Skip to main content

PERSPECTIVE article

Front. Physiol., 08 March 2023
Sec. Invertebrate Physiology
This article is part of the Research Topic Cephalopods in the Anthropocene: Multiple Challenges in a Changing Ocean View all 16 articles

Cuttlefish color change as an emerging proxy for ecotoxicology

  • 1Ethologie Animale et Humaine (EthoS), UMR 6552 CNRS, Université Caen Normandie, Caen, France
  • 2Stress Environnementaux et Biosurveillance des Milieux Aquatiques (SEBIO), UMR-I 02, Université Le Havre Normandie, Le Havre, France
  • 3Littoral Environnement et Sociétés (LIENSs), UMR 7266 CNRS-La Rochelle Université, La Rochelle, France

Lately, behavioral ecotoxicology has flourished because of increasing standardization of analyses of endpoints like movement. However, research tends to focus on a few model species, which limits possibilities of extrapolating and predicting toxicological effects and adverse outcomes at the population and ecosystem level. In this regard, it is recommended to assess critical species-specific behavioral responses in taxa playing key roles in trophic food webs, such as cephalopods. These latter, known as masters of camouflage, display rapid physiological color changes to conceal themselves and adapt to their surrounding environments. The efficiency of this process depends on visual abilities and acuity, information processing, and control of chromatophores dynamics through nervous and hormonal regulation with which many contaminants can interfere. Therefore, the quantitative measurement of color change in cephalopod species could be developed as a powerful endpoint for toxicological risk assessment. Based on a wide body of research having assessed the effect of various environmental stressors (pharmaceutical residues, metals, carbon dioxide, anti-fouling agents) on the camouflage abilities of juvenile common cuttlefish, we discuss the relevance of this species as a toxicological model and address the challenge of color change quantification and standardization through a comparative review of the available measurement techniques.

1 Introduction

For ethical reasons and given the growing need for risk assessment of environmental pollutants, the field of ecotoxicology is undergoing substantial changes (Campbell et al., 2022). In line with the 3Rs principle (Replace, Reduce, Refine), alternatives are being sought to replace conventional toxicity tests, which frequently measure mortality as an endpoint. Among them, the development of sublethal behavioral endpoints is not new (Dell’Omo, 2002), but attracts increasing attention. Indeed, it has been shown that a variety of behavioral responses to contaminants, both in vertebrates and invertebrates, appear to reflect alterations in sensory, hormonal, neurological, and/or metabolic systems (Saaristo et al., 2018; El-Gendy et al., 2021). Several authors have suggested that behavior could be among the most sensitive, flexible and conspicuous expressions of an animal’s integrated physiological response, making it a suitable endpoint for early toxicity testing (e.g., Clotfelter et al., 2004; Hellou, 2011; Melvin and Wilson, 2013; Peterson et al., 2017).

A large proportion of the research conducted so far in aquatic species has targeted behaviors such as predator avoidance, locomotor activity, exploration and anxiety (Melvin and Wilson, 2013). They are doubtless highly relevant to assess ecological risks as they reflect a wide range of biological functions, including predation, feeding, migration and mating (El-Gendy et al., 2021). Moreover, some behavioral tests using these endpoints allow high-throughput screening assays in laboratories and can be adapted to various species. However, diversifying the approaches, toxicological endpoints and model organisms is necessary (1) to account for inter-specific differences in sensitivity and toxicodynamics and (2) to make extrapolations and predictions of toxicological and ecological effects possible (Segner and Baumann, 2016). In this regard, some critical species-specific behavioral responses must be assessed in species playing key roles in trophic food webs, such as cephalopods. As mollusks—an important phylum among marine invertebrates—they certainly offer a particularly large and rich behavioral repertoire, including remarkable color changes that may serve as a proxy, notably for neurotoxic effects of environmental chemicals (Hanlon and Messenger, 2018).

Color change is a widespread ability among animals, known to fulfill various biological functions such as thermoregulation, UV protection, crypsis or communication (Figon and Casas, 2018). It is generally achieved either by the production, degradation or chemical modification of pigmented structures (morphological color change) or by changes in intracellular pigment distribution within specialized skin organs called chromatophores (physiological color change). In cephalopods, the chromatophores consist of pigment-containing elastic sacculi attached to a set of neuromuscular fibers, whose mechanical action controls the dispersion or the concentration of pigments (Messenger, 2001; Figure 1A). These basic structural elements are organized in units, themselves organized in chromatic components, which, combined with textural (i.e., expression of skin papillae), postural (e.g., arm posture) and locomotor components, form a palette of species-specific body patterns (Hanlon and Messenger, 2018).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. (A). Diagram of the neuronal control of chromatophores in the European cuttlefish: The visual information is transmitted from the eye (1) to the central nervous system (through the optic lobes, the lateral basal lobes, then the chromatophore lobes) that controls the motor activity of chromatophores (4) through the stellate ganglion (3), (B). Example of cuttlefish hatchlings exposed for 1 month to CO2 (i.e., pH 7.54), Hg (i.e., 3 μgg-1 dry weight in muscle) or CO2+Hg, with a low disruptive score (left) compared to a cuttlefish from control conditions (i.e., pH 8.02; right) with a high disruptive score, (C). Example of cuttlefish hatchlings exposed for 3 days to dexmedetomidine during 3 days displaying a dark uniform pattern on both checkerboard and uniform backgrounds compared to cuttlefish from solvent control conditions (DMSO: dimethyl sulfoxide; DS: disruptive score; HP: homochromy percentage).

Multiple experiments have shown that the choice of body pattern relied on a fine visual analysis of the animal’s immediate surroundings, considering, not only the nature of the substrate, but also the presence of objects, conspecifics, prey or predators (Allen et al., 2009; Barbosa et al., 2012). This information is processed and integrated by the brain which generates a motor program that selectively activates the expansion/retraction of chromatophores (Figure 1A; Supplementary Figure S1).

A range of environmental stressors might interfere with these neurally controlled mechanisms and result in the alteration of chromatophores’ activity—and, subsequently, of color change. Therefore, the quantitative measurement of physiological color change in cephalopod species could provide new opportunities for toxicological risk assessment. Following a body of research that has evaluated the effect of various environmental stressors on camouflage in juvenile European common cuttlefish, we here discuss the relevance of this species as a toxicological model and address the challenge of color change quantification and standardization through a comparative review of the available measurement techniques.

1.1 Sepia officinalis as an emerging model in behavioral ecotoxicology

The European common cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis) is one of the most studied cephalopod species, especially for the exploration of color change and advanced cognitive functions (Darmaillacq et al., 2014). This necto-benthic species heavily relies on rapid adaptive camouflage to avoid being detected by predators. Cuttlefish are known to exploit a combination of background color matching, disruptive coloration, masquerade and distractive markings, involving more than 30 different chromatic components (Hanlon and Messenger, 1988). Although S. officinalis could theoretically produce billions of body patterns, it appears that some combinations of components are preferentially used (Osorio et al., 2022), resulting in the expression of three main camouflage patterns called uniform, mottle and disruptive (Hanlon and Messenger, 2018). Uniform patterns are usually displayed on uniform backgrounds, whereas cuttlefish exhibit more complex patterns such as mottle pattern on sandy bottoms or disruptive pattern on contrasted substrates containing gravel, pebbles or shells. These body patterns are said to be chronic (i.e., maintained over long time periods) as opposed to acute body patterns (i.e., displayed during seconds to minutes for communication, predation or defensive purposes). Their efficiency can be improved by behavioral stillness and sand-digging (Mather, 1986).

The chromatophores of S. officinalis are known to be under the control of serotonin, which induces a retraction of the pigment-containing sacculi, and glutamate, which induces their expansion. This antagonistic system would control rapid color changes for the production of acute body patterns, whereas FMRFamide-related peptides and nitric oxide would be involved in the maintenance of chronic patterns by modulating the action of glutamate (Loi and Tublitz, 2000; Mattiello et al., 2010). A proven malfunction of these control systems could therefore reveal a neurotoxic effect or neuroendocrine disruption.

At the time of hatching, the chromatophores are functional and the juveniles are immediately capable of color changes—although their camouflage abilities significantly improve with age (Dickel et al., 2006). This gives the opportunity to implement behavioral tests on the earliest-life stages, with several practical advantages: (1) hatchlings can be available in large numbers from egg clusters collected in the environment, (2) the hatching success is close to 100% and (3) juveniles are relatively easy to maintain in laboratory conditions. Their small size (about 10 mm in dorsal mantle length at hatching against 45 cm for the adults) requires little space for rearing facilities (0.005 m2 and 2 L per animal according to Fiorito et al., 2015) and experimental devices. Most importantly, the use of early-life stages is particularly relevant from an ecotoxicological and ecological point of view. Indeed, hatchlings and juveniles are more vulnerable to environmental contaminants than adults, since they are at key stages of their cerebral, cognitive and behavioral development (Dickel et al., 2006). They are also more exposed to contaminants as they spend all embryonic stages and the first post-hatching months in coastal waters subjected to anthropogenic pollutants, before migrating offshore to their wintering grounds (Miramand et al., 2006). In addition, the digestive gland, a key organ for detoxification, has not yet reached its full physiological development in the first month of juvenile life, raising the question of its efficiency to cope with the toxic effects of contaminants (Lacoue-Labarthe et al., 2016).

More generally, the recent interest in using cephalopods in ecotoxicological studies lies in their potential for bioaccumulation (Bustamante et al., 1998b), which should be favored by their short life span and rapid growth (i.e., the accumulation of contaminants followed by reproduction is less energetically expensive than their elimination for survival purpose). In addition, cephalopods are recognized vectors for chemicals transfer along the trophic web as they are predators and prey of numerous marine organisms (Bustamante et al., 1998a). In cuttlefish, bioaccumulation occurs from different uptake pathways, including seawater, sediment (for sand-digging species) and trophic routes (see Bustamante et al., 2002, Bustamante et al., 2004). Consequently, the concentrations of contaminents vary along the environmental conditions affecting bioavailability and physiology (e.g., temperature, pCO2) and many life traits, such as age, size or diet and trophic levels (for reviews: Lacoue-Labarthe et al., 2016; Penicaud et al., 2017).

To date, neither cuttlefish nor any cephalopod species has been recognized as a model species per se. However, S. officinalis was acknowledged as an emerging model for research in the fields of cognitive neuro-ethology, sensory ecology and behavioral ecotoxicology (Bassaglia et al., 2013). With an effort toward the harmonization of existing analytical tools and resources, studies devoted to cephalopods could benefit a wider scientific community.

1.2 The challenge of color change quantification

The experimental analysis of cuttlefish color change requires (1) properly designing the visual stimuli meant to elicit specific behavioral responses (color change or body pattern) and (2) collecting images of sufficient quality, allowing (3) relevant quantitative image analysis.

To meet the first requirement, fundamental research conducted on cuttlefish camouflage is a valuable resource. While cuttlefish are known to integrate multiple visual features from their surroundings to elaborate a camouflage response, it has been demonstrated that the use of simple artificial substrata can reproducibly elicit typical body patterns. For example, the disruptive body pattern is routinely induced in response to black and white checkerboards displayed on the bottom and walls of the arena. A top requirement is that the checkerboard squares are between 40 and 120% of the size of the animals’ white square component area (see Barbosa et al., 2007). The use of smaller or larger checkerboard squares is likely to induce mottle or uniform body patterns (Chiao and Hanlon, 2001).

The photographs used for color change analyses must also meet certain criteria. The topic was covered by Stevens and collaborators (2007), who emphasize the utmost importance of manually controlling the white balance and light exposure. The lighting should also be tuned so as not to stress tested individuals (<350 lux at water surface according to Fiorito et al., 2015) while being as close as possible to the spectrum of sunlight to offer ecologically relevant testing conditions.

Finally, the camouflage patterns can be characterized using a range of descriptors (see Table 8 three from Josef and Shashar, 2014 for a review). For ecotoxicological purposes, the simple discrimination of uniform, mottle and disruptive patterns can already provide ample information. This can be done manually (Dickel et al., 2006) but also automatically, based on their distinct spatial frequency spectra (Barbosa et al., 2008), or with the help of learning algorithms (Orenstein et al., 2016).

The analysis can be extended to an evaluation of the efficiency of each camouflage. The uniform camouflage pattern can be assessed by a measurement of homochromy, i.e., the matching of the animal mantle compared to the background luminance according to the mean grey value (see Poirier et al., 2005; Di Poï et al., 2014; Supplementary Figure S2). In this case, the standard deviation of the grey values of the animal’s body can also be considered as a descriptor of the heterogeneity of the uniform pattern. Finally, the disruptiveness can be assessed by assigning a score—from 0 (not expressed), 1 (weakly expressed), 2 (moderately expressed) to 3 (strongly expressed)—to each of the eleven chromatic components commonly forming disruptive patterning (Barbosa et al., 2008). Individual cuttlefish can then be assigned a total grade, called disruptive score, ranging from 0, for maximal homochromy, to 33, for highly disruptive patterning (Supplementary Figure S2).

It is also possible to add a temporal dimension to the analyses. It may consist in assessing the latency of a chronic pattern (Court et al., 2022) or its stability over short time periods (Di Poï et al., 2014; Chabenat et al., 2021; Gouveneaux et al., in prep.). Otherwise, the comparison of camouflage quality at different ages during the first months is a simple way to assess whether the typically observed post-embryonic maturation of camouflage abilities is altered (Dickel et al., 2006).

These tests are relatively quick and inexpensive to implement, not to mention that photographic imaging is non-invasive. The reproducibility of image analysis is also facilitated by the fact that cuttlefish usually stay still on the substrate and, therefore, exhibit a portion of its mantle relatively unchanged in size and extension (as opposed to octopodiforms which take on shapes and postures that usually conceal part of their skin). This feature could facilitate the adaptation of more complex quantification methods (e.g., the analysis of grey values profiles along some body axes; Court et al., 2022 adapted from Chiao and Hanion, 2001) to reveal more subtle color change alterations. Indeed, image analysis can be technically challenging and time-consuming, especially if the method is manual and several annotators have to analyze in parallel to avoid bias on the results.

1.3 Recent applications in ecotoxicology

A number of environmental stressors have been shown to alter Sepia officinalis’ color change abilities. Several studies have focused on the effect of waterborne fluoxetine and venlafaxine, two antidepressants of the class of selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors and serotonin-norepinephrine reuptake inhibitors, respectively (Table 1). Changes were mainly observed in the efficiency of the uniform pattern, which appeared more heterogeneous in animals exposed to one or the other of these compounds (Bidel et al., 2016a, 2016b). However, a mixture of both resulted in a more uniform pattern (i.e., lower disruptive score) when calculated in animals tested on uniform backgrounds (Chabenat et al., 2021). Although venlafaxine did not appear to modulate the serotonin levels in the central nervous system of cuttlefish (Bidel et al., 2016a), it was observed to cause the dose-dependent relaxation of chromatophore-associated muscles in isolated skin patches, as observed in response to serotonin (Gouveneaux et al., in prep).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Summary of uses of body patterns in ecotoxicology (dph: days post-hatching; NE: no effect).

More recently, these tests have been adapted to monitor the effects associated with trophic mercury, a potent neurotoxin under its methylated form (MeHg) (Minet, 2022; Figure 1B), as well as the effects of suspected neuroendocrine disruptors (i.e., dexmedetomidine) (Gouveneaux et al., in prep). In contrast to previously cited studies, juvenile cuttlefish were shown to struggle mostly with the disruptive pattern: the chronic exposure to MeHg resulted in the display of a light uniform pattern on black and white checkerboards. In contrast, acute exposure to the antifouling agent dexmedetomidine resulted in a dark uniform pattern regardless of the type of background presented (light or dark grey uniform backgrounds, black and white checkerboards; Figure 1C). Finally, seawater carbon dioxide (pCO2) is of growing interest due to its effects on nervous system and behaviors in cephalopods (e.g., Spady et al., 2018).

It must be noted that, while one-month-old juveniles exposed to elevated pCO2 (∼1,600 µatm) failed to produce a disruptive pattern on the black and white checkerboards (Minet, 2022), exposure of hatchlings to ∼1,000 µatm did not affect the camouflage display as assessed with a different quantification method regardless of the substrate nature (Court et al., 2022). This raises the question of how the animals’ response depends on its age, the contaminants’ pressure levels and the descriptors used for color patterns. In sum, this emphasizes the need to harmonize the camouflage assays.

2 Discussion

Cephalopods combine the advantages of being keystone species in marine food webs, important fishing and aquaculture resources, and valuable experimental animals. As such, they stand at the crossroads of several of the environmental problems of the Anthropocene. As a matter of fact, human activities have substantially changed the world’s marine environments in recent decades. While some populations and species are paying a high price for these changes, up to the point of collapse or extinction. Cephalopods, however, seem to take advantage of these major changes (Doubleday et al., 2016). This success is partly attributed to the “live fast, die young” life history strategies of cephalopods, which confer them great plasticity and adaptability to environmental changes (O’Brien et al., 2018). However, these overall trends of populations can mask a continuous decrease in the European cuttlefish stocks in the last decade (FAO, 2019), suggesting that this species may be vulnerable to multiple, co-acting stressors (Clarke, 1996; Bustamante et al., 1998b). Therefore, their study might shed light on our understanding and assessment of the risks associated with pollutants and related stresses in marine environments.

In this perspective, the quantification of physiological color change in cuttlefish is emerging as an easily accessible, non-invasive, sensitive and holistic ecotoxicological endpoint. Indeed, color change determines camouflage, whose efficiency depends on (1) visual abilities and acuity (from eye functioning to pigments structure), (2) information processing (nervous system) and (3) control of chromatophore dynamics (nervous and hormonal control pathways) (see Reiter and Laurent, 2020) with which many contaminants can interfere. Previous studies, notably on fish, crustaceans and cephalopods (e.g., Lennquist et al., 2010; Ford and Feuerhelm, 2020; Chabenat et al., 2021), have highlighted various effects of contaminants on color change, ranging from impaired acclimatization abilities to changing visual environments up to persistent darkness or paleness. The underlying causes of these alterations remain poorly understood, although in most cases they are likely explained by the disruption of color change control mechanisms. It appears so for venlafaxine, whose topical effects on S. officinalis (i.e., the concentration of pigments and skin paling), are consistent with its mode of action, namely, an increase in serotonin levels (Gouveneaux et al., in prep).

Yet, unusual mantle color changes or discoloration are commonly used in cephalopods—although quantitative assessment methods are rarely implemented—as indicators of anesthesia or general ill-being (Polese et al., 2014; Fiorito et al., 2015). As it generally goes with behavior, this emphasizes that color change is the expression of an integrated physiological state and carries the potential to reveal a wide spectrum of disruptions beyond those affecting the chromatophore control mechanisms themselves. This points to the possibility of confounding factors, which exist for many toxicological endpoints. Thus, the assessment of color change must be completed by sensory tests (such as visual acuity assessment; see Cartron et al., 2013) but also neurotransmitters quantification or genomic approaches, to highlight neurotoxic effects.

The use of S. officinalis as a model proved to be convenient, with fundamental studies about its color change offering a solid knowledge base and a wide range of analytical tools. However, scoring methods, such as the disruptive score, are still time-consuming and experimenter-dependent with a qualitative interpretation of the expression of chromatic components. Thus, one of the main challenges in the development of cuttlefish color change as a proxy for ecotoxicology will be the development of automatized and, above all, standardized methods to ensure the reproducibility and comparability of tests. This applies to the methods of image acquisition and analysis, but also to the conditions of exposure to contaminants, as well as the age of the animals tested. Finally, it is essential to develop positive controls, i.e., to use chemical compounds with known modes of action, such as neuromodulators, to characterize and mark out the nature and magnitude of effects that can be expected on cuttlefish color changes.

Most studies conducted so far have focused on the description of chronic body patterns and their components. However, a variety of subtle color changes and body patterns, including the repertoire of acute body patterns displayed by S. officinalis, remain to be assessed under stressful conditions for possible derivation as toxicological endpoints. Besides, finer scale studies could lead to a better understanding of the hierarchical organization of chromatophores and thus of the impact of contaminants on neuronal activity. This could rely on existing methods for automatic analysis of chromatophores activity, which have significantly improved (Goodwin and Tublitz, 2013; Hadjisolomou and El-Haddad, 2017). In fact, it is currently possible, using deep learning algorithms, to perform automatic delineation and tracking of individual chromatophores, to classify them as light or dark and to discriminate overlapping structures (https://git.unicaen.fr/nicolas.elie/redpol-open). Such a tool was recently developed from video recordings of S. officinalis’ skin explants, following the bioassay protocol implemented by Loi and collaborators (1996) for highlighting the pharmacological basis of chromatophore control. It will likely be able to process high-resolution images acquired from motile cuttlefish, as recent works have shown (Reiter et al., 2018; Hadjisolomou et al., 2021). Yet, the number of animals needed for ex vivo/in vitro tests is reduced by the potential for high-throughput using of several skin patches from one animal only.

In conclusion, color change appears to be an integrated endpoint of the subtle effects of contaminants that can interact at different processing levels (visual perception, neuro-hormonal control, motor dynamic of chromatophores, or even decision-making) and thus provide early warning information of the cuttlefish vulnerability to environmental stressors. In turn, improving the knowledge on the toxicodynamics of contaminants targeting specific pathways would help understand the complex biological processes governing camouflage. Finally, the ecological consequences of affected camouflage abilities on cuttlefish fitness remain to be explored.

Data availability statement

The raw data supporting the conclusion of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics statement

The animal study was reviewed and approved by the Comité d'éthique Normandie en matière d'expérimentation animale (CENOMEXA).

Author contributions

AG developed the initial idea for the article. AG and AM organized manuscript sections, directed editing, made the figures and finalized the manuscript. All authors have made a substantial and direct intellectual contribution to this work and approved it for publication.

Funding

This work is supported by the RedPol France (Channel) England Interreg Programme, which is co-financed by the European Regional Development Fund (to AG, CB, CJ-A, and TK) and the Région Nouvelle Aquitaine through the EXPO project (to AM, PB, and TL-L). It is a contribution to the MERCy project, funded by la Fondation pour la Recherche sur la Biodiversité and the Ministère de la Transition Écologique et Solidaire.

Acknowledgments

The Institute Universitaire de France (IUF) is acknowledged for its support to PB as a Senior Member.

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fphys.2023.1162709/full#supplementary-material

References

Allen, J. J., Mäthger, L. M., Barbosa, A., and Hanlon, R. T. (2009). Cuttlefish use visual cues to control three-dimensional skin papillae for camouflage. J. Comp. Physiology A 195, 547–555. doi:10.1007/s00359-009-0430-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbosa, A., Allen, J. J., Mäthger, L. M., and Hanlon, R. T. (2012). Cuttlefish use visual cues to determine arm postures for camouflage. Proc. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 279, 84–90. doi:10.1098/rspb.2011.0196

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbosa, A., Mäthger, L. M., Buresch, K. C., Kelly, J., Chubb, C., Chiao, C.-C., et al. (2008). Cuttlefish camouflage: The effects of substrate contrast and size in evoking uniform, mottle or disruptive body patterns. Vis. Res. 48, 1242–1253. doi:10.1016/j.visres.2008.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Barbosa, A., Mäthger, L. M., Chubb, C., Florio, C., Chiao, C.-C., and Hanlon, R. T. (2007). Disruptive coloration in cuttlefish: A visual perception mechanism that regulates ontogenetic adjustment of skin patterning. J. Exp. Biol. 210, 1139–1147. doi:10.1242/jeb.02741

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bassaglia, Y., Buresi, A., Franko, D., Andouche, A., Baratte, S., and Bonnaud, L. (2013). Sepia officinalis: A new biological model for eco-evo-devo studies. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 447, 4–13. doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2013.02.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidel, F., Di Poi, C., Budzinski, H., Pardon, P., Callewaert, W., Arini, A., et al. (2016a). The antidepressant venlafaxine may act as a neurodevelopmental toxicant in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). Neurotoxicology 55, 142–153. doi:10.1016/j.neuro.2016.05.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bidel, F., Di Poi, C., Imarazene, B., Koueta, N., Budzinski, H., Van Delft, P., et al. (2016b). Pre-hatching fluoxetine-induced neurochemical, neurodevelopmental, and immunological changes in newly hatched cuttlefish. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 23, 5030–5045. doi:10.1007/s11356-015-4591-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bustamante, P., Caurant, F., Fowler, S. W., and Miramand, P. (1998a). Cephalopods as a vector for the transfer of cadmium to top marine predators in the north-east Atlantic Ocean. Sci. Total Environ. 220, 71–80. doi:10.1016/S0048-9697(98)00250-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bustamante, P., Cherel, Y., Caurant, F., and Miramand, P. (1998b). Cadmium, copper and zinc in octopuses from kerguelen islands, southern Indian ocean. Polar Biol. 19, 264–271. doi:10.1007/s003000050244

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bustamante, P., Teyssié, J.-L., Fowler, S. W., Cotret, O., Danis, B., Miramand, P., et al. (2002). Biokinetics of zinc and cadmium accumulation and depuration at different stages in the life cycle of the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 231, 167–177. doi:10.3354/meps231167

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bustamante, P., Danis, J.-L. B., Fowler, S. W., and Cotret, P. O. (2004). Uptake, transfer and distribution of silver and cobalt in tissues of the common cuttlefish Sepia officinalis at different stages of its life cycle. Mar. Ecol. Prog. Ser. 269, 185–195.

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Campbell, P. G., Hodson, P. V., Welbourn, P. M., and Wright, D. A. (2022). Ecotoxicology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Cartron, L., Shashar, N., Dickel, L., and Darmaillacq, A.-S. (2013). Effects of stimuli shape and polarization in evoking deimatic patterns in the European cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis, under varying turbidity conditions. Invertebr. Neurosci. 13, 19–26. doi:10.1007/s10158-013-0148-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chabenat, A., Knigge, T., and Bellanger, C. (2021). Antidepressants modify cryptic behavior in juvenile cuttlefish at environmentally realistic concentrations. Environ. Toxicol. Chem. 40, 2571–2577. doi:10.1002/etc.5149

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chiao, C.-C., and Hanlon, R. T. (2001). Cuttlefish camouflage: Visual perception of size, contrast and number of white squares on artificial checkerboard substrata initiates disruptive coloration. J. Exp. Biol. 204, 2119–2125. doi:10.1242/jeb.204.12.2119

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clarke, M. R. (1996). Cephalopods as prey. III. Cetaceans. Philosophical Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 351, 1053–1065. doi:10.1098/rspb.1996.0093

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clotfelter, E. D., Bell, A. M., and Levering, K. R. (2004). The role of animal behaviour in the study of endocrine-disrupting chemicals. Anim. Behav. 68, 665–676. doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2004.05.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Court, M., Paula, J. R., Macau, M., Otjacques, E., Repolho, T., Rosa, R., et al. (2022). Camouflage and exploratory avoidance of newborn cuttlefish under warming and acidification. Biology 11, 1394. doi:10.3390/biology11101394

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Darmaillacq, A.-S., Dickel, L., and Mather, J. (2014). Cephalopod cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

Google Scholar

Dell’Omo, G. (2002). Behavioural ecotoxicology. New York: John Wiley and Sons.

Google Scholar

Di Poi, C., Bidel, F., Dickel, L., and Bellanger, C. (2014). Cryptic and biochemical responses of young cuttlefish Sepia officinalis exposed to environmentally relevant concentrations of fluoxetine. Aquat. Toxicol. 151, 36–45. doi:10.1016/j.aquatox.2013.12.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dickel, L., Darmaillacq, A.-S., Poirier, R., Agin, V., Bellanger, C., and Chichery, R. (2006). Behavioural and neural maturation in the cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Vie Milieu/Life Environ. 56, 89–95.

Google Scholar

Doubleday, Z. A., Prowse, T. A., Arkhipkin, A., Pierce, G. J., Semmens, J., Steer, M., et al. (2016). Global proliferation of cephalopods. Curr. Biol. 26, R406–R407. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

El-Gendy, K. S., Gad, A. F., and Radwan, M. A. (2021). Physiological and behavioral responses of land molluscs as biomarkers for pollution impact assessment: A review. Environ. Res. 193, 110558. doi:10.1016/j.envres.2020.110558

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

FAO (2019). FAO yearbook 2017. Fishery and aquaculture statistics. Rome: FAO.

Google Scholar

Figon, F., and Casas, J. (2018). Morphological and physiological colour changes in the animal kingdom. eLS, 1–11. doi:10.1002/9780470015902.a0028065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fiorito, G., Affuso, A., Basil, J., Cole, A., de Girolamo, P., D’angelo, L., et al. (2015). Guidelines for the care and welfare of cephalopods in research–a consensus based on an initiative by CephRes, FELASA and the Boyd Group. Lab. Anim. 49, 1–90. doi:10.1177/0023677215580006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ford, A. T., and Feuerhelm, E. (2020). Effects of the antidepressant fluoxetine on pigment dispersion in chromatophores of the common sand shrimp, Crangon crangon: Repeated experiments paint an inconclusive picture. Ecotoxicology 29, 1368–1376. doi:10.1007/s10646-020-02272-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goodwin, E., and Tublitz, N. (2013). Video analyses of chromatophore activity in the European cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. J. Exp. Mar. Biol. Ecol. 447, 156–159. doi:10.1016/j.jembe.2013.02.024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadjisolomou, S. P., and El-Haddad, G. (2017). SpotMetrics: An open-source image-analysis software plugin for automatic chromatophore detection and measurement. Front. physiology 8, 106. doi:10.3389/fphys.2017.00106

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hadjisolomou, S. P., El-Haddad, R., Kloskowski, K., Chavarga, A., and Abramov, I. (2021). Quantifying the speed of chromatophore activity at the single-organ level in response to a visual startle stimulus in living, intact squid. Front. physiology 12, 675252. doi:10.3389/fphys.2021.675252

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanlon, R. T., and Messenger, J. B. (1988). Adaptive coloration in young cuttlefish:(Sepia officinalis L.) the morphology and development of body patterns and their relation to behaviour. Philos. Trans. B 320, 437–487. doi:10.1098/rstb.1988.0087

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hanlon, R. T., and Messenger, J. B. (2018). “Chapter 3 - body patterning and colour change,” in Cephalopod behaviour (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), 45–73.

Google Scholar

Hellou, J. (2011). Behavioural ecotoxicology, an “early warning” signal to assess environmental quality. Environ. Sci. Pollut. Res. 18, 1–11. doi:10.1007/s11356-010-0367-2

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Josef, N., and Shashar, N. (2014). “Camouflage in benthic cephalopods: What does it teach us,” in Cephalopod cognition (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press).

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lacoue-Labarthe, T., Le Pabic, C., and Bustamante, P. (2016). Ecotoxicology of early-life stages in the common cuttlefish Sepia officinalis: Review and perspectives. Vie Milieu/Life Environ. 66, 65–79.

Google Scholar

Lennquist, A., Hilvarsson, A., and Förlin, L. (2010). Responses in fish exposed to medetomidine, a new antifouling agent. Mar. Environ. Res. 69, S43–S45. doi:10.1016/j.marenvres.2010.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loi, P. K., and Tublitz, N. J. (2000). Roles of glutamate and FMRFamide-related peptides at the chromatophore neuromuscular junction in the cuttlefish, Sepia officinalis. J. Comp. Neurology 420, 499–511. doi:10.1002/(sici)1096-9861(20000515)420:4<499::aid-cne7>3.0.co;2-e

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loi, P., Saunders, R., Young, D., and Tublitz, N. (1996). Peptidergic regulation of chromatophore function in the European cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. J. Exp. Biol. 199, 1177–1187. doi:10.1242/jeb.199.5.1177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mather, J. A. (1986). Sand digging in Sepia officinalis: Assessment of a Cephalopod mollusc’s" fixed" behavior pattern. J. Comp. Psychol. 100, 315–320. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.100.3.315

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mattiello, T., Fiore, G., Brown, E. R., d’Ischia, M., and Palumbo, A. (2010). Nitric oxide mediates the glutamate-dependent pathway for neurotransmission in Sepia officinalis chromatophore organs. J. Biol. Chem. 285, 24154–24163. doi:10.1074/jbc.M109.083428

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melvin, S. D., and Wilson, S. P. (2013). The utility of behavioral studies for aquatic toxicology testing: A meta-analysis. Chemosphere 93, 2217–2223. doi:10.1016/j.chemosphere.2013.07.036

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Messenger, J. B. (2001). Cephalopod chromatophores: Neurobiology and natural history. Biol. Rev. 76, 473–528. doi:10.1017/s1464793101005772

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minet, A. (2022). PhD thesis. La Rochelle: La Rochelle Université.Bioaccumulation and effects of mercury in cuttlefish in the context of ocean acidification

Google Scholar

Miramand, P., Bustamante, P., Bentley, D., and Kouéta, N. (2006). Variation of heavy metal concentrations (Ag, Cd, Co, Cu, Fe, Pb, V, and Zn) during the life cycle of the common cuttlefish Sepia officinalis. Sci. Total Environ. 361, 132–143. doi:10.1016/j.scitotenv.2005.10.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Brien, C. E., Roumbedakis, K., and Winkelmann, I. E. (2018). The current state of cephalopod science and perspectives on the most critical challenges ahead from three early-career researchers. Front. Physiology 9, 700. doi:10.3389/fphys.2018.00700

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Orenstein, E. C., Haag, J. M., Gagnon, Y. L., and Jaffe, J. S. (2016). Automated classification of camouflaging cuttlefish. Methods Oceanogr. 15, 21–34. doi:10.1016/j.mio.2016.04.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Osorio, D., Ménager, F., Tyler, C. W., and Darmaillacq, A.-S. (2022). Multi-level control of adaptive camouflage by European cuttlefish. Curr. Biol. 32, 2556–2562.e2. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2022.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penicaud, V., Lacoue-Labarthe, T., and Bustamante, P. (2017). Metal bioaccumulation and detoxification processes in cephalopods: A review. Environ. Res. 155, 123–133. doi:10.1016/j.envres.2017.02.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peterson, E. K., Buchwalter, D. B., Kerby, J. L., LeFauve, M. K., Varian-Ramos, C. W., and Swaddle, J. P. (2017). Integrative behavioral ecotoxicology: Bringing together fields to establish new insight to behavioral ecology, toxicology, and conservation. Curr. Zool. 63, 185–194. doi:10.1093/cz/zox010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poirier, R., Chichery, R., and Dickel, L. (2005). Early experience and postembryonic maturation of body patterns in cuttlefish (Sepia officinalis). J. Comp. Psychol. 119, 230–237. doi:10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.230

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Polese, G., Winlow, W., and Di Cosmo, A. (2014). Dose-dependent effects of the clinical anesthetic isoflurane on Octopus vulgaris: A contribution to cephalopod welfare. J. aquatic animal health 26, 285–294. doi:10.1080/08997659.2014.945047

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiter, S., Hülsdunk, P., Woo, T., Lauterbach, M. A., Eberle, J. L., Akay, L. A., et al. (2018). Elucidating the control and development of skin patterning in cuttlefish. Nature 562, 361–366. doi:10.1038/s41586-018-0591-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reiter, S., and Laurent, G. (2020). Visual perception and cuttlefish camouflage. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 60, 47–54. doi:10.1016/j.conb.2019.10.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saaristo, M., Brodin, T., Balshine, S., Bertram, M. G., Brooks, B. W., Ehlman, S. M., et al. (2018). Direct and indirect effects of chemical contaminants on the behaviour, ecology and evolution of wildlife. Proc. R. Soc. B 285, 20181297. doi:10.1098/rspb.2018.1297

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Segner, H., and Baumann, L. (2016). What constitutes a model organism in ecotoxicology? Integr. Environ. Assess. Manag. 12, 199–200. doi:10.1002/ieam.1727

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spady, B. L., Munday, P. L., and Watson, S.-A. (2018). Predatory strategies and behaviours in cephalopods are altered by elevated CO2. Glob. change Biol. 24, 2585–2596. doi:10.1111/gcb.14098

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, M., Párraga, C. A., Cuthill, I. C., Partridge, J. C., and Troscianko, T. S. (2007). Using digital photography to study animal coloration. Biol. J. Linn. Soc. 90, 211–237. doi:10.1111/j.1095-8312.2007.00725.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: behavior, body pattern, camouflage, cephalopod, chromatophore, crypsis, mollusk, neurotoxicity

Citation: Gouveneaux A, Minet A, Jozet-Alves C, Knigge T, Bustamante P, Lacoue-Labarthe T and Bellanger C (2023) Cuttlefish color change as an emerging proxy for ecotoxicology. Front. Physiol. 14:1162709. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2023.1162709

Received: 09 February 2023; Accepted: 27 February 2023;
Published: 08 March 2023.

Edited by:

Rui Rosa, University of Lisbon, Portugal

Reviewed by:

Daniel Osorio, University of Sussex, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2023 Gouveneaux, Minet, Jozet-Alves, Knigge, Bustamante, Lacoue-Labarthe and Bellanger. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Cécile Bellanger, Y2VjaWxlLmJlbGxhbmdlckB1bmljYWVuLmZy

These authors have contributed equally to this work

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.