Skip to main content

SYSTEMATIC REVIEW article

Front. Physiol., 25 September 2020
Sec. Autonomic Neuroscience

Placebo Analgesia, Nocebo Hyperalgesia, and the Cardiovascular System: A Qualitative Systematic Review

\nHojjat DanialiHojjat DanialiMagne Arve Flaten
Magne Arve Flaten*
  • Department of Psychology, Norwegian University of Science and Technology, Trondheim, Norway

Background: Placebo/nocebo effects involve the autonomic nervous system, including cardiac activity, but studies have reported inconsistent findings on how cardiac activity is modulated following a placebo/nocebo effect. However, no systematic review has been conducted to provide a clear picture of cardiac placebo responses.

Objective: The main goal of the present study is to review the effects of placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia on cardiac activity as measured by blood pressure, heart rate, and heart rate variability.

Methods: Using several Boolean keyword combinations, the PubMed, EMBASE, PsycINFO, Cochrane Review Library, and ISI Web of Knowledge databases were searched until January 5, 2020, to find studies that analyzed blood pressure, heart rate, or heart rate variability indexes following a placebo analgesic/nocebo hyperalgesic effect.

Results: Nineteen studies were found, with some reporting more than one index of cardiac activity; eight studies were on blood pressure, 14 studies on heart rate, and six on heart rate variability. No reliable association between placebo/nocebo effects and blood pressure or heart rate was found. However, placebo effects reduced, and nocebo effects increased low-frequency heart rate variability, and heart rate variability significantly predicted placebo effects in two studies.

Conclusion: Placebo/nocebo effects can have reliable effects on heart rate variability, but not on heart rate and blood pressure.

Introduction

Placebo analgesia is a reduction in pain due to the administration of an inert substance with information that the substance effectively alleviates pain (Flaten et al., 2006; Benedetti, 2008). Moreover, inert factors previously associated with effective treatment, for example, through learning procedures such as classical (e.g., Ader, 1997; Flaten et al., 1997; Flaten, 2013), observational (e.g., Colloca and Benedetti, 2009; Hunter et al., 2014; and Bajcar and Babel, 2018), and operant conditioning (Adamczyk et al., 2019; Babel, 2020), are also capable of generating placebo effects. Both verbal information and learning procedures can generate expectations about positive outcomes, which are known as one of the mechanisms of placebo effects (Kirsch, 2004; Flaten et al., 2013). However, recent evidence suggests that conditioning procedures can be regarded as a distinctive mechanism of placebo effects, as placebo effects induced by conditioning are not always mediated via expectations (Babel et al., 2018; Babel, 2019). On the opposite side, negative expectations/experiences about a treatment, induced by either verbal suggestions (e.g., Stovner et al., 2008) or learning mechanisms (e.g., Bajcar et al., 2020), can lead to higher pain, anxiety, and physiological stress levels (e.g., Flaten et al., 1999, 2011; Aslaksen and Lyby, 2015; Roderigo et al., 2017). The body of research on nocebo effects is relatively small, however.

Placebo/nocebo responses impact various physiological processes, especially those controlled by the autonomic nervous system (ANS) (Meissner, 2014). The ANS controls visceral organs and tissues through sympathetic and parasympathetic nervous divisions to preserve homeostasis. Homeostasis is the ongoing process of maintaining physiological equilibrium, which comprises autonomic, neuroendocrine, and behavioral mechanisms (Craig, 2002; Meissner, 2014).

The cardiovascular system is under ANS control and has an essential role in the maintenance of homeostasis (Berntson et al., 2017). Pain, as a stressor, increases sympathetic activity (SA) (Craig, 2003; Loggia et al., 2011) and leads to corresponding changes in cardiac activity. In a systematic review (SR), Koenig et al. (2014) found 20 studies on experimentally induced pain that also reported subsequent cardiac activity. The authors concluded that following painful stimulation, cardiovascular SA increased and parasympathetic activity (PA) decreased (Koenig et al., 2014). Based on such studies, it can be assumed that if a placebo effect reduces pain, then it also reduces cardiac SA and increases cardiac PA. However, the use of various cardiac activity metrics makes it challenging to draw clear conclusions about cardiac placebo responses. Blood pressure (BP), heart rate (HR), and HR variability (HRV) (described in Materials and Methods) are three cardiac activity indexes that have been used to observe cardiac reactions to pain (for a review, see Koenig et al., 2014) and placebo effects (e.g., Amigo et al., 1993; Aslaksen and Flaten, 2008; Hrobjartsson and Gotzsche, 2010; Zimmermann-Viehoff et al., 2013).

That is why an SR is warranted to synthesize the findings on cardiovascular placebo analgesic/nocebo hyperalgesic responses. To our knowledge, this is the first SR aimed to unravel the modulatory effects of placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia on cardiac activity. To do so, the following questions were investigated: (a) Which cardiac metric, among BP, HR, and HRV, gives the best picture of cardiac activity following a placebo/nocebo effect on pain in adults? (b) Is a change in cardiac activity essential for a placebo/nocebo effect to occur on pain?

Materials and Methods

Search Strategy

The Boolean keyword combinations presented in Table 1 were used to search the PubMed, EMBASE, PsycINFO, Cochrane Review Library, and ISI Web of Knowledge databases until January 5, 2020. The systematic approach used for this SR is in accordance with the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic Reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) statement (Moher et al., 2009).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Boolean terms used to search databases.

Data Extraction

The first author extracted the data, and both authors reviewed them. The searches resulted in 2,613 hits, which were reduced to 1,806 after duplicates were removed. In this SR, the main target outcomes were BP, HR, or HRV indexes of cardiac activity following a placebo/nocebo effect, as compared to the control or a comparison group/condition. The secondary target outcomes were pain reports (e.g., pain intensity, threshold, duration, tolerance, unpleasantness, symptom severity, pain treatment efficacy, and pain expectation). Therefore, only studies in English that reported either BP and/or HR and/or HRV following a placebo/nocebo effect on pain (either experimental or clinical) were included.

A placebo effect was defined as a reduction in pain due to information that a treatment/manipulation would reduce pain, compared to the natural history control group/condition or the group/condition with different manipulation. The same definition was applied for a nocebo effect, except that instead of the reduction in pain, an increase in pain was assumed (Flaten et al., 2006). Either of the following situations was considered as a cardiac placebo response: a change in BP, HR, or HRV metrics following the administration of a placebo/nocebo treatment/condition compared to the control or the comparison group/condition (Meissner, 2014). Studies on both healthy subjects and patients experiencing pain were included. However, due to potential differences in cardiac activity (e.g., faster HR in children or irregular HR in cardiac patients), studies on children, animals, and cardiac patient populations were excluded. After reviewing the titles and abstracts, 109 relevant articles were identified for a thorough article review. The same inclusion criteria as in sorting out the hits were applied to read the articles: only (a) peer-reviewed studies (b) in English on (c) human adults and on (d) pain (either experimental or clinical) which (e) have investigated cardiac activity following a placebo/nocebo effect with (f) at least two comparison groups/conditions or a control group/condition (e.g., a natural history control group or a control group/condition) were included. After review of the 109 articles, 19 were included in the present SR (Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The review flow diagram.

Although this SR does not have a protocol review, there is a list of excluded studies, accessible by contacting the first author (HD) (Figure 1).

Cardiac Measurement Indexes

BP

BP is the pressure of blood flowing in vessels, expressed through systolic BP (SBP; the maximum pressure through one heartbeat) and diastolic BP (DBP; the minimum pressure between two beats) (Berntson et al., 2017). BP is controlled through the baroreceptor reflex of afferent nerves placed in the aortic and carotid artery walls that signal acute stretches in the artery walls or pressure changes to the nucleus tractus solitarius (NTS) in the medulla. NTS provides a negative feedback (i.e., baroreflex activity), meaning that in response to a baroreflex signal of an elevation in BP, NTS increases PA, which will result in HR and BP decreases (France and Ditto, 1996). This relationship implies that decreases in BP are associated with elevated PA, whereas increases in BP are associated with elevated SA (Sved, 2009).

HR

HR is computed by counting the number of heart contractions over a period of time, usually 1 min. The heart is mostly under inhibitory influence of PA (Thayer, 2009). SA increases lead to HR increases, and increases in PA result in HR decreases. The basic data to compute HR are the time interval between two adjacent beats (R spikes), the interbeat interval (IBI) (Thayer, 2009).

HRV

The parasympathetic and sympathetic branches both modulate cardiac activity, and HRV is computed to extract and separate these influences (Koenig et al., 2014; Berntson et al., 2017). There are three main methods to calculate HRV: time domain, frequency domain, and non-linear domain. However, the basic data to compute all types of HRV are the IBI. Time domain measures compute HRV in short-term (minutes or seconds) to long-term (hours) recordings, although short-term recordings are more common in pain and placebo studies (e.g., Aslaksen and Flaten, 2008; Koenig et al., 2014). The main method to quantify HRV from time domain metrics is R–R intervals, which is the calculation of the time interval between two R spikes in an electrocardiogram. R–R intervals are also called normal-to-normal (N–N) intervals when it is assured that the R spikes are intact and not affected by artifacts (Citi et al., 2012). Common statistical methods to analyze HRV are the standard deviation of N–N intervals (SDNN) and the root mean square of successive differences between normal R spikes (RMSSD). SDNN represents autonomic effects on a 5-min HRV recording (Minarini, 2020) and represents PA influences (Shaffer et al., 2014). RMSSD is obtained by computing successive time differences between R spikes. Then, each value is squared and averaged before obtaining the root for the total value. RMSSD is primarily used to measure changes in HRV that are mediated by the vagal nerve (Task force of the European society of cardiology and the north American society of pacing electrophysiology, 1996). Frequency domain methods use power spectral analysis to calculate a certain set of frequency bands in the IBI. Two frequency bands are usually discernable from short-term recordings: the high-frequency (HF; 0.15–0.4 Hz) band that is primarily mediated by PA and represents the respiratory influences and also a combination of SA and vagal activity. The HF band is highly correlated with respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA; i.e., heart period changes due to the respiratory cycles) and RMSSD and is related to vagal effects (Koenig et al., 2014). The low-frequency (LF; 0.04–0.15 Hz) band is indicative of PA and baroreflex activity (Thayer et al., 2008; Goldstein et al., 2011) and can be derived from short timescale recordings. To assess pain-related HRV, both time and frequency domain indices are used. The HF/LF ratio is another measure that represents the sympathovagal balance (Thayer, 2009). Non-linear methods analyze the irregularity and complexity of the cardiac time series data (Thayer, 2009), are little used, and will not be assessed here.

Bias Risk Assessment and Grading the Quality of Evidence

Biased reporting (i.e., changing the study aims based on findings) and selection bias (i.e., lack of access to all available data) are two potential sources of bias that threaten the reliability of SRs (Drucker et al., 2016). To avoid biased reporting and selection bias, the aims of the study did not change throughout the project, and the reference and citation lists of all included studies were manually searched (Drucker et al., 2016). Moreover, the revised version of the Cochrane Risk of Bias Tool (RoB 2) was used to assess the risk of bias in included studies. RoB 2 assesses level of risk of bias (high, low, or some concerns) based on the randomization process (i.e., biases in randomization and random assignment), deviations from intended interventions (i.e., differences between the intervention and the comparison/control group), missing outcome data, measurement of the outcome (e.g., the objectiveness of the outcome assessment), and selection of the reported result (i.e., considering both reported and unreported data) (for more information, see Sterne et al., 2019). To evaluate the certainty of evidence found in this SR, the Grades of Recommendation, Assessment, Development, and Evaluation (GRADE) approach was used. GRADE classifies the equality of evidence as high (i.e., there is little chance that further research will change the confidence in evidence), moderate (i.e., future research is likely to impact the confidence in evidence), low (i.e., future research is very likely to impact the confidence in evidence), and very low (i.e., the evidence is very uncertain) based on the studies' design, inconsistency of the results, indirectness of the evidence, imprecision of the results, and publication bias (for more information, see Brozek et al., 2009).

Results

A total of 19 studies were included. Eight studies reported more than one cardiac measurement; hence, eight studies reported BP, 14 studies reported HR, and six studies reported HRV. Included studies were classified based on the author names and publication year, sample size, study design, sample type, pain type and test, placebo/nocebo effects, and cardiac placebo responses (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Characteristics of studies investigating placebo/nocebo responses on blood pressure, heart rate, and heart rate variability.

BP and Placebo/Nocebo Responses

A total of eight studies investigated BP following placebo/nocebo responses.

Four studies showed that participants had lower SBP in the placebo group and higher SBP in the nocebo group: Aslaksen et al. (2014) showed that exposure to a sham treatment reduced pain and SBP compared with the control or treatment groups. Geers et al. (2015) showed that participants without experience with a pain task in the placebo group had lower pain and SBP as compared with the control group. Elsenbruch et al. (2019) reported that positive suggestions plus a relaxation program lowered pain and SBP as compared with the control or negative information group. On the other hand, Aslaksen et al. (2015) showed that compared with the control group, nocebo information induced higher pain and SBP regardless of whether an analgesic cream or a placebo was administered. However, in four other studies, the placebo/nocebo effect had no impact on BP (Robertson et al., 1991; Geers et al., 2010; Jegindø et al., 2013; Roderigo et al., 2017) (Table 2).

To sum up, four studies showed that the placebo effect was associated with reduced SBP (Aslaksen et al., 2014; Geers et al., 2015; Elsenbruch et al., 2019) and that the nocebo effect was associated with increased SBP (Aslaksen et al., 2015). However, four other studies reported that the placebo/nocebo effects were not associated with a significant change in BP (Robertson et al., 1991; Geers et al., 2010; Jegindø et al., 2013; Roderigo et al., 2017) (Table 2).

Therefore, no reliable association is found between the placebo/nocebo effect and BP.

HR and Placebo/Nocebo Responses

HR placebo responses were investigated in 14 studies (the study by Pollo et al., 2003, is counted twice, as it investigated HR in two different samples).

Six studies showed that the placebo effect reduced HR: Robertson et al. (1991) showed that females in the placebo group had lower pain and HR, as compared with the treatment and control groups. Pollo et al. (2003) studied two different samples and reported lower HR in the placebo group/conditions in both samples, as compared with the control group/conditions. Nishikawa et al. (2005) showed that regardless of the treatment type, adequate information lowered pain, anxiety, and HR, as compared with the minimal-information groups. Benedetti et al. (2015) showed that a conditioned sham treatment lowered headache, fatigue, and HR, as compared with the control group. Elsenbruch et al. (2019) showed that positive suggestions plus relaxation lowered pain and HR as compared with the control group. However, eight other studies reported that placebo/nocebo effects had no impact on HR (Aslaksen and Flaten, 2008; Colloca and Benedetti, 2009; Geers et al., 2010, 2015; Bjørkedal and Flaten, 2012; Peerdeman et al., 2015; Roderigo et al., 2017; Rhudy et al., 2018) (Table 2).

In sum, six studies showed that placebo effects reduced HR (Robertson et al., 1991; Pollo et al., 2003; Nishikawa et al., 2005; Benedetti et al., 2015; Elsenbruch et al., 2019), and eight studies showed that placebo effects did not impact HR (Aslaksen and Flaten, 2008; Colloca and Benedetti, 2009; Geers et al., 2010, 2015; Bjørkedal and Flaten, 2012; Peerdeman et al., 2015; Roderigo et al., 2017; Rhudy et al., 2018). No nocebo effects on HR was reported (Table 2).

Thus, the results do not suggest a reliable association between the placebo effect and HR.

HRV and Placebo/Nocebo Responses

A total of six studies investigated HRV and placebo.

All six studies used power spectral analysis including time and frequency domains. Moreover, three studies analyzed the LF/HF ratio (Aslaksen and Flaten, 2008; Chae et al., 2008; De Pascalis and Scacchia, 2019); one study considered the coefficient component variance in LF and HF bands (Jegindø et al., 2013); and another study analyzed only HF-HRV (Adler-Neal et al., 2019).

Pollo et al. (2003) showed a decrease in LF-HRV in the placebo condition, as compared with the treatment conditions. Chae et al. (2008) showed that compared with positive information, negative information increased LF-HRV; notably, negative information had no effect on the LF/HF ratio. Aslaksen and Flaten (2008) showed that although the placebo effect did not impact HRV frequency, it lowered the LF/HF ratio. Moreover, HRV predicted a placebo effect on negative emotions. De Pascalis and Scacchia (2019) reported lower pain and increased time-HRV with a slowed-down pace in the placebo condition, as compared with the pain-only condition. Furthermore, although the LF/HF ratio was not impacted, linear HRV measures predicted the placebo effect. Adler-Neal et al. (2019) reported that the sham meditation program lowered pain and HF-HRV (only HF was analyzed). Finally, Jegindø et al. (2013) reported that religious beliefs did not impact HRV components (Table 2).

In sum, two studies showed that placebo effects decreased LF-HRV (Pollo et al., 2003) and that nocebo information increased LF-HRV (Chae et al., 2008), whereas one study showed that expectations did not affect HRV (Jegindø et al., 2013). Two studies showed that HRV predicted placebo responses on negative emotions and pain (Aslaksen and Flaten, 2008; De Pascalis and Scacchia, 2019). One study showed that sham meditation produced lower pain and HF-HRV (Adler-Neal et al., 2019). One study reported a lower LF/HF ratio following the placebo effect (Aslaksen and Flaten, 2008), but two studies showed the placebo effect failed to impact the LF/HF ratio (Chae et al., 2008; De Pascalis and Scacchia, 2019) (Table 2).

The results indicate that the placebo analgesic effect is associated with a decrease in LF-HRV, that the nocebo hyperalgesic effect is associated with an increase in LF-HRV, and that HRV is a predictor for placebo effects. However, there is no reliable effect of placebo on the LF/HF ratio and HF-HRV.

Results of Bias Risk Assessment and Grading the Quality of Evidence

Of the 19 included studies, 14 had a low risk of bias in the randomization process, 13 had a low risk of bias in deviation from intended interventions, 18 had a low risk of bias in missing outcome data, 16 had a low risk of bias in measurement of the outcome, and 16 had a low risk of bias in selection of reported results. Overall, 12 studies were judged as having a low risk of bias (Table 3).

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Cochrane Risk of Bias tool for the included studies.

As presented in Table 4, the first and second findings (i.e., the BP and HR are not reliable cardiac indexes for the assessment of placebo/nocebo cardiac responses) are evaluated as having high certainty. The third finding (i.e., the HRV is a reliable cardiac metric for the assessment of cardiac placebo/nocebo responses), on the other hand, is judged as having low certainty, mainly due to heterogeneity of studies and manipulations, selective reporting, lack of natural history control groups, small sample size, and various statistical methods being used in some of the included studies.

TABLE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Table 4. Grading the certainty of findings.

Discussion

The results of this SR disclosed the following: (1) Of eight studies on placebo and BP, four showed that there is no reliable association between placebo effect and BP, whereas four studies showed that placebo and nocebo effects are followed by lower and higher SBP, respectively. (2) Six of 14 studies on placebo and HR showed that placebo effect lowered HR, while eight studies showed that placebo effects did not impact HR. (3) Of six studies on placebo/nocebo and HRV, two showed that placebo and nocebo effects are associated with lower and higher LF-HRV, respectively. Two studies reported that HRV predicted the placebo effect, and two showed that placebo did not impact the LF/HF ratio. Only one study reported no effects of placebo on HRV (some studies had more than one result). Therefore, these results indicate that the placebo effects have reliable impact on HRV, but not on HR and BP.

Cardiac Placebo Responses and BP

Although four studies showed that placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia were associated with lower and higher SBP, respectively, four other studies did not observe a significant change in BP following a placebo/nocebo effect.

This finding opposes the assumption of an association between placebo effects and lower SBP. All of the four studies that reported no significant relationship between placebo effect and BP used BP as an index to measure physiological stress following a placebo/nocebo effect (Robertson et al., 1991; Geers et al., 2010; Jegindø et al., 2013; Roderigo et al., 2017). Stress and negative emotions can be mediated by placebo effects; however, a placebo effect is not always accompanied with a reduction in stress. For instance, Flaten et al. (2006) showed that although positive information about a weak treatment elicited a placebo effect, it had no impact on stress level. Roderigo et al. (2017) studied the effects of stress on placebo and nocebo effects and showed that although positive information elicited lower pain, it did not impact the physiological stress level as measured by BP and HR. Therefore, a placebo effect is not necessarily associated with a lower BP.

Another reason for the lack of a placebo effect in BP is related to limitations of BP in representing cardiac SA and PA. DBP is highly affected by respiration and does not add important information about cardiac placebo responses and thus is not analyzed in most of the studies, whereas lower and higher SBP indicate higher and lower PA, respectively (Sved, 2009). Therefore, BP will not be sensitive to changes in cardiac SA following a placebo/nocebo effect. Additionally, BP is highly influenced by measurement errors such as individual (e.g., underlying hypertension history) and contextual factors (e.g., white coat hypertension effects) (Pickering et al., 2002), which partially explains the inconsistency in cardiac placebo responses in BP.

Cardiac Placebo Responses and HR

Six studies reported an association between placebo effects and reduced HR, and eight other reported no significant association. This finding contradicts the hypothesis that there should be a reduction in HR following a placebo effect. Our previous explanation in which we suggested that a placebo response can be elicited without significantly affecting the stress level including the autonomic cardiac output (e.g., Geers et al., 2010; Bjørkedal and Flaten, 2012; Roderigo et al., 2017) is applicable here as well.

Another explanation refers to the contribution of both SA and PA on HR. This dual impact of SA and PA on HR may mask cases in which the placebo responses are more relevant to one of the autonomic branches than the other. For instance, Rhudy et al. (2018) showed that although the placebo effect modulated the skin conductance response (SCR), which is a physiological index more related to SA (Dawson et al., 2000), it did not impact HR. This finding suggests that HR failed to detect the placebo response on SA. Moreover, Schneider et al. (2007) reported that although sham acupuncture did not influence HR, it lowered cortisol levels. However, Peerdeman et al. (2015) showed that verbal suggestions did not affect either HR or the SCR.

Cardiac Placebo Responses and HRV

Two of six studies showed that placebo effect reduced LF-HRV and that nocebo effect increased LF-HRV (Pollo et al., 2003; Chae et al., 2008). This is consistent with the hypothesis that presumes lower LF-HRV due to a placebo effect and higher LF-HRV due to a nocebo effect. Several experimental studies have reported higher LF-HRV during pain (e.g., Terkelsen et al., 2005; Pollatos et al., 2012; for a review, see Koenig et al., 2014). For instance, Aslaksen et al. (2007) showed that participants had higher LF-HRV during a heat pain task.

Therefore, contrary to the pain that increases the SA and baroreflex activity, the placebo response reduces the pain and lowers the sympathetic baroreflex activity indexed by lower LF-HRV (Pollo et al., 2003; Chae et al., 2008).

Pollo et al. (2003) showed that both the placebo effect and reduced LF-HRV remained after the muscarine antagonist atropine was administered. This finding, thus, reduces the probability of the involvement of PA in both placebo analgesia and the following cardiac placebo responses. The inverse happens for nocebo effects, in which following an increase in pain due to a nocebo treatment, the sympathetic baroreflex activity increases and leads to higher LF-HRV (Chae et al., 2008).

Two studies reported a change in LF-HRV following a placebo/nocebo effect. Reduction in LF-HRV following a placebo effect indicates decreases in SA, especially the baroreflex activity, and indicates a reduction in physiological stress. The inverse happens for a nocebo effect. Pollo et al. (2003) showed that the reduced LF-HRV following a placebo effect was reversed by the opioid antagonist naloxone and concluded that under stressful circumstances, ANS is controlled by the endogenous opioid system. However, a reduction in physiological stress is not necessarily bonded to a placebo effect, as out of six studies, only two reported such effect; this partially confirms that a placebo effect does not necessarily impact cardiac activity unless it modulates physiological stress level and negative emotions.

Two studies reported that HRV predicted the placebo effect on negative emotions and pain. Along the same line, this predictive value may be seen if the physiological stress level is modulated by placebo. Aslaksen and Flaten (2008) showed that although the placebo capsules did not impact HRV frequency, the placebo group had a lower LF/HF ratio than the control group. However, Chae et al. (2008) and De Pascalis and Scacchia (2019) reported that the placebo had no impact on the LF/HF ratio. This suggests that the LF/HF ratio is not a reliable measure for how placebo impacts cardiac SA and PA. The reliability of the LF/HF ratio has been questioned in previous studies (e.g., Eckberg, 1997). The sympathovagal balance index calculated by the LF/HF ratio has been demonstrated to be theoretically flawed (e.g., Eckberg, 1997) and not empirically supported (e.g., Billman, 2013). The most serious concern is that LF does not index SA (e.g., Houle and Billman, 1999; Goldstein et al., 2011). Thus, there is a lack of rationale and compelling evidence that the LF/HF ratio indexes the relative impact of both vagal activity and SA (e.g., Hopf et al., 1995; Reyes del Paso et al., 2013).

Conclusion

This review investigated the effects of placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia on cardiac activity indexed by BP, HR, and HRV and shows that HRV seems as a better index to detect the effects of placebo/nocebo on cardiac activity.

Cardiac activity is dually controlled by sympathetic and parasympathetic systems, and it is not possible to track the separate effects of each autonomic branch from HR and BP recordings (Thayer and Lane, 2000; Sved, 2009). In line with previous studies (e.g., Pollo et al., 2003; Aslaksen and Flaten, 2008; Koenig et al., 2014), this SR concludes that HRV is a more reliable method to study cardiac placebo analgesic and nocebo hyperalgesic responses since this method can represent both sympathetic and parasympathetic influences on cardiac activity (Malik, 1996; Task force of the European society of cardiology the north American society of pacing electrophysiology., 1996).

Furthermore, the results of this SR confirm that the elicitation of a placebo effect is not necessarily dependent on the modulation of autonomic and physiological stress levels, since a placebo can occur without significantly affecting the physiological stress (e.g., Flaten et al., 2006). This assumption partially explains why a considerable number of studies fail to observe a cardiac placebo response (e.g., Colloca and Benedetti, 2009; Geers et al., 2010; Bjørkedal and Flaten, 2012).

Recommendation for Prospective Studies

Firstly, to measure cardiac placebo/nocebo responses, the present SR provides evidence that BP and HR are not optimal in detecting cardiac placebo responses and therefore recommends HRV over BP and HR; however, as the quality of evidence supporting this recommendation is rated as low, more documentation is needed to underpin the advantages of HRV in placebo studies. Secondly, sex differences in cardiac activity were reported in five studies; however, only three analyzed sex differences on cardiac placebo responses (Robertson et al., 1991; Bjørkedal and Flaten, 2012; Roderigo et al., 2017) (see Table 2); two studies reported that females had lower SBP than males, but the effect of sex on cardiac placebo responses was not reported (Aslaksen et al., 2014, 2015). However, two other studies reported that participants' sex did not affect BP following a placebo effect (Robertson et al., 1991; Roderigo et al., 2017). Of three studies that analyzed sex differences in HR following a placebo effect, one reported that females' placebo responses were accompanied with a reduced HR as compared with males (Robertson et al., 1991), whereas two studies reported no sex differences on HR following a placebo (Bjørkedal and Flaten, 2012; Roderigo et al., 2017). Although Aslaksen and Flaten (2008) showed that male subjects reported higher placebo effect to male experimenters, participants' sex differences in HRV were not analyzed; and no other studies considered sex differences in HRV following a placebo/nocebo effect. Therefore, potential sex differences in cardiac placebo responses remain to be investigated by future studies. Thirdly, this SR focused on the effects of placebo analgesia and nocebo hyperalgesia on cardiac activity; however, the effects of placebo on cardiac activity in other symptoms (e.g., itch, hypertension, and fatigue) are not well-known and need more investigation. Fourth, this SR investigated cardiac placebo responses and concluded that a placebo effect does not necessarily lower physiological stress. This speculation needs to be investigated using other physiological stress measurements (SCR, endocrine secretion, electroencephalography, etc.) as well.

Limitations

There are some limitations to this SR. Firstly, as this is a qualitative SR, generalizing findings to other fields and symptoms requires caution. Secondly, the majority of the included studies were on healthy volunteer samples. Therefore, extrapolating findings to, e.g., patients with chronic pain must be done with caution. Thirdly, due to different terms and inconsistent keywords used across studies, access to all relevant studies may have been limited. However, to ensure the inclusion of all relevant studies, we manually reviewed the citation and reference lists of all included studies, as in similar previous SRs (Daniali and Flaten, 2019). A fourth limitation is the lack of a thorough report on the method of cardiac data recording as in Aslaksen et al. (2014) or a thorough report on cardiac analysis method as in Roderigo et al. (2017), which hinders a clear conclusion. Fifthly, although HRV is recommended over BP or HR, the lower number of HRV studies than that of BP or HR studies has limited the quality of evidence supporting this notion. Finally, although this SR did not have a review protocol, the scientific nature of the study was precisely characterized by specifying a priori questions and a relevant review procedure.

Data Availability Statement

Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This is a review article based on published Works.

Author Contributions

HD and MF both had significant contribution in planning the study, reviewing the articles, analyzing the results, writing the draft, and preparing the final manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

The Norwegian University of Science and Technology (NTNU) funded this study.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Adamczyk, W. M., Wiercioch-Kuzianik, K., Bajcar, E. A., and Babel, P. (2019). Rewarded placebo analgesia: a new mechanism of placebo effects based on operant conditioning. Eur. J. Pain 23, 923–935. doi: 10.1002/ejp.1360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ader, R. (1997). “The role of conditioning in pharmacotherapy,” in The Placebo Effect: An Interdisciplinary Exploration, ed A, Harrington (Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press), 138–165.

Google Scholar

Adler-Neal, A. L., Waugh, C. E., Garland, E. L., Shaltout, H. A., Diz, D. I., and Zeidan, F. (2019). The role of heart rate variability in mindfulness-based pain relief. J. Pain. 21, 306–323. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2019.07.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Amigo, I., Cuesta, V., Fernandez, A., and Gonzalez, A. (1993). The effect of verbal instructions on blood pressure measurement. J. Hypertens. 11, 293–296. doi: 10.1097/00004872-199303000-00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aslaksen, P. M., and Flaten, M. A. (2008). The roles of physiological and subjective stress in the effectiveness of a placebo on experimentally induced pain. Psychosom Med. 70, 811–818. doi: 10.1097/PSY.0b013e31818105ed

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aslaksen, P. M., and Lyby, P. S. (2015). Fear of pain potentiates nocebo hyperalgesia. J. Pain Res. 8:703. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S91923

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aslaksen, P. M., Myrbakk, I. N., Høifødt, R. S., and Flaten, M. A. (2007). The effect of experimenter gender on autonomic and subjective responses to pain stimuli. Pain 129, 260–268. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aslaksen, P. M., Vasylenko, O., and Fagerlund, A. J. (2014). The effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on experimentally induced heat pain. Exp. Brain Res. 232, 1865–1873. doi: 10.1007/s00221-014-3878-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Aslaksen, P. M., Zwarg, M. L., Eilertsen, H.-I. H., Gorecka, M. M., and Bjørkedal, E. (2015). Opposite effects of the same drug: reversal of topical analgesia by nocebo information. Pain 156, 39–46. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.0000000000000004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babel, P. (2019). Classical conditioning as a distinct mechanism of placebo effects. Front. Psychiatry 10:449. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00449

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babel, P. (2020). Operant conditioning as a new mechanism of placebo effects. Eur. J. Pain 24, 902–908. doi: 10.1002/ejp.1544

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Babel, P., Adamczyk, W., Swider, K., Bajcar, E. A., Kicman, P., and Lisińska, N. (2018). How classical conditioning shapes placebo analgesia: hidden versus open conditioning. Pain Med. 19, 1156–1169. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnx177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bajcar, E. A., and Babel, P. (2018). How does observational learning produce placebo effects? A model integrating research findings. Front. Psychol. 9:2041. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02041

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bajcar, E. A., Wiercioch-Kuzianik, K., Adamczyk, W. M., and Babel, P. (2020). To experience or to be informed? Classical conditioning induces nocebo hyperalgesia even when placebo analgesia is verbally suggested—results of a preliminary study. Pain Med. 21, 548–560. doi: 10.1093/pm/pnz123

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedetti, F. (2008). Mechanisms of placebo and placebo-related effects across diseases and treatments. Annu. Rev. Pharmacol. Toxicol. 48, 33–60. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.48.113006.094711

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benedetti, F., Durando, J., Giudetti, L., Pampallona, A., and Vighetti, S. (2015). High-altitude headache: the effects of real vs sham oxygen administration. Pain 156, 2326–2336. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Berntson, G. G., Quigley, K. S., Norman, G. J., and Lozano, D. L. (2017). “Cardiovascular psychophysiology,” in Cambridge Handbooks in Psychology. Handbook of Psychophysiology, eds. J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, and G. G. Berntson (New York, NY: Cambridge University Press), 183–216.

Google Scholar

Billman, G. E. (2013). The LF/HF ratio does not accurately measure cardiac sympatho-vagal balance. Front. Physiol. 4:26. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2013.00026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bjørkedal, E., and Flaten, M. A. (2012). Expectations of increased and decreased pain explain the effect of conditioned pain modulation in females. J. Pain Res. 5:289. doi: 10.2147/JPR.S33559

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brozek, J. L., Akl, E. A., Alonso-Coello, P., Lang, D., Jaeschke, R., Williams, J. W., et al. (2009). Grading quality of evidence and strength of recommendations in clinical practice guidelines: part 1 of 3. An overview of the GRADE approach and grading quality of evidence about interventions. Allergy 64, 669–677. doi: 10.1111/j.1398-9995.2009.01973.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chae, Y., Kim, S.-Y., Park, H.-S., Lee, H., and Park, H.-J. (2008). Experimentally manipulating perceptions regarding acupuncture elicits different responses to the identical acupuncture stimulation. Physiol. Behav. 95, 515–520. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2008.07.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Citi, L., Brown, E. N., and Barbieri, R. (2012). A real-time automated point-process method for the detection and correction of erroneous and ectopic heartbeats. IEEE Trans. Biomed. Eng. 59, 2828–2837. doi: 10.1109/TBME.2012.2211356

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colloca, L., and Benedetti, F. (2009). Placebo analgesia induced by social observational learning. Pain 144, 28–34. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2009.01.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. (2003). A new view of pain as a homeostatic emotion. Trends Neurosci. 26, 303–307. doi: 10.1016/S0166-2236(03)00123-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Craig, A. D. (2002). How do you feel? Interoception: the sense of the physiological condition of the body. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3:655. doi: 10.1038/nrn894

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Daniali, H., and Flaten, M. A. (2019). A qualitative systematic review of effects of provider characteristics and nonverbal behavior on pain, and placebo and nocebo effects. Front. Psychiatry 10:242. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00242

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dawson, M. E., Schell, A. M., and Filion, D. L. (2000). “The electrodermal system,” in Handbook of Psychophysiology, 2nd ed. eds. J. T. Cacioppo, L. G. Tassinary, and G. B. Bernston (Cambridge: Cambridge Press), 200–223.

Google Scholar

De Pascalis, V., and Scacchia, P. (2019). The influence of reward sensitivity, heart rate dynamics and EEG-delta activity on placebo analgesia. Behav. Brain Res. 359, 320–332. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.11.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Drucker, A. M., Fleming, P., and Chan, A.-W. (2016). Research techniques made simple: assessing risk of bias in systematic reviews. J. Invest. Dermatol. 136, e109–e114. doi: 10.1016/j.jid.2016.08.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eckberg, D. L. (1997). Sympathovagal balance: a critical appraisal. Circulation 96, 3224–3232. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.96.9.3224

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Elsenbruch, S., Roderigo, T., Enck, P., and Benson, S. (2019). Can a brief relaxation exercise modulate placebo or nocebo effects in a visceral pain model? Front. Psychiatry 10:144. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2019.00144

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaten, M. A. (2013). “Placebo responses, antagonistic responses, and homeostasis,” in Placebo and Pain: From Bench to Bedside, eds L. Colloca, M. A. Flaten, and K. Meissner (San Diego: Academic), 103–113. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-397928-5.00011-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaten, M. A., Aslaksen, P. M., Finset, A., Simonsen, T., and Johansen, O. (2006). Cognitive and emotional factors in placebo analgesia. J. Psychos. Res. 61, 81–89. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2005.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaten, M. A., Aslaksen, P. M., and Lyby, P. S. (2013). “Positive and negative emotions and placebo analgesia,” in Placebo and Pain (Elsevier) 73–81. doi: 10.1016/B978-0-12-397928-5.00008-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaten, M. A., Aslaksen, P. M., Lyby, P. S., and Bjørkedal, E. (2011). The relation of emotions to placebo responses. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 366, 1818–1827. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2010.0407

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaten, M. A., Simonsen, T., and Olsen, H. (1999). Drug-related information generates placebo and nocebo responses that modify the drug response. Psychos. Med. 61, 250–255. doi: 10.1097/00006842-199903000-00018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flaten, M. A., Simonsen, T., Waterloo, K. K., and Olsen, H. (1997). Pharmacological classical conditioning in humans. Hum. Psychopharmacol. Clin. Exp. 12, 369–377. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1099-1077(199707/08)12:4<369::AID-HUP881>3.0.CO;2-D

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

France, C., and Ditto, B. (1996). Risk for high blood pressure and decreased pain perception. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 5, 120–125. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.ep11452781

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geers, A. L., Fowler, S. L., Wellman, J. A., Helfer, S. G., Close, S., and France, C. R. (2015). Prior experience with a pain stimulus as a predictor of placebo analgesia. J. Behav. Med. 38, 136–142. doi: 10.1007/s10865-014-9586-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Geers, A. L., Wellman, J. A., Fowler, S. L., Helfer, S. G., and France, C. R. (2010). Dispositional optimism predicts placebo analgesia. J. Pain 11, 1165–1171. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2010.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goldstein, D. S., Bentho, O., Park, M. Y., and Sharabi, Y. (2011). Low-frequency power of heart rate variability is not a measure of cardiac sympathetic tone but may be a measure of modulation of cardiac autonomic outflows by baroreflexes. Exp. Physiol. 96, 1255–1261. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.2010.056259

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopf, H. B., Skyschally, A., Heusch, G., and Peters, J. (1995). Low-frequency spectral power of heart rate variability is not a specific marker of cardiac sympathetic modulation. Anesthesiology 82, 609–619. doi: 10.1097/00000542-199503000-00002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Houle, M. S., and Billman, G. E. (1999). Low-frequency component of the heart rate variability spectrum: a poor marker of sympathetic activity. Am. J. Physiol. Heart Circulat. Physiol. 267, H215–H223. doi: 10.1152/ajpheart.1999.276.1.H215

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hrobjartsson, A., and Gotzsche, P. C. (2010). Placebo interventions for all clinical conditions. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 1:CD003974. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003974.pub3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunter, T., Siess, F., and Colloca, L. (2014). Socially induced placebo analgesia: a comparison of a pre-recorded versus live face-to-face observation. Eur. J. Pain 18, 914–922. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00436.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jegindø, E.-M. E., Vase, L., Skewes, J. C., Terkelsen, A. J., Hansen, J., Geertz, A. W., et al. (2013). Expectations contribute to reduced pain levels during prayer in highly religious participants. J. Behav. Med. 36, 413–426. doi: 10.1007/s10865-012-9438-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kirsch, I. (2004). Conditioning, Expectancy, and the Placebo Effect: Comment on Stewart-Williams and Podd. Psychol. Bull. 130, 341–343. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.2.341

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koenig, J., Jarczok, M., Ellis, R., Hillecke, T., and Thayer, J. F. (2014). Heart rate variability and experimentally induced pain in healthy adults: a systematic review. Eur. J. Pain 18, 301–314. doi: 10.1002/j.1532-2149.2013.00379.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loggia, M. L., Juneau, M., and Bushnell, M. C. (2011). Autonomic responses to heat pain: heart rate, skin conductance, and their relation to verbal ratings and stimulus intensity. Pain, 152, 592–598. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2010.11.032

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Malik, M. (1996), Heart rate variability. Ann. Noninvasive Electrocardiol. 1, 151–181. doi: 10.1111/j.1542-474X.1996.tb00275.x.

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meissner, K. (2014). “Placebo responses on cardiovascular, gastrointestinal, and respiratory organ functions,” in Placebo. Handbook of Experimental Pharmacology, Vol. 225. eds. F. Benedetti, P. Enck, E. Frisaldi, M. Schedlowski (Berlin, Heidelberg: Springer), 183–215. doi: 10.1007/978-3-662-44519-8_11

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Minarini, G. (2020). “Root mean square of the successive differences as marker of the parasympathetic system and difference in the outcome after ANS stimulation,” in Autonomic Nervous System Monitoring-Heart Rate Variability. (IntechOpen). doi: 10.5772/intechopen.89827

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moher, D., Liberati, A., Tetzlaff, J., and Altman, D. G. PRISMA Group. (2009). Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ 339:b2535. doi: 10.1136/bmj.b2535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nishikawa, H., Nakamura, K., and Nakano, S. (2005). Adequate information to patients on lorazepam and its expected actions enhances the antianxiety effect of this drug during dental treatment. J. Clin. Pharmacol. Ther. 36, 89–100. doi: 10.3999/jscpt.36.2_89

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peerdeman, K. J., van Laarhoven, A. I., Donders, A. R. T., Hopman, M. T., Peters, M. L., and Evers, A. W. (2015). Inducing expectations for health: effects of verbal suggestion and imagery on pain, itch, and fatigue as indicators of physical sensitivity. PLoS ONE 10:e0139563. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0139563

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pickering, T. G., Gerin, W., and Schwartz, A. R. (2002). What is the white-coat effect and how should it be measured? Blood Pressure Monitor. 7, 293–300. doi: 10.1097/00126097-200212000-00001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Füstös, J., and Critchley, H. D. (2012). On the generalised embodiment of pain: how interoceptive sensitivity modulates cutaneous pain perception. Pain 153, 1680–1686. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2012.04.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollo, A., Vighetti, S., Rainero, I., and Benedetti, F. (2003). Placebo analgesia and the heart. Pain 102, 125–133. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00345-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reyes del Paso, G. A., Langewitz, W., Mulder, L. J., Roon, A., and Duschek, S. (2013). The utility of low frequency heart rate variability as an index of sympathetic cardiac tone: a review with emphasis on a reanalysis of previous studies. Psychophysiology 50, 477–487. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rhudy, J. L., Güereca, Y. M., Kuhn, B. L., Palit, S., and Flaten, M. A. (2018). The influence of placebo analgesia manipulations on pain report, the nociceptive flexion reflex, and autonomic responses to pain. J. Pain 19, 1257–1274. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2018.04.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, C., Gatchel, R. J., and Fowler, C. (1991). Effectiveness of a videotaped behavioral intervention in reducing anxiety in emergency oral surgery patients. Behav. Med. 17, 77–85. doi: 10.1080/08964289.1991.9935162

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roderigo, T., Benson, S., Schöls, M., Hetkamp, M., Schedlowski, M., Enck, P., et al. (2017). Effects of acute psychological stress on placebo and nocebo responses in a clinically relevant model of visceroception. Pain 158, 1489–1498. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schneider, A., Weiland, C., Enck, P., Joos, S., Streitberger, K., Maser-Gluth, C., et al. (2007). Neuroendocrinological effects of acupuncture treatment in patients with irritable bowel syndrome. Complement. Ther. Med. 15, 255–263. doi: 10.1016/j.ctim.2006.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shaffer, F., McCraty, R., and Zerr, C. L. (2014). A healthy heart is not a metronome: an integrative review of the heart's anatomy and heart rate variability. Front. Psychol. 5:1040. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01040

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sterne, J. A. C., Page, M. J., Elbers, R. G., Blencowe, N. S., Boutron, I., et al. (2019). RoB 2: a revised tool for assessing risk of bias in randomised trials. BMJ 366:l4898. doi: 10.1136/bmj.l4898

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stovner, L. J., Oftedal, G., Straume, A., and Johnsson, A. (2008). Nocebo as headache trigger: evidence from a sham-controlled provocation study with RF fields. Acta Neurol. Scand. 117, 67–71. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0404.2008.01035.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sved, A. F. (2009). #x0201C;Blood pressure: baroreceptors,” in Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, ed. L. R. Squire (Oxford: Academic Press), 259–264. doi: 10.1016/B978-008045046-9.00468-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Task force of the European society of cardiology and the north American society of pacing electrophysiology. (1996). Heart rate variability. Standards of measurements, physiological interpretation, and clinical use. Circulation 93, 1043–1065. doi: 10.1161/01.CIR.93.5.1043

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Terkelsen, A. J., Mølgaard, H., Hansen, J., Andersen, O. K., and Jensen, T. S. (2005). Acute pain increases heart rate: differential mechanisms during rest and mental stress. Auton. Neurosci. 121, 101–109. doi: 10.1016/j.autneu.2005.07.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F. (2009). “Heart rate variability: a neurovisceral integration model,” in Encyclopedia of Neuroscience, ed L. R. Squire (Oxford: Academic Press), 1041–1047. doi: 10.1016/B978-008045046-9.01991-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., Hansen, A. L., and Johnsen, B. H. (2008). “Noninvasive assessment of autonomic influences on the heart: Impedance cardiography and heart rate variability,” in Handbook of Physiological Research Methods in Health Psychology, eds. L. J. Luecken and L. C. Gallo (Thousand oaks: Sage Publications Inc.), 183–209.

Google Scholar

Thayer, J. F., and Lane, R. D. (2000). A model of neurovisceral integration in emotion regulation and dysregulation. J. Aff. Disord. 61, 201–216. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(00)00338-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimmermann-Viehoff, F., Meissner, K., Koch, J., Weber, C. S., Richter, S., and Deter, H. C. (2013). Autonomic effects of suggestive placebo interventions to increase or decrease blood pressure: a randomized controlled trial in healthy subjects. J. Psychoso. Res. 75, 32–35. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2013.03.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: placebo effects, nocebo effects, cardiac activity, blood pressure, heart rate, heart rate variability, pain, review—systematic

Citation: Daniali H and Flaten MA (2020) Placebo Analgesia, Nocebo Hyperalgesia, and the Cardiovascular System: A Qualitative Systematic Review. Front. Physiol. 11:549807. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2020.549807

Received: 05 June 2020; Accepted: 17 August 2020;
Published: 25 September 2020.

Edited by:

Vitor Engracia Valenti, São Paulo State University, Brazil

Reviewed by:

Przemysław Babel, Jagiellonian University, Poland
Rebecca Webster, The University of Sheffield, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Daniali and Flaten. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Magne Arve Flaten, magne.a.flaten@ntnu.no

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.