- 1Department of Political Sciences, University of Perugia, Perugia, Italy
- 2Department of Neurosciences, Imaging, and Clinical Sciences, University of Chieti-Pescara “G. D'Annunzio”, Chieti, Italy
- 3Department of Humanities, University of Calabria, Rende, Italy
- 4Department of Neuroscience, Reproductive Sciences, and Dentistry, University of Naples Federico II, Naples, Italy
- 5Department of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY, United States
Theory and research in social, evolutionary, and political psychology indicates that subjective feelings of threat and exposure to objectively threatening circumstances—including pandemic diseases—may contribute to increased affinities for political conservatism, right-wing authoritarianism, and prejudice against out-group members. We investigated these possibilities in the context of Italy, which was the first Western country to be severely affected by the spread of COVID-19. Early on in the pandemic, from March 3–8, 2020, we surveyed 757 Italian adults ranging in age from 18 to 78 years. Results revealed that antipathy toward immigrants and sexual minorities was predicted by (male) sex, COVID-19 anxiety, RWA, and political distrust. Furthermore, COVID-19 anxiety magnified the effect of RWA on disliking of immigrants and sexual minorities (but not obese or disabled people). Contrary to prediction, political trust failed to attenuate the effects of COVID-19 anxiety or RWA on out-group antipathy. Implications for the theories of right-wing authoritarianism and political ideology as motivated social cognition, as well as the state of contemporary Italian politics are discussed.
“It is a cultural fact that China has paid a big price for this epidemic because we have seen them all eat mice live or things like that.”
(Luca Zaia, Governor of the Veneto Region of Italy).
Introduction
In December of 2019, a novel coronavirus (SARS-COV-2) was identified in China and for more than 2 years, COVID-19, the related disease, has been spreading globally. Italy was the first Western country affected by the outbreak of epidemic, with more than 150,000 Italian deaths as of February 2022. The spread of pandemic diseases not only poses a severe threat for the health and wellbeing of individuals and families; it threatens the social fabric of society as well. Throughout human history, xenophobia has often followed the outbreak of disease (e.g., Parmet, 2007; Prati and Pietrantoni, 2016; Clissold et al., 2020; Huo, 2020). So, too, has homophobia—and not only in the case of AIDS (Smith, 1998; Snowden, 2019). According to Altay (2022), the COVID-19 pandemic has worsened prejudice against sexual minorities in Turkey.
There is a great deal of psychological research demonstrating that exposure to circumstances that are highly threatening and uncertainty-inducing—such as terrorist attacks and pandemic diseases—triggers an affinity for authoritarianism and right-wing politics (e.g., Echebarria-Echabe and Fernández-Guede, 2006; Onraet et al., 2013; Beall et al., 2016; Jost et al., 2017; Schaller et al., 2017; O'Shea et al., 2022). For example, Godefroidt (2022) meta-analyzed 728 cross-national tests of the hypothesis that terrorist threat would trigger conservative, right-wing shift and found clear and consistent support for this hypothesis (r = 0.13, p < 0.001), as well as support for the hypotheses that terrorist threats would increase out-group hostility (r = 0.12, p < 0.001) and rally ‘round the flag effects, that is, support for whichever leaders happen to be in power (r = 0.09, p < 0.001).
Of course, not all threats are likely to have the same political consequences. Surely it matters how one measures threat, what type of threat it is, and what the political situation in a given country is (e.g., Brandt et al., 2021; Brandt and Bakker, 2022). Eadeh and Chang (2020) proposed that public health crises should increase support for liberal-socialist policies that include expansions of affordable health care. Their idea, which is shared by Brandt et al. (2021), is that people behave rationally by attempting to calculate which political leaders are most likely to solve a particular problem. However, studies focusing on reactions the Ebola outbreak in 2014 found the opposite: at least in the US, the threat of Ebola appears to have made people more conservative—not more liberal (Beall et al., 2016; Schaller et al., 2017). This suggest that people may not always behave rationally, at least in the way in which Eadeh and Chang and Brandt et al. have suggested.
With regard to COVID-19 in particular, there is some evidence that the outbreak may have increased authoritarianism (Fischer et al., 2020; Roccato et al., 2020, 2021; Cavazza et al., 2021) and out-group prejudice (Huo, 2020) and moved people to the political right in the US and elsewhere (Karwowski et al., 2020; Golec de Zavala et al., 2021). Findings such as these are thought to support the pathogen avoidance theory of evolutionary psychology, which suggests that fear of diseases prompts in-group cohesion, adherence to tradition, conformity, and out-group prejudice (Faulkner et al., 2004; Murray et al., 2013; Green et al., 2016; Tybur et al., 2016; Zmigrod et al., 2021). In addition, sexual disgust—which, for many heterosexuals, may be elicited by gay men, lesbians, bisexuals, and other sexual minorities, who may be stereotyped as sexually promiscuous, and therefore more likely to spread disease (Pinsof and Haselton, 2016)—appears to play a significant role in social conservatism (e.g., Billingsley et al., 2018). According to the Gallup Poll, the percentage of Americans who identified as politically conservative rose from 37% in 2019 to 40% in January/February of 2020, but this change was apparently short-lived, lasting only a few months (Saad, 2020). Still other research suggests that coronavirus deaths strengthened support for the current head of government—whether left or right—in Australia, Canada, France, Germany, Hong Kong, India, and the UK (Yam et al., 2020).
The goal of the present research program was to understand how the COVID outbreak affected Italian politics in the early days of the pandemic (see also Roccato et al., 2020, 2021). The Italian context is an especially interesting one in which to investigate these dynamics, and not only because it was the first Western country to be significantly affected by COVID-19. At the time of the outbreak, the Italian government was a divided one. The Prime Minster was Giuseppe Conte, a center-right politician from the Five Star Party, who had lost his coalition with the far-right Lega Party in September 2019 and was in coalition with three parties on the left when coronavirus struck. In some sense, then, Italy could have moved further to the left (Eadeh and Chang, 2020; Brandt et al., 2021) or, consistent with the theory of political ideology as motivated social cognition, back to the right in an effort to assuage psychological needs to reduce uncertainty and threat (see Jost, 2021).
The COVID outbreak in Italy progressed in three stages. From December of 2019 to February 17, 2020, Italy had no confirmed cases of infection, except for two Chinese tourists who were hospitalized in Rome on January 29. The outbreak was described as a Chinese issue, and no restrictions were imposed on Italian citizens. The second phase, which lasted from February 18 to March 9, 2020, began with the certification of the first Italian case of infection, which occurred in Codogno, a small city in Lombardy. Other infections in Italians were reported in the following days, with some cities and provinces in Northern Italy isolated. On March 8, the entire region of Lombardy and other Northern provinces were suddenly declared “red zones.” The third phase, from March 9 to May 18, began with a government decree imposing a nationwide lockdown. We collected data during the second phase of the outbreak in Italy, from March 3–7, 2020, shortly before the national lockdown. This was during a phase of great chaos, because COVID-19 cases were increasing daily, and very little was known about the disease.
In the present study, we investigated whether, and if so, how anxiety elicited by the sudden emergence of COVID-19—in relation to right-wing authoritarianism and individual differences in levels of political trust—affected prejudice against out-groups. We focused especially on immigrants and sexual minorities because both of these groups are frequently the targets of prejudice among rightists in Italy and other European nations, especially during periods of high uncertainty and threat (e.g., Echebarria-Echabe and Fernández-Guede, 2006; Roets and Van Hiel, 2011; Echebarria-Echabe, 2013; Procellato, 2019; Dhanani and Franz, 2021; Golec de Zavala et al., 2021). We also measured attitudes toward obese and disabled people, but presumably these target groups are perceived as less threatening—both physically (in terms of the spread of disease) and socially (in terms of threats to the status quo)—compared to immigrants and sexual minorities, and they are also less likely to be victims of prejudice in Europe. Following studies conducted by Hartman et al. (2021) in the UK and Republic of Ireland, we investigated the hypothesis that COVID anxiety would intensify the effects of right-wing authoritarianism on out-group prejudice.
We also hypothesized that political trust would attenuate the effects of COVID anxiety on disliking of out-group members, even among people who were higher in terms of right-wing authoritarianism. This is because trust is positively associated with support for democratic values and negatively associated with right-wing authoritarianism and out-group prejudice (e.g., Dhont and Van Hiel, 2011; Miklikowska, 2012). We also anticipated that trust in political authorities to handle the crisis might assuage popular anxiety and prevent it from being channeled in destructive directions, as in the scapegoating of minorities. Trust is especially important when it comes to situations of high uncertainty and threat, such as the spread of pandemic disease (Renn and Levine, 1991; Blair et al., 2017; Paolini et al., 2020). During the Liberian Ebola outbreak in 2014–15, for instance, citizens who trusted health officials were more willing to follow public health recommendations than those who did not (Tsai et al., 2020). Cross-cultural research has confirmed that trust in government is robustly associated with compliance with COVID-19 rules (Pagliaro et al., 2021).
In summary, then, we explored the following five hypotheses in the context of the early days of the coronavirus pandemic in Italy: (H1) Anxiety about COVID-19 would be positively associated with antipathy toward immigrants and sexual minorities; (H2) Right-wing authoritarianism (RWA) would be positively associated with antipathy toward immigrants and sexual minorities; (H3) Political trust would be negatively associated with antipathy toward immigrants and sexual minorities; (H4) Anxiety about COVID-19 would magnify the positive association between RWA and antipathy toward immigrants and sexual minorities; and (H5) Political trust would diminish the positive association between RWA and antipathy toward immigrants and sexual minorities.
Method
Like the majority of studies conducted during the pandemic's first wave, we administered an online, cross-sectional survey and recruited participants using a snowball sampling procedure. The survey was advertised by the Italian members of the research team, who used their own social networking sites as well as those of the laboratories they directed. The invitation to participate established a start date (March 3, 2020) and an end date (March 7, 2020). The decision was made to recruit a minimum of 500 participants.
We surveyed residents of every Italian region via public online regional groups and successfully recruited individuals from each of the twenty regions. Seven hundred and fifty-seven Italian participants completed the survey (183 males; 571 females, 3 transgender). Participants ranged in age from 18 to 78 years old (M = 34.96, SD = 11.88). In terms of education, 27.1% completed high school (only), and 71.6% earned a college degree. We measured subjective socioeconomic status by asking participants to use a scale ranging from 1 (“extremely low”) to 5 (“extremely high”) to answer the following question: “How would you define your socioeconomic status?” Three-fourths (74.8 %) identified themselves as having a medium socioeconomic status. After completing the questionnaire anonymously, participants were thanked and fully debriefed.1
To measure political trust, we asked participants to “Please think about the morality of different social actors” and to indicate on a scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much) the extent to which they considered “institutions” and “politicians” to be moral. We then averaged responses to these two targets to create a composite political trust score (r = 0.47, p < 0.001).
We measured participants' COVID-19 contagion anxiety using 16 items adapted from previous research by Wong et al. (2007) during an earlier SARS outbreak in Hong Kong. On a 4-point scale ranging from “strongly disagree” to “strongly agree,” participants were asked to think about their emotions related to the COVID-19 pandemic in Italy and to answer questions about how fearful and anxious they felt (alpha = 0.89).
Participants completed a widely used 10-item scale to measure right-wing authoritarianism (or RWA) in the Italian language (Roccato and Russo, 2015). A sample item was “Obedience and discipline serve above all to live well.” Responses were provided on a scale ranging from 1 (completely disagree) to 4 (completely agree). For this sample scale reliability was satisfactory (alpha = 0.72).
Finally, participants completed feeling thermometers so that we could gauge out-group antipathy. Specifically, on scales ranging from 0 to 10, they reported how “cold” or “warm” they felt toward immigrants, sexual minorities, obese people, and disabled people. After a reverse-scoring procedure, higher scores reflect less favorable attitudes.
Results
Descriptive statistics and zero-order correlations for all study variables are provided in Table 1. Consistent with (H1), COVID anxiety was significantly correlated with the expression of less favorable attitudes toward immigrants (r = 0.17, p < 0.001) and sexual minorities (r = 0.10, p < 0.01), and it was also correlated with RWA (r = 0.10, p < 0.01). COVID anxiety was not significantly correlated with attitudes toward obese or disabled people. In support of (H2), RWA was strongly correlated with disliking of immigrants (r = 0.50, p < 0.001) and sexual minorities (r = 0.41, p < 0.001), and both types of antipathy were highly correlated with one another (r = 0.53, p < 0.001). RWA was significantly but modestly correlated with disliking of obese people (r = 0.12, p < 0.01), and marginally correlated with disliking of disabled people (r = 0.07, p < 0.10). With respect to (H3), political trust was negatively correlated with COVID anxiety, RWA, and the holding of unfavorable attitudes toward immigrants—but it was uncorrelated with attitudes toward the other three groups.
To further explore the robustness of these associations and to test the interaction hypotheses described in (H4) and (H5) we conducted a series of multiple regression analyses. The results concerning attitudes toward immigrants are summarized in Table 2, and those concerning attitudes toward sexual minorities are summarized in Table 3. After adjusting for socio-demographic variables listed in Model 1, we observed in Model 2 that COVID anxiety and RWA were independently and positively associated with disliking of immigrants and sexual minorities, in further support of (H1) and (H2). In Model 2, political trust was negatively associated with disliking of immigrants (Table 2), consistent with (H3), but it was positively associated with disliking of sexual minorities (Table 3). In Model 3, which includes the interaction terms, the effect of political trust on antipathy toward sexual minorities was negative but only marginally significant (p = 0.083), and it was non-significant in the case of attitudes toward immigrants. Thus, the regression analysis provided only partial support for (H3). These models accounted for 28% of the statistical variance in attitudes toward immigrants and 24% of the variance in attitudes toward sexual minorities.
Table 2. Unfavorable attitudes toward immigrants as a function of demographic variables, COVID-19 anxiety, RWA, and political trust.
Table 3. Unfavorable attitudes toward sexual minorities as a function of demographic variables, COVID-19 anxiety, RWA, and political trust.
In (H4) we predicted that anxiety about the pandemic would magnify the positive association between RWA and out-group dislike. In Model 3, we see that the interaction between COVID anxiety and RWA was indeed statistically significant for disliking of immigrants (b = 1.06, SE = 0.26, ß = 0.71, p < 0.001) and sexual minorities (b = 0.49, SE = 0.21, ß = 0.42, p = 0.021). To further probe the pattern of interactions bearing on (H4) we used the PROCESS macro developed by Hayes (2017). Specifically, we entered RWA as the independent variable, COVID anxiety as a moderating variable, and out-group attitudes as the dependent variables. In both cases, the analysis yielded a clearly identifiable linear trend in which the positive association between RWA and out-group dislike increased in strength at higher levels of the moderating variable (COVID anxiety). For example, the association between RWA and the expression of unfavorable attitudes toward immigrants was b = 1.78 (95% CI: LL = 1.3333; UL = 2.2351) at low levels of COVID-19 anxiety; b = 2.41 (95% CI: LL = 2.0957; UL = 2.7216) at moderate levels of COVID-19 anxiety; and b = 2.96 (95% CI: LL = 2.5792; UL = 3.3482) at high levels of COVID-19 anxiety (see Figure 1). Likewise, the association between RWA and disliking of sexual minorities was b = 1.28 at low levels of COVID-19 anxiety (95% CI: LL = 0.9073; UL = 1.6572); b = 1.56 at moderate levels of COVID-19 anxiety (95% CI: LL = 1.3018; UL = 1.8222); and b = 1.81 (95% CI: LL = 1.4909; UL = 2.1304) at high levels of COVID-19 anxiety (see Figure 2). Thus, (H4) was clearly supported.
In (H5) we predicted that political trust would diminish the positive association between RWA and out-group dislike. As shown in Table 2, the interaction between political trust and RWA was non-significant for attitudes toward immigrants, and so was the interaction between political trust and COVID anxiety. As shown in Table 3, the interaction between political trust and RWA was non-significant for disliking of sexual minorities, but there was a marginal interaction between political trust and COVID anxiety (p = 0.061). Thus, we obtained very little support for (H5).
Discussion
For 2 years and counting, the COVID-19 pandemic has posed a severe threat to public health and elevated levels of uncertainty and threat in the population. At the time of writing, the disease has killed nearly 6 million people worldwide and plunged the global economy into recession. Theory and research in social, evolutionary, and political psychology converge on the prediction that highly threatening circumstances such as these are likely to activate right-wing authoritarian tendencies in the general public, with the predictable result of prejudice against out-group members, especially foreigners and those who are perceived as deviant, such as sexual minorities (e.g., see Altemeyer, 1988; Murray et al., 2013; Green et al., 2016; Tybur et al., 2016; Jost et al., 2017; Schaller et al., 2017; Procellato, 2019; Jost, 2021; O'Shea et al., 2022). Our study, which was conducted in the early days of the COVID-19 outbreak in the first Western country affected by it, suggests that disease-related anxiety and authoritarian dispositions may have interacted to decrease liking for immigrants and sexual minorities. In this respect, we replicated the results of studies conducted in the UK and the Republic of Ireland by Hartman et al. (2021). However, we did not find that attitudes toward obese and disabled people were affected by COVID anxiety, presumably because they were perceived as less threatening than immigrants and sexual minorities.
Of course, political leaders bear some responsibility for stoking and exploiting their followers' authoritarianism and out-group antipathy, as the opening quotation from the Italian politician Luca Zaia suggests. Similarly, the leader of the far-right Italian party, Lega, Matteo Salvini—along with Hungary's Prime Minister Viktor Orbán—linked the virus to asylum-seekers and used the crisis strategically to call for additional border closures. In the US, similar sentiments were expressed by President Donald Trump, who referred to COVID-19 as the “China virus” and “Wuhan virus.” The results of our study suggest that in Italy COVID-related anxiety may have strengthened the association between authoritarianism and disliking of immigrants and sexual minorities, two groups that are often scapegoated by authoritarians in Western societies (e.g., Adorno et al., 1950; Altemeyer, 1988).
Although Trump lost his bid for re-election in 2020, it appears that the right-wing in Italy is again surging. According to a public opinion poll conducted in February of 2022 by the media agency La7, 17% of Italians supported Lega; 21% supported the Brothers of Italy party, which is another far-right party; and nearly 8% supported Forza Italia (former Prime Minister Silvio Berlusconi's party). Only a quarter of Italians supported one of the leftist parties, including the Democratic Party (see https://tg.la7.it/listing/sondaggi). It is noteworthy that in September 2019, only a few months before the novel coronavirus struck, these same leftist parties were invited to join the governing coalition.
Our study also suggests that political trust and perceptions of the government's morality may be relevant to effective leadership during a public health crisis (Giannella et al., 2022). Italians who perceived institutions and politicians as morally trustworthy experienced less anxiety related to the spread of COVID-19 and less antipathy toward immigrants overall. In this sense, trust in the social system may have attenuated some of the most troubling manifestations of system-level crises and facilitate democratic (as opposed to anti-democratic, authoritarian) responses (see also Langer et al., 2022). However, we obtained very little support for the hypothesis that political trust would reduce the effects of RWA and COVID anxiety on disliking of out-group members. It is conceivable that levels of political trust in the left-leaning government were simply too low among those who were high in RWA or out-group antipathy to have much of an effect. The fact that Italy was the first Western country to face the spread of the virus might have contributed to a general feeling of distrust that Italian politicians were incapable of protecting their citizens from the threat. Indeed, a different study conducted in Italy found that COVID-related threats were associated with the tendency to criticize the government's handling of the pandemic and, as a consequence, anti-democratic sentiment (Roccato et al., 2021). In contrast, a study conducted in the US while Trump was President found that confidence in the government to handle COVID-19 was positively (rather than negatively) associated with RWA and xenophobia (Pazhoohi and Kingstone, 2021). Thus, when it comes to understanding and predicting the effects of political trust, it is important to consider the political context, including the president or party that is in power (see also Morisi et al., 2019).
On the topic of trust in government, it is perhaps relevant that many Western commentators praised the Chinese government's response to COVID-19 and have speculated that authoritarian states like China may be better equipped to handle pandemic diseases than their more democratic counterparts. However, this disregards the fact that in the earliest stages of the outbreak, when the possibility of containment existed, misinformation campaigns initiated by the authoritarian government in China—in conjunction with distrust between local and national politicians—caused the virus to spread within China and, ultimately, around the world (Kavanagh, 2020).
The present study was theoretically grounded in well-established frameworks in social, evolutionary, and political psychology. Nevertheless, a clear limitation of our work stems from the cross-sectional nature of the data; it is not possible to draw causal conclusions on the basis of our study. We are reassured, to some extent, by the fact that longitudinal and experimental research has shown that situationally induced threats are indeed capable of eliciting authoritarian and conservative shifts (e.g., see Jost, 2021, p. 73–76, 187–193, 281, 285, and 288). Given the overwhelming media coverage that has rightfully accompanied COVID-19, it would be difficult, but probably not impossible, to successfully manipulate the salience of the threat posed by the disease in the laboratory. And it would be obviously unethical to make research participants believe that they were being exposed to the virus directly in order to observe the effects of anxiety on social and political attitudes, although this is the kind of experiment that would be most internally valid. For all of these reasons, we believe that the research methods we have adopted are sensible and defensible, given the totality of the circumstances.
Another clear limitation of our study is that we used a snowball sampling technique to contact respondents, which is not ideal and surely failed to produce a nationally representative sample, despite the efforts that were made to reach citizens in all regions of the country. Given the sudden and dramatic onset of the pandemic and the challenges it posed, this was the only strategy available to us to collect data about COVID anxiety in a timely manner. Because of strict limits on the length of the survey, we were unable to measure political orientation or potential psychological mediators such as the feeling of disgust, which plays a key role in evolutionary theorizing about disease avoidance and authoritarian prejudice. Nevertheless, many other studies conducted elsewhere have indeed addressed the connections between disgust sensitivity and a wide variety of political attitudes, including attitudes toward immigrants (e.g., Hodson and Costello, 2007; Inbar et al., 2012; Tybur et al., 2016; Aarøe et al., 2017; Shook et al., 2017).
Conclusion
In conclusion, we suggest that the spread of pandemic disease not only represents a threat to public health, but also a potential threat to democratic norms of acceptance, openness, tolerance, civility, pluralism, and cooperation across group boundaries. In Italy, as in other countries, there is reason to worry that the novel coronavirus may have increased the prevalence and impact of right-wing authoritarian tendencies that contribute to ethnocentrism and out-group antipathy. If there is an antidote at hand, it may simply be the enhancement of public trust in those democratic political institutions that are, at the end of the day, responsible for risk management and public health in the West. We can only hope that in the future governments will be able to gain more trust—for the right reasons—than they have thus far.
Author's Note
This manuscript is an invited revision for a special issue of Frontiers in Political Science on “Authoritarianism in the Context of COVID-19,” guest edited by D. Stevens, K. Dunn, M. Jackson, and J. Transue. We thank three anonymous reviewers for helpful comments on an earlier draft.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.
Ethics Statement
The study was designed to respect all the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki on Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects and was approved by the Ethical Committee of the University of Calabria (protocol number 8104). The participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.
Author Contributions
MP, SP, VB, and CS designed the study and contributed to the acquisition of data and had full access to all the data in the study and take responsibility for the integrity of the data. JJ, MP, and SP analyzed and interpreted the data and drafted the manuscript. All authors have read the manuscript and have agreed with its submission.
Conflict of Interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher's Note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
Footnotes
1. ^The survey focused on two major areas of investigation, one pertaining to clinical psychology (about the connection between personality profiles and COVID anxiety) and another focused on social/political psychology (on which this article is based). The two investigations had very different aims and hypotheses; a decision was made to collect data for both together because of the difficulty in surveying people during the emergency in Italy. COVID-related anxiety and RWA were explored in both investigations but from quite different perspectives (see also Bochicchio et al., 2021).
References
Aarøe, L., Petersen, M. B., and Arceneaux, K. (2017). The behavioral immune system shapes political intuitions: why and how individual differences in disgust sensitivity underlie opposition to immigration. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 111, 277–294. doi: 10.1017/S0003055416000770
Adorno, T., Frenkel-Brunswick, E., Levinson, D., and Sanford, R. (1950). The Authoritarian Personality. New York, NY: Harper.
Altay, T. (2022). The pink line across digital publics: Political homophobia and the queer strategies of everyday life during COVID-19 in Turkey. Eur. J. Womens Stud. 1–15. doi: 10.1177/13505068221076329
Altemeyer, B. (1988). Enemies of Freedom: Understanding Right-Wing Authoritarianism. San Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass.
Beall, A. T., Hofer, M. K., and Schaller, M. (2016). Infections and elections: did an Ebola outbreak influence the 2014 US federal elections (and if so, how)? Psychol. Sci. 27, 595–605. doi: 10.1177/0956797616628861
Billingsley, J., Lieberman, D., and Tybur, J. M. (2018). Sexual disgust trumps pathogen disgust in predicting voter behavior during the 2016 US Presidential Election. Evol. Psychol. 16, 1474704918764170. doi: 10.1177/1474704918764170
Blair, R. A., Morse, B. S., and Tsai, L. L. (2017). Public health and public trust: survey evidence from the Ebola Virus disease epidemic in Liberia. Soc. Sci. Med. 172, 89–97. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.11.016
Bochicchio, V., Winsler, A., Pagliaro, S., Pacilli, M. G., Dolce, P., and Scandurra, C. (2021). Negative affectivity, authoritarianism, and anxiety of infection explain early maladjusted behavior during the COVID-19 outbreak. Front. Psychol. 12, 536. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.583883
Brandt, M. J., and Bakker, B. N. (2022). The complicated but solvable threat–politics relationship. Trends Cogn. Sci. 26. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2022.02.005
Brandt, M. J., Turner-Zwinkels, F. M., Karapirinler, B., Van Leeuwen, F., Bender, M., van Osch, Y., et al. (2021). The association between threat and politics depends on the type of threat, the political domain, and the country. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 47, 324–343. doi: 10.1177/0146167220946187
Cavazza, N., Russo, S., Colloca, P., and Roccato, M. (2021). Household crowding can have political effects: an empirical study on support for anti-democratic political systems during the COVID-19 lockdown in Italy. J. Environ. Psychol. 76, 101628. doi: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2021.101628
Clissold, E., Nylander, D., Watson, C., and Ventriglio, A. (2020). Pandemics and prejudice. Int. J. Soc. Psychiatry 66, 421–423. doi: 10.1177/0020764020937873
Dhanani, L. Y., and Franz, B. (2021). Why public health framing matters: an experimental study of the effects of COVID-19 framing on prejudice and xenophobia in the United States. Soc. Sci. Med. 269, 113572. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2020.113572
Dhont, K., and Van Hiel, A. (2011). Direct contact and authoritarianism as moderators between extended contact and reduced prejudice: Lower threat and greater trust as mediators. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 14, 223–237. doi: 10.1177/1368430210391121
Eadeh, F. R., and Chang, K. K. (2020). Can threat increase support for liberalism? New insights into the relationship between threat and political attitudes. Soc. Psychol. Person. Sci. 11, 88–96. doi: 10.1177/1948550618815919
Echebarria-Echabe, A. (2013). Mortality salience and uncertainty: similar effects but different processes? Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 43, 185–191. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.1938
Echebarria-Echabe, A., and Fernández-Guede, E. (2006). Effects of terrorism on attitudes and ideological orientation. Eur. J. Soc. Psychol. 36, 259–265. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.294
Faulkner, J., Schaller, M., Park, J. H., and Duncan, L. A. (2004). Evolved disease-avoidance mechanisms and contemporary xenophobic attitudes. Group Process. Intergroup Relat. 7, 333–353. doi: 10.1177/1368430204046142
Fischer, K., Chaudhuri, A., and Atkinson, Q. (2020). Responses to the COVID-19 pandemic reflect the dual evolutionary foundations of political ideology. PsyArxiv [Preprint]. doi: 10.31234/osf.io/qeap8
Giannella, V. A., Pagliaro, S., and Barreto, M. (2022). Leader's morality, prototypicality, and followers' reactions. Leadership Q. doi: 10.1016/j.leaqua.2021.101596
Godefroidt, A. (2022). How terrorism does (and does not) affect citizens' political attitudes: a meta-analysis. Am. J. Pol. Sci. forthcoming.
Golec de Zavala, A., Bierwiaczonek, K., Baran, T., Keenan, O., and Hase, A. (2021). The COVID-19 pandemic, authoritarianism, and rejection of sexual dissenters in Poland. Psychol. Sex. Orient. Gender Divers. 8, 250–260. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000446
Green, E. G., Sarrasin, O., Baur, R., and Fasel, N. (2016). From stigmatized immigrants to radical right voting: a multilevel study on the role of threat and contact. Polit. Psychol. 37, 465–480. doi: 10.1111/pops.12290
Hartman, T. K., Stocks, T. V., McKay, R., Gibson-Miller, J., Levita, L., Martinez, A. P., et al. (2021). The authoritarian dynamic during the COVID-19 pandemic: effects on nationalism and anti-immigrant sentiment. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 12, 1274–1285. doi: 10.1177/1948550620978023
Hayes, A. F. (2017). Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis (2nd ed.): A Regression-Based Approach. New York, NY: Guilford.
Hodson, G., and Costello, K. (2007). Interpersonal disgust, ideological orientations, and dehumanization as predictors of intergroup attitudes. Psychol. Sci. 18, 691–698. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01962.x
Huo, Y. (2020). “Prejudice and discrimination,” in Together Apart: The Psychology of COVID-19, eds J. Jetten, S. D. Reicher, S. A. Haslam, and T. Cruws (London: Sage), 113–118.
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D., Iyer, R., and Haidt, J. (2012). Disgust sensitivity, political conservatism, and voting. Soc. Psychol. Personal. Sci. 3, 537–544. doi: 10.1177/1948550611429024
Jost, J. T. (2021). Left & Right: The Psychological Significance of a Political Distinction. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Jost, J. T., Stern, C., Rule, N. O., and Sterling, J. (2017). The politics of fear: is there an ideological asymmetry in existential motivation? Soc. Cogn. 35, 324–353. doi: 10.1521/soco.2017.35.4.324
Karwowski, M., Kowal, M., Groyecka, A., Białek, M., Lebuda, I., Sorokowska, A., et al. (2020). When in danger, turn right: does Covid-19 threat promote social conservatism and right-wing presidential candidates? Hum. Ethol. 35, 37–48. doi: 10.22330/he/35/037-048
Kavanagh, M. M. (2020). Authoritarianism, outbreaks, and information politics. Lancet Public Health 5, e135–e136. doi: 10.1016/S2468-2667(20)30030-X
Langer, M., Vasilopoulos, P., and Jost, J. T. (2022). Does system justification promote establishment voting? Mainstream politics in France, Germany, and the United Kingdom. Manuscript submitted for publication.
Miklikowska, M. (2012). Psychological underpinnings of democracy: empathy, authoritarianism, self-esteem, interpersonal trust, normative identity style, and openness to experience as predictors of support for democratic values. Pers. Individ. Dif. 53, 603–608. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2012.04.032
Morisi, D., Jost, J. T., and Singh, V. (2019). An asymmetrical “President-in-Power” effect. Am. Polit. Sci. Rev. 113, 614–620. doi: 10.1017/S0003055418000850
Murray, D. R., Schaller, M., and Suedfeld, P. (2013). Pathogens and politics: further evidence that parasite prevalence predicts authoritarianism. PLoS ONE 8, e62275. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062275
Onraet, E., Van Hiel, A., Dhont, K., and Pattyn, S. (2013). Internal and external threat in relationship with right-wing attitudes. J. Pers. 81, 233–248. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12011
O'Shea, B. A., Vitriol, J. A., Federico, C. M., Appleby, J., and Williams, A. L. (2022). Exposure and aversion to human transmissible diseases predict conservative ideological and partisan preferences. Polit. Psychol. 43, 65–88. doi: 10.1111/pops.12741
Pagliaro, S., Sacchi, S., Pacilli, M. G., Brambilla, M., Lionetti, F., Bettache, K., et al. (2021). Trust predicts COVID-19 prescribed and discretionary behavioral intentions in 23 countries. PLoS ONE 16, e0248334. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248334
Paolini, D., Maricchiolo, F., Pacilli, M. G., and Pagliaro, S. (2020). COVID-19 lockdown in Italy: the role of social identification and social and political trust on well-being and distress. Curr. Psychol. 1–8. doi: 10.1007/s12144-020-01141-0
Parmet, W. E. (2007). Legal power and legal rights—Isolation and quarantine in the case of drug-resistant tuberculosis. N. Engl. J. Med. 357, 433–435. doi: 10.1056/NEJMp078133
Pazhoohi, F., and Kingstone, A. (2021). Associations of political orientation, xenophobia, right-wing authoritarianism, and concern of COVID-19: cognitive responses to an actual pathogen threat. Pers. Individ. Dif. 182, 111081. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.111081
Pinsof, D., and Haselton, M. (2016). The political divide over same-sex marriage: mating strategies in conflict?. Psychol. Sci. 27, 435–442. doi: 10.1177/0956797615621719
Prati, G., and Pietrantoni, L. (2016). Knowledge, risk perceptions, and xenophobic attitudes: evidence from Italy during the Ebola outbreak. Risk Anal. 36, 2000–2010. doi: 10.1111/risa.12537
Procellato, N. (2019). I più discriminati? Rom, immigrati e Musulmani. Available online at: http://www.demos.it/a01672.php (accessed April 04, 2022).
Renn, O., and Levine, D. (1991). “Credibility and trust in risk communication,” in Communicating Risks to the Public, eds R. E. Kasperson, and P. J. M. Stallen (Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic), 175–218.
Roccato, M., Cavazza, N., Colloca, P., and Russo, S. (2020). A democratic emergency after a health emergency? Exposure to COVID-19, perceived economic threat and support for anti-democratic political systems. Soc. Sci. Q. 101, 2193–2202. doi: 10.1111/ssqu.12865
Roccato, M., and Russo, S. (2015). Two short, balanced, unidimensional, invariant across genders parallel forms of Altemeyer's (1996) Right-Wing Authoritarianism Scale. Psicol. Soc. 10, 257–272. doi: 10.1482/81371
Roccato, M., Russo, S., Colloca, P., and Cavazza, N. (2021). The lasting effects of the COVID-19 pandemic on support for anti-democratic political systems: a six-month longitudinal study. Soc. Sci. Q. 102, 2285–2295. doi: 10.1111/ssqu.12958
Roets, A., and Van Hiel, A. (2011). The role of need for closure in essentialist entitativity beliefs and prejudice: an epistemic needs approach to racial categorization. Br. J. Soc. Psychol. 50, 52–73. doi: 10.1348/014466610X491567
Saad, L. (2020). U.S. Conservatism Down Since Start of 2020. Gallup. Available online at: https://news.gallup.com/poll/316094/conservatism-down-start-2020.aspx (accessed April 04, 2022).
Schaller, M., Hofer, M. K., and Beall, A. T. (2017). Evidence that an Ebola outbreak influenced voting preferences, even after controlling (mindfully) for autocorrelation: reply to Tiokhin and Hruschka (2017). Psychol. Sci. 28, 1361–1363. doi: 10.1177/0956797617718183
Shook, N. J., Oosterhoff, B., Terrizzi, J. A J.r, and Brady, K. M. (2017). “Dirty politics”: the role of disgust sensitivity in voting. Transl. Issues Psychol. Sci. 3, 284–297. doi: 10.1037/tps0000111
Smith, R. A. (1998). “GRID (Gay-Related Immune Deficiency),” in Encyclopedia of AIDS: A Social, Political, Cultural, and Scientific Record of the HIV Epidemic, ed R. A. Smith (London: Routledge), 408–410.
Snowden, F. M. (2019). Epidemics and Society: From the Black Death to the Present. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Tsai, L. L., Morse, B. S., and Blair, R. A. (2020). Building credibility and cooperation in low-trust settings: persuasion and source accountability in Liberia during the 2014–2015 Ebola crisis. Comp. Polit. Stud. 53, 1582–1618. doi: 10.1177/0010414019897698
Tybur, J. M., Inbar, Y., Aarøe, L., Barclay, P., Barlow, F. K., De Barra, M., et al. (2016). Parasite stress and pathogen avoidance relate to distinct dimensions of political ideology across 30 nations. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 113, 12408–12413. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1607398113
Wong, T. W., Gao, Y., and Tam, W. S. (2007). Anxiety among university students during the SARS epidemic in Hong Kong. Stress Health 23, 31–35. doi: 10.1002/smi.1116
Yam, K. C., Jackson, J. C., Barnes, C. M., Lau, J., Qin, X., and Lee, H. Y. (2020). The rise of COVID-19 cases is associated with support for world leaders. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci. U. S. A.117, 25429–25433. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2009252117
Keywords: right-wing authoritarianism, antipathy toward immigrants, political trust, pandemic, antipathy toward sexual minorities, COVID-19 anxiety
Citation: Pacilli MG, Pagliaro S, Bochicchio V, Scandurra C and Jost JT (2022) Right-Wing Authoritarianism and Antipathy Toward Immigrants and Sexual Minorities in the Early Days of the Coronavirus Pandemic in Italy. Front. Polit. Sci. 4:879049. doi: 10.3389/fpos.2022.879049
Received: 18 February 2022; Accepted: 20 April 2022;
Published: 01 June 2022.
Edited by:
Melinda S. Jackson, San Jose State University, United StatesReviewed by:
Alexander Jedinger, GESIS Leibniz Institute for the Social Sciences, GermanyEmily Fisher, Hobart and William Smith Colleges, United States
John Duckitt, The University of Auckland, New Zealand
Copyright © 2022 Pacilli, Pagliaro, Bochicchio, Scandurra and Jost. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.
*Correspondence: John T. Jost, am9obi5qb3N0JiN4MDAwNDA7bnl1LmVkdQ==