Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Psychiatry, 04 April 2014
Sec. Mood Disorders

Posttraumatic stress disorder: a theoretical model of the hyperarousal subtype

  • Independent Researcher, Marlborough, UK

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a frequent and distressing mental disorder, about which much remains to be learned. It is a heterogeneous disorder; the hyperarousal subtype (about 70% of occurrences and simply termed PTSD in this paper) is the topic of this article, but the dissociative subtype (about 30% of occurrences and likely involving quite different brain mechanisms) is outside its scope. A theoretical model is presented that integrates neuroscience data on diverse brain regions known to be involved in PTSD, and extensive psychiatric findings on the disorder. Specifically, the amygdala is a multifunctional brain region that is crucial to PTSD, and processes peritraumatic hyperarousal on grounded cognition principles to produce hyperarousal symptoms. Amygdala activity also modulates hippocampal function, which is supported by a large body of evidence, and likewise amygdala activity modulates several brainstem regions, visual cortex, rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), and medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC), to produce diverse startle, visual, memory, numbing, anger, and recklessness symptoms. Additional brain regions process other aspects of peritraumatic responses to produce further symptoms. These contentions are supported by neuroimaging, neuropsychological, neuroanatomical, physiological, cognitive, and behavioral evidence. Collectively, the model offers an account of how responses at the time of trauma are transformed into an extensive array of the 20 PTSD symptoms that are specified in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Fifth edition. It elucidates the neural mechanisms of a specific form of psychopathology, and accords with the Research Domain Criteria framework.

Introduction

Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) is a frequent mental disorder, with a lifetime prevalence in the US general population estimated at 6.8–7.8% (1, 2). In regions of civil disorder or armed conflict, rates are substantially higher (3, 4). PTSD is disabling, distressing, and commonly persistent (2, 5), and leads to serious impairment of economic and social functioning (69), as well as increased mortality from multiple causes (10). Although some interventions can be very effective, a substantial proportion of PTSD sufferers achieve limited improvement (1113), so new and theory-based treatment options require early development. More generally, theoretical progress on this disorder may illuminate the mechanisms of anxiety disorders, the most prevalent class of psychological disorders, with a lifetime prevalence of 28.8% in the US general population (1).

The character of PTSD is well illustrated by the following vignette that epitomizes the condition of severely traumatized Vietnam veterans decades after active combat in Vietnam:

I can’t get the memories out of my mind! The images come flooding back in vivid detail, triggered by the most inconsequential things, like a door slamming or the smell of stir-fried pork. Last night, I went to bed, was having a good sleep for a change. Then in the early morning a storm front passed through and there was a bolt of crackling thunder. I awoke instantly, frozen in fear. I am right back in Vietnam, in the middle of the monsoon season at my guard post. I am sure I’ll get hit in the next volley and convinced I will die. My hands are freezing, yet sweat pours from my entire body. I feel each hair on the back of my neck standing on end. I can’t catch my breath and my heart is pounding. I smell a damp sulphur smell. Suddenly I see what’s left of my buddy Troy, his head on a bamboo platter, sent back to our camp by the Viet Cong. Propaganda messages are stuffed between his clenched teeth. The next bolt of lightning and clap of thunder makes me jump so much that I fall to the floor….” [(14), p. 470].

The Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, fifth edition (DSM-5) (15) and its predecessors have set out criteria for PTSD for over 30 years. Extensive research has generated a valuable body of findings, but current hypotheses of PTSD have yet to achieve a comprehensive account of the accumulated findings. Recent reviews [cf. Ref. (1618)] explain the mechanisms of rather few of the 20 DSM-5 PTSD symptoms. Moreover, fear conditioning mediated by the amygdala dominates current PTSD theorizing, and is central to major theoretical models (1922). Nevertheless, fear is not essential to the pathogenesis of PTSD, because there is good evidence that PTSD can develop in the absence of fear at the time of trauma [i.e., peritraumatic fear; Ref. (2327)]. Together, it is suggested that a broader view of peritraumatic responses, and different theoretical perspectives, should enrich and progress understanding of PTSD.

A theoretical model is presented that elaborates how peritraumatic responses are transformed into an extensive array of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms, to manifest as the hyperarousal subtype of PTSD. In brief, the model elucidates how peritraumatic responses, particularly those participating in hyperarousal, are processed by the amygdala, which subsequently replicates those responses on later encounters with trauma-related stimuli, to produce hyperarousal symptoms. This perspective is consistent with grounded cognition, which contends that brain regions that are active during an event, are reactivated when that event is recalled, and is known to operate in diverse brain regions (2832). Hyperarousal further drives the other symptom clusters (33, 34), and correspondingly, the amygdala modulates the activity of diverse brain regions known to be involved in PTSD. For example, there is an abundant literature that arousal-enhanced amygdala activity influences hippocampal function, and thus episodic memory performance [see for reviews, Ref. (35, 36) and summary later]. Similar amygdala processes are suggested to modulate diverse other brain regions involved in PTSD, namely, brainstem, visual cortex, rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), and medial orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC). The result is a theoretical model, summarized in Figure 2, which integrates neuroscientific data on multiple brain regions, and extensive psychiatric findings on PTSD, to elucidate the etiology, symptomatology, and neurocircuitry of the hyperarousal subtype of PTSD, and to suggest new directions for theoretical, prevention, and intervention research.

Major Features of PTSD

Posttraumatic stress disorder comprises four symptom clusters according to DSM-5, namely, hyperarousal, persistent re-experiencing of the trauma, avoidance of trauma-related stimuli, and negative alterations in cognitions and mood. Empirical findings have further characterized PTSD symptomatology, as summarized next. PTSD is a heterogeneous disorder, and there are several subtypes of it. One is characterized by hyperarousal, and a second by dissociation, numbness, and physiological unresponsiveness, accounting for approximately 70 and 30% of occurrences, respectively (3739). Quite different brain mechanisms are likely involved (3739), and only those relating to the former, hyperarousal subtype are considered, which is simply termed PTSD in this article. That is, the theoretical model elucidates the neural mechanisms of a specific form of psychopathology, and accords with the Research Domain Criteria (RDoC) framework (40, 41). Such PTSD represents the extreme of a continuum of stress reactions rather than a discrete syndrome (4244). The etiology of PTSD is multifactorial (45, 46), and hyperarousal is of particular importance as detailed later. In contrast, peritraumatic intense fear commonly contributes to symptomatology, but is not a crucial factor as it is not experienced by a substantial minority of those who subsequently develop PTSD (2325, 27). Pain, injury, and dysphoric emotions besides fear (e.g., shame, guilt, sadness, and anger) are further factors that can substantially contribute to PTSD development (24, 25, 4750).

Peritraumatic hyperarousal is supported by the majority of behavioral and physiological findings (5156). Reactivation of hyperarousal and other symptoms is elicited by specific trauma-related stimuli or reminders of them, even in conditions of no current danger (7, 18, 21, 5759). Nevertheless, no significant differences between PTSD patients and controls are generally reported for physiological responses to control scripts, and to standard physical and mental challenges (5861). Resting levels of physiological variables did not differ significantly between PTSD patients and controls in several studies that measured these variables under appropriate conditions [such as at home; Ref. (60, 62)]. More generally, the meta-analysis of Pole (63) found small and significant weighted mean effect sizes for resting heart rate (r = 0.18) and skin conductance level (r = 0.08) in PTSD, but not for other individual resting physiological variables. Basal levels of cortisol do not differ significantly between PTSD patients and controls, according to the meta-analysis of Meewisse et al. (64). There is, however, significant heterogeneity, and specific subgroups differ significantly from controls (64). More broadly, hyperarousal is the predominant PTSD symptom cluster (33, 34). A longitudinal study collected data on patients’ symptom levels through face to face interviews at several days, 3 and 12 months post-trauma. The findings were that hyperarousal strongly influences the development of the other symptom clusters but is little influenced by them (34). A subsequent study of similar design but with methodological modifications to increase generalizability, replicated the earlier findings (33).

Major sequelae of PTSD are disrupted economic and social behaviors, manifested in high levels of marital and parenting problems, divorce, unemployment, isolation, homelessness, and imprisonment (69). Comorbid disorders occur at high rates in PTSD sufferers (2, 65, 66). The relationship between PTSD and comorbid disorders appears complex and bidirectional. Specifically, a history of pre-trauma psychiatric disorders is a weak risk factor for PTSD (45, 46). Conversely, the time course of onsets suggests PTSD may often cause comorbid depression, anxiety disorders, and alcohol and other substance abuse/dependence disorders (65, 66). In sum, PTSD comprises a complex symptomatology, which continues to be elaborated and revised, and which must be explained by models of PTSD.

PTSD Theoretical Model

The model proposes that arousal/hyperarousal is processed by the (multifunctional) amygdala, which does so on grounded cognition principles, and these are summarized below. Hyperarousal is rarely defined, but simply means a high-level of physiological arousal. Arousal is a common component of diverse emotions, and may also occur in their absence (6769). Dysfunctional hyperarousal is suggested to be the overriding feature of PTSD, and its independence of particular emotions is consistent with the diversity of peritraumatic emotions reported earlier. It is dysfunctional because its co-occurrence with traumatic reminders is automatic, involuntary, severe, and uncontrollable, even when there is no current danger or other challenge, as illustrated by the vignette quoted earlier, and as elucidated later. Besides processing hyperarousal symptoms, the amygdala also modulates the activity of multiple brain regions, thereby generating further DSM-5 PTSD symptoms. The hypothesis of the amygdala’s contribution to PTSD is summarized in Figure 1.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Diagram summarizing hyperarousal processing by the amygdala, and consequent array of PTSD symptoms. During traumatic interactions, the amygdala processes diverse hyperarousal inputs, producing amygdala hyperactivity. Consolidation in the amygdala generates persisting representations of hyperactivity, which participate in distributed trauma-related representations. Reminders reactivate the distributed trauma-related representations, including those mediated by the amygdala, which influence further brain regions and thereby drive multiple PTSD symptoms. E.g., amygdala projections to brainstem reactivate hyperarousal symptoms automatically and involuntarily. ▬, Heavy interconnections; ─, moderate interconnections; →, amygdala hyperactivity representations drive PTSD symptoms. Italics indicate symptoms. Abbreviations: dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; mOFC, medial orbitofrontal cortex; PHC, parahippocampal cortex; PRC, perirhinal cortex; STG, superior temporal gyrus; STS, superior temporal sulcus; VTC, ventral temporal cortex.

Grounded Cognition

Grounded cognition operates widely in the brain (as do many other processes), including it is suggested, in the amygdala. It offers an account of the transformation of peritraumatic hyperarousal into enduring PTSD hyperarousal symptoms, and is the overall process that is performed in Box 2 of Figure 1. Grounded cognition proposes that enduring knowledge representations are grounded in or based in sensory, motor, and other specialized brain systems. Sensory systems, for example, which process the perception of a feature, also mediate enduring representations of that feature, usually in higher-level subregions. That is, sensory systems are engaged in both perceptual processing and in the mediation of sensory knowledge representations. Hence, a sensory system that is active during the perception of a stimulus, is later reactivated when the corresponding memory representation is retrieved. Similar principles apply to the motor system, which is involved in both action execution processes and in the mediation of action knowledge representations. In short, enduring knowledge representations are grounded in or based in the systems that mediate perceptual, motor, and other processes, hence the term grounded cognition (2831). More broadly, a complex knowledge representation is constituted by a network of component representations distributed across multiple sensory, motor, and other specialized brain regions (2831, 70, 71), and this is also termed a distributed representation. Further, such distributed networks may be integrated by means of nodes or hubs, which also efficiently reactivate such distributed networks so as to achieve knowledge reactivation and retrieval (2831, 72).

Sensory property knowledge (e.g., object color knowledge) is represented in corresponding sensory systems (color perception system), and this is evidenced by diverse neuroimaging findings. In a neuroimaging study using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), healthy subjects were scanned while they performed a color perception task (Farnsworth–Munsell 100 hue test) and a color knowledge task (a property verification task). The findings were that partially overlapping subregions of the color processing system mediated color perception and color knowledge representation (32). Findings that likewise support overlapping of perceptual processing and of knowledge representation within a sensory modality, have been reported by a number of neuroimaging studies using various experimental designs, and covering multiple sensory modalities including visual object form, auditory, somatosensory, and gustatory modalities (7375).

Action knowledge is likewise represented in the motor system and participates in distributed knowledge representations. This is exemplified by the distributed networks that represent tool concepts. The robust findings of numerous neuroimaging studies using diverse designs are that tool concepts are represented across multiple brain regions, including ventral premotor cortex, and intraparietal sulcus in parietal cortex. These regions likely represent the typical actions performed with tools, and contribute to tool identification (76). Convergent support is provided by neuropsychological findings that motor system dysfunction is associated with impaired action knowledge. Several studies of substantial groups of patients (n = 29 and n = 90) with circumscribed brain lesions, presented diverse tests of action knowledge, and mapped lesion locations from structural MRI data. The findings were that impaired knowledge of actions was associated with lesions to motor cortical areas, and regions interconnected with them (77, 78). In summary, numerous brain systems that mediate perceptual or motor processes also mediate corresponding persisting knowledge representations, in accordance with grounded cognition. It is suggested that the amygdala implements corresponding operations, as described in the next sections.

The Amygdala, Peritraumatic Hyperarousal, and Hyperarousal Symptoms

The amygdala and its projections are crucially involved in PTSD symptomatology, and this is evidenced by a neuropsychological study. The study used Vietnam Head Injury Study (VHIS) participants who had experienced heavy combat exposure, and had been assessed for PTSD using standard methods. The findings were that of the combatants with amygdala lesions (n = 15), none developed PTSD, whereas in comparison groups of combatants with lesions to control regions or no brain lesions, 40–48% developed PTSD (79).

The model contends that the amygdala, which processes arousal (80, 81), also produces related representations on grounded cognition principles. Correspondingly, at the severe intensity levels that characterize PTSD, peritraumatic hyperarousal is transformed into enduring hyperarousal symptoms. In more detail, multiple forms of arousal and peritraumatic responses (Boxes 1a–d, Figure 1) input to the amygdala, enhancing amygdala activity (Box 2, Figure 1). Consolidation is implemented in the amygdala to produce enduring representations of amygdala hyperactivity (Box 2, Figure 1). Amygdala-mediated representations participate in and are reactivated with distributed stimulus representations (Box 3, Figure 1), and via projections to brainstem can reactivate arousal/hyperarousal (Box 4a and related symptom, Figure 1). The result in PTSD is hyperarousal that is automatic, involuntary, and severe, even in conditions of no current danger or other challenge. Arousal that is integral with challenging stimulus representations mobilizes necessary resources, so is normally adaptive [cf. Ref. (81)], but PTSD is the extreme of the stress reactions continuum (4244), so the effects are extreme and disabling, and constitute hyperarousal symptoms.

Arousal/Hyperarousal Processing in the Amygdala

The amygdala receives extensive arousal/hyperarousal inputs (Boxes 1a–d, Figure 1), which is evidenced by neuroanatomical, neuroimaging, and physiological findings. The cardiovascular and respiratory systems project via multiple pathways to the amygdala in rodent and monkey. Visceral sensory nerves of the autonomic nervous system (ANS) that innervate these organs project to spinal cord neurons, which relay information sequentially via thalamus and insula to the amygdala. A major medullary pathway has similar origins, and projects sequentially through the visceral region of the nucleus of the solitary tract (NTS) in the medulla, parabrachial complex (PBC), thalamus, and insula to multiple amygdaloid nuclei (8285). Functional activation of the human respiratory system can be elicited by such methods as adding resistive respiratory loads to an external breathing circuit, and the resulting brain activations have been neuroimaged. A common finding of such studies is that the activated brain regions include the amygdala (8689).

The hypothalamus is a further arousal-related system, which projects substantially to multiple amygdaloid nuclei, in rodent neuroanatomical studies (84). Consistent with the neuroanatomy, electrical stimulation of parts of the hypothalamus and PBC was found to evoke responses in amygdala neurons, predominantly in the central nucleus (90).

Elevated levels of stress hormones also influence amygdala activity. Cortisol binds directly to glucocorticoid receptors on the amygdala, and to glucocorticoid receptors on NTS neurons, which project to the amygdala as described above (35, 85, 91). Epinephrine binds to vagus neurons of the parasympathetic division of the ANS, which projects to the amygdala via routes described above (35, 85, 9193). Consistent with the neuroanatomy, electrical stimulation of vagus neurons in rats was found to produce substantially enhanced neurotransmitter release in the basolateral amygdala (94).

In human neuroimaging studies, enhanced levels of arousal are brought about by stimuli such as pictures or words, and these have been found to elicit enhanced amygdala activation (9598). In PTSD, the occurrence of peritraumatic hyperarousal, often assessed hours or days after trauma, is commonly supported by studies of heart rate, respiration rate, and subjective arousal (5156). Moreover, elevated heart rate and respiration rate shortly after the trauma significantly predict subsequent PTSD diagnosis or PTSD symptoms (52, 53, 55, 56). In addition, a linear relationship between severity of combat as measured by the number of firefights and prevalence of PTSD diagnosis, was found in military personnel deployed to Iraq (99). This finding was replicated in personnel deployed to Afghanistan (99). Further, elevated pre-trauma amygdala reactivity predicts a greater increase in PTSD symptoms after exposure to stressful events (100). Peritraumatic hyperarousal is likely relayed to the amygdala by the pathways described above; nevertheless, empirical verification of this in human is required.

Consolidation in the Amygdala

Consolidation is the process whereby initially transient neural activity is transformed into persisting neural representations (101103), and there is substantial evidence it occurs in the amygdala (Box 2, Figure 1). At the neural level, consolidation occurs in the period after learning events (101103), and corresponding processing occurs in the amygdala. In a lever-pressing paradigm, cats that had already learned to lever-press received on a single trial a footshock. Neurons in several amygdaloid nuclei were recorded and the findings were that their activity increased in the period after the footshock for several hours (mean 137 ± 5 min) relative to control levels (104).

Long-term potentiation (LTP) is a persistent enhancement in a neuron’s responsiveness (105), and is thought to be a major mechanism that contributes to the formation of long-term representations and memories (106). There is substantial evidence for its occurrence in the amygdala. For example, rats were trained with a classical fear conditioning design, involving a tone repeatedly paired with electric shock, and neural activity was measured in the amygdala. The findings were that learning that the tone predicted shock was paralleled by persisting enhancements in responses of amygdala neurons to the tone (107). Moreover, such enhanced responsiveness corresponds to that brought about in the same amygdala areas through the experimental induction of LTP (108). Results consistent with these mechanisms and related ones have been reported by multiple studies (109112). In addition, fear-related conditioning in rats can be severely impaired by infusing into the amygdala pharmacological agents that block LTP and other processes in the consolidation cascade (110, 113115). Moreover, destruction of parts of the amygdala well after fear conditioning training has been completed, abolishes that fear-related conditioning (116118). Collectively, the hypothesis that the amygdala implements consolidation is well supported.

Amygdala-Mediated Representations Participate in Distributed Representations

Amygdala-mediated representations participate in distributed representations, and via projections to brainstem can reactivate arousal/hyperarousal (Boxes 3, 4a, and related symptom, Figure 1). The multiple components of distributed representations are linked together by perirhinal cortex [PRC; Brodmann’s Areas 35, 36, and part of 38, which constitutes the temporal pole; Ref. (119, 120)]. The binding role of PRC is evidenced by robust findings that lesions to PRC produce visual recognition and visual discrimination impairments for complex visual stimuli, but not for more basic ones, in monkey and human (72, 121124). PRC interconnects heavily with ventral temporal cortex (VTC), superior temporal sulcus (STS), parahippocampal cortex, and amygdala, and moderately with superior temporal gyrus (STG), insula, and OFC, according to neuroanatomical tracing studies in monkey (120, 125). Thus, amygdala–PRC interconnections, which are dense, pervasive, and reciprocal in primate and rat (83, 84, 125), enable the amygdala to contribute specialized representations to distributed representations in parallel with other regions. Moreover, neuroimaging investigations of the neural representation of diverse objects, reveal that such neural representations are constituted by the above regions, including the amygdala [see for reviews, Ref. (30, 71)].

In PTSD, a common neuroimaging paradigm involves presentation of individualized auditory scripts of highly traumatic and of neutral experiences to subjects while they are neuroimaged. Common findings are that relative to comparison groups or conditions, PTSD patients during traumatic recollections, which are likely constituted by distributed trauma-related representations [cf. Ref. (63)], exhibit enhanced activations in the amygdala, as well as other brain regions [see for reviews and meta-analyses, Ref. (1618, 21, 126)]. Further, amygdala involvement in distributed representations suggests that such amygdala activation should be automatic, and this is supported by findings from neuroimaging studies of amygdala activation during unattended processing of feared stimuli (127, 128). Thus, convergent evidence supports amygdala participation in distributed representations, including trauma-related ones.

The amygdala can reactivate arousal/hyperarousal, via its heavy projections to hypothalamus and brainstem. Specifically, the amygdala sends heavy projections to hypothalamus, NTS, and PBC, and it projects extensively to further brainstem regions (14, 84, 92, 129, 130). Convergent support is provided by findings from neuroimaging studies that PTSD symptom severity is positively correlated with amygdala activity, as measured by PET or fMRI during exposure to trauma-related or experimental stimuli (131135). Additional brain regions, specifically insula (BA 13), anterior parts of ACC (BAs 25, 32), and dorsomedial PFC (BAs 9, 10) may also contribute to components of hyperarousal (136, 137).

Several cautions concerning the above hypotheses and evidence should be noted. Enhanced peritraumatic stress hormone levels are not supported by recent studies (138, 139); nevertheless, field studies of stressed animals demonstrate enormous increases (140142). The contradictory findings may arise because of diverse methodological issues. For example, human stress hormone measurements were typically taken several hours after trauma, whereas stress hormone half-lives measured in rat blood range from 70 s to 20 min (143145). Further, stress hormone levels are strongly influenced by psychological factors [e.g., Ref. (146, 147)], so are likely to have been lowered by such factors as being rescued, knowing that medical care and other support is at hand, and so forth. As regards measures such as resting heart rate, these are typically taken hours or days after trauma, so are only coarse approximations of peritraumatic processes, and are unlikely to reflect the intensity of responses involving, for example, faintness, vomiting, and defecation, as can occur in combat (148). Medication administered during ambulance transport or in the emergency room are further issues.

There is considerable variability in neuroimaging findings. Nevertheless, such variability has been ascribed to numerous technical reasons, including low spatial and temporal resolution of scanners, limited subject numbers, lack of segregation of the several subtypes of PTSD, comorbid disorders, the amygdala’s deep location, small size and signal changes, and so forth [see for reviews, Ref. (18, 126)]. Brainstem regions probably participate in the reactivation of hyperarousal co-occurring with trauma-related stimuli, but brainstem activations are rarely reported in neuroimaging studies of PTSD, perhaps again for technical reasons. Amygdala hyperactivity could be a pre-trauma vulnerability factor which with others could substantially explain PTSD symptomatology (100, 149). Nevertheless, several meta-analyses, twin, and other studies have found that pre-trauma vulnerability factors have generally small effects, which interact with the crucial traumatic events in the generation of PTSD symptomatology and amygdala hyperactivity (45, 46, 49, 150152).

In summary, hyperarousal is the predominant symptom of PTSD (33, 34), and it is suggested to be processed by the amygdala. Specifically, peritraumatic hyperarousal is transformed into enduring hyperarousal symptoms, and this is likely mediated by a network comprising the amygdala, its inputs, and its interconnections with PRC and brainstem.

The Amygdala Modulates Diverse Brain Regions, so Driving Further PTSD Symptoms

The primate amygdala interconnects with approximately 90% of cerebral cortical areas (153), and numerous subcortical areas (92, 130). It is suggested amygdala hyperactivity modulates the activity of diverse interconnected brain regions, and thereby drives the development of further DSM-5 PTSD symptoms, which are generally markedly different in nature from hyperarousal. This is summarized in Boxes 4a–e and related symptoms, Figure 1.

Exaggerated Startle, Sleep Disturbance, and Modulation of Further Brainstem Regions

There is abundant evidence that the amygdala crucially participates in the enhancement of startle [see for reviews, Ref. (14, 154)], so amygdala hyperactivity likely produces the exaggerated startle symptom of PTSD (Box 4a and related symptom, Figure 1). Startle is elicited by a sudden unexpected stimulus, such as a noise, and involves involuntary reactions by muscular, hormonal, and visceral systems (105). In laboratory paradigms, heightened startle responses are reliably elicited by presentation of conditioned stimuli (e.g., a light formerly paired with electric shocks) and an unexpected sudden stimulus (e.g., a noise burst); together these constitute the fear-potentiated startle paradigm (14, 154). The network underlying such heightened startle responses involves a major modulatory role for the amygdala and its brainstem projections (14, 154). This claim is evidenced by findings that in rodents trained and tested in the fear-potentiated startle paradigm, lesions of the amygdala consistently abolish the heightened fear-potentiated startle response (14, 154). Conversely, low-level electrical stimulation applied to the rodent amygdala heightens the startle response, but when applied to adjacent brain regions, does not do so (14). Moreover, such amygdala stimulation enhances activation of the brainstem region that implements the startle response (14, 154). Thus, the amygdala and its brainstem projections can enhance startle responses. It is suggested that amygdala hyperactivity in PTSD and the consequent brainstem activation, likely produce the exaggerated startle symptom of PTSD.

The amygdala likely participates also in the sleep disturbance symptom of PTSD [(155); Box 4a and related symptom, Figure 1]. The networks that regulate wakefulness and sleep substantially overlap ANS regions that modulate arousal (156). These shared regions include midbrain reticular formation, NTS, posterior hypothalamus, anterior hypothalamus, and preoptic area (156). The amygdala interconnects with components of the wakefulness and sleep regulating networks, including brainstem subregions, NTS, anterior hypothalamus, preoptic area, basal nucleus of Meynert, and OFC (84, 130, 155157). Non-rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep is a state of attenuated arousal, during which overall brain activity is reduced substantially, according to neuroimaging findings in healthy human subjects (155, 158, 159). After controlling for this overall decline, brainstem subregions that generate and maintain sleep, as well as the amygdala and other areas, are relatively activated compared to waking (155, 156, 159). NREM sleep recurs throughout the sleep period, in alternation with rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. The latter is a state of heightened activity in numerous brain regions and deactivations in others. There is increased activity relative to wakefulness and NREM sleep, in brainstem subregions, amygdala, and other regions, whereas high-level sensory and lateral prefrontal cortices are deactivated (155, 157, 158, 160).

It has been hypothesized that amygdala hyperactivity in PTSD likely drives the sleep disturbance symptom of this disorder, through modulation of components of the wakefulness and sleep networks (155). Specifically, amygdala hyperactivity likely activates brainstem subregions promoting wakefulness, and reduces activation of brainstem subregions that generate and maintain sleep (155). The amygdala also suppresses mOFC activity as evidenced in a later section. OFC importantly influences sleep; electrical stimulation of it in animals produces sleep, and neuroimaging studies of humans report increased activation during NREM and REM sleep. Furthermore, lesions of OFC in experimental animals reduce sleep (156, 157). Thus, amygdala suppression of mOFC likely further disturbs sleep. The amygdala may also modulate other components of the wakefulness and sleep networks, although further investigations are required. In sum, amygdala hyperactivity modulates brainstem subregions, mOFC, and perhaps other components of the networks that regulate wakefulness and sleep, and so may drive the sleep disturbance symptom of PTSD.

Hypervigilance, Visual Intrusions, and Flashbacks and Modulation of Visual Cortex

Posttraumatic stress disorder symptoms that are primarily visual symptoms, including visual sensory components of flashbacks, are likely mediated by a network including visual cortex, a region which is modulated by the amygdala (Box 4b and related symptoms, Figure 1). Visual perceptual processing in PTSD is enhanced for trauma-related stimuli (161). This is evidenced by findings that identification accuracy is higher for trauma-related stimuli than control stimuli, in PTSD patients but not healthy trauma-exposed comparison subjects (161). Moreover, the authors suggest that such enhancement likely contributes to intrusions and other re-experiencing PTSD symptoms. Several neuroimaging studies have examined brain activations during traumatic flashbacks or analogs of them. The findings were that visual flashbacks and their analogs consistently recruited early visual cortex; less consistent activations were reported in somatosensory cortex, insula, and motor-related areas (162164). A further neuroimaging study using fMRI examined the non-traumatic visual hallucinations that are the hallmark of Charles Bonnet syndrome patients, who are otherwise neuropsychiatrically normal, and the related brain activations. The findings were that visual hallucinations of particular stimuli (e.g., color, faces) co-occurred with activations of visual cortical subregions that are specialized for representing those stimuli (165). Convergent evidence for visual cortex involvement in PTSD is provided by a longitudinal study of brain structure changes in veterans with and without PTSD. The findings were that significantly greater atrophy of visual cortex (and other brain regions) occurred in veterans with PTSD whose symptoms were worsening, whereas such atrophy did not co-occur in those whose PTSD symptoms were improving. These findings may arise from the neurotoxic effects of overactivation (166). Thus, the findings of the limited investigations to date suggest that enhanced activation in visual cortex occurs in PTSD, and likely contributes to the visual sensory symptoms of PTSD.

The amygdala likely drives enhancements of visual cortex functioning. The amygdala projects densely to all levels of the ventral visual stream, from anterior TE to posterior V1, according to neuroanatomical tracing studies in monkey (167169). Moreover, examination of amygdaloid boutons adjacent to TE and V1 neurons, revealed that the boutons formed asymmetric synapses and predominantly onto dendritic spines. Such synapses are normally excitatory, suggesting amygdala-visual cortex projections enhance visual function (170). This is further supported by behavioral and neuroimaging studies. In an attentional blink paradigm, neutral and negative words were briefly presented at short temporal intervals, and the findings were that healthy controls processed negative words with greater accuracy than neutral words. This effect, however, was not found in patients with bilateral or left amygdala lesions (171). In an fMRI study, fearful faces relative to neutral faces elicited enhanced activation in visual cortical areas in healthy controls. Such visual cortex enhancement, however, did not occur in patients who had amygdala lesions due to sclerosis, but structurally intact visual cortex (172). Together, these findings are consistent with amygdala dependent enhancement of visual function.

Amygdala modulation of visual cortex may also have more fundamental plasticity effects. In a rat study, a training pure tone was presented on 30 trials in a single session, concurrently with electrical stimulation of the basolateral amygdala. A microelectrode array implanted in the primary auditory cortex (A1) measured plasticity changes in that system. The findings were that relative to pretraining baseline measurements, the frequency tuning of a majority of the tested A1 sites shifted toward or to match the frequency of the training tone. These changes were specific to the training tone, as they failed to occur for a tone that was not accompanied by basolateral amygdala stimulation. Such plasticity developed rapidly after a single training session, underwent consolidation processes, and was enduring as it persisted throughout the 3-week duration of testing. There was also some evidence for increased sensitivity (reduced threshold) and increased selectivity (reduced bandwidth) at these A1 sites, although these effects were less enduring (173, 174). Plasticity is ubiquitous in the brain, so corresponding plasticity likely occurs in human visual cortex.

Collectively, multiple findings suggest that trauma-specific hyperactivity of the amygdala in PTSD likely drives enhancement of visual perceptual processing, as well as specialization and enlargement of visual cortex representations, of trauma-related stimuli. These effects likely support hypervigilance, visual intrusions, and visual flashbacks/hallucinations symptoms of PTSD, although further investigations in human are required. In addition, the amygdala projects to further unimodal sensory regions (130, 167, 168), and likely replicates parallel effects in those sensory domains also.

Memory Impairment and Modulation of the Hippocampus

The hippocampus is disturbed in PTSD, likely producing memory impairment for important aspects of the traumatic event, a DSM-5 PTSD symptom [(17, 18, 21, 126); Box 4c and related symptom, Figure 1]. Numerous functional neuroimaging studies have reported aberrant activations of the hippocampus in PTSD, with reports of both diminished and increased activations, although the reasons for these variations are not clear (17, 18, 21, 126). In addition, structural neuroimaging studies commonly report reduced hippocampal volume (18, 21). In a VHIS study, a patient group was identified with damage to the hippocampus but none to the amygdala (n = 9). Four of these patients had developed PTSD, a prevalence that was not significantly different from that of the control groups (79). These findings suggest that the hippocampus does not play a crucial pathogenic role in PTSD; it is further suggested that hippocampal dysfunction is probably driven by amygdala influence, as elucidated below.

The hippocampus is a high-level region in a hierarchy of interconnected brain regions, which together support progressively more comprehensive representations, which are used in perception, memory, cognition, and so forth. The particular function of the hippocampus is to integrate complex representations from earlier regions (e.g., PRC and entorhinal cortex) with spatio-temporal representations, to generate episodic memory [i.e., event memory; Ref. (72, 175, 176)]. Thus, impaired memory for important aspects of the traumatic event is likely brought about by hippocampal dysfunction.

Numerous studies have provided evidence that the amygdala influences the hippocampus. The primate amygdala sends heavy anatomical projections directly and extensively to the hippocampus, and indirectly via the entorhinal cortex, a major input to the hippocampus. The reciprocal projections are substantially weaker and restricted in extent (83, 92, 130, 168). Functionally, the amygdala enhances hippocampal consolidation of emotionally arousing information, leading to greater memory for such information. This is supported by abundant evidence from neuroimaging, neuropsychological, behavioral, and animal studies (35, 36, 177179).

Nevertheless, the amygdala may also impair memory retrieval processes in the hippocampus, and these effects have many parallels with the memory enhancement effects; that is, participation of stress hormones, common neurotransmitters, and restriction to emotionally arousing memories (177, 179). A neuroimaging study found that the amygdala and hippocampus participate in the recollective retrieval of emotional relative to neutral memories. Further, correlation analyses revealed that the amygdala and subregions of hippocampus were strongly coactivated during such recollection of emotional memories (180). A number of studies have presented stressful challenges that elicit endogenous release of stress hormones or have directly administered glucocorticoids (e.g., cortisol), to healthy human subjects before memory testing. The findings were that memory retrieval was impaired during arousal and elevation of stress hormone levels, predominantly for emotionally arousing information (179, 181184). Such retrieval deficits, however, may be modulated by multiple factors, such as duration and extent of hormone elevations, timing and type of retrieval tests, and so forth (177, 183). The amygdala is responsive to stress hormones as described earlier, and releases noradrenaline. Consistent with the involvement of this system, cortisol induced memory impairment is abolished by treatment with propranolol, an antagonist of adrenergic receptors (182). In addition, further neuroimaging studies found that elevated cortisol levels reduce activation of parts of the medial temporal network during retrieval of memories (185, 186).

Much of the evidence above, as well as the amygdala’s peritraumatic involvement, the amygdala’s established modulation under stress of hippocampal consolidation, and the latter phenomenon’s parallels with retrieval impairment, suggests the amygdala likely modulates hippocampal retrieval rather than the reverse. Further findings are that hippocampal volumes are commonly reduced in severe PTSD (18, 21, 187), and that reduced hippocampal volume is a risk factor for PTSD (187). The theoretical implications of these findings are currently unclear.

Collectively, there is abundant evidence that amygdala activity can enhance hippocampal consolidation, and these brain regions likely participate also in stress-induced impairment of emotional memory retrieval. The evidence generally suggests the amygdala may drive hippocampal dysfunction to produce the PTSD symptom of impaired memory for important aspects of the traumatic event, although further studies are needed.

Impairments of Motivation, Emotion, and Cognition, and Modulation of Rostral Anterior Cingulate Cortex

The rostral (anterior) subregion of anterior cingulate cortex (rACC), and adjacent medial subregion of orbitofrontal cortex (mOFC) are often treated together as ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC). Nevertheless, as these regions have both commonalities and substantial differences in connectivity, functions, and contributions to PTSD symptomatology, they are reviewed separately. The rACC participates in PTSD, likely producing impairments of motivation, emotion, and cognition (Box 4d and related symptoms, Figure 1). Moreover, impaired motivation and related symptoms likely contribute to the social and economic sequelae of PTSD (6, 9), adding to their significance. The rACC manifests in PTSD diminished responsiveness to trauma-related stimuli, which is a robust finding of functional neuroimaging studies (1618, 126). Such diminished activation is inversely correlated with PTSD symptom severity (134, 188190). In addition, following therapy rACC activity increases and this is associated with reduction of PTSD symptoms (191, 192). Together, these findings indicate that rACC is hypoactive in PTSD, and that such hypoactivation likely contributes substantively to PTSD symptomatology.

The normal ACC participates in networks that process wide-ranging motivated behaviors, emotion, cognition, and attention (193195). In addition, lesions, degeneration, or hypometabolism located predominantly in rACC have been associated with impairments of these functions, including motivational and emotional deficits such as apathy, disinterest, aimlessness, inactivity, and uncommunicativeness (196200). PTSD patients manifest DSM-5 symptoms of impaired interest and motivation, withdrawal and detachment, and emotional numbing [collectively termed numbing symptoms; Ref. (26, 201)], as well as impaired cognitive and attentional functioning according to neuropsychological tests (202204). There is evident overlap between impairments caused by rACC pathology, and multiple PTSD numbing symptoms, suggesting rACC dysfunction participates in the latter also. Convergent evidence is provided by an fMRI study of numbing symptoms in women with PTSD predominantly due to childhood abuse. The findings were that emotional numbing symptoms in the PTSD group relative to healthy controls were associated with hypoactivation of rACC and adjacent medial PFC (205).

The diminished rACC activation in PTSD is inversely correlated with amygdala activation according to the meta-analysis of Hayes et al. (16), suggesting one region may functionally influence the other. Contrary to the conventional view, it is suggested the predominant influence is that trauma-specific amygdala hyperactivity in PTSD likely drives the diminution of rACC activity, and consequent impairment of rACC functions. The ACC is reciprocally interconnected with the amygdala in primates; ACC (mainly BAs 24, 25) projections to the amygdala are moderate, whereas amygdala projections to ACC (BAs 24, 25, 32) are dense throughout the ventral to dorsal extent of rACC (130, 168, 206). In addition, in rodent experiments, amygdala activation elicited by fear conditioned stimuli or by electrical stimulation, suppresses rACC and medial PFC activity (207, 208). Further, in an auditory oddball task, targets that elicited arousal as measured by skin conductance responses, were associated with reduced activation in rACC as measured by fMRI, in PTSD subjects relative to controls. In contrast, non-arousing targets had no such rACC effects (209). Together, these findings support the amygdala’s suppressive influence on rACC. Consistent with the overall hypothesis of this section, Litz et al. (201) found that, after controlling for demographic variables, trauma exposure, comorbid disorders, and PTSD symptom clusters, hyperarousal symptoms were the best predictor of numbing symptoms. Moreover, these findings were supported in subsequent studies of diverse trauma samples by the same and other research groups [e.g., Ref. (210, 211)]. In sum, trauma-specific amygdala hyperactivity, which derives from peritraumatic responses, likely drives the diminished activity of rACC. This diminished activity likely contributes to the motivational, emotional, cognitive, and attentional symptoms of PTSD and their sequelae.

Anger, Irritability, Aggression, and Recklessness, and Modulation of Medial Orbitofrontal Cortex

Anger, irritability, and aggression are closely related, and anger/irritability may be expressed in multiple forms including as aggression, or in extremes as violence (212214). In addition, restraints on angry impulses are an important component of anger (212214). The mOFC is located adjacent to rACC on the vmPFC. In PTSD, mOFC is hypoactive, which is suggested to be driven by amygdala hyperactivity, and it crucially participates in anger, irritability, aggression, and recklessness symptoms of PTSD (Box 4e and related symptoms, Figure 1). There are many similarities in findings between mOFC and rACC regions. The mOFC manifests in PTSD reduced activation to trauma-related stimuli (16, 17, 126). The mOFC is substantially and reciprocally interconnected with the amygdala, according to neuroanatomical tracing studies in monkey (168, 215, 216). In addition, mOFC hypoactivation is inversely correlated with amygdala activation according to the meta-analysis of Hayes et al. (16). Further, electrical stimulation of the rat amygdala suppresses activity in medial PFC (208). Together, these similar findings suggest that the amygdala may suppress mOFC activation, paralleling the amygdala’s suppression of activation of the adjacent rACC.

Impaired function of mOFC produces behavioral disinhibition, which is the expression of socially inappropriate and aberrant behaviors, as well as impaired regulation, and these likely participate crucially in anger/aggression (217), and recklessness. In a study of patients with frontotemporal dementia, behavioral disinhibition, which was measured with the Neuropsychiatric Inventory, was found to be significantly correlated with mOFC hypometabolism, which was measured with PET and the [18F]fluorodeoxyglucose method (218). A further study of patients with dementia used the same behavioral measures, together with structural MRI and voxel-based morphometry. A significant association was found between reduced gray matter tissue in mOFC and several further regions, and several behavioral impairments including disinhibition (200). Lesions to the OFC have also been acquired through head injuries, and such patients have been found to manifest elevated levels of anger and aggression as well as aberrant social behaviors (219, 220). For example, in the latter study of a large sample of VHIS patients, elevated levels of aggression were found in patients with lesions to vmPFC, relative to patients with lesions to other brain regions and to healthy controls (220).

A different experimental paradigm compared patients with major depressive disorder with anger attacks, patients with major depressive disorder without anger attacks, and healthy controls. The study involved individualized anger and neutral scripts, an adaptation of the Pitman et al. (61) paradigm, and PET neuroimaging. The findings were that depressive patients with anger attacks relative to controls, during anger provocation manifested hypoactivation of mOFC and adjacent medial BA 10 (221). In an fMRI neuroimaging study, healthy subjects recalled anger eliciting events, and executed specified anger regulation strategies. The findings of the conjunction analysis were that anger regulation strategies activated mOFC and adjacent OFC regions, lateral PFC, and additional regions outside PFC (222).

Taken together, there is convergent evidence from diverse experimental paradigms that the OFC, particularly the mOFC, mediates inhibitory and regulatory processes (among other functions), and impairment of these leads to elevated levels of disinhibition, anger, and aggression. In PTSD, amygdala hyperactivity likely drives suppression of mOFC activity, which contributes to the corresponding symptoms of anger, irritability, and aggression. This overall hypothesis is further supported by behavioral findings that hyperarousal is robustly and significantly associated with measures of anger and aggression in PTSD (223, 224).

Recklessness or self-destructive behaviors are heterogeneous, and include diverse potentially destructive activities: sexual excesses, risky driving, substance abuse, aggression, violence, and so forth (225). Such diversity is likely promoted by multiple and poorly understood mechanisms. Recklessness or self-destructive behaviors are evidently related to aggression, which includes self-directed injurious or destructive activities (217). Similarly, impaired inhibitory and regulatory processes, as well as impaired processing of adverse feedback and of risk, are likely components for such behaviors to become manifest, and these flow from mOFC disruption [see earlier; Ref. (226, 227)]. Hence, mOFC dysfunction may play a crucial role in this PTSD symptom also. In sum, amygdala hyperactivity likely drives mOFC hypoactivation, which likely participates in anger/aggression, and recklessness symptoms of PTSD.

Pain, Distress, and the Amygdala

Severe pain importantly contributes to PTSD development (47, 49, 50), and the amygdala may process this similarly to hyperarousal (Figure 1). The amygdala receives heavy nociceptive inputs via several neuroanatomical pathways according to neuroanatomical tracing studies in monkey and rodent. Nociceptors in skin and throughout the body project to spinal cord neurons, which relay nociceptive information via dense projections sequentially through spinal cord, several nuclei of the thalamus, insula, or to SII which in turn projects heavily to insula, thence to multiple amygdaloid nuclei (83, 84, 228, 229). A further pathway relaying nociceptive information involves similar origins that project via NTS, PBC, to the central amygdaloid nucleus (228, 229). Human neuroimaging studies further demonstrate that painful stimulation activates the amygdala, and multiple additional areas (230233). Nevertheless, lesions to the amygdala do not affect pain sensitivity levels nor latency of pain responses (129, 234, 235), so the amygdala may mediate chronic or high-level pain-related functions. Pain-related amygdala activity undergoes consolidation (236238), and may produce noxiousness representations that participate in distributed representations. Mental pain or distress and physical pain are processed by largely overlapping networks of brain regions (239241), including the amygdala (241), so mental pain also may be processed similarly to hyperarousal. Pain processes are normally adaptive, but in PTSD are likely extreme and consequently disabling.

To summarize the above sections, the amygdala interconnects with and modulates the activity of multiple brain regions, and thereby drives the development of diverse DSM-5 PTSD symptoms. This is consistent with behavioral findings that hyperarousal drives the development of the other PTSD symptom clusters (33, 34).

Fear and the Insula

Persistent dysphoric emotions such as fear are a further DSM-5 PTSD symptom. Theorizing about fear has been dominated by the amygdala’s contribution [see for reviews, Ref. (111, 154)], but the amygdala solely cannot account for fear. The amygdala participates in fear, but also in diverse other emotions (81, 231, 232, 242, 243). In addition, fear is frequently elicited in laboratory animals by inflicting pain, and the latter is also processed in the amygdala (see earlier), suggesting the operation of possible confounds. Furthermore, rodents with amygdala lesions manifest impaired fear conditioning when assessed with fear-potentiated startle or freezing, yet largely intact fear conditioning when assessed by avoidance behaviors (35, 244, 245). Likewise, several neuropsychological studies have found that patients with bilateral or unilateral amygdala lesions continue to experience apparently normal subjective fear and anxiety, although with some subtle abnormalities of emotion generally (246, 247). Similarly, in a VHIS study using the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR disorders, it was found that the frequency of any fear and anxiety disorders (which did not include PTSD) was not significantly different among patient groups with lesions to the amygdala or to control regions (79). Thus, additional brain regions are crucial for the processing and representation of fear. In particular, the insula is a region that is reliably activated in PTSD and in anxiety disorders (18, 126), and it may contribute to the mediation of the intense and persistent fear symptom of PTSD (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. In addition to amygdala processing, the insula participates in the processing of fear and likely drives avoidance symptoms. Together, these mechanisms account for some 80% of DSM-5 PTSD symptoms. Abbreviations: dACC, dorsal anterior cingulate cortex; rACC, rostral anterior cingulate cortex; mOFC, medial orbitofrontal cortex; PHC, parahippocampal cortex; PRC, perirhinal cortex; STG, superior temporal gyrus; STS, superior temporal sulcus; VTC, ventral temporal cortex.

Fear is poorly understood, but is widely regarded as elicited predominantly by danger, real, or perceived (105, 248250). Nevertheless, this assumption is rarely tested and is challenged by empirical findings. Several studies of military personnel have demonstrated the co-occurrence of high danger levels, but little subjective fear or physiological activation (251, 252). In addition, correlations between dangerousness and fear were found to be low and non-significant in parachute trainees (253). Instead, expectations of harm may be the chief elicitors of fear (254, 255). Hence, dangers that can be controlled, normally do not cause harm nor elicit fear (251, 252, 256), whereas dangers that cannot be controlled, may produce harm and elicit fear.

It is well established in human studies that fear comprises three loosely linked components: subjective feelings, fear-related behaviors including flexible changes and avoidance, and physiological arousal (248, 250, 257). The insula is well-suited to participate in the mediation of fear and its three components. For the subjective component, the insula represents bodily state (85, 258, 259), and is involved in cognitive processing (260, 261), so can generate bodily harm expectations. For behaviors, the insula projects via the corticospinal tract to spinal cord, and together with adjacent SII accounts for 3.4% of corticospinal projection neurons in monkey (262). Consequently, it can directly, rapidly, and efficiently modulate spinal motoneuron populations, so can generate flight and urgent motor behaviors. In addition, the insula is strongly functionally interconnected with ACC, including the cingulate motor area [CMA; Ref. (263265)]. Further, a region of dorsal ACC (dACC) that likely includes part of CMA, was found to be reliably activated during fear experience in healthy humans, in a meta-analysis of fMRI neuroimaging studies (266). This brain area plays a major role in organizing diverse complex behaviors (195), including avoidance behaviors and emotional facial expression [see next section; Ref. (266, 267)]. Consequently, the insula can heighten appropriate behaviors, such effects being apparent during fear (268). For the physiological component, the insula sends substantial projections to ANS regulatory areas, and can modulate diverse autonomic functions (269).

Functional neuroimaging findings provide convergent support for insula mediation of fear (126, 243, 266, 270). Moreover, Caseras et al. (270) reported overlap of insula (and ACC) areas engaged in interoception and in fear. Another study compared fMRI activations across feared, pleasant, and unpleasant stimuli, and found enhanced insular subregion activation was specific for fear, whereas amygdala activation was common to multiple emotions (243). Further relevant observations are that the insula has moderate interconnections with PRC (120), so can contribute enduring fear representations to distributed representations. Despite the accumulated evidence, there is a scarcity of studies that directly investigate the contribution of the insula to fear, and such studies are needed. In sum, the insula commonly participates in the network that mediates PTSD, and it likely processes and represents the intense and persistent fear symptom of PTSD.

Avoidance and Dorsal ACC

Dorsal ACC is activated in PTSD, according to findings from multiple neuroimaging studies [see for reviews, Ref. (16, 18)], and is suggested to mediate avoidance symptoms (Box 4f and related symptoms, Figure 2). The quantitative meta-analysis of Hayes et al. (16) covering all reviewed PTSD studies estimates the location of the activated dACC region, and it appears to encompass part of the CMA. The CMA (BA 24c) sends heavy projections to spinal cord motoneurons, M1, supplementary motor area, presupplementary motor area, premotor cortex, putamen, cerebellum, and lateral prefrontal cortex [lateral PFC; Ref. (264, 271)]. The CMA receives heavy to substantial inputs from diverse areas, including anterior cingulate gyrus (BAs 24a, 24b), insula, PRC, and lateral PFC (264, 271). The primate CMA comprises two subregions, both somatotopically organized, but with poorly understood functional differences, although the anterior subregion’s functions include representation of abstract aspects of behavior (271).

The ACC participates in networks that mediate wide-ranging motivated behaviors (195), including avoidance behaviors (272274). In a rodent study, rats with freedom of movement were given mildly noxious stimulation when in part of a chamber, and neural activation was assessed by c-Fos expression. The findings were that the level of c-Fos expression in ACC was significantly and linearly related to behavioral avoidance of the noxious stimulation region (273). Rodent ACC is thought to correspond to primate BA 24 (275). In a study of surgical patients, painful laser stimulation was applied to the dorsum of the hand, and early neural responses were measured with chronically implanted intracerebral electrodes. The findings were that there were early activations within dACC, which was considered to correspond to CMA, and such activations were interpreted as mediating withdrawal/avoidance behaviors (272).

More generally, dACC is reliably activated in neuroimaging studies of pain, and has been suggested to mediate avoidance and other pain-related behaviors (274, 276). Also, dACC likely mediates avoidance during fear, as noted earlier. In addition, dACC activations during pain, fear, anger, other negative emotions, and cognition manifest substantial overlap (276). Thus, dACC and CMA representations likely support behavioral avoidance across multiple states. These representations may also support cognitive avoidance, in view of these regions’ involvement in cognitive processing (276), anterior CMA’s involvement in abstract aspects of behavior (271), and both regions’ heavy interconnections with lateral PFC (271). The CMA is also involved in emotional facial expression (267), which needs to be taken into account in theoretical and empirical studies. In sum, the commonly activated dACC region in PTSD which likely encompasses part of CMA, CMA’s heavy and extensive motor system projections, behavioral and neuroimaging findings across multiple states, and these regions’ cognitive functions and heavy interconnections with lateral PFC, together support dACC’s involvement in the behavioral avoidance and cognitive avoidance symptoms of PTSD. Moreover, the inputs to CMA arise from several regions involved in PTSD, suggesting such avoidance can be driven by several facets of PTSD.

Predictions and Directions for Future Research

The proposed model of the hyperarousal subtype of PTSD, offers an account of how peritraumatic responses are transformed into an extensive array of the 20 DSM-5 PTSD symptoms, and is summarized in Figure 2. The amygdala is crucial to PTSD (79), and the model provides a causal account of amygdala activation, of insula activation, and explains how they influence diverse brain regions. The model thereby provides an etiological account of an array of PTSD symptoms, and explains the contributions to PTSD made by multiple brain regions, which are consistent with their known functions and connectivities, but whose contributions to PTSD were otherwise unresolved. The model is also consistent with findings that hyperarousal drives the other symptom clusters (33, 34), and that PTSD represents the extreme of a stress reactions continuum (4244). Thus, the model integrates neuroscience and psychiatry to provide an extensive and dimensional account of a specific form of psychopathology, hyperarousal PTSD, and accords with the RDoC framework [cf. Ref. (40, 41)]. Together, the level of explanatory power and the number of symptoms elucidated exceeds those of existing accounts [cf. Ref. (17, 18, 21)], as summarized below. The model requires empirical tests in some areas, and also offers predictions and directions for future research, which should facilitate progress toward a comprehensive model of the hyperarousal subtype of PTSD, as also summarized below.

Traumatic events induce full PTSD in a proportion of those exposed, subthreshold PTSD in a larger proportion, and no disturbance in the majority; respectively, 12.5, 25.6, and 61.4% in a large Vietnam veterans sample (277), and this variation is an unresolved puzzle. The predominant existing models of PTSD involve fear conditioning supplemented by other concepts (18, 2022), and thus focus on traumatic stimuli and their conditioning. In contrast, the current model proposes that peritraumatic responses are the major determinant of subsequent symptomatology, so the puzzle may be explained in part by variability of traumatic responses. That is, it is predicted that a given traumatic event may elicit differing levels of peritraumatic responses across individuals, leading to differing levels of subsequent symptomatology. A continuum of responses is well supported, and ranges from the adaptive to the extreme and maladaptive (4244, 81). More generally, this hypothesis is consistent with the dimensional approach advocated by the RDoC framework (40, 41). An implication for prevention strategies is that amelioration of peritraumatic responses, in particular hyperarousal should ameliorate subsequent PTSD symptoms. There is some support for this from the preliminary findings that of a group of subjects (n = 11) intoxicated with alcohol at the time of trauma, none manifested early PTSD symptomatology (278). More generally, peritraumatic hyperarousal, stress hormones, heart rate, and so forth, need to be measured by real-time methods in empirical studies; current methods involve delays and other methodological shortcomings that likely produce significant underestimates, as noted earlier. Real-time measurements could be obtained remotely from personnel engaged in occupations with elevated rates of PTSD, and a prediction is that higher levels of peritraumatic arousal, stress hormones, and so forth, would be revealed than in existing studies. This seems likely as a significant proportion of combatants, for example, report such intense responses as faintness, vomiting, or defecation (148). Neuroanatomical, animal, and human neuroimaging studies suggest that multiple components of peritraumatic hyperarousal should heighten amygdala function, but empirical verification of this in human is needed.

Posttraumatic stress disorder develops in the absence of peritraumatic fear in a significant minority of patients [e.g., Ref. (2325)], and this is difficult for existing models to explain. In contrast, the current model proposes that hyperarousal, which is processed by the multifunctional amygdala, is sufficient for the development of diverse PTSD symptoms (Figure 1), which are able to satisfy DSM-IV (279) diagnostic criteria for PTSD. That is, the model explains how PTSD developed in the absence of peritraumatic fear. Hyperarousal to trauma-related reminders likely arises because amygdala and brainstem projection sites participate in distributed trauma-related representations. This prediction could be assessed using neuroimaging optimized for measuring brainstem activity. A therapeutic implication is that functional lesions of the amygdala–brainstem pathway by deep brain stimulation should have therapeutic potential, and there is preliminary rodent evidence for this proposal (280). This may need to be combined with treatment to reduce hyperactivity of the insula [which also modulates arousal; Ref. (136, 137, 269)] in patients with involvement of severe fear or pain symptoms. A number of medications have shown some efficacy in ameliorating PTSD symptoms, but rational pharmacotherapy options require development (12, 281). The proposed model suggests some directions for progress toward this goal. Specifically, the amygdala is hypothesized to modulate multiple brain regions to produce a diversity of symptoms (Figure 1), and diverse neurotransmitters may be involved in these pathways [cf. Ref. (92)]. Once the neurotransmitters involved in such pathways and symptoms are determined, then corresponding antagonists are predicted to attenuate those symptoms. Likewise, amygdala hyperactivity may be driven by severe pain as well as hyperarousal, so may be activated by additional neurotransmitters that require additional pharmacotherapy. Thus, the model may open the possibility of efficient, personalized interventions, in which pharmacotherapy and other treatments are matched to a patient’s particular pattern of symptoms.

The amygdala tunes the selectivity of auditory cortex neurons in rodents. Corresponding tuning of visual (and other sensory) cortex neurons and consequent enlargement of visual trauma-related representations is predicted to occur in human PTSD patients. Such plasticity effects likely participate in visual symptoms of PTSD, and merit investigation. The processing and participation of physical pain and of mental pain/dysphoria in PTSD are elucidated by the current model, but are unexplained by most existing models. Mental pain may be the common feature of peritraumatic dysphoric emotions (e.g., fear, shame, guilt, sadness, and anger) that can contribute to PTSD development, and this deserves further investigation.

The numbing symptoms of diminished interest, withdrawal, and blunted emotions, are unexplained by most existing PTSD models. The function of rACC is interpreted as the regulation of the amygdala, but the mechanisms producing its dysfunction in PTSD are unexplained (17, 18, 21). In contrast, the current model predicts that amygdala hyperactivity drives rACC hypoactivation, and this can be investigated at several levels. At the neuroanatomical level, amygdala neuronal boutons that synapse with rACC neurons are predicted to be predominantly inhibitory, and so should mainly form symmetric rather than asymmetric synapses [cf. Ref. (170)]. Amygdala–rACC projections are denser and more extensive than the reciprocal ones (130, 168, 206). Quantitative estimates of these projections may provide an index of the relative frequency of recruitment, and thus of the predominant direction of activation. At the functional level, replications of Garcia et al. (207) and Pérez-Jaranay and Vives (208), would determine whether amygdala suppression of rACC is a robust finding. Such studies may perhaps be possible in humans with intracerebral electrodes implanted over these regions as part of presurgical investigations, combined with, for example, presentation of feared stimuli [cf. Ref. (272)]. Also, fear-elicited rACC hypoactivation should be abolished by amygdala lesions (e.g., through Urbach–Wiethe disease) or inactivation. The script driven imagery paradigm with PTSD patients may be combined with fMRI and effective connectivity analyses to further determine the direction of activation between rACC and amygdala. More generally, rACC hypoactivation is predicted to produce impaired motivation (and other numbing symptoms), and to be positively correlated with quantitative measures of impaired motivation, such as those of Robert et al. (282) and Starkstein et al. (283). More broadly, the above investigations could be extended to encompass mOFC, whose activation is also hypothesized to be suppressed by amygdala activity.

An unresolved puzzle is the explanation for the findings of Koenigs et al. (79), that lesions to vmPFC reduce the occurrence of PTSD. Lesions to ACC and OFC including vmPFC, and regions interconnected with them, produce apathy and disturbances of emotion and sensation [see earlier; Ref. (284)]. For example, failure to be offered food for 24 h, unexpected bereavement of a much loved spouse, or reminders of painful experiences, were found to produce little complaint or distress in single case studies of patients (196, 197). Substantial levels of pain assessed with the visual analog scale or other measures, are reported by some ACC lesioned patients not to be bothersome (285, 286). A testable speculation is that similar processes may operate in PTSD following lesions to vmPFC. That is, objectively measurable symptoms such as elevated physiological arousal and startle responses, may be regarded by the patients as not bothersome or distressing, so lowering the subjective ratings of these and other symptoms. Moreover, this pattern may be associated with some degree of apathy as assessed by apathy questionnaires.

Fear is poorly understood, and the widespread assumption of its activation simply by danger requires empirical testing. The insula appears to play a crucial role in the mediation of fear, and this may explain the findings that patients with amygdala lesions continue to experience fear (79, 246, 247). Accordingly, lesions or inactivation of the insula, for example, should cause impairment of fear, but currently there are few empirical studies of the insula’s contribution to fear. In addition, the insula and its projection sites can account for the three components of fear, but empirical tests are required. Avoidance behaviors are not explained by existing PTSD models (17, 18, 21), and the amygdala sends few projections directly to the motor system (111, 153). The current model, however, predicts that dACC activations derive from several facets of PTSD, that their locations encompass part of CMA, and that they likely mediate avoidance symptoms. This may be tested with more precise specification of the location of dACC activations in PTSD, whether this includes CMA, and whether this overlaps the location of dACC activations in pain, fear, and so forth. The dACC activations in PTSD may encompass one or both subregions of CMA, which could be localized and their functions broadly examined by means of motor and facial expression tasks in the same PTSD subjects.

Poor or negative social support after a traumatic event substantially promotes PTSD (20, 45, 46), and this effect requires explanation. Possible accounts are that poor or negative social support may bring about further stress and mental pain that exacerbate or reactivate existing PTSD symptoms [cf. Ref. (287)]. In contrast, positive social support may have corresponding ameliorative effects. There is limited literature on a number of PTSD symptoms (e.g., distressing dreams and distorted cognitions), so these require early investigation. More generally, enriched understanding of the mechanisms and symptomatology of hyperarousal PTSD should facilitate development of further novel prevention strategies and targeted therapies.

Conclusion

A rich and valuable body of findings is accumulating on PTSD. The proposed theoretical model integrates wide-ranging findings about the hyperarousal subtype of PTSD into an extensive synthesis, which is detailed and parsimonious, and consistent with basic science and cognitive neuroscience literatures. It offers an account of the etiology and neurocircuitry of hyperarousal PTSD, explains a high proportion of the 20 DSM-5 PTSD symptoms, offers testable predictions to further illuminate the mechanisms of the disorder and of fear more generally, and has implications for prevention and treatment of the disorder.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The author declares that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

1. Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2005) 62:593–602. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.62.6.593

CrossRef Full Text

2. Kessler RC, Sonnega A, Bromet E, Hughes M, Nelson CB. Posttraumatic stress disorder in the National Comorbidity survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1995) 52:1048–60. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1995.03950240066012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

3. de Jong JT, Komproe IH, Van Ommeren M, El Masri M, Araya M, Khaled N, et al. Lifetime events and posttraumatic stress disorder in 4 postconflict settings. JAMA (2001) 286:555–62. doi:10.1001/jama.286.5.555

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

4. Hinton DE, Chhean D, Pich V, Pollack MH, Orr SP, Pitman RK. Assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder in Cambodian refugees using the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale: psychometric properties and symptom severity. J Trauma Stress (2006) 19:405–9. doi:10.1002/jts.20115

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

5. Friedman MJ. Posttraumatic stress disorder among military returnees from Afghanistan and Iraq. Am J Psychiatry (2006) 163:586–93. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.163.4.586

CrossRef Full Text

6. Galovski T, Lyons JA. Psychological sequelae of combat violence: a review of the impact of PTSD on the veteran’s family and possible interventions. Aggress Violent Behav (2004) 9:477–501. doi:10.1016/S1359-1789(03)00045-4

CrossRef Full Text

7. Keane TM. Psychological effects of military combat. In: Dohrenwend BP, editor. Adversity, Stress, and Psychopathology. New York: Oxford University Press (1998). p. 52–65.

8. Schnurr PP, Lunney CA, Bovin MJ, Marx BP. Posttraumatic stress disorder and quality of life: extension of findings to veterans of the wars in Iraq and Afghanistan. Clin Psychol Rev (2009) 29:727–35. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

9. Taft CT, Schumm JA, Panuzio J, Proctor SP. An examination of family adjustment among Operation Desert Storm veterans. J Consult Clin Psychol (2008) 76:648–56. doi:10.1037/a0012576

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

10. Boscarino JA. Posttraumatic stress disorder and mortality among US Army veterans 30 years after military service. Ann Epidemiol (2006) 16:248–56. doi:10.1016/j.annepidem.2005.03.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

11. Agorastos A, Marmar CR, Otte C. Immediate and early behavioral interventions for the prevention of acute and posttraumatic stress disorder. Curr Opin Psychiatry (2011) 24:526–32. doi:10.1097/YCO.obo13e32834cdde2

CrossRef Full Text

12. Friedman MJ. PTSD: pharmacotherapeutic approaches. Focus (2013) 11:315–20. doi:10.1176/appi.focus.11.3.315

CrossRef Full Text

13. Roberts NP, Kitchiner NJ, Kenardy J, Bisson JI. Systematic review and meta-analysis of multiple-session early interventions following traumatic events. Am J Psychiatry (2009) 166:293–301. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.08040590

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

14. Davis M. Analysis of aversive memories using the fear-potentiated startle paradigm. In: Squire LR, Butters N, editors. Neuropsychology of Memory. 2nd ed. New York: Guilford Press (1992). p. 470–84.

15. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (2013).

16. Hayes JP, Hayes SM, Mikedis AM. Quantitative meta-analysis of neural activity in posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Mood Anxiety Disord (2012) 2:9. doi:10.1186/2045-5380-2-9

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

17. Patel R, Spreng RN, Shin LM, Girard TA. Neurocircuitry models of posttraumatic stress disorder and beyond: a meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2012) 36:2130–42. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2012.06.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

18. Shin LM, Liberzon I. The neurocircuitry of fear, stress, and anxiety disorders. Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35:169–91. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.83

CrossRef Full Text

19. Brewin CR. Encoding and retrieval of traumatic memories. In: Vasterling JJ, Brewin CR, editors. Neuropsychology of PTSD. London: Guilford Press (2005). p. 131–50.

20. Brewin CR, Holmes EA. Psychological theories of posttraumatic stress disorder. Clin Psychol Rev (2003) 23:339–76. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(03)00033-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

21. Pitman RK, Rasmusson AM, Koenen KC, Shin LM, Orr SP, Gilbertson MW, et al. Biological studies of post-traumatic stress disorder. Nat Rev Neurosci (2012) 13:769–87. doi:10.1038/nrn3339

CrossRef Full Text

22. Zoellner LA, Rothbaum BO, Feeny NC. PTSD not an anxiety disorder? DSM committee proposal turns back the hands of time. Depress Anxiety (2011) 28:853–6. doi:10.1002/da.20899

CrossRef Full Text

23. Adler AB, Wright KM, Bliese PD, Eckford R, Hoge CW. A2 diagnostic criterion for combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress (2008) 21:301–8. doi:10.1002/jts.20336

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

24. Brewin CR, Andrews B, Rose S. Fear, helplessness, and horror in posttraumatic stress disorder: investigating DSM-IV criterion A2 in victims of violent crime. J Trauma Stress (2000) 13:499–509. doi:10.1023/A:1007741526169

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

25. Carson MA, Paulus LA, Lasko NB, Metzger LJ, Wolfe J, Orr SP, et al. Psychophysiologic assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder in Vietnam nurse veterans who witnessed injury or death. J Consult Clin Psychol (2000) 68:890–7. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.68.5.890

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

26. Friedman MJ, Resick PA, Bryant RA, Brewin CR. Considering PTSD for DSM-5. Depress Anxiety (2011) 28:750–69. doi:10.1002/da.20767

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

27. O’Donnell ML, Creamer M, McFarlane AC, Silove D, Bryant RA. Should A2 be a diagnostic requirement for posttraumatic stress disorder in DSM-V? Psychiatry Res (2010) 176:257–60. doi:10.1016/j.psychres.2009.05.012

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

28. Barsalou LW. Grounded cognition. Annu Rev Psychol (2008) 59:617–45. doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

29. Barsalou LW, Simmons WK, Barbey AK, Wilson CD. Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends Cogn Sci (2003) 7:84–91. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)00029-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

30. Martin A. Circuits in mind: the neural foundations for object concepts. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The Cognitive Neurosciences. 4th ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2009). p. 1031–45.

31. Meyer K, Damasio A. Convergence and divergence in a neural architecture for recognition and memory. Trends Neurosci (2009) 32:376–82. doi:10.1016/j.tins.2009.04.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

32. Simmons WK, Ramjee V, Beauchamp MS, McRae K, Martin A, Barsalou LW. A common neural substrate for perceiving and knowing about color. Neuropsychologia (2007) 45:2802–10. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

33. Marshall GN, Schell TL, Glynn SM, Shetty V. The role of hyperarousal in the manifestation of posttraumatic psychological distress following injury. J Abnorm Psychol (2006) 115:624–8. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.115.3.624

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

34. Schell TL, Marshall GN, Jaycox LH. All symptoms are not created equal: the prominent role of hyperarousal in the natural course of posttraumatic psychological distress. J Abnorm Psychol (2004) 113:189–97. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.113.2.189

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

35. McGaugh JL. The amygdala modulates the consolidation of memories of emotionally arousing experiences. Annu Rev Neurosci (2004) 27:1–28. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144157

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

36. McIntyre CK, McGaugh JL, Williams CL. Interacting brain systems modulate memory consolidation. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2012) 36:1750–62. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.11.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

37. Lanius RA, Bluhm R, Lanius U, Pain C. A review of neuroimaging studies in PTSD: heterogeneity of response to symptom provocation. J Psychiatr Res (2006) 40:709–29. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2005.07.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

38. Lanius RA, Hopper JW, Menon R. Individual differences in a husband and wife who developed PTSD after a motor vehicle accident: a functional MRI case study. Am J Psychiatry (2003) 160:667–9. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.4.667

CrossRef Full Text

39. Lanius RA, Vermetten E, Loewenstein RJ, Brand B, Schmahl C, Bremner JD, et al. Emotion modulation in PTSD: clinical and neurobiological evidence for a dissociative subtype. Am J Psychiatry (2010) 167:640–7. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2009.09081168

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

40. Cuthbert BN. The RDoC framework: facilitating transition from ICD/DSM to dimensional approaches that integrate neuroscience and psychopathology. World Psychiatry (2014) 13:28–35. doi:10.1002/wps.20087

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

41. Cuthbert BN, Insel TR. Toward the future of psychiatric diagnosis: the seven pillars of RDoC. BMC Med (2013) 11:126. doi:10.1186/1741-7015-11-126

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

42. Broman-Fulks JJ, Ruggiero KJ, Green BA, Kilpatrick DG, Danielson CK, Resnick HS, et al. Taxometric investigation of PTSD: data from two nationally representative samples. Behav Ther (2006) 37:364–80. doi:10.1016/j.beth.2006.02.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

43. Forbes D, Haslam N, Williams BJ, Creamer M. Testing the latent structure of posttraumatic stress disorder: a taxometric study of combat veterans. J Trauma Stress (2005) 18:647–56. doi:10.1002/jts.20073

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

44. Ruscio AM, Ruscio J, Keane TM. The latent structure of posttraumatic stress disorder: a taxometric investigation of reactions to extreme stress. J Abnorm Psychol (2002) 111:290–301. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.111.2.290

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

45. Brewin CR, Andrews B, Valentine JD. Meta-analysis of risk factors for posttraumatic stress disorder in trauma-exposed adults. J Consult Clin Psychol (2000) 68:748–66. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.68.5.748

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

46. Ozer EJ, Best SR, Lipsey TL, Weiss DS. Predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder and symptoms in adults: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull (2003) 129:52–73. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.129.1.52

CrossRef Full Text

47. Bryant RA, Creamer M, O’Donnell M, Silove D, McFarlane AC. A study of the protective function of acute morphine administration on subsequent posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2009) 65:438–40. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.10.032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

48. Marx BP, Foley KM, Feinstein BA, Wolf EJ, Kaloupek DG, Keane TM. Combat-related guilt mediates the relations between exposure to combat-related abusive violence and psychiatric diagnoses. Depress Anxiety (2010) 27:287–93. doi:10.1002/da.20659

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

49. Norman SB, Stein MB, Dimsdale JE, Hoyt DB. Pain in the aftermath of trauma is a risk factor for post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med (2008) 38:533–42. doi:10.1017/S0033291707001389

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

50. Zatzick DF, Rivara FP, Nathens AB, Jurkovich GJ, Wang J, Fan M-Y, et al. A nationwide US study of post-traumatic stress after hospitalization for physical injury. Psychol Med (2007) 37:1469–80. doi:10.1017/S0033291707000943

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

51. Bryant RA. Longitudinal psychophysiological studies of heart rate: mediating effects and implications for treatment. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2006) 1071:19–26. doi:10.1196/annals.1364.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

52. Bryant RA, Creamer M, O’Donnell M, Silove D, McFarlane AC. A multisite study of initial respiration rate and heart rate as predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry (2008) 69:1694–701. doi:10.4088/JCP.v69n1104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

53. Coronas R, Gallardo O, Moreno MJ, Suarez D, Garcia-Pares G, Menchon JM. Heart rate measured in the acute aftermath of trauma can predict post-traumatic stress disorder: a prospective study in motor vehicle accident survivors. Eur Psychiatry (2011) 26:508–12. doi:10.1016/j.eurpsy.2010.06.006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

54. Nixon RDV, Bryant RA. Peritraumatic and persistent panic attacks in acute stress disorder. Behav Res Ther (2003) 41:1237–42. doi:10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00150-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

55. O’Donnell ML, Creamer M, Elliott P, Bryant R. Tonic and phasic heart rate as predictors of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychosom Med (2007) 69:256–61. doi:10.1097/PSY.obo13e3180417d04

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

56. Zatzick DF, Russo J, Pitman RK, Rivara F, Jurkovich G, Roy-Byrne P. Reevaluating the association between emergency department heart rate and the development of posttraumatic stress disorder: a public health approach. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 57:91–5. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.10.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

57. Orr SP, McNally RJ, Rosen GM, Shalev AY. Psychophysiologic reactivity: implications for conceptualizing PTSD. In: Rosen GM, editor. Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Issues and Controversies. Chichester: Wiley (2004). p. 101–26.

58. Orr SP, Metzger LJ, Miller MW, Kaloupek DG. Psychophysiological assessment of PTSD. In: Wilson JP, Keane TM, editors. Assessing Psychological Trauma and PTSD. 2nd ed. London: Guilford Press (2004). p. 289–343.

59. Pitman RK, Shalev AY, Orr SP. Posttraumatic stress disorder: emotion, conditioning, and memory. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The New Cognitive Neurosciences. 2nd ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2000). p. 1133–47.

60. Orr SP, Meyerhoff JL, Edwards JV, Pitman RK. Heart rate and blood pressure resting levels and responses to generic stressors in Vietnam veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress (1998) 11:155–64. doi:10.1023/A:1024421502881

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

61. Pitman RK, Orr SP, Forgue DF, de Jong JB, Claiborn JM. Psychophysiologic assessment of posttraumatic stress disorder imagery in Vietnam combat veterans. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1987) 44:970–5. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1987.01800230050009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

62. McFall ME, Veith RC, Murburg MM. Basal sympathoadrenal function in posttraumatic distress disorder. Biol Psychiatry (1992) 31:1050–6. doi:10.1016/0006-3223(92)90097-J

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

63. Pole N. The psychophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull (2007) 133:725–46. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.133.5.725

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

64. Meewisse M-L, Reitsma JB, De Vries G-J, Gersons BPR, Olff M. Cortisol and post-traumatic stress disorder in adults. Br J Psychiatry (2007) 191:387–92. doi:10.1192/bjp.bp.106.024877

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

65. Brady KT, Killeen TK, Brewerton T, Lucerini S. Comorbidity of psychiatric disorders and posttraumatic stress disorder. J Clin Psychiatry (2000) 61(Suppl 7):22–32.

66. Brown TA, Campbell LH, Lehman CL, Grisham JR, Mancill RB. Current and lifetime comorbidity of the DSM-IV anxiety and mood disorders in a large clinical sample. J Abnorm Psychol (2001) 110:585–99. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.110.4.585

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

67. Frijda NH. Moods, emotion episodes, and emotions. In: Lewis M, Haviland JM, editors. Handbook of Emotions. New York: Guilford Press (1993). p. 381–403.

68. Oatley K. Best Laid Schemes: The Psychology of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (1992).

69. Ortony A, Clore GL, Collins A. The Cognitive Structure of Emotions. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press (1988).

70. Allport DA. Distributed memory, modular subsystems and dysphasia. In: Newman S, Epstein R, editors. Current Perspectives in Dysphasia. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone (1985). p. 32–60.

71. Haxby JV, Gobbini MI, Montgomery K. Spatial and temporal distribution of face and object representations in the human brain. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The Cognitive Neurosciences. 3rd ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2004). p. 889–904.

72. Murray EA, Bussey TJ, Saksida LM. Visual perception and memory: a new view of medial temporal lobe function in primates and rodents. Annu Rev Neurosci (2007) 30:99–122. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.29.051605.113046

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

73. Goldberg RF, Perfetti CA, Schneider W. Perceptual knowledge retrieval activates sensory brain regions. J Neurosci (2006) 26:4917–21. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5389-05.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

74. Simmons WK, Martin A, Barsalou LW. Pictures of appetizing foods activate gustatory cortices for taste and reward. Cereb Cortex (2005) 15:1602–8. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhi038

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

75. Wheeler ME, Petersen SE, Buckner RL. Memory’s echo: vivid remembering reactivates sensory-specific cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2000) 97:11125–9. doi:10.1073/pnas.97.20.11125

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

76. Beauchamp MS, Martin A. Grounding object concepts in perception and action: evidence from fMRI studies of tools. Cortex (2007) 43:461–8. doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70470-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

77. Saygin AP, Wilson SM, Dronkers NF, Bates E. Action comprehension in aphasia: linguistic and non-linguistic deficits and their lesion correlates. Neuropsychologia (2004) 42:1788–804. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.04.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

78. Tranel D, Kemmerer D, Adolphs R, Damasio H, Damasio AR. Neural correlates of conceptual knowledge for actions. Cogn Neuropsychol (2003) 20:409–32. doi:10.1080/02643290244000248

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

79. Koenigs M, Huey ED, Raymont V, Cheon B, Solomon J, Wassermann EM, et al. Focal brain damage protects against post-traumatic stress disorder in combat veterans. Nat Neurosci (2008) 11:232–7. doi:10.1038/nn2032

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

80. Kapp BS, Whalen PJ, Supple WF, Pascoe JP. Amygdaloid contributions to conditioned arousal and sensory information processing. In: Aggleton JP, editor. The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss (1992). p. 229–54.

81. Shabel SJ, Janak PH. Substantial similarity in amygdala neuronal activity during conditioned appetitive and aversive emotional arousal. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2009) 106:15031–6. doi:10.1073/pnas.0905580106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

82. Herbert H, Moga MM, Saper CB. Connections of the parabrachial nucleus with the nucleus of the solitary tract and the medullary reticular formation in the rat. J Comp Neurol (1990) 293:540–80. doi:10.1002/cne.902930404

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

83. McDonald AJ. Cortical pathways to the mammalian amygdala. Prog Neurobiol (1998) 55:257–332. doi:10.1016/S0301-0082(98)00003-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

84. Pitkänen A. Connectivity of the rat amygdaloid complex. In: Aggleton JP, editor. The Amygdala: A Functional Analysis. 2nd ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2000). p. 31–115.

85. Saper CB. The central autonomic nervous system: conscious visceral perception and autonomic pattern generation. Annu Rev Neurosci (2002) 25:433–69. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.25.032502.111311

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

86. Brannan S, Liotti M, Egan G, Shade R, Madden L, Robillard R, et al. Neuroimaging of cerebral activations and deactivations associated with hypercapnia and hunger for air. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2001) 98:2029–34. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.4.2029

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

87. Evans KC, Banzett RB, Adams L, McKay L, Frackowiak RSJ, Corfield DR. Bold fMRI identifies limbic, paralimbic, and cerebellar activation during air hunger. J Neurophysiol (2002) 88:1500–11. doi:10.1152/jn.00957.2001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

88. Liotti M, Brannan S, Egan G, Shade R, Madden L, Abplanalp B, et al. Brain responses associated with consciousness of breathlessness (air hunger). Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2001) 98:2035–40. doi:10.1073/pnas.98.4.2035

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

89. von Leupoldt A, Sommer T, Kegat S, Baumann HJ, Klose H, Dahme B, et al. Dyspnea and pain share emotion-related brain network. Neuroimage (2009) 48:200–6. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

90. Cechetto DF, Calaresu FR. Central pathways relaying cardiovascular afferent information to amygdala. Am J Physiol (1985) 248:R38–45.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

91. Roozendaal B, Quirarte GL, McGaugh JL. Stress-activated hormonal systems and the regulation of memory storage. Ann N Y Acad Sci (1997) 821:247–58. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.1997.tb48284.x

CrossRef Full Text

92. Amaral DG, Price JL, Pitkänen A, Carmichael ST. Anatomical organization of the primate amygdaloid complex. In: Aggleton JP, editor. The Amygdala: Neurobiological Aspects of Emotion, Memory, and Mental Dysfunction. New York: Wiley-Liss (1992). p. 1–66.

93. Schreurs J, Seelig T, Schulman H. ß2-Adrenergic receptors on peripheral nerves. J Neurochem (1986) 46:294–6. doi:10.1111/j.1471-4159.1986.tb12961.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

94. Hassert DL, Miyashita T, Williams CL. The effects of peripheral vagal nerve stimulation at a memory-modulating intensity on norepinephrine output in the basolateral amygdala. Behav Neurosci (2004) 118:79–88. doi:10.1037/0735-7044.118.1.79

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

95. Canli T, Zhao Z, Brewer J, Gabrieli JDE, Cahill L. Event-related activation in the human amygdala associates with later memory for individual emotional experience. J Neurosci (2000) 20(RC99):1–5.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

96. Cunningham WA, Raye CL, Johnson MK. Implicit and explicit evaluation: fMRI correlates of valence, emotional intensity, and control in the processing of attitudes. J Cogn Neurosci (2004) 16:1717–29. doi:10.1162/0898929042947919

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

97. Lewis PA, Critchley HD, Rotshtein P, Dolan RJ. Neural correlates of processing valence and arousal in affective words. Cereb Cortex (2007) 17:742–8. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhk024

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

98. Taylor SF, Liberzon I, Koeppe RA. The effect of graded aversive stimuli on limbic and visual activation. Neuropsychologia (2000) 38:1415–25. doi:10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00032-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

99. Hoge CW, Castro CA, Messer SC, McGurk D, Cotting DI, Koffman RL. Combat duty in Iraq and Afghanistan, mental health problems, and barriers to care. N Engl J Med (2004) 351:13–22. doi:10.1056/NEJMoa040603

CrossRef Full Text

100. Admon R, Lubin G, Stern O, Rosenberg K, Sela L, Ben-Ami H, et al. Human vulnerability to stress depends on amygdala’s predisposition and hippocampal plasticity. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2009) 106:14120–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.0903183106

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

101. Brashers-Krug T, Shadmehr R, Bizzi E. Consolidation in human motor memory. Nature (1996) 382:252–5. doi:10.1038/382252a0

CrossRef Full Text

102. Karni A, Meyer G, Rey-Hipolito C, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R, et al. The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1998) 95:861–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.3.861

CrossRef Full Text

103. Maquet P. The role of sleep in learning and memory. Science (2001) 294:1048–52. doi:10.1126/science.1062856

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

104. Pelletier JG, Likhtik E, Filali M, Paré D. Lasting increases in basolateral amygdala activity after emotional arousal: implications for facilitated consolidation of emotional memories. Learn Mem (2005) 12:96–102. doi:10.1101/lm.88605

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

105. Reber AS, Allen R, Reber ES. Penguin Dictionary of Psychology. 4th ed. London: Penguin Books (2009).

106. Cooke SF, Bliss TVP. Plasticity in the human central nervous system. Brain (2006) 129:1659–73. doi:10.1093/brain/awl082

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

107. Rogan MT, Stäubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature (1997) 390:604–7. doi:10.1038/37601

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

108. Rogan MT, LeDoux JE. LTP is accompanied by commensurate enhancement of auditory-evoked responses in a fear conditioning circuit. Neuron (1995) 15:127–36. doi:10.1016/0896-6273(95)90070-5

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

109. Collins DR, Paré D. Differential fear conditioning induces reciprocal changes in the sensory responses of lateral amygdala neurons to the CS(+) and CS(-). Learn Mem (2000) 7:97–103. doi:10.1101/lm.7.2.97

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

110. Goosens KA, Maren S. NMDA receptors are essential for the acquisition, but not expression, of conditional fear and associative spike firing in the lateral amygdala. Eur J Neurosci (2004) 20:537–48. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03513.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

111. LeDoux JE, Schiller D, Cain C. Emotional reaction and action: from threat processing to goal-directed behavior. In: Gazzaniga MS, editor. The Cognitive Neurosciences. 4th ed. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (2009). p. 905–24.

112. Rosenkranz JA, Grace AA. Dopamine-mediated modulation of odour-evoked amygdala potentials during pavlovian conditioning. Nature (2002) 417:282–7. doi:10.1038/417282a

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

113. Maren S, Ferrario CR, Corcoran KA, Desmond TJ, Frey KA. Protein synthesis in the amygdala, but not the auditory thalamus, is required for consolidation of Pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Eur J Neurosci (2003) 18:3080–8. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2003.03063.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

114. Schafe GE, Doyère V, LeDoux JE. Tracking the fear engram: the lateral amygdala is an essential locus of fear memory storage. J Neurosci (2005) 25:10010–5. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3307-05.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

115. Wilensky AE, Schafe GE, Kristensen MP, LeDoux JE. Rethinking the fear circuit: the central nucleus of the amygdala is required for the acquisition, consolidation, and expression of pavlovian fear conditioning. J Neurosci (2006) 26:12387–96. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4316-06.2006

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

116. Anglada-Figueroa D, Quirk GJ. Lesions of the basal amygdala block expression of conditioned fear but not extinction. J Neurosci (2005) 25:9680–5. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2600-05.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

117. Fanselow MS, Gale GD. The amygdala, fear and memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2003) 985:125–34. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07077.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

118. Gale GD, Anagnostaras SG, Godsil BP, Mitchell S, Nozawa T, Sage JR, et al. Role of the basolateral amygdala in the storage of fear memories across the adult lifetime of rats. J Neurosci (2004) 24:3810–5. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4100-03.2004

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

119. Davies RR, Graham KS, Xuereb JH, Williams GB, Hodges JR. The human perirhinal cortex and semantic memory. Eur J Neurosci (2004) 20:2441–6. doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03710.x

CrossRef Full Text

120. Suzuki WA, Amaral DG. Perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices of the macaque monkey: cortical afferents. J Comp Neurol (1994) 350:497–533. doi:10.1002/cne.903500402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

121. Buckley MJ, Gaffan D. Perirhinal cortical contributions to object perception. Trends Cogn Sci (2006) 10:100–7. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.01.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

122. Lee ACH, Buckley MJ, Pegman SJ, Spiers H, Scahill VL, Gaffan D, et al. Specialization in the medial temporal lobe for processing of objects and scenes. Hippocampus (2005) 15:782–97. doi:10.1002/hipo.20101

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

123. Lee ACH, Bussey TJ, Murray EA, Saksida LM, Epstein RA, Kapur N, et al. Perceptual deficits in amnesia: challenging the medial temporal lobe ‘mnemonic’ view. Neuropsychologia (2005) 43:1–11. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.07.017

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

124. Taylor KI, Stamatakis EA, Tyler LK. Crossmodal integration of object features: voxel-based correlations in brain-damaged patients. Brain (2009) 132:671–83. doi:10.1093/brain/awn361

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

125. Stefanacci L, Suzuki WA, Amaral DG. Organization of connections between the amygdaloid complex and the perirhinal and parahippocampal cortices in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol (1996) 375:552–82. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19961125)

CrossRef Full Text

126. Etkin A, Wager TD. Functional neuroimaging of anxiety: a meta-analysis of emotional processing in PTSD, social anxiety disorder, and specific phobia. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164:1476–88. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.07030504

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

127. Schmidt S, Mohr A, Miltner WHR, Straube T. Task-dependent neural correlates of the processing of verbal threat-related stimuli in social phobia. Biol Psychol (2010) 84:304–12. doi:10.1016/j.biopsycho.2010.03.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

128. Straube T, Mentzel H-J, Miltner WHR. Neural mechanisms of automatic and direct processing of phobogenic stimuli in specific phobia. Biol Psychiatry (2006) 59:162–70. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2005.06.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

129. Bernard JF, Bester H, Besson JM. The spino-parabrachio-amygdaloid and -hypothalamic nociceptive pathways. In: Besson JM, Guilbaud G, Ollat H, editors. Forebrain Areas involved in Pain Processing. Paris: John Libbey Eurotext (1995). p. 27–48.

130. Freese JL, Amaral DG. Neuroanatomy of the primate amygdala. In: Whalen PJ, Phelps EA, editors. The Human Amygdala. London: Guilford Press (2009). p. 3–42.

131. Fredrikson M, Furmark T. Amygdaloid regional cerebral blood flow and subjective fear during symptom provocation in anxiety disorders. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2003) 985:341–7. doi:10.1111/j.1749-6632.2003.tb07092.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

132. Protopopescu X, Pan H, Tuescher O, Cloitre M, Goldstein M, Engelien W, et al. Differential time courses and specificity of amygdala activity in posttraumatic stress disorder subjects and normal control subjects. Biol Psychiatry (2005) 57:464–73. doi:10.1016/j.biopsych.2004.12.026

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

133. Rauch SL, Whalen PJ, Shin LM, McInerney SC, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, et al. Exaggerated amygdala response to masked facial stimuli in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional MRI study. Biol Psychiatry (2000) 47:769–76. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(00)00828-3

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

134. Shin LM, Orr SP, Carson MA, Rauch SL, Macklin ML, Lasko NB, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow in the amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex during traumatic imagery in male and female Vietnam veterans with PTSD. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2004) 61:168–76. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.2.168

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

135. Shin LM, Rauch SL, Pitman RK. Amygdala, medial prefrontal cortex, and hippocampal function in PTSD. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2006) 1071:67–79. doi:10.1196/annals.1364.007

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

136. King AP, Abelson JL, Britton JC, Phan KL, Taylor SF, Liberzon I. Medial prefrontal cortex and right insula activity predict plasma ACTH response to trauma recall. Neuroimage (2009) 47:872–80. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.05.088

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

137. Liberzon I, King AP, Britton JC, Phan KL, Abelson JL, Taylor SF. Paralimbic and medial prefrontal cortical involvement in neuroendocrine responses to traumatic stimuli. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164:1250–8. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06081367

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

138. Shalev AY, Videlock EJ, Peleg T, Segman R, Pitman RK, Yehuda R. Stress hormones and post-traumatic stress disorder in civilian trauma victims: a longitudinal study. Part I: HPA axis responses. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol (2008) 11:365–72. doi:10.1017/S1461145707008127

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

139. Videlock EJ, Peleg T, Segman R, Yehuda R, Pitman RK, Shalev AY. Stress hormones and post-traumatic stress disorder in civilian trauma victims: a longitudinal study. Part II: the adrenergic response. Int J Neuropsychopharmacol (2008) 11:373–80. doi:10.1017/S1461145707008139

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

140. Bateson P, Bradshaw EL. Physiological effects of hunting red deer (Cervus elaphus). Proc Biol Sci (1997) 264:1707–14. doi:10.1098/rspb.1997.0237

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

141. Carragher JF, Ingram JR, Matthews LR. Effects of yarding and handling procedures on stress responses of red deer stags (Cervus elaphus). Appl Anim Behav Sci (1997) 51:143–58. doi:10.1016/S0168-1591(96)01090-8

CrossRef Full Text

142. Mason G. The physiology of the hunted deer. Nature (1998) 391:22. doi:10.1038/34045

CrossRef Full Text

143. Kitay JI. Sex differences in adrenal cortical secretion in the rat. Endocrinology (1961) 68:818–24. doi:10.1210/endo-68-5-818

CrossRef Full Text

144. Matsuyama H, Ruhmann-Wennhold A, Johnson LR, Nelson DH. Disappearance rates of exogenous and endogenous ACTH from rat plasma measured by bioassay and radioimmunoassay. Metabolism (1972) 21:30–5. doi:10.1016/0026-0495(72)90017-0

CrossRef Full Text

145. McCarty R. Stress, behavior and experimental hypertension. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (1983) 7:493–502. doi:10.1016/0149-7634(83)90029-5

CrossRef Full Text

146. Coover GD, Sutton BR, Heybach JP. Conditioning decreases in plasma corticosterone level in rats by pairing stimuli with daily feedings. J Comp Physiol Psychol (1977) 91:716–26. doi:10.1037/h0077363

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

147. Mason JW, Maher JT, Hartley LH, Mougey EH, Perlow MJ, Jones LG. Selectivity of corticosteroid and catecholamine responses to various natural stimuli. In: Serban G, editor. Psychopathology of Human Adaptation. New York: Plenum (1976). p. 147–171.

148. Stouffer SA, Lumsdaine AA, Lumsdaine MH, Williams RM, Smith MB, Janis IL, et al. The American Soldier: Combat and Its Aftermath. New York: Wiley (1949).

149. McHugh PR, Treisman G. PTSD: a problematic diagnostic category. J Anxiety Disord (2007) 21:211–22. doi:10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.09.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

150. Gilbertson MW, McFarlane AC, Weathers FW, Keane TM, Yehuda R, Shalev AY, et al. Is trauma a causal agent of psychopathologic symptoms in posttraumatic stress disorder? Findings from identical twins discordant for combat exposure. J Clin Psychiatry (2010) 71:1324–30. doi:10.4088/JCP.10m06121blu

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

151. Pitman RK, Gilbertson MW, Gurvits TV, May FS, Lasko NB, Metzger LJ, et al. Clarifying the origin of biological abnormalities in PTSD through the study of identical twins discordant for combat exposure. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2006) 1071:242–54. doi:10.1196/annals.1364.019

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

152. Shin LM, Lasko NB, Macklin ML, Karpf RD, Milad MR, Orr SP, et al. Resting metabolic activity in the cingulate cortex and vulnerability to posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2009) 66:1099–107. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.138

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

153. Young MP, Scannell JW, Burns GAPC, Blakemore C. Analysis of connectivity: neural systems in the cerebral cortex. Rev Neurosci (1994) 5:227–49.

154. Davis M, Walker DL, Miles L, Grillon C. Phasic vs sustained fear in rats and humans: role of the extended amygdala in fear vs anxiety. Neuropsychopharmacology (2010) 35:105–35. doi:10.1038/npp.2009.109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

155. Germain A, Buysse DJ, Nofzinger EA. Sleep-specific mechanisms underlying posttraumatic stress disorder: integrative review and neurobiological hypotheses. Sleep Med Rev (2008) 12:185–95. doi:10.1016/j.smrv.2007.09.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

156. Jones BE. Basic mechanisms of sleep-wake states. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 4th ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier Saunders (2005). p. 136–53.

157. Nofzinger EA, Maquet P. What brain imaging reveals about sleep generation and maintenance. In: Kryger MH, Roth T, Dement WC, editors. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. 5th ed. Amsterdam: Elsevier Saunders (2011). p. 201–14.

158. Braun AR, Balkin TJ, Wesensten NJ, Carson RE, Varga M, Baldwin P, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. Brain (1997) 120:1173–97. doi:10.1093/brain/120.7.1173

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

159. Nofzinger EA, Buysse DJ, Miewald JM, Meltzer CC, Price JC, Sembrat RC, et al. Human regional cerebral glucose metabolism during non-rapid eye movement sleep in relation to waking. Brain (2002) 125:1105–15. doi:10.1093/brain/awf103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

160. Maquet P, Péters J-M, Aerts J, Delfiore G, Degueldre C, Luxen A, et al. Functional neuroanatomy of human rapid-eye-movement sleep and dreaming. Nature (1996) 383:163–6. doi:10.1038/383163aO

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

161. Kleim B, Ehring T, Ehlers A. Perceptual processing advantages for trauma-related visual cues in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med (2012) 42:173–81. doi:10.1017/S0033291711001048

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

162. Bourne C, Mackay CE, Holmes EA. The neural basis of flashback formation: the impact of viewing trauma. Psychol Med (2013) 43:1521–32. doi:10.1017/S0033291712002358

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

163. Osuch EA, Benson B, Geraci M, Podell D, Herscovitch P, McCann UD, et al. Regional cerebral blood flow correlated with flashback intensity in patients with posttraumatic stress disorder. Biol Psychiatry (2001) 50:246–53. doi:10.1016/S0006-3223(01)01107-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

164. Whalley MG, Kroes MCW, Huntley Z, Rugg MD, Davis SW, Brewin CR. An fMRI investigation of posttraumatic flashbacks. Brain Cogn (2013) 81:151–9. doi:10.1016/j.bandc.2012.10.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

165. Ffytche DH, Howard RJ, Brammer MJ, David A, Woodruff P, Williams S. The anatomy of conscious vision: an fMRI study of visual hallucinations. Nat Neurosci (1998) 1:738–42. doi:10.1038/3738

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

166. Cardenas VA, Samuelson K, Lenoci M, Studholme C, Neylan TC, Marmar CR, et al. Changes in brain anatomy during the course of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res (2011) 193:93–100. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2011.01.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

167. Amaral DG, Behniea H, Kelly JL. Topographic organization of projections from the amygdala to the visual cortex in the macaque monkey. Neuroscience (2003) 118:1099–120. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(02)01001-1

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

168. Amaral DG, Price JL. Amygdalo-cortical projections in the monkey (Macaca fascicularis). J Comp Neurol (1984) 230:465–96. doi:10.1002/cne.902300402

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

169. Freese JL, Amaral DG. The organization of projections from the amygdala to visual cortical areas TE and V1 in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol (2005) 486:295–317. doi:10.1002/cne.20520

CrossRef Full Text

170. Freese JL, Amaral DG. Synaptic organization of projections from the amygdala to visual cortical areas TE and V1 in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol (2006) 496:655–67. doi:10.1002/cne.20945

CrossRef Full Text

171. Anderson AK, Phelps EA. Lesions of the human amygdala impair enhanced perception of emotionally salient events. Nature (2001) 411:305–9. doi:10.1038/35077083

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

172. Vuilleumier P, Richardson MP, Armony JL, Driver J, Dolan RJ. Distant influences of amygdala lesion on visual cortical activation during emotional face processing. Nat Neurosci (2004) 7:1271–8. doi:10.1038/nn1341

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

173. Chavez CM, McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM. The basolateral amygdala modulates specific sensory memory representations in the cerebral cortex. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2009) 91:382–92. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2008.10.010

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

174. Chavez CM, McGaugh JL, Weinberger NM. Activation of the basolateral amygdala induces long-term enhancement of specific memory representations in the cerebral cortex. Neurobiol Learn Mem (2013) 101:8–18. doi:10.1016/j.nlm.2012.12.013

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

175. Cowell RA, Bussey TJ, Saksida LM. Components of recognition memory: dissociable cognitive processes or just differences in representational complexity? Hippocampus (2010) 20:1245–62. doi:10.1002/hipo.20865

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

176. Nadel L, Hardt O. Update on memory systems and processes. Neuropsychopharmacology (2011) 36:251–73. doi:10.1038/npp.2010.169

CrossRef Full Text

177. de Quervain DJF, Aerni A, Schelling G, Roozendaal B. Glucocorticoids and the regulation of memory in health and disease. Front Neuroendocrinol (2009) 30:358–70. doi:10.1016/j.yfrne.2009.03.002

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

178. Murty VP, Ritchey M, Adcock RA, LaBar KS. fMRI studies of successful emotional memory encoding: a quantitative meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia (2010) 48:3459–69. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.07.030

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

179. Smeets T, Otgaar H, Candel I, Wolf OT. True or false? Memory is differentially affected by stress-induced cortisol elevations and sympathetic activity at consolidation and retrieval. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2008) 33:1378–86. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.07.009

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

180. Dolcos F, LaBar KS, Cabeza R. Remembering one year later: role of the amygdala and the medial temporal lobe memory system in retrieving emotional memories. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2005) 102:2626–31. doi:10.1073/pnas.0409848102

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

181. Buchanan TW, Tranel D, Adolphs R. Impaired memory retrieval correlates with individual differences in cortisol response but not autonomic response. Learn Mem (2006) 13:382–7. doi:10.1101/lm.206306

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

182. de Quervain DJF, Aerni A, Roozendaal B. Preventive effects of ß-adrenoceptor blockade on glucocorticoid-induced memory retrieval deficits. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164:967–9. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.164.6.967

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

183. Het S, Ramlow G, Wolf OT. A meta-analytic review of the effects of acute cortisol administration on human memory. Psychoneuroendocrinology (2005) 30:771–84. doi:10.1016/j.psyneuen.2005.03.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

184. Kuhlmann S, Piel M, Wolf OT. Impaired memory retrieval after psychosocial stress in healthy young men. J Neurosci (2005) 25:2977–82. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5139-04.2005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

185. de Quervain DJF, Henke K, Aerni A, Treyer V, McGaugh JL, Berthold T, et al. Glucocorticoid-induced impairment of declarative memory retrieval is associated with reduced blood flow in the medial temporal lobe. Eur J Neurosci (2003) 17:1296–302. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02542.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

186. Oei NY, Elzinga BM, Wolf OT, de Ruiter MB, Damoiseaux JS, Kuijer JPA, et al. Glucocorticoids decrease hippocampal and prefrontal activation during declarative memory retrieval in young men. Brain Imaging Behav (2007) 1:31–41. doi:10.1007/s11682-007-9003-2

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

187. Gilbertson MW, Shenton ME, Ciszewski A, Kasai K, Lasko NB, Orr SP, et al. Smaller hippocampal volume predicts pathologic vulnerability to psychological trauma. Nat Neurosci (2002) 5:1242–7. doi:10.1038/nn958

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

188. Hopper JW, Frewen PA, van der Kolk BA, Lanius RA. Neural correlates of reexperiencing, avoidance, and dissociation in PTSD: symptom dimensions and emotion dysregulation in responses to script-driven trauma imagery. J Trauma Stress (2007) 20:713–25. doi:10.1002/jts.20284

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

189. Kim MJ, Chey J, Chung A, Bae S, Khang S, Ham B, et al. Diminished rostral anterior cingulate activity in response to threat-related events in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Psychiatr Res (2008) 42:268–77. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2007.02.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

190. Shin LM, Wright CI, Cannistraro PA, Wedig MM, McMullin K, Martis B, et al. A functional magnetic resonance imaging study of amygdala and medial prefrontal cortex responses to overtly presented fearful faces in posttraumatic stress disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2005) 62:273–81. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.62.3.273

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

191. Felmingham K, Kemp A, Williams L, Das P, Hughes G, Peduto A, et al. Changes in anterior cingulate and amygdala after cognitive behavior therapy of posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychol Sci (2007) 18:127–9. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01860.x

CrossRef Full Text

192. Peres JF, Newberg AB, Mercante JP, Simao M, Albuquerque VE, Peres MJ, et al. Cerebral blood flow changes during retrieval of traumatic memories before and after psychotherapy: a SPECT study. Psychol Med (2007) 37:1481–91. doi:10.1017/S003329170700997x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

193. Bush G. Dorsal anterior midcingulate cortex: roles in normal cognition and disruption in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. In: Vogt BA, editor. Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease. New York: Oxford University Press (2009). p. 245–74.

194. Vogt BA. Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease. New York: Oxford University Press (2009).

195. Weston CSE. Another major function of the anterior cingulate cortex: the representation of requirements. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2012) 36:90–110. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.04.014

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

196. Degos JD, da Fonseca N, Gray F, Cesaro P. Severe frontal syndrome associated with infarcts of the left anterior cingulate gyrus and the head of the right caudate nucleus. Brain (1993) 116:1541–8. doi:10.1093/brain/116.6.1541

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

197. Laplane D, Degos JD, Baulac M, Gray F. Bilateral infarction of the anterior cingulate gyri and of the fornices. J Neurol Sci (1981) 51:289–300. doi:10.1016/0022-510X(81)90107-6

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

198. Marshall GA, Monserratt L, Harwood D, Mandelkern M, Cummings JL, Sultzer DL. Positron emission tomography metabolic correlates of apathy in Alzheimer disease. Arch Neurol (2007) 64:1015–20. doi:10.1001/archneur.64.7.1015

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

199. Migneco O, Benoit M, Koulibaly PM, Dygai I, Bertogliati C, Desvignes P, et al. Perfusion brain SPECT and statistical parametric mapping analysis indicate that apathy is a cingulate syndrome: a study in Alzheimer’s disease and nondemented patients. Neuroimage (2001) 13:896–902. doi:10.1006/nimg.2000.0741

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

200. Rosen HJ, Allison SC, Schauer GF, Gorno-Tempini ML, Weiner MW, Miller BL. Neuroanatomical correlates of behavioural disorders in dementia. Brain (2005) 128:2612–25. doi:10.1093/brain/awh628

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

201. Litz BT, Schlenger WE, Weathers FW, Caddell JM, Fairbank JA, La Vange LM. Predictors of emotional numbing in posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress (1997) 10:607–18. doi:10.1002/jts.2490100407

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

202. Marx BP, Brailey K, Proctor SP, MacDonald HZ, Graefe AC, Amoroso P, et al. Association of time since deployment, combat intensity, and posttraumatic stress symptoms with neuropsychological outcomes following Iraq war deployment. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2009) 66:996–1004. doi:10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2009.109

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

203. Moores KA, Clark CR, McFarlane AC, Brown GC, Puce A, Taylor DJ. Abnormal recruitment of working memory updating networks during maintenance of trauma-neutral information in post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res (2008) 163:156–70. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2007.08.011

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

204. Vasterling JJ, Brailey K, Constans JI, Sutker PB. Attention and memory dysfunction in posttraumatic stress disorder. Neuropsychology (1998) 12:125–33. doi:10.1037/0894-4105.12.1.125

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

205. Frewen PA, Dozois DJA, Neufeld RWJ, Lane RD, Densmore M, Stevens TK, et al. Emotional numbing in posttraumatic stress disorder: a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. J Clin Psychiatry (2012) 73:431–6. doi:10.4088/JCP.10m06477

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

206. Stefanacci L, Amaral DG. Some observations on cortical inputs to the macaque monkey amygdala: an anterograde tracing study. J Comp Neurol (2002) 451:301–23. doi:10.1002/cne.10339

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

207. Garcia R, Vouimba R-M, Baudry M, Thompson RF. The amygdala modulates prefrontal cortex activity relative to conditioned fear. Nature (1999) 402:294–6. doi:10.1038/46286

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

208. Pérez-Jaranay JM, Vives F. Electrophysiological study of the response of medial prefrontal cortex neurons to stimulation of the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala in the rat. Brain Res (1991) 564:97–101. doi:10.1016/0006-8993(91)91357-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

209. Felmingham KL, Williams LM, Kemp AH, Rennie C, Gordon E, Bryant RA. Anterior cingulate activity to salient stimuli is modulated by autonomic arousal in posttraumatic stress disorder. Psychiatry Res (2009) 173:59–62. doi:10.1016/j.pscychresns.2008.12.005

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

210. Flack WF, Litz BT, Hsieh FY, Kaloupek DG, Keane TM. Predictors of emotional numbing, revisited: a replication and extension. J Trauma Stress (2000) 13:611–8. doi:10.1023/A:1007806132319

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

211. Palyo SA, Clapp JD, Beck JG, Grant DM, Marques L. Unpacking the relationship between posttraumatic numbing and hyperarousal in a sample of help-seeking motor vehicle accident survivors: replication and extension. J Trauma Stress (2008) 21:235–8. doi:10.1002/jts.20308

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

212. Averill JR. Studies on anger and aggression. Am Psychol (1983) 38:1145–60. doi:10.1037/0003-066x.38.11.1145

CrossRef Full Text

213. Berkowitz L. Affect, aggression, and antisocial behavior. In: Davidson RJ, Scherer KR, Goldsmith HH, editors. Handbook of Affective Sciences. New York: Oxford University Press (2003). p. 804–23.

214. Taft CT, Street AE, Marshall AD, Dowdall DJ, Riggs DS. Posttraumatic stress disorder, anger, and partner abuse among Vietnam combat veterans. J Fam Psychol (2007) 21:270–7. doi:10.1037/0893-3200.21.2.270

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

215. Carmichael ST, Price JL. Limbic connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol (1995) 363:615–41. doi:10.1002/cne.903630408

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

216. Price JL. Connections of orbital cortex. In: Zald DH, Rauch SL, editors. The Orbitofrontal Cortex. New York: Oxford University Press (2006). p. 39–55.

217. Siever LJ. Neurobiology of aggression and violence. Am J Psychiatry (2008) 165:429–42. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2008.07111774

CrossRef Full Text

218. Peters F, Perani D, Herholz K, Holthoff V, Beuthien-Baumann B, Sorbi S, et al. Orbitofrontal dysfunction related to both apathy and disinhibition in frontotemporal dementia. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord (2006) 21:373–9. doi:10.1159/000091898

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

219. Blair RJR, Cipolotti L. Impaired social response reversal: a case of acquired sociopathy. Brain (2000) 123:1122–41. doi:10.1093/brain/123.6.1122

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

220. Grafman J, Schwab K, Warden D, Pridgen A, Brown HR, Salazar AM. Frontal lobe injuries, violence, and aggression: a report of the Vietnam Head Injury Study. Neurology (1996) 46:1231–8. doi:10.1212/wnl.46.5.1231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

221. Dougherty DD, Rauch SL, Deckersbach T, Marci C, Loh R, Shin LM, et al. Ventromedial prefrontal cortex and amygdala dysfunction during an anger induction positron emission tomography study in patients with major depressive disorder with anger attacks. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2004) 61:795–804. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.61.8.795

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

222. Fabiansson EC, Denson TF, Moulds ML, Grisham JR, Schira MM. Don’t look back in anger: neural correlates of reappraisal, analytical rumination, and angry rumination during recall of an anger-inducing autobiographical memory. Neuroimage (2012) 59:2974–81. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.078

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

223. Elbogen EB, Wagner HR, Fuller SR, Calhoun PS, Kinneer PM, Mid-Atlantic Mental Illness Research, et al. Correlates of anger and hostility in Iraq and Afghanistan war veterans. Am J Psychiatry (2010) 167:1051–8. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2010.09050739

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

224. Savarese VW, Suvak MK, King LA, King DW. Relationships among alcohol use, hyperarousal, and marital abuse and violence in Vietnam veterans. J Trauma Stress (2001) 14:717–32. doi:10.1023/A:1013038021175

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

225. Strom TQ. An exploratory examination of risk-taking behavior and PTSD symptom severity in a veteran sample. Mil Med (2012) 177:390–6. doi:10.7205/MILMED-D-11-00133

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

226. Clark L, Bechara A, Damasio H, Aitken MRF, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Differential effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions on risky decision-making. Brain (2008) 131:1311–22. doi:10.1093/brain/awn066

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

227. Wheeler EZ, Fellows LK. The human ventromedial frontal lobe is critical for learning from negative feedback. Brain (2008) 131:1323–31. doi:10.1093/brain/awn041

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

228. Gauriau C, Bernard J-F. Pain pathways and parabrachial circuits in the rat. Exp Physiol (2002) 87:251–8. doi:10.1113/eph8702357

CrossRef Full Text

229. Saper CB. Pain as a visceral sensation. Prog Brain Res (2000) 122:237–243. doi:10.1016/S0079-6123(08)62142-1

CrossRef Full Text

230. Apkarian AV, Bushnell MC, Treede R-D, Zubieta J-K. Human brain mechanisms of pain perception and regulation in health and disease. Eur J Pain (2005) 9:463–84. doi:10.1016/j.ejpain.2004.11.001

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

231. Bingel U, Quante M, Knab R, Bromm B, Weiller C, Büchel C. Subcortical structures involved in pain processing: evidence from single-trial fMRI. Pain (2002) 99:313–21. doi:10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00157-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

232. Bornhövd K, Quante M, Glauche V, Bromm B, Weiller C, Büchel C. Painful stimuli evoke different stimulus-response functions in the amygdala, prefrontal, insula and somatosensory cortex; a single-trial fMRI study. Brain (2002) 125:1326–36. doi:10.1093/brain/awf137

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

233. Schneider F, Habel U, Holthusen H, Kessler C, Posse S, Müller-Gärtner H-W, et al. Subjective ratings of pain correlate with subcortical-limbic blood flow: an fMRI study. Neuropsychobiology (2001) 43:175–85. doi:10.1159/000054887

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

234. Manning BH, Martin WJ, Meng ID. The rodent amygdala contributes to the production of cannabinoid-induced antinociception. Neuroscience (2003) 120:1157–70. doi:10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00356-7

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

235. Manning BH, Merin NM, Meng ID, Amaral DG. Reduction in opioid- and cannabinoid-induced antinociception in rhesus monkeys after bilateral lesions of the amygdaloid complex. J Neurosci (2001) 21:8238–46.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

236. Bird GC, Lash LL, Han JS, Zou X, Willis WD, Neugebauer V. Protein kinase A-dependent enhanced NMDA receptor function in pain-related synaptic plasticity in rat amygdala neurones. J Physiol (2005) 564:907–21. doi:10.1113/jphysiol.2005.084780

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

237. Han JS, Neugebauer V. Synaptic plasticity in the amygdala in a visceral pain model in rats. Neurosci Lett (2004) 361:254–7. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2003.12.027

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

238. Ikeda R, Takahashi Y, Inoue K, Kato F. NMDA receptor-independent synaptic plasticity in the central amygdala in the rat model of neuropathic pain. Pain (2007) 127:161–72. doi:10.1016/j.pain.2006.09.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

239. Eisenberger NI. The pain of social disconnection: examining the shared neural underpinnings of physical and social pain. Nat Rev Neurosci (2012) 13:421–34. doi:10.1038/nrn3231

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

240. Kross E, Berman MG, Mischel W, Smith EE, Wager TD. Social rejection shares somatosensory representations with physical pain. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2011) 108:6270–5. doi:10.1073/pnas.1102693108

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

241. Mee S, Bunney BG, Reist C, Potkin SG, Bunney WE. Psychological pain: a review of evidence. J Psychiatr Res (2006) 40:680–90. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2006.03.003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

242. Tettamanti M, Rognoni E, Cafiero R, Costa T, Galati D, Perani D. Distinct pathways of neural coupling for different basic emotions. Neuroimage (2012) 59:1804–17. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.08.018

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

243. Wendt J, Lotze M, Weike AI, Hosten N, Hamm AO. Brain activation and defensive response mobilization during sustained exposure to phobia-related and other affective pictures in spider phobia. Psychophysiology (2008) 45:205–15. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00620.x

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

244. Lehmann H, Treit D, Parent MB. Spared anterograde memory for shock-probe fear conditioning after inactivation of the amygdala. Learn Mem (2003) 10:261–9. doi:10.1101/lm.54103

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

245. Vazdarjanova A, McGaugh JL. Basolateral amygdala is not critical for cognitive memory of contextual fear conditioning. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (1998) 95:15003–7. doi:10.1073/pnas.95.25.15003

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

246. Anderson AK, Phelps EA. Is the human amygdala critical for the subjective experience of emotion? Evidence of intact dispositional affect in patients with amygdala lesions. J Cogn Neurosci (2002) 14:709–20. doi:10.1162/08989290260138618

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

247. Jacobson R. Disorders of facial recognition, social behaviour and affect after combined bilateral amygdalotomy and subcaudate tractotomy – a clinical and experimental study. Psychol Med (1986) 16:439–50. doi:10.1017/S0033291700009272

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

248. Craske MG, Rauch SL, Ursano R, Prenoveau J, Pine DS, Zinbarg RE. What is an anxiety disorder? Depress Anxiety (2009) 26:1066–85. doi:10.1002/da.20633

CrossRef Full Text

249. Concise Medical Dictionary. 8th ed. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2010). 272 p.

250. Rachman SJ. Anxiety. Hove: Psychology Press (1998).

251. Bourne PG, Coli WM, Datel WE. Affect levels of ten Special Forces soldiers under threat of attack. Psychol Rep (1968) 22:363–6. doi:10.2466/pr0.1968.22.2.363

CrossRef Full Text

252. Bourne PG, Rose RM, Mason JW. 17-OHCS levels in combat. Arch Gen Psychiatry (1968) 19:135–40. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.1968.01740080007002

CrossRef Full Text

253. Rachman SJ. Fear and fearlessness among trainee parachutists. Adv Behav Res Ther (1983) 4:153–60. doi:10.1016/0146-6402(83)90025-5

CrossRef Full Text

254. Di Nardo PA, Guzy LT, Jenkins JA, Bak RM, Tomasi SF, Copland M. Etiology and maintenance of dog fears. Behav Res Ther (1988) 26:241–4. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(88)90005-8

CrossRef Full Text

255. Doogan S, Thomas GV. Origins of fear of dogs in adults and children: the role of conditioning processes and prior familiarity with dogs. Behav Res Ther (1992) 30:387–94. doi:10.1016/0005-7967(92)90050-Q

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

256. Bandura A, Taylor CB, Williams SL, Mefford IN, Barchas JD. Catecholamine secretion as a function of perceived coping self-efficacy. J Consult Clin Psychol (1985) 53:406–14. doi:10.1037/0022-006x.53.3.406

CrossRef Full Text

257. Lang PJ, Levin DN, Miller GA, Kozak MJ. Fear behavior, fear imagery, and the psychophysiology of emotion: the problem of affective response integration. J Abnorm Psychol (1983) 92:276–306. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.92.3.276

CrossRef Full Text

258. Contreras M, Ceric F, Torrealba F. Inactivation of the interoceptive insula disrupts drug craving and malaise induced by lithium. Science (2007) 318:655–8. doi:10.1126/science.1145590

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

259. Craig AD. How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat Rev Neurosci (2002) 3:655–66. doi:10.1038/nrn894

CrossRef Full Text

260. Dosenbach NU, Visscher KM, Palmer ED, Miezin FM, Wenger KK, Kang HC, et al. A core system for the implementation of task sets. Neuron (2006) 50:799–812. doi:10.1016/j.neuron.2006.04.031

CrossRef Full Text

261. Dosenbach NU, Fair DA, Miezin FM, Cohen AL, Wenger KK, Dosenbach RA, et al. Distinct brain networks for adaptive and stable task control in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A (2007) 104:11073–8. doi:10.1073/pnas.0704320104

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

262. Galea MP, Darian-Smith I. Multiple corticospinal neuron populations in the macaque monkey are specified by their unique cortical origins, spinal terminations, and connections. Cereb Cortex (1994) 4:166–94. doi:10.1093/cercor/4.2.166

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

263. Deen B, Pitskel NB, Pelphrey KA. Three systems of insular functional connectivity identified with cluster analysis. Cereb Cortex (2011) 21:1498–506. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhq186

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

264. Morecraft RJ, Van Hoesen GW. Convergence of limbic input to the cingulate motor cortex in the rhesus monkey. Brain Res Bull (1998) 45:209–32. doi:10.1016/S0361-9230(97)00344-4

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

265. Taylor KS, Seminowicz DA, Davis KD. Two systems of resting state connectivity between the insula and cingulate cortex. Hum Brain Mapp (2009) 30:2731–45. doi:10.1002/hbm.20705

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

266. Mechias M-L, Etkin A, Kalisch R. A meta-analysis of instructed fear studies: implications for conscious appraisal of threat. Neuroimage (2010) 49:1760–8. doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.09.040

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

267. Morecraft RJ, Stilwell-Morecraft KS, Rossing WR. The motor cortex and facial expression: new insights from neuroscience. Neurologist (2004) 10:235–49. doi:10.1097/01.nrl.0000138734.45742.8d

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

268. Griffin DR. Animal Thinking. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press (1984).

269. Cechetto DF, Saper CB. Role of the cerebral cortex in autonomic function. In: Loewy AD, Spyer KM, editors. Central Regulation of Autonomic Functions. Oxford: Oxford University Press (1990). p. 208–23.

270. Caseras X, Murphy K, Mataix-Cols D, Lopez-Sola M, Soriano-Mas C, Ortriz H, et al. Anatomical and functional overlap within the insula and anterior cingulate cortex during interoception and phobic symptom provocation. Hum Brain Mapp (2013) 34:1220–9. doi:10.1002/hbm.21503

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

271. Morecraft RJ, Tanji J. Cingulofrontal interactions and the cingulate motor areas. In: Vogt BA, editor. Cingulate Neurobiology and Disease. New York: Oxford University Press (2009). p. 113–44.

272. Frot M, Mauguière F, Magnin M, Garcia-Larrea L. Parallel processing of nociceptive A-δ inputs in SII and midcingulate cortex in humans. J Neurosci (2008) 28:944–52. doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2934-07.2008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

273. Uhelski ML, Morris-Bobzean SA, Dennis TS, Perrotti LI, Fuchs PN. Evaluating underlying neuronal activity associated with escape/avoidance behavior in response to noxious stimulation in adult rats. Brain Res (2012) 1433:56–61. doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.11.016

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

274. Vogt BA. Pain and emotion interactions in subregions of the cingulate gyrus. Nat Rev Neurosci (2005) 6:533–44. doi:10.1038/nrn1704

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

275. Price JL. Definition of the orbital cortex in relation to specific connections with limbic and visceral structures and other cortical regions. Ann N Y Acad Sci (2007) 1121:54–71. doi:10.1196/annals.1401.008

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

276. Shackman AJ, Salomons TV, Slagter HA, Fox AS, Winter JJ, Davidson RJ. The integration of negative affect, pain, and cognitive control in the cingulate cortex. Nat Rev Neurosci (2011) 12:154–67. doi:10.1038/nrn2994

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

277. Steenkamp MM, Nickerson A, Maguen S, Dickstein BD, Nash WP, Litz BT. Latent classes of PTSD symptoms in Vietnam veterans. Behav Modif (2012) 36:857–74. doi:10.1177/0145445512450908

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

278. Mellman TA, Ramos J, David D, Williams L, Augenstein JS. Possible inhibition of early PTSD symptoms by alcohol intoxication. Depress Anxiety (1998) 7:145. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1520-6394(1998)

CrossRef Full Text

279. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association (1994).

280. Langevin J-P, De Salles AAF, Kosoyan HP, Krahl SE. Deep brain stimulation of the amygdala alleviates post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms in a rat model. J Psychiatr Res (2010) 44:1241–5. doi:10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.04.022

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

281. Friedman MJ. Toward rational pharmacotherapy for posttraumatic stress disorder: reprise. Am J Psychiatry (2013) 170:944–6. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.13060768

CrossRef Full Text

282. Robert PH, Clairet S, Benoit M, Koutaich J, Bertogliati C, Tible O, et al. The Apathy Inventory: assessment of apathy and awareness in Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry (2002) 17:1099–105. doi:10.1002/gps.755

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

283. Starkstein SE, Ingram L, Garau ML, Mizrahi R. On the overlap between apathy and depression in dementia. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry (2005) 76:1070–4. doi:10.1136/jnnp.2004.052795

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

284. Levy R, Dubois B. Apathy and the functional anatomy of the prefrontal cortex – basal ganglia circuits. Cereb Cortex (2006) 16:916–28. doi:10.1093/cercor/bhj043

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

285. Cohen RA, Kaplan RF, Zuffante P, Moser DJ, Jenkins MA, Salloway S, et al. Alteration of intention and self-initiated action associated with bilateral anterior cingulotomy. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci (1999) 11:444–53.

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text

286. Foltz EL, White LE. Pain “relief” by frontal cingulumotomy. J Neurosurg (1962) 19:89–100. doi:10.3171/jns.1962.19.2.0089

CrossRef Full Text

287. Andrews B, Brewin CR, Philpott R, Stewart L. Delayed-onset posttraumatic stress disorder: a systematic review of the evidence. Am J Psychiatry (2007) 164:1319–26. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.2007.06091491

Pubmed Abstract | Pubmed Full Text | CrossRef Full Text

Keywords: PTSD, fear, amygdala, insula, ACC, RDoC

Citation: Weston CSE (2014) Posttraumatic stress disorder: a theoretical model of the hyperarousal subtype. Front. Psychiatry 5:37. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2014.00037

Received: 20 February 2014; Paper pending published: 13 March 2014;
Accepted: 20 March 2014; Published online: 04 April 2014.

Edited by:

Michael Noll-Hussong, University Ulm, Germany

Reviewed by:

Seth Davin Norrholm, Emory University School of Medicine, USA
Casimiro Cabrera Abreu, Queen’s University and Providence Care, Canada

Copyright: © 2014 Weston. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Charles Stewart E. Weston, 4 Ashley Piece, Ramsbury, Marlborough, Wiltshire SN8 2QE, UK e-mail: weston_charles@hotmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.