcoholism. Psychopharmacology (2007) 192:291–8. doi:10.1007/s00213-006-0695-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

29. Noël X, Van der Linden M, Schmidt N, Sferrazza R, Hanak C, Le Bon O, et al. Supervisory attentional system in nonamnesic alcoholic men. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2001) 58:1152–8. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.58.12.1152

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

30. Noël X, Van der Linden M, Brevers D, Campanella S, Hanak C, Kornreich C, et al. The contribution of executive functions deficits to impaired episodic memory in individuals with alcoholism. Psychiatry Res (2012) 198:116–22. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2011.10.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

31. Oscar-Berman M, Marinkovic K. Alcohol: effects on neurobehavioral functions and the brain. Neuropsychol Rev (2007) 17:239–57. doi:10.1007/s11065-007-9038-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

32. Ratti MT, Bo P, Giardini A, Soragna D. Chronic alcoholism and the frontal lobe: which executive functions are imparied? Acta Neurol Scand (2002) 105:276–81. doi:10.1034/j.1600-0404.2002.0o315.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

33. Uekermann J, Daum I, Schlebusch P, Wiebel B, Trenckmann U. Depression and cognitive functioning in alcoholism. Addiction (2003) 98:1521–9. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2003.00526.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

34. Sullivan EV. Compromised pontocerebellar and cerebellothalamocortical systems: speculations on their contributions to cognitive and motor impairment in nonamnesic alcoholism. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2003) 27:1409–19. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000085586.91726.46

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

35. Stavro K, Pelletier J, Potvin S. Widespread and sustained cognitive deficits in alcoholism: a meta-analysis. Addict Biol (2013) 18:203–13. doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2011.00418.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

36. Ron MA. The alcoholic brain: CT scan and psychological findings. Psychol Med Monogr Suppl (1983) 3:1–33. doi:10.1017/S0264180100000345

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

37. Moselhy HF, Georgiou G, Kahn A. Frontal lobe changes in alcoholism: a review of the literature. Alcohol Alcohol (2001) 36:357–68. doi:10.1093/alcalc/36.5.357

CrossRef Full Text

38. Kril JJ, Halliday GM, Svoboda MD, Cartwright H. The cerebral cortex is damaged in chronic alcoholics. Neuroscience (1997) 79:983–98. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(97)00083-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

39. Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV, Mathalon DH, Lim KO. Frontal lobe volume loss observed with magnetic resonance imaging in older chronic alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (1997) 21:521–9. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.1997.tb03798.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

40. Oscar-Berman M, Marinkovic K. Alcoholism and the brain: an overview. Alcohol Res Health (2003) 27:125–33.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

41. Agartz I, Momenan R, Rawlings RR, Kerich MJ, Hommer DW. Hippocampal volume in patients with alcohol dependence. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1999) 56:356–63. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.56.4.356

CrossRef Full Text

42. Beresford TP, Arciniegas DB, Alfers J, Clapp L, Martin B, Du Y, et al. Hippocampus volume loss due to chronic heavy drinking. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2006) 30:1866–70. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00223.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

43. Bleich S, Sperling W, Degner D, Graesel E, Bleich K, Wilhelm J, et al. Lack of association between hippocampal volume reduction and first-onset alcohol withdrawal seizure. A volumetric MRI study. Alcohol Alcohol (2003) 38:40–4. doi:10.1093/alcalc/agg017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

44. Sullivan EV, Marsh L, Mathalon DH, Lim KO, Pfefferbaum A. Anterior hippocampal volume deficits in nonamnesic, aging chronic alcoholics. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (1995) 19:110–22. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.1995.tb01478.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

45. Copersino ML, Fals-Stewart W, Fitzmaurice G, Schretlen DJ, Sokoloff J, Weiss RD. Rapid cognitive screening of patients with substance use disorders. Exp Clin Psychopharmacol (2009) 17:337–44. doi:10.1037/a0017260

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

46. Popovic IM, Seric V, Demarin V. Mild cognitive impairment in symptomatic and asymptomatic cerebrovascular disease. J Neurol Sci (2007) 257:185–93. doi:10.1016/j.jns.2007.01.029

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

47. Noël X, Brevers D, Bechara A. A neurocognitive approach to understanding the neurobiology of addiction. Curr Opin Neurobiol (2013) 23:632–8. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2013.01.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

48. Bechara A, Noël X, Crone EA. Loss of willpower: abnormal neural mechanism of impulse control and decision making in addiction. In: Weirs RW, Stacy AW, editors. Handbook of Implicit Cognition and Addiction. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publishers (2006). p. 215–32.

49. Thush C, Wiers RW, Ames SL, Grenard JL, Sussman S, Stacy AW. Interactions between implicit and explicit cognition and working memory capacity in the prediction of alcohol use in at-risk adolescents. Drug Alcohol Depend (2008) 94:116–24. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2007.10.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

50. Houben K, Havermans RC, Nederkoorn C, Jansen A. Beer à no-go: learning to stop responding to alcohol cues reduces alcohol intake via reduced affective associations rather than increased response inhibition. Addiction (2012) 107:1280–7. doi:10.1111/j.1360-0443.2012.03827.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

51. Houben K, Nederkoorn C, Wiers RW, Jansen A. Resisting temptation: decreasing alcohol-related affect and drinking behavior by training response inhibition. Drug Alcohol Depend (2011) 116:132–6. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2010.12.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

52. Houben K, Wiers RW, Jansen A. Getting a grip on drinking behavior: training working memory to reduce alcohol abuse. Psychol Sci (2011) 22:968–75. doi:10.1177/0956797611412392

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

53. Houben K, Wiers RW. Implicitly positive about alcohol? Implicit positive associations predict drinking behavior. Addict Behav (2008) 33:979–86. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2008.03.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

54. Jajodia A, Earleywine M. Measuring alcohol expectancies with the implicit association test. Psychol Addict Behav (2003) 17:126–33. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.17.2.126

CrossRef Full Text

55. Gazdzinski S, Durazzo TC, Meyerhoff DJ. Temporal dynamics and determinants of whole brain tissue volume changes during recovery from alcohol dependence. Drug Alcohol Depend (2005) 78:263–73. doi:10.1016/j.drugalcdep.2004.11.004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

56. Gazdzinski S, Durazzo TC, Mon A, Yeh P-H, Meyerhoff DJ. Cerebral white matter recovery in abstinent alcoholics – a multimodality magnetic resonance study. Brain (2010) 133:1043–53. doi:10.1093/brain/awp343

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

57. Monnig MA, Tonigan JS, Yeo RA, Thoma RJ, McCrady BS. White matter volume in alcohol use disorders: a meta-analysis. Addict Biol (2013) 18:581–92. doi:10.1111/j.1369-1600.2012.00441.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

58. Bendszus M, Weijers HG, Wiesbeck G, Warmuth-Metz M, Bartsch AJ, Engels S, et al. Sequential MR imaging and proton MR spectroscopy in patients who underwent recent detoxification for chronic alcoholism: correlation with clinical and neuropsychological data. AJNR Am J Neuroradiol (2001) 22:1926–32.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

59. Agartz I, Brag S, Franck J, Hammarberg A, Okugawa G, Svinhufvud K, et al. MR volumetry during acute alcohol withdrawal and abstinence: a descriptive study. Alcohol Alcohol (2003) 38:71–8. doi:10.1093/alcalc/agg020

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

60. Bartsch AJ, Homola G, Biller A, Smith SM, Weijers H-G, Wiesbeck GA, et al. Manifestations of early brain recovery associated with abstinence from alcoholism. Brain (2007) 130:36–47. doi:10.1093/brain/awl303

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

61. Fein G, Torres J, Price LJ, Di Sclafani V. Cognitive performance in long-term abstinent alcoholic individuals. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2006) 30:1538–44. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2006.00185.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

62. Pitel AL, Rivier J, Beaunieux H, Vabret F, Desgranges B, Eustache F. Changes in the episodic memory and executive functions of abstinent and relapsed alcoholics over a 6-month period. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2009) 33:490–8. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00859.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

63. Mann K, Günther A, Stetter F, Ackermann K. Rapid recovery from cognitive deficits in abstinent alcoholics: a controlled test-retest study. Alcohol Alcohol (1999) 34:567–74. doi:10.1093/alcalc/34.4.567

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

64. Manning V, Wanigaratne S, Best D, Hill RG, Reed LJ, Ball D, et al. Changes in neuropsychological functioning during alcohol detoxification. Eur Addict Res (2008) 14:226–33. doi:10.1159/000156479

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

65. Sullivan EV, Rosenbloom MJ, Lim KO, Pfefferbaum A. Longitudinal changes in cognition, gait, and balance in abstinent and relapsed alcoholic men: relationships to changes in brain structure. Neuropsychology (2000) 14:178–88. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.14.2.178

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

66. Fama R, Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV. Perceptual learning in detoxified alcoholic men: contributions from explicit memory, executive function, and age. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2004) 28:1657–65. doi:10.1097/01.ALC.0000145690.48510.DA

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

67. Marinkovic K, Oscar-Berman M, Urban T, O’Reilly CE, Howard JA, Sawyer K, et al. Alcoholism and dampened temporal limbic activation to emotional faces. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2009) 33:1880–92. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2009.01026.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

68. Chanraud S, Pitel A-L, Müller-Oehring EM, Pfefferbaum A, Sullivan EV. Remapping the brain to compensate for impairment in recovering alcoholics. Cereb Cortex (2013) 23:97–104. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhr381

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

69. Day E, Bentham PW, Callaghan R, Kuruvilla T, George S. Thiamine for prevention and treatment of Wernicke-Korsakoff Syndrome in people who abuse alcohol. Cochrane Database Syst Rev (2013) 7:CD004033. doi:10.1002/14651858.CD004033.pub3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

70. Morgenstern J, Longabaugh R. Cognitive-behavioral treatment for alcohol dependence: a review of evidence for its hypothesized mechanisms of action. Addiction (2000) 95:1475–90. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.951014753.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

71. Premkumar P, Fannon D, Kuipers E, Peters ER, Anilkumar APP, Simmons A, et al. Structural magnetic resonance imaging predictors of responsiveness to cognitive behaviour therapy in psychosis. Schizophr Res (2009) 115:146–55. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2009.08.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

72. Kumari V, Peters ER, Fannon D, Antonova E, Premkumar P, Anilkumar AP, et al. Dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity predicts responsiveness to cognitive-behavioral therapy in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 66:594–602. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2009.04.036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

73. Goldapple K, Segal Z, Garson C, Lau M, Bieling P, Kennedy S, et al. Modulation of cortical-limbic pathways in major depression: treatment-specific effects of cognitive behavior therapy. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2004) 61:34–41. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.1.34

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

74. Hunt SA, Baker AL, Michie PT, Kavanagh DJ. Neurocognitive profiles of people with comorbid depression and alcohol use: implications for psychological interventions. Addict Behav (2009) 34:878–86. doi:10.1016/j.addbeh.2009.03.036

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

75. Mohlman J, Gorman JM. The role of executive functioning in CBT: a pilot study with anxious older adults. Behav Res Ther (2005) 43:447–65. doi:10.1016/j.brat.2004.03.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

76. Granholm E, McQuaid JR, Link PC, Fish S, Patterson T, Jeste DV. Neuropsychological predictors of functional outcome in Cognitive Behavioral Social Skills Training for older people with schizophrenia. Schizophr Res (2008) 100:133–43. doi:10.1016/j.schres.2007.11.032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

77. Allen DN, Goldstein G, Seaton BE. Cognitive rehabilitation of chronic alcohol abusers. Neuropsychol Rev (1997) 7:21–39. doi:10.1007/BF02876971

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

78. Goldstein G, Haas GL, Shemansky WJ, Barnett B, Salmon-Cox S. Rehabilitation during alcohol detoxication in comorbid neuropsychiatric patients. J Rehabil Res Dev (2005) 42:225–34. doi:10.1682/JRRD.2004.03.0040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

79. Goldman MS. Experience-dependent neuropsychological recovery and the treatment of chronic alcoholism. Neuropsychol Rev (1990) 1:75–101. doi:10.1007/BF01108859

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

80. Le Berre A-P, Rauchs G, La Joie R, Segobin S, Mézenge F, Boudehent C, et al. Readiness to change and brain damage in patients with chronic alcoholism. Psychiatry Res (2013) 213:202–9. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2013.03.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

81. Allsop S, Saunders B, Phillips M. The process of relapse in severely dependent male problem drinkers. Addiction (2000) 95:95–106. doi:10.1046/j.1360-0443.2000.9519510.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

82. Noël X, Sferrazza R, Van Der Linden M, Paternot J, Verhas M, Hanak C, et al. Contribution of frontal cerebral blood flow measured by (99m)Tc-Bicisate spect and executive function deficits to predicting treatment outcome in alcohol-dependent patients. Alcohol Alcohol (2002) 37:347–54. doi:10.1093/alcalc/37.4.347

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

83. Bates ME, Buckman JF, Nguyen TT. A role for cognitive rehabilitation in increasing the effectiveness of treatment for alcohol use disorders. Neuropsychol Rev (2013) 23:27–47. doi:10.1007/s11065-013-9228-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

84. Prochaska JO, DiClemente CC. Stages and processes of self-change of smoking: toward an integrative model of change. J Consult Clin Psychol (1983) 51:390–5. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.51.3.390

CrossRef Full Text

85. Rupp CI, Kemmler G, Kurz M, Hinterhuber H, Fleischhacker WW. Cognitive remediation therapy during treatment for alcohol dependence. J Stud Alcohol Drugs (2012) 73:625–34.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

86. Grenard JL, Ames SL, Wiers RW, Thush C, Sussman S, Stacy AW. Working memory capacity moderates the predictive effects of drug-related associations on substance use. Psychol Addict Behav (2008) 22:426–32. doi:10.1037/0893-164X.22.3.426

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

87. Houben K, Wiers RW. Response inhibition moderates the relationship between implicit associations and drinking behavior. Alcohol Clin Exp Res (2009) 33:626–33. doi:10.1111/j.1530-0277.2008.00877.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

88. Seron X, Van der Linden M. Objectifs et stratégies de la revalidation neuropsychologique. In: Seron X, Van der Linden M, editors. Traité de Neuropsychologie Clinique: Tome 2. Marseille: Solal (2000). p. 9–16.

Keywords: alcohol-dependence, brain atrophy, cognitive impairments, inhibition, working memory, implicit cognition, cognitive remediation therapy

Citation: Bernardin F, Maheut-Bosser A and Paille F (2014) Cognitive impairments in alcohol-dependent subjects. Front. Psychiatry 5:78. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00078

Received: 27 April 2014; Accepted: 23 June 2014;
Published online: 16 July 2014.

Edited by:

Michel Lejoyeux, Université Paris Diderot, France

Reviewed by:

Antonio Mirijello, Catholic University of Rome, Italy
Liangsuo Ma, University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, USA

Copyright: © 2014 Bernardin, Maheut-Bosser and Paille. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: François Paille, Service d’Addictologie, CHU Nancy, Bâtiment Philippe Canton, Hôpitaux de Brabois, Vandoeuvre F-54550, France e-mail: secretariat.pr.paille@chu-nancy.fr

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.