
94% of researchers rate our articles as excellent or good
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.
Find out more
GENERAL COMMENTARY article
Front. Psychiatry, 03 January 2013
Sec. Addictive Disorders
Volume 3 - 2012 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00113
A commentary on
Neural correlates of moral sensitivity in obsessive compulsive disorder
by Harrison, B. J., Pujol, J., Soriano-Mas, C., Hernández-Ribas, R., López-Solà, M., Ortiz, H., et al. (2012). Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 741–749.
In a recent issue of Archives of General Psychiatry, Harrison et al. (2012) provided the first evidence of neural correlates on moral sensitivity in Obsessive Compulsive Disorder (OCD), which is believed to be heightened in this clinical population. This study aimed to explore whether subjects with OCD showed increased ventromedial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortex responses in a functional magnetic resonance imaging study of difficult moral decision-making.
These brain areas were found to be active on healthy humans experiencing moral emotions such as indignation or moral disgust (see Moll et al., 2005 for a complete review).
The results are striking in so far as they show that patients with OCD demonstrated significantly increased activation of the ventral frontal cortex, particularly of the medial orbitofrontal cortex. Moreover, significant positive associations were documented between the patients' DY-BOCS ratings of total symptom severity and the activation of the anterior insula (Harrison et al., 2012).
The evidence from this study offers the opportunity to discuss two emerging related issues.
First, it adds further support on a clinical model to the view that visceral and moral disgust share, at least in part, common neural and cognitive mechanisms (Jones, 2007). In fact, OCD is also characterized by an altered representation of disgust at sensory (Tsao and McKay, 2004) and affective (Shapira et al., 2003) level.
Thus, their heightened moral sensitivity might reflect an enhanced disgust for immoral outcomes.
This hypothesis is supported by previous evidence coming from neuroimaging studies. For instance, an overlapped recruitment of ventromedial prefrontal and orbitofrontal cortices have already been reported during the processing of both sensorial and moral disgust (Moll et al., 2005).
A similar result was announced for the insula. For example, Wicker et al. (2003) have discovered that the anterior insula was activated both during the observation of disgusted facial expressions and during the feeling of disgust evoked by unpleasant odors. Moreover, Sanfey et al. (2003) found an activation of this region in healthy subjects who received unfair monetary offers, which are known to elicit moral disgust.
All these findings suggest that, the heightened moral sensitivity in OCD can be grounded on the same neural mechanisms responsible for their altered sensitivity to outcomes which can induce sensory and emotional disgust (Shapira et al., 2003; Tsao and McKay, 2004).
Another remarkable aspect deserving some discussion is that OCD participants were not different from healthy control subjects in the subjective rating of the moral dilemmas, despite the difference reported in neural activation. This result, which probably argues some distinction between the patients' increased neural responses and their perceived emotional experience during a moral dilemma (Harrison et al., 2012), can be discussed calling into question the interoceptive awareness which might be deranged in this clinical population.
This suggestion is supported by the recent evidence of a negative correlation between interoception and anxiety (Pollatos et al., 2009), which is known to affects OCD. Moreover, Zaki et al. (2012) have recently found a common cluster of activation for interoception and emotional experience in the anterior insula, a region clearly deranged in this clinical population (Song et al., 2011).
Harrison, B. J., Pujol, J., Soriano-Mas, C., Hernández-Ribas, R., López-Solà, M., Ortiz, H., et al. (2012). Neural correlates of moral sensitivity in obsessive compulsive disorder. Arch. Gen. Psychiatry 69, 741–749.
Moll, J., Zahn, R., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Krueger, F., and Grafman, J. (2005). Opinion: the neural basis of human moral cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 6, 799–809.
Pollatos, O., Traut-Mattausch, E., and Schandry, R. (2009). Differential effects of anxiety and depression on interoceptive accuracy. Depress. Anxiety 26, 167–173.
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., and Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science 300, 1755–1758.
Shapira, N. A., Liu, Y., He, A. G., Bradley, M. M., Lessig, M. C., James, G. A., et al. (2003). Brain activation by disgust-inducing pictures in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Biol. Psychiatry 54, 751–756.
Song, A., Jung, W. H., Jang, J. H., Kim, E., Shim, G., Park, H. Y., et al. (2011). Disproportionate alterations in the anterior and posterior insular cortices in obsessive-compulsive disorder. PLoS ONE 6:e22361. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0022361
Tsao, S. D., and McKay, D. (2004). Behavioral avoidance tests and disgust in contamination fears: distinctions from trait anxiety. Behav. Res. Ther. 42, 207–216.
Wicker, B., Keysers, C., Plailly, J., Royet, J. P., Gallese, V., and Rizzolatti, G. (2003). Both of us disgusted in My insula: the common neural basis of seeing and feeling disgust. Neuron 40, 655–664.
Citation: Vicario CM (2013) Morality and disgust: insights from obsessive compulsive disorder. Front. Psychiatry 3:113. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2012.00113
Received: 07 December 2012; Accepted: 14 December 2012;
Published online: 03 January 2013.
Edited by:
Ramesh C. Gupta, Nagaland University India, IndiaCopyright © 2013 Vicario. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits use, distribution and reproduction in other forums, provided the original authors and source are credited and subject to any copyright notices concerning any third-party graphics etc.
*Correspondence:Y2FybWVsby52aWNhcmlvQHVuaXJvbWExLml0
Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Research integrity at Frontiers
Learn more about the work of our research integrity team to safeguard the quality of each article we publish.