Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Physiol., 29 October 2019
Sec. Craniofacial Biology and Dental Research
This article is part of the Research Topic Orofacial Functions: From Neural Mechanisms to Rehabilitation View all 50 articles

Effects of Chronic and Experimental Acute Masseter Pain on Precision Biting Behavior in Humans

\r\nSamaa Al Sayegh,*Samaa Al Sayegh1,2*Annie BorgwardtAnnie Borgwardt1Krister G. SvenssonKrister G. Svensson3Abhishek Kumar,Abhishek Kumar1,2Anastasios Grigoriadis,Anastasios Grigoriadis1,2Nikolaos Christidis,Nikolaos Christidis1,2
  • 1Division of Oral Diagnostics and Rehabilitation, Department of Dental Medicine, Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge, Sweden
  • 2Scandinavian Center for Orofacial Neurosciences, Huddinge, Sweden
  • 3Swedish Armed Forces Headquarters, Medical Service, Stockholm, Sweden

Chronic pain in the orofacial region is common worldwide. Pain seems to affect the jaw motor control. Hence, temporomandibular disorders (TMD) are often accompanied by pain upon chewing, restricted mouth opening and impaired maximal bite forces. However, little is known on the effects of pain, in particular the effects of chronic jaw muscle pain on precision biting. The aim of the study was to investigate the effect of chronic and acute jaw muscle pain on oral motor control during precision biting in humans. Eighteen patients with chronic masseter muscle pain and 18 healthy participants completed the experiment. All participants were examined according to the Diagnostic Criteria for TMD. Experimental acute pain was induced by bilateral, simultaneous sterile hypertonic saline infusions into the healthy masseter muscles. A standardized hold and split biting task was used to assess the precision biting. The data was analyzed with non-parametric statistical tests. The results showed no significant differences in the hold forces, split forces, durations of split or peak split rates within or between the pain and pain-free conditions. The mean split rate increased significantly compared to baseline values both in the chronic patients and the pain-free condition. However, this increase was not evident in the experimental acute pain condition. Further, there were no significant differences in the mean split rates between the conditions. The data suggest that jaw muscle pain does not seem to alter precision biting in humans, however, the possibility that a nociceptive modulation of spindle afferent activity might have occurred but compensated for cannot be ruled out.

Introduction

Chronic pain is a large and worldwide health problem with approximately 20% of the population reporting chronic pain of moderate to severe intensity (Breivik et al., 2006). The orofacial region is one of the most frequent locations for chronic pains, with a prevalence of 5–33% worldwide (Macfarlane et al., 2001, 2002, 2004; Isong et al., 2008; Schiffman et al., 2014). The most common diagnosis of the temporomandibular disorders (TMD) is masticatory muscle pain, i.e., myalgia (LeResche, 1997). Previous studies have shown that pain affects jaw motor control (Lund et al., 1991; Svensson and Graven-Nielsen, 2001; Murray and Peck, 2007) hence TMD is often accompanied by pain upon chewing, restricted mouth opening and impaired maximal (premolar/molar) bite force (Felício et al., 2002; Goiato et al., 2017; Xu et al., 2017). In the past, different theories have attempted to explain how pain and jaw function interrelate (Parker, 1990; Lund et al., 1991; Stohler, 1999; Svensson and Graven-Nielsen, 2001; Lobbezoo et al., 2006; Murray and Peck, 2007; Svensson and Kumar, 2016). However, several studies have either failed to or have presented contrary data to the fundaments of these theories (Stohler et al., 1988; Lund et al., 1991; Svensson et al., 1997, 1998; Svensson and Graven-Nielsen, 2001; Sae-Lee et al., 2008a, b). This contrasting evidence in the association between pain-related TMD and masticatory muscle function led to the integrated pain adaptation model which was presented in 2007 (Murray and Peck, 2007). Accordingly, pain is biopsychosocial and the relationship between pain and function may be more complex than proposed by the previous theories (Ohrbach et al., 2011, 2013; Bair et al., 2013). The theory proposes that just as an individual’s experience of pain varies, so will also an individual’s motor response.

Most evidence for how pain and jaw function interact comes from studies where pain was induced by injecting chemical substances in healthy subjects (Svensson and Graven-Nielsen, 2001; Graven-Nielsen, 2006; Kumar et al., 2015a, b). The use of pain models to mimic clinical pain is essential in research because the cause of pain is known and the effect can be controlled in such models. However, even if these pain models resemble clinical TMD-pain it is not certain that the induced pain can be exactly compared with the real, chronic TMD-condition. During a chronic pain condition changes in the nervous system occur causing central sensitization, which in turn may affect the motor function (Woolf and Walters, 1991; Woolf and Salter, 2000; Latremoliere and Woolf, 2009). Therefore, there is a greater need for studies that aim to investigate the mechanisms behind chronic musculoskeletal pain as well as further investigations of the contrasting evidence for the association between pain-related TMD and masticatory muscle function.

Human biting and chewing behaviors are controlled by a complex sensory-motor regulation involving the face primary motor cortex, the cortical masticatory area, and the central pattern generator (CPG) and coordinated by the sensory information from the periodontal mechanoreceptors (PMR’s), jaw muscle spindles, articular temporomandibular joint (TMJ) receptors, and pulpal mechanoreceptors as well as other orofacial features (Dellow and Lund, 1971; Nozaki et al., 1986; Lund, 1991; Trulsson and Johansson, 1996a, 2002; Türker, 2002; Lund and Kolta, 2006; Trulsson, 2006; van der Bilt et al., 2006; Grigoriadis et al., 2014; Kumar et al., 2014). Further, orofacial pain may be a potential modifier of mastication and jaw motor control (Clark et al., 1984) and therefore it is of great importance to investigate how pain affects human jaw function. Therefore, the aim of the study was to investigate the effect of chronic pain on oral motor force control during precision biting and compare it with experimentally induced acute pain in healthy controls. Our hypothesis was that chronic jaw muscle pain would affect the precise biting behavior and this would be reflected in the higher holding forces during the biting task (Capra et al., 2007), similar to the higher forces caused by alterations of the PMR’s (Svensson and Trulsson, 2011). Furthermore, we also hypothesized that the duration of the split phase should increase in chronic pain patients due to the fear of pain increase (Nicholas, 2007). On the other hand, the experimental acute pain model was not hypothesized to alter the fine motor control and would not affect the hold force and split duration in healthy participants similarly to other previously used models (Kumar et al., 2015a).

Materials and Methods

This controlled, experimental, clinical study was conducted at the Department of Dental Medicine, Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge, Sweden. The study was performed in accordance with the declaration of Helsinki and approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Stockholm (DNR: 2014/1394-3). The chronic TMD-pain patients were recruited at the Specialist Clinic at the University Dental Clinic, Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge, Sweden and Eastmaninstitutet Folktandvården Stockholms län AB, Stockholm, Sweden. The healthy participants were recruited at the Department of Dental Medicine or the University Dental Clinic, Karolinska Institutet, Huddinge, Sweden. Before inclusion, all participants were given both verbal as well as written information about this study and an informed written consent was obtained.

Participants

A sample size calculation revealed that 17 participants in each group was required to show a mean (SD) difference of 30% between the groups, with a power of 80% and a significance level of 0.05 (Svensson and Trulsson, 2011). Hence, 22 patients (mean age ± SD = 34 ± 12 years) with chronic myalgia including myofascial pain with referred pain in the masseter muscles with/without temporal myalgia were recruited in the study. Additionally, 22 pain-free, sex- and age-matched healthy volunteers (mean age ± SD = 33 ± 11 years), were also included in the study as controls. Therefore, the patient group comprised of 16 women (mean age ± SD = 35 ± 13 years) and six men (mean age ± SD = 30 ± 7 years), the control group also comprised of 16 women (mean age ± SD = 34 ± 13 years) and six men (mean age ± SD = 30 ± 6 years). The healthy controls also acted as the experimental acute pain group.

Further, out of the 22 included participants in each group 18 participants (14 women and 4 men) from the chronic patient group and 18 participants (12 women and 6 men) from the control group provided complete data for the analysis. The four remaining participants in each group had either incomplete data or were identified as outliers. This data was handled as missing data in the statistical analyses (identification of outliers is described further below).

The inclusion criteria for participation in the study were: (a) age over 18 years; (b) intact natural central incisors with normal relation to antagonistic teeth; (c) individuals capable to protrude the lower jaw in order to be able to perform the hold and split task. Exclusion criteria were: (1) a diagnosis of arthralgia, degenerative joint disease painful jaw clicking or locking according to (DC/TMD) (Schiffman et al., 2014); (2) clinically visible dental pathology or mobility, toothache, severe malocclusions, tooth wear grade 3 = exposure of pulp or secondary dentine according to the simplified scoring criteria for tooth wear index I (López-Frías et al., 2012), (3) earlier trauma to the anterior teeth; (4) root-canal treatments in the anterior teeth, orthodontic retainer, fixed prosthodontics (implants, bridges, crowns) in the anterior teeth, dentures; (5) systemic inflammatory diseases (i.e., rheumatoid arthritis, fibromyalgia, etc.), neuropathic pain or neurological disease; (6) whiplash associated disorder; (7) use of any medication that might influence the response of pain i.e., analgesics during 24 h preceding the experiment, use of cannabinoids, or any medication that might influence the neurological function; (8) allergy to any of the substances or food used in the experiment; (9) pregnancy or lactation; and (10) cognitive or physical disability that prevent participation.

Additional inclusion criteria for patient group were: (d) a diagnosis of local myalgia or myofascial pain or myofascial pain with referred pain in the masseter muscle according to the diagnostic criteria for temporomandibular disorders (DC/TMD); (e) a pain duration of at least 3 months; (f) current pain with a minimum score of 3 according to numeric rating scale (NRS 0-10).

For the healthy individuals the additional inclusion criteria were: (d) good general health. Additional exclusion criteria were: (11) a diagnosis of myalgia or myofascial pain according to the DC/TMD; (12) additional palpatory tenderness of the masseter, temporalis muscles or over the TMJ.

Experimental Protocol

Participants answered questionnaires regarding anxiety (generalized anxiety disorder scale-7; GAD-7) (Löwe et al., 2008), depression (the patient health questionnaire for depression-9; PHQ-9) (Kroenke et al., 2001), physical symptoms (PHQ-15, the patient health questionnaire for physical symptoms-15) (Kroenke et al., 2002), and stress (PSS-10, perceived stress scale-10) (Nordin and Nordin, 2013). Patients with myalgia answered questionnaires regarding chronic pain (graded chronic pain scale-7; G-7) (Von Korff et al., 1992). Prior to the inclusion, all participants were clinically examined according to DC/TMD. The experimental protocol and time points of assessments are illustrated in Figure 1A for the chronic pain and 1B for the pain-free and experimental acute pain. During the single experimental session of about 1 h, the chronic pain patients as well as the pain-free healthy controls were asked to perform ten trials of a standardized hold and split biting task (Trulsson and Johansson, 1996b) after performing five training trials. In order to simulate the experimental acute pain condition, simultaneous bilateral infusions of 0.4 ml of sterile hypertonic saline (58.5 mg/ml) into both masseter muscles were performed in the healthy controls during 20 s. The infusions were performed by an infusion pump (infusion rate 1200 μl/min; Harvard Infusion Pump 22, Harvard Apparatus, Great Britain) (Christidis et al., 2008), as shown in Figure 1C. The healthy controls were asked to perform ten trials while in pain. For all participants in all condition-groups, pain intensity was recorded before the task (for the experimental acute pain condition immediately after injection), at peak pain and at the end of the task. The experimental acute pain participants were also instructed to inform when the pain intensity was below the level of three on the 11-graded NRS-scale (0–10). While the chronic pain patients were asked to comment if their pain increased, decreased or remained unchanged after performing the hold and split task. After the task, all participants were asked to mark the maximum pain spread on a pain drawing. In the digital analysis, the scanned pain areas (i.e., the marks on the pain drawings) were analyzed in arbitrary units (au). Pain drawings provided visual illustration and quantitatively described the pattern and location of pain as well as referred pain (Wright, 2000). Three examiners (SA, AB, and KS) led the trials and were trained together in giving the instructions in the same manner according to a standardized protocol.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Flow-chart illustrates the experimental protocol for chronic pain (A). BL, baseline before the task; BL, start of the task; E, end of the task. Flow-chart illustrates the experimental protocol for pain-free and experimental acute pain (B). BL, baseline before the task; s, second; E, end of the task. DC/TMD, Diagnostic criteria for temporomandibular disorders; NRS, numeric rating scale. Participant performing the hold and split task and the equipment used during the experiment (C). Estrella half peanut was placed on force transducer during acute induced pain in the masseter muscles bilaterally by infusion of hypertonic saline with Harvard infusion pump. A written informed consent was obtained from the participant in the figure for the publication of the image. A representative schematic force profile (upper trace) and force rate profile (lower trace) of one hold and split trial as shown in the WinZoom program (D). (a) Initial contact with the food, (b) Initiation of splitting, (c) the split force and end of the split phase, (d) duration of the split phase, (e) peak rate of split force, and (f) hold phase, interval beginning 0.2 s after initial contact with the food and ending 0.2 s prior to the onset of the split phase.

Hold and Split Task

The hold and split task simulated the natural behavior of positioning/holding and contracting the jaw muscles to apply the optimum force needed for splitting food. The task was firstly described by Trulsson and Johansson in 1996 (Trulsson and Johansson, 1996b) using peanuts as test food. Many studies embraced the same methodology later on using a custom-built apparatus (Umeå University, Physiology Section, IMB, Umeå, Sweden) of the same design (Johnsen et al., 2007; Svensson and Trulsson, 2009, 2011; Kumar et al., 2014, 2015a, 2017, 2019). The apparatus consisted of 11 cm-long plastic-covered metal handle with a diameter of 7 mm connected to two duralumin blocks that terminate in two parallel rectangular plates. The total weight of the plates was 48 g and the stiffness between the plates was 50 N/mm while the total length of the apparatus was 17 cm. The upper duralumin block contained strain gauge force transducers for assessment of the forces applied to the plate (DC 200 Hz). The apparatus was designed to insure that the force assessment is independent of where the force was applied to the plate (Svensson and Trulsson, 2009). A half of a roasted and salted peanut (Estrella salta jordnötter; Estrella AB, Angered, Sweden) was placed on the free-end of the plate. A less than 0.1 mm thin piece of plastic-coated fabric tape on the top of the upper plate prevents the peanut from slipping while the apparatus was being positioned. The lower plate, which was placed with 8 mm distance between surfaces from the upper plate, was equipped with a piece of plexiglass designed to function as an anterior stop while positioning of the lower incisors. Participants used their preferred hand to place the apparatus between the upper and lower right or left central incisors in order to hold the apparatus in a horizontal position. The participants placed the lower plate on the lower incisor and slid the apparatus until the anterior stop was reached and the edge of the upper antagonist central incisor was positioned near the middle of the peanut, as shown in Figure 1C. The participants were instructed to hold the peanut between their incisors and not to use more force than necessary to control the peanut (Trulsson and Gunne, 1998). After approximately 3–5 s, the participants were asked to split the peanut. The forces applied by the incisors were continuously monitored during the task. If the peanut was dropped before the holding phase, a new trial with a new peanut was recorded. On the other hand, if the peanut was dropped before splitting, the trial was observed as a failure.

Data Analyses

The force data were collected and analyzed using a customized software (WinSC/WinZoom v1.52.0.1; Umeå University, Umeå, Sweden) with 12-bit resolution at 800 Hz. Force rates were obtained by symmetrical numerical time differentiation ± 5 points from/to the force signal. The initial contact (a) and the onset of the split phase (b) were both reliably identified from the force-rate signal. The beginning of the split phase was detected at the point at which the force rate exceeded 5 N/s, the minimum rate of force increase that could be reliably detected in a single trial. The hold force was measured as the mean value of the force during the interval (f) – beginning 0.2 s after initial contact with the food (a) and ending 0.2 s before the onset (b) of the split phase. The split phase was characterized by a distinct rapid increase in force (b to c), which eventually split the food morsel. The split force/the end of the split phase (c) was defined as the peak force prior to the moment the morsel split, indicated by a rapid decrease in the force. The duration of the split phase (d) was defined as the time from the onset of the split phase (b) to the end of the split phase (c). The mean split force rate was defined as the force increase from the onset (b) to the end (c) of the split phase, divided by the duration of the split phase (d). The peak split force rate (e) defined as the max split force rate (steepest slope in the split phase profile) was identified by the WinZoom program. This is schematically shown in Figure 1D (Svensson and Trulsson, 2009).

Incomplete force trials as well as trials that were observed as failures due to dropping the peanut before splitting were handled as missing data. The peanut slippage was observed five times in the chronic condition (two patients), while this occurred once in the pain-free condition and once during acute pain condition (the same participant). Since the participants were not allowed to train more than five times before performing the hold and split task all trials’ profiles were manually checked and outliers were detected by using Adjusted Boxplot Method for skewed distributions (Brys et al., 2004, 2005; Hubert and Vandervieren, 2008). Outliers were handled as missing data. In all the three conditions (chronic pain, pain-free and experimental acute pain) only the first five “inlying” trials were included in the analyses (as for the pain conditions only trials during pain intensity of NRS ≥ 3). One participant from the healthy group did not answer the questionnaires hence; psychosocial data for that particular participant was handled as missing data.

The normality of the entire data was evaluated by using the Adjusted Boxplot Method and the Shapiro-Wilk test. The data showed a non-normal distribution with majority of the variables skewed to the right. Therefore, non-parametric tests were used to analyze the data. For each participant, data from all five included trials provided a participant median for each measurement and all data are further presented as group median (IQR, interquartile range). The data were analyzed with the SigmaStat software (version14.0; Systat Software Inc., San Jose, CA, United States) and for all tests, the level of significance was set at P < 0.05. Comparisons of psychosocial variables were done with Mann-Whitney Rank Sum test. For within condition’s comparisons, the non-parametric Friedman’s analysis of variance (ANOVA) for repeated measures and Tukey post hoc test for the associated multiple comparisons were used to test changes in all variables versus the baseline value of each variable (the first trial for each participant and condition was considered as baseline). For between conditions’ comparisons, Wilcoxon Signed Rank test and Mann–Whitney Rank Sum test were used. The variables included in the analyses were therefore conditions (chronic, pain-free, acute) and trials (Macfarlane et al., 2001, 2002; Breivik et al., 2006; Isong et al., 2008; Schiffman et al., 2014). For descriptive purposes, sex (men, women) was an additional variable included in the analyses. Spearman correlation test was applied to detect any correlation between force and pain variables.

Results

Psychosocial Characteristics

There were no significant differences between the chronic pain patients and the healthy participants regarding any of the psychosocial symptoms (Mann-Whitney Rank Sum test; P > 0.05) (Table 1).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Pain and pyschosocial data.

Pain Characteristics

Majority of the chronic pain patients (61%) were diagnosed with local myalgia in the masseter muscles (with/without temporal myalgia) and 39% were diagnosed with myofascial pain with referred pain in the masseter muscles (with/without temporal myalgia) according to DC/TMD. 61% of the chronic patients had low pain intensity and low grade of disability (GCPS-7), 33% had high pain intensity and low grade of disability, about 6% had moderately limiting high disability and none of them had severely limiting high disability. The pain intensity, pain area and pain duration in the chronic patients are presented in Table 1.

The induced experimental acute pain intensity, pain area, and pain duration in the healthy participants are presented in Table 1.

Force Characteristics

Healthy participants, both during pain-free and experimental acute pain condition, as well as chronic pain patients applied low forces during the hold phase followed by a rapid-ramp increase in force until the peanut split. There were no significant differences in the hold forces (pain-free: 0.99 N, acute pain: 1.46 N, chronic pain: 1.00 N) and the split forces (pain-free: 28.95 N, acute pain: 27.35 N, chronic pain: 30.79 N) within (Friedman/Tukey; P > 0.05) (Table 2) or between the conditions (Wilcoxon Signed Rank and Mann-Whitney Rank Sum; P > 0.05) (Table 3). Further, there were also no significant differences in the durations of split (pain-free: 0.26 s, acute pain: 0.30 s, chronic pain: 0.30 s) and the peak split force rates (pain-free: 307.38 N/s, acute pain: 285.28 N/s, chronic pain: 278.34 N/s) both within (Friedman/Tukey; P > 0.05) (Table 2) and between the conditions (Wilcoxon Signed Rank and Mann-Whitney Rank Sum; P > 0.05) (Table 3). The mean split force rate increased significantly compared to baseline values both in the chronic condition and the pain-free condition (Friedman/Tukey; P = 0.04 and 0.01, respectively) (Table 2). This increase in rate was not evident in the experimental acute pain condition (Friedman; P = 0.11) (Table 2). Observations of the mean split rates revealed no significant differences (pain-free: 99.76 N/s, acute pain: 82.07 N/s, chronic pain: 83.62 N/s) between the conditions (Wilcoxon Signed Rank and Mann-Whitney Rank Sum; P > 0.05) (Table 3).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Changes compared to baseline values in force, duration and rate variables in the three conditions.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Comparisons between the three conditions regarding force, duration, and rate variables.

Pain Intensity in Relation to Force Variables

The worst reported pain intensity during trial (NRS = 4.25) in the chronic condition correlated negatively with the hold force (1.00 N) (Spearman correlation; P = 0.04). The pain intensity 20s after infusion (NRS = 7.00), and the worst reported pain intensity during trial (NRS = 7.00) showed a negative correlation with the duration of the split phase in the experimental acute pain condition (0.30 s) (Spearman correlation; P < 0.05).

Discussion

The findings of the present study showed that neither experimental acute pain nor chronic muscular pain affects the human jaw motor control during a standardized precision biting task involving “holding” and “splitting” of peanuts with anterior teeth. The findings seem to indicate that jaw muscle pain does not alter the sensorimotor regulation and precision control of the jaws.

The PMR’s are innervated by fibers terminating in subnucleus interpolaris. The signaling in these fibers is faster and probably not affected by noxious stimuli from trigeminal facial areas that terminate more caudally in subnucleus caudalis. The rostral projections are larger in diameter and thereby faster than the more caudally located ones (Capra and Dessem, 1992). Further, the muscle spindles are innervated by efferent nerve fibers, motorneurons, which receive information from the central nervous system (CNS). The motorneurons convey nervous impulses to produce muscular effect, causing muscle fibers to shorten and contract. To enhance contraction either the firing frequency of each neuron increases or more motor units are activated/recruited (McComas, 1998; Loeb and Ghez, 2000; Miles, 2004). The fine-tuning of this activity is achieved by sensory information from orofacial receptors including the PMR’s and muscle spindles to the CNS. The results seem to indicate that the presence of healthy PMR’s and pulpal receptors provide the CNS with accurate sensory information despite the muscular pain. There is a possibility that a nociceptive modulation of spindle afferent activity might have occurred (Capra et al., 2007) but compensated for, hence resulting in undetectable effect on the force parameters in the present study. Activity along slower conducting nociceptive afferents could still modify the activity of faster afferents via intra-nuclear connections within the trigeminal brainstem sensory nuclear complex. Some masseter nociceptive afferents might provide axon collaterals to the rostral trigeminal subnuclei (Capra and Dessem, 1992). Nevertheless, this study did not assess nociceptive afferent activity. Furthermore, a potential effect of the experimentally induced pain (Nash et al., 2010; Minami et al., 2013) might have been compensated by other unaffected muscle parts/muscles. This assumption could be applicable on the chronic pain condition as well. The chronic pain patients may over time have developed compensating motor and behavioral strategies (Mohn et al., 2011; Fillingim et al., 2018).

Hold Phase

The fact that all the conditions showed no significant differences in the hold forces compared to the baseline trials indicated no trial order effect within the conditions. This is in line with results from previous studies where the hold and split task was used (Kumar et al., 2014; Zhang et al., 2016). The hold force in the pain-free condition was found to be 0.99 N in this study and in line with the previously mentioned study (Kumar et al., 2014) as well as earlier reports where the hold force for peanuts in healthy participants with natural dentition during normal conditions varied between 0.59 and 0.79 N (Trulsson and Johansson, 1996b; Trulsson and Gunne, 1998; Johnsen et al., 2007; Svensson and Trulsson, 2009, 2011).

The chronic pain patients applied similar hold force as the pain-free participants. During the holding phase in pain-free healthy individuals, it has been hypothesized that the PMR’s besides the afferent nerve fibers in muscle spindles play a role in signaling the early contact-state information about the peanut in a predictive feed-forward manner in order to activate the motor commands that are needed for initiating the split phase (Svensson and Trulsson, 2009, 2011). The jaw muscle activity increases in response to an increased food hardness. It was shown that the destruction of either periodontal or muscle afferents in animals, reduced such increase in jaw muscle activity (Lavigne et al., 1987; Morimoto et al., 1989; Hidaka et al., 1997). However, chronic muscle pain did not seem to have any effect on the forces during the holding phase in this study. One probable explanation could be that the presence of healthy PMRs are of greater importance in oral fine motor control than the muscle spindles (Kumar et al., 2019).

Split Phase

The split phase was characterized by a sudden rapid ramp increase in the bite force. When the peanut was split, the force dropped down and an unloading of the teeth occurred (Svensson and Trulsson, 2009, 2011). The unloading contributes to a reflex response in the masseter muscle activity that results in stopping the jaw closing movement (Turker and Jenkins, 2000). All conditions showed no differences in the split force and duration of split phase compared to baseline values. This finding is in line with previous studies where experimental acute pain did not show any robust effects on the split forces (Kumar et al., 2014, 2015a). It was previously suggested that split forces and duration of split are mainly dependent on the mechanical properties of the food and the bevel of the incisal edges during biting with anterior teeth (Johnsen et al., 2007; Svensson and Trulsson, 2009, 2011; Kumar et al., 2014, 2015c, 2017). In healthy individuals in previous studies, the split forces varied between 17.6 and 35.9 N and the duration of the split phase varied between 0.22 and 0.34 s (Trulsson and Johansson, 1996b; Svensson and Trulsson, 2009, 2011). In our study the split force was 28.95 N and the duration of the split phase was 0.26 s in healthy pain-free participants. Our findings were within the range of the previous results.

The significant increase in the mean split force rate in the pain-free individuals and chronic pain patients indicates that reaching to the split of the peanut from the initiation of the split phase was faster compared to baseline. On the other hand, no statistically significant difference in the mean split force rate in the acute condition compared to baseline may indicate that the high intensity of the induced pain and the protective effect of the acute pain affected the rate. However, the differences were only found within the conditions (not between them) and there were no differences in the duration of the split (one of the two variables that the mean split force rate depends on) in all the three conditions. Further statistical analyses showed that there was a significant difference in the split force increase (the other variable that the mean split force rate depends on) between the acute and the pain-free conditions where the acute condition showed a smaller increase (Table 3). This significant difference could not be found between the chronic and the pain-free conditions or between the chronic and acute conditions. Moreover, no significant differences could be found within the three groups in the split force increase (Table 2). Therefore, this finding should be considered with caution and further investigations would be needed in order to make a more conclusive interpretation. It was suggested that the individual’s reaction to pain might depend on the specifics of the performed task (Sae-Lee et al., 2008a, b). An individual’s confidence that they can manage pain (self-efficacy beliefs) predicts avoidance behavior (Nicholas, 2007) and the ability to persist with a task (Turner et al., 2005). The smaller split force increase in the acute condition could be an indication that the participants could predict the split force needed easier than the two other condition groups. The acute condition group did not need to increase the mean split force rate as much as the other two condition groups since the split force onset was already high and more approximate to the split force needed. That is in line with the significant increase in the mean split force rate in the pain-free and chronic conditions. A possible explanation could be a training effect since the participants in the acute condition were the same persons as in the pain-free condition. However, the previously cited studies (Kumar et al., 2014, 2015a; Zhang et al., 2016) did not report any findings about rate variables. There were no significant differences in the mean and peak split force rates between the three conditions indicating that the pain did not impair the neural control needed for achieving the necessary force magnitude needed for splitting the peanut.

For the acute jaw muscle pain, the results confirms our hypothesis and are in line with previous studies using other experimental jaw muscle pain models and in other populations which had shown that experimental acute pain had no detectable or robust effect on the hold and split forces and on the variability of the forces, duration of the split phase, electromyographic muscle activity (EMG) or jaw movement amplitude in comparison with healthy controls during biting and mastication (Svensson et al., 1997; Kumar et al., 2015a, b) indicating the absence of motor impairment even in subjects who reported moderate to intense levels of pain. Interestingly, the results showed that this was the case even for the chronic jaw muscle pain which was contradictory to our hypothesis. On the other hand, this is in line with results from a previous study (Goiato et al., 2017) showing that maximal bite forces in the incisor region was similar prior to and after jaw muscle pain relief, suggesting that the presence of pain did not affect the maximal forces in this region.

Study Limitations and Strengths

The baseline pain intensity in chronic pain patients was rather low, none of them had a severely limiting high disability according to GCPS-7 and pain catastrophizing according to pain catastrophizing scale-13 (PCS-13) (Boonstra et al., 2016) was not assessed in this study which could be considered as a limitation. The fact that there were no significant differences between chronic patients and healthy participants regarding the psychosocial characteristics minimized the possible confounding effect, and therefore could be considered a strength of the current study.

Incisors were used in this study in order to minimize confounding factors as it had earlier been reported that participants felt it was easier to master the task when anterior teeth were used compared to posterior teeth (Johnsen et al., 2007). It had been also shown that the masseter muscle was significantly more active (higher EMG) than the temporal muscle during tasks involving incisal biting and jaw protrusion (Farella et al., 2008) and that biting in a protrusive position was accompanied by the highest activation of the masseter muscle (Lu et al., 2013). One can assume that there would be a practicing bias in the experimental acute pain condition since the same individuals performed the task twice, pain-free and while in experimental acute pain. However, the circumstances differed and a previous study showed that there was no apparent or optimization of jaw motor control when this specific task was repeated up to sixty times, in participants with healthy periodontium (Kumar et al., 2014).

Conclusion

The current study shows that jaw muscle pain does not seem to alter precision biting in humans. Specifically, chronic myalgia in the jaw muscles as well as experimentally induced acute pain did not show any effects on the hold or split forces when compared to healthy pain-free individuals. However, a possibility that a nociceptive modulation of spindle afferent activity might have occurred but compensated for cannot completely be ruled out.

Data Availability Statement

The datasets for this study will be made available by the authors, without undue reservations, to any qualified researcher.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Regional Ethical Review Board in Stockholm (DNR: 2014/1394-3). The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

SA wrote the manuscript, performed the research, analyzed the data, and made the tables and figures. AB performed the research and participated in manuscript editing. KS participated in the design of the project, performed the research, and participated in manuscript editing. AK participated in data analyses and manuscript editing. AG designed the project and participated in manuscript editing. NC designed the project, and participated in data analyses, making the figures, and manuscript editing.

Funding

The study was funded from the Stockholm County Council and the Karolinska Institutet (SOF; Styrgruppen för Odontologisk Forskning).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

We are grateful to Reika Makita for the contribution as a model in Figure 1.

Abbreviations

ANOVA, analysis of variance; au, arbitrary units; cm, centimeter; CNS, central nervous system; CPG, central pattern generator; DC-TMD, diagnostic criteria for temporomandibular disorders; EMG, electromyographic muscle activity; G, gram; GAD-7, generalized anxiety disorder scale-7; GCP-7, graded chronic pain scale-7; Hz, hertz; IQR, interquartile range; mg, milligram; min, minute; ml, milliliter; mm, millimeter; N, Newton; NRS, numeric rating scale; PHQ-15, the patient health questionnaire for physical symptoms-15; PHQ-9, the patient health questionnaire for depression-9; PMR, periodontal mechanoreceptor; PSS-10, perceived stress scale-10; s, second; SCON, Scandinavian Center for Orofacial Neurosciences; SD, standard deviation; TMD, temporomandibular disorders; TMJ, temporomandibular joint; US, United States; USD, United States dollar; μ l, microliter.

References

Bair, E., Ohrbach, R., Fillingim, R. B., Greenspan, J. D., Dubner, R., Diatchenko, L., et al. (2013). Multivariable modeling of phenotypic risk factors for first-onset TMD: the OPPERA prospective cohort study. J. Pain 14(12 Suppl.), T102–T115. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boonstra, A. M., Stewart, R. E., Köke, A. J., Oosterwijk, R. F., Swaan, J. L., Schreurs, K. M., et al. (2016). Cut-Off points for mild, moderate, and severe pain on the numeric rating scale for pain in patients with chronic musculoskeletal pain: variability and influence of sex and catastrophizing. Front. Psychol. 7:1466. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01466

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Breivik, H., Collett, B., Ventafridda, V., Cohen, R., and Gallacher, D. (2006). Survey of chronic pain in Europe: prevalence, impact on daily life, and treatment. Eur. J. Pain 10, 287–333.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Brys, G., Hubert, M., and Rousseeuw, P. J. (2005). A robustification of independent component analysis. J. Chemom. 19, 364–375. doi: 10.1002/cem.940

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brys, G., Hubert, M., and Struyf, A. (2004). A robust measure of skewness. J. Comput. Graph. Stat. 13, 996–1017. doi: 10.1198/106186004x12632

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capra, N. F., and Dessem, D. (1992). Central connections of trigeminal primary afferent neurons: topographical and functional considerations. Crit. Rev. Oral Biol. Med. 4, 1–52. doi: 10.1177/10454411920040010101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Capra, N. F., Hisley, C. K., and Masri, R. M. (2007). The influence of pain on masseter spindle afferent discharge. Arch. Oral Biol. 52, 387–390. doi: 10.1016/j.archoralbio.2006.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Christidis, N., Ioannidou, K., Milosevic, M., Segerdahl, M., and Ernberg, M. (2008). Changes of hypertonic saline-induced masseter muscle pain characteristics, by an infusion of the serotonin receptor type 3 antagonist granisetron. J. Pain 9, 892–901. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2008.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clark, G. T., Jacobson, R., and Beemsterboer, P. L. (1984). Interdental thickness discrimination in myofascial pain dysfunction subjects. J. Oral Rehabil. 11, 381–386. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2842.1984.tb00589.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dellow, P. G., and Lund, J. P. (1971). Evidence for central timing of rhythmical mastication. J. Physiol. 215, 1–13. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1971.sp009454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Farella, M., Palla, S., Erni, S., Michelotti, A., and Gallo, L. M. (2008). Masticatory muscle activity during deliberately performed oral tasks. Physiol. Meas. 29, 1397–1410. doi: 10.1088/0967-3334/29/12/004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Felício, C. M., Mazzetto, M. O., and Perri Angote Dos Santos, C. (2002). Masticatory behavior in individuals with temporomandibular disorders. Minerva Stomatol. 51, 111–120.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Fillingim, R. B., Slade, G. D., Greenspan, J. D., Dubner, R., Maixner, W., Bair, E., et al. (2018). Long-term changes in biopsychosocial characteristics related to temporomandibular disorder: findings from the OPPERA study. Pain 159, 2403–2413. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001348

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goiato, M. C., Zuim, P. R. J., Moreno, A., Dos Santos, D. M., da Silva, E. V. F., de Caxias, F. P., et al. (2017). Does pain in the masseter and anterior temporal muscles influence maximal bite force? Arch. Oral Biol. 83, 1–6. doi: 10.1016/j.archoralbio.2017.06.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graven-Nielsen, T. (2006). Fundamentals of muscle pain, referred pain, and deep tissue hyperalgesia. Scand. J. Rheumatol. Suppl. 122, 1–43. doi: 10.1080/03009740600865980

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grigoriadis, A., Johansson, R. S., and Trulsson, M. (2014). Temporal profile and amplitude of human masseter muscle activity is adapted to food properties during individual chewing cycles. J. Oral Rehabil. 41, 367–373. doi: 10.1111/joor.12155

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hidaka, O., Morimoto, T., Masuda, Y., Kato, T., Matsuo, R., Inoue, T., et al. (1997). Regulation of masticatory force during cortically induced rhythmic jaw movements in the anesthetized rabbit. J. Neurophysiol. 77, 3168–3179. doi: 10.1152/jn.1997.77.6.3168

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubert, M., and Vandervieren, E. (2008). An adjusted boxplot for skewed distributions. Comput. Stat. Data Anal. 52, 5186–5201. doi: 10.1016/j.csda.2007.11.008

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Isong, U., Gansky, S. A., and Plesh, O. (2008). Temporomandibular joint and muscle disorder-type pain in U.S. adults: the national health interview survey. J. Orofac. Pain 22, 317–322.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Johnsen, S. E., Svensson, K. G., and Trulsson, M. (2007). Forces applied by anterior and posterior teeth and roles of periodontal afferents during hold-and-split tasks in human subjects. Exp. Brain Res. 178, 126–134. doi: 10.1007/s00221-006-0719-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., and Williams, J. B. (2001). The PHQ-9: validity of a brief depression severity measure. J. Gen. Intern. Med. 16, 606–613. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kroenke, K., Spitzer, R. L., and Williams, J. B. (2002). The PHQ-15: validity of a new measure for evaluating the severity of somatic symptoms. Psychosom. Med. 64, 258–266. doi: 10.1097/00006842-200203000-00008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, A., Castrillon, E., Svensson, K. G., Baad-Hansen, L., Trulsson, M., and Svensson, P. (2015a). Effects of experimental craniofacial pain on fine jaw motor control: a placebo-controlled double-blinded study. Exp. Brain Res. 233, 1745–1759. doi: 10.1007/s00221-015-4245-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, A., Castrillon, E., and Svensson, P. (2015b). Can experimentally evoked pain in the jaw muscles or temporomandibular joint affect anterior bite force in humans? J. Oral Facial Pain Headache 29, 31–40. doi: 10.11607/ofph.1268

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, A., Grigoriadis, J., Trulsson, M., Svensson, P., and Svensson, K. G. (2015c). Effects of short-term training on behavioral learning and skill acquisition during intraoral fine motor task. Neuroscience 306, 10–17. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.06.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, A., Castrillon, E., Trulsson, M., Svensson, K. G., and Svensson, P. (2017). Fine motor control of the jaw following alteration of orofacial afferent inputs. Clin. Oral Investig. 21, 613–626. doi: 10.1007/s00784-016-1939-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, A., Svensson, K. G., Baad-Hansen, L., Trulsson, M., Isidor, F., and Svensson, P. (2014). Optimization of jaw muscle activity and fine motor control during repeated biting tasks. Arch. Oral Biol. 59, 1342–1351. doi: 10.1016/j.archoralbio.2014.08.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kumar, A., Tanaka, Y., Takahashi, K., Grigoriadis, A., Wiesinger, B., Svensson, P., et al. (2019). Vibratory stimulus to the masseter muscle impairs the oral fine motor control during biting tasks. J. Prosthodont. Res. 63, 354–360. doi: 10.1016/j.jpor.2018.10.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Latremoliere, A., and Woolf, C. J. (2009). Central sensitization: a generator of pain hypersensitivity by central neural plasticity. J. Pain 10, 895–926. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2009.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lavigne, G., Kim, J. S., Valiquette, C., and Lund, J. P. (1987). Evidence that periodontal pressoreceptors provide positive feedback to jaw closing muscles during mastication. J. Neurophysiol. 58, 342–358. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.2.342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

LeResche, L. (1997). Epidemiology of temporomandibular disorders: implications for the investigation of etiologic factors. Crit. Rev. Oral Biol. Med. 8, 291–305. doi: 10.1177/10454411970080030401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lobbezoo, F., van Selms, M. K., and Naeije, M. (2006). Masticatory muscle pain and disordered jaw motor behaviour: literature review over the past decade. Arch. Oral Biol. 51, 713–720. doi: 10.1016/j.archoralbio.2006.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Loeb, G. E., and Ghez, C. (2000). “The motor unit and Muscle action,” in Principles of Neural Science, eds E. R. Kandel, J. Schwartz, and T. M. Jesell, (New York, NY: McGraw-Hill Companies Inc.).

Google Scholar

López-Frías, F. J., Castellanos-Cosano, L., Martín-González, J., Llamas-Carreras, J. M., and Segura-Egea, J. J. (2012). Clinical measurement of tooth wear: tooth wear indices. J. Clin. Exp. Dent. 4, e48–e53. doi: 10.4317/jced.50592

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Löwe, B., Decker, O., Müller, S., Brähler, E., Schellberg, D., Herzog, W., et al. (2008). Validation and standardization of the generalized anxiety disorder screener (GAD-7) in the general population. Med. Care 46, 266–274. doi: 10.1097/MLR.0b013e318160d093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lu, S., Baad-Hansen, L., Zhang, Z., and Svensson, P. (2013). One hour jaw muscle training does not evoke plasticity in the corticomotor control of the masseter muscle. Arch. Oral Biol. 58, 1483–1490. doi: 10.1016/j.archoralbio.2013.06.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lund, J. P. (1991). Mastication and its control by the brain stem. Crit. Rev. Oral Biol. Med. 2, 33–64. doi: 10.1177/10454411910020010401

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lund, J. P., Donga, R., Widmer, C. G., and Stohler, C. S. (1991). The pain-adaptation model: a discussion of the relationship between chronic musculoskeletal pain and motor activity. Can. J. Physiol. Pharmacol. 69, 683–694. doi: 10.1139/y91-102

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lund, J. P., and Kolta, A. (2006). Generation of the central masticatory pattern and its modification by sensory feedback. Dysphagia 21, 167–174. doi: 10.1007/s00455-006-9027-6

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macfarlane, T. V., Blinkhorn, A. S., Davies, R. M., Kincey, J., and Worthington, H. V. (2004). Predictors of outcome for orofacial pain in the general population: a four-year follow-up study. J. Dent. Res. 83, 712–717. doi: 10.1177/154405910408300911

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macfarlane, T. V., Blinkhorn, A. S., Davies, R. M., Ryan, P., Worthington, H. V., and Macfarlane, G. J. (2002). Orofacial pain: just another chronic pain? Results from a population-based survey. Pain 99, 453–458. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(02)00181-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macfarlane, T. V., Glenny, A. M., and Worthington, H. V. (2001). Systematic review of population-based epidemiological studies of oro-facial pain. J. Dent. 29, 451–467. doi: 10.1016/s0300-5712(01)00041-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McComas, A. J. (1998). Oro-facial muscles: internal structure, function and ageing. Gerodontology 15, 3–14. doi: 10.1111/j.1741-2358.1998.00003.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miles, T. S. (2004). “Mastication,” in Clinical Oral Physiology, eds T. S. Miles, N. Birgitte, and P. Svensson, (Copenhagen: Quintessence Publishing Company Ltd.).

Google Scholar

Minami, I., Akhter, R., Albersen, I., Burger, C., Whittle, T., Lobbezoo, F., et al. (2013). Masseter motor unit recruitment is altered in experimental jaw muscle pain. J. Dent. Res. 92, 143–148. doi: 10.1177/0022034512470832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mohn, C., Vassend, O., and Knardahl, S. (2011). Psychophysiological responses to pain stimulation and cognitive tasks in female temporomandibular disorder patients. Scand. J. Pain 2, 72–82. doi: 10.1016/j.sjpain.2010.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morimoto, T., Inoue, T., Masuda, Y., and Nagashima, T. (1989). Sensory components facilitating jaw-closing muscle activities in the rabbit. Exp. Brain Res. 76, 424–440.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Murray, G. M., and Peck, C. C. (2007). Orofacial pain and jaw muscle activity: a new model. J. Orofac. Pain 21, 263–278.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nash, P. G., Macefield, V. G., Klineberg, I. J., Gustin, S. M., Murray, G. M., and Henderson, L. A. (2010). Changes in human primary motor cortex activity during acute cutaneous and muscle orofacial pain. J. Orofac. Pain 24, 379–390.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Nicholas, M. K. (2007). The pain self-efficacy questionnaire: taking pain into account. Eur. J. Pain 11, 153–163. doi: 10.1016/j.ejpain.2005.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nordin, M., and Nordin, S. (2013). Psychometric evaluation and normative data of the Swedish version of the 10-item perceived stress scale. Scand. J. Psychol. 54, 502–507. doi: 10.1111/sjop.12071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nozaki, S., Iriki, A., and Nakamura, Y. (1986). Localization of central rhythm generator involved in cortically induced rhythmical masticatory jaw-opening movement in the guinea pig. J. Neurophysiol. 55, 806–825. doi: 10.1152/jn.1986.55.4.806

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohrbach, R., Bair, E., Fillingim, R. B., Gonzalez, Y., Gordon, S. M., Lim, P. F., et al. (2013). Clinical orofacial characteristics associated with risk of first-onset TMD: the OPPERA prospective cohort study. J. Pain 14(12 Suppl.), T33–T50. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2013.07.018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohrbach, R., Fillingim, R. B., Mulkey, F., Gonzalez, Y., Gordon, S., Gremillion, H., et al. (2011). Clinical findings and pain symptoms as potential risk factors for chronic TMD: descriptive data and empirically identified domains from the OPPERA case-control study. J. Pain 12(11 Suppl.), T27–T45. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2011.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parker, M. W. (1990). A dynamic model of etiology in temporomandibular disorders. J. Am. Dent. Assoc. 120, 283–290. doi: 10.14219/jada.archive.1990.0045

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sae-Lee, D., Whittle, T., Forte, A. R., Peck, C. C., Byth, K., Sessle, B. J., et al. (2008a). Effects of experimental pain on jaw muscle activity during goal-directed jaw movements in humans. Exp. Brain Res. 189, 451–462. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1439-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sae-Lee, D., Whittle, T., Peck, C. C., Forte, A. R., Klineberg, I. J., and Murray, G. M. (2008b). Experimental jaw-muscle pain has a differential effect on different jaw movement tasks. J. Orofac. Pain 22, 15–29.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Schiffman, E., Ohrbach, R., Truelove, E., Look, J., Anderson, G., Goulet, J. P., et al. (2014). Diagnostic criteria for temporomandibular disorders (DC/TMD) for clinical and research applications: recommendations of the international RDC/TMD consortium network and orofacial pain special interest group†. J. Oral Facial Pain Headache 28, 6–27. doi: 10.11607/jop.1151

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stohler, C. S. (1999). Craniofacial pain and motor function: pathogenesis, clinical correlates, and implications. Crit. Rev. Oral Biol. Med. 10, 504–518. doi: 10.1177/10454411990100040601

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stohler, C. S., Ashton-Miller, J. A., and Carlson, D. S. (1988). The effects of pain from the mandibular joint and muscles on masticatory motor behaviour in man. Arch. Oral Biol. 33, 175–182. doi: 10.1016/0003-9969(88)90042-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, K. G., and Trulsson, M. (2009). Regulation of bite force increase during splitting of food. Eur. J. Oral Sci. 117, 704–710. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0722.2009.00691.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, K. G., and Trulsson, M. (2011). Impaired force control during food holding and biting in subjects with tooth- or implant-supported fixed prostheses. J. Clin. Periodontol. 38, 1137–1146. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-051X.2011.01781.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, P., and Graven-Nielsen, T. (2001). Craniofacial muscle pain: review of mechanisms and clinical manifestations. J. Orofac. Pain 15, 117–145.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Svensson, P., Graven-Nielsen, T., Matre, D., and Arendt-Nielsen, L. (1998). Experimental muscle pain does not cause long-lasting increases in resting electromyographic activity. Muscle Nerve 21, 1382–1389. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1097-4598(199811)21:11<1382::aid-mus4>3.0.co;2-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, P., Houe, L., and Arendt-Nielsen, L. (1997). Bilateral experimental muscle pain changes electromyographic activity of human jaw-closing muscles during mastication. Exp. Brain Res. 116, 182–185. doi: 10.1007/pl00005738

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Svensson, P., and Kumar, A. (2016). Assessment of risk factors for oro-facial pain and recent developments in classification: implications for management. J. Oral Rehabil. 43, 977–989. doi: 10.1111/joor.12447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trulsson, M. (2006). Sensory-motor function of human periodontal mechanoreceptors. J. Oral Rehabil. 33, 262–273. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2842.2006.01629.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trulsson, M., and Gunne, H. S. (1998). Food-holding and -biting behavior in human subjects lacking periodontal receptors. J. Dent. Res. 77, 574–582. doi: 10.1177/00220345980770041001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trulsson, M., and Johansson, R. S. (1996a). Encoding of tooth loads by human periodontal afferents and their role in jaw motor control. Prog. Neurobiol. 49, 267–284. doi: 10.1016/s0301-0082(96)00016-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trulsson, M., and Johansson, R. S. (1996b). Forces applied by the incisors and roles of periodontal afferents during food-holding and -biting tasks. Exp. Brain Res. 107, 486–496.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Trulsson, M., and Johansson, R. S. (2002). Orofacial mechanoreceptors in humans: encoding characteristics and responses during natural orofacial behaviors. Behav. Brain Res. 135, 27–33. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(02)00151-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Türker, K. S. (2002). Reflex control of human jaw muscles. Crit. Rev. Oral Biol. Med. 13, 85–104. doi: 10.1177/154411130201300109

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turker, K. S., and Jenkins, M. (2000). Reflex responses induced by tooth unloading. J. Neurophysiol. 84, 1088–1092. doi: 10.1152/jn.2000.84.2.1088

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Turner, J. A., Ersek, M., and Kemp, C. (2005). Self-efficacy for managing pain is associated with disability, depression, and pain coping among retirement community residents with chronic pain. J. Pain 6, 471–479. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2005.02.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van der Bilt, A., Engelen, L., Pereira, L. J., van der Glas, H. W., and Abbink, J. H. (2006). Oral physiology and mastication. Physiol. Behav. 89, 22–27.

Google Scholar

Von Korff, M., Ormel, J., Keefe, F. J., and Dworkin, S. F. (1992). Grading the severity of chronic pain. Pain 50, 133–149. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(92)90154-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Woolf, C. J., and Salter, M. W. (2000). Neuronal plasticity: increasing the gain in pain. Science 288, 1765–1769.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Woolf, C. J., and Walters, E. T. (1991). Common patterns of plasticity contributing to nociceptive sensitization in mammals and Aplysia. Trends Neurosci. 14, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/0166-2236(91)90024-o

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wright, E. F. (2000). Referred craniofacial pain patterns in patients with temporomandibular disorder. J. Am. Dent. Assoc. 131, 1307–1315. doi: 10.14219/jada.archive.2000.0384

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Xu, L., Fan, S., Cai, B., Fang, Z., and Jiang, X. (2017). Influence of sustained submaximal clenching fatigue test on electromyographic activity and maximum voluntary bite forces in healthy subjects and patients with temporomandibular disorders. J. Oral Rehabil. 44, 340–346. doi: 10.1111/joor.12497

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, H., Kumar, A., Kothari, M., Luo, X., Trulsson, M., Svensson, K. G., et al. (2016). Can short-term oral fine motor training affect precision of task performance and induce cortical plasticity of the jaw muscles? Exp. Brain Res. 234, 1935–1943. doi: 10.1007/s00221-016-4598-4

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: jaw motor control, chronic pain, myalgia, experimental pain, hypertonic saline

Citation: Al Sayegh S, Borgwardt A, Svensson KG, Kumar A, Grigoriadis A and Christidis N (2019) Effects of Chronic and Experimental Acute Masseter Pain on Precision Biting Behavior in Humans. Front. Physiol. 10:1369. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2019.01369

Received: 09 July 2019; Accepted: 15 October 2019;
Published: 29 October 2019.

Edited by:

Limor Avivi-Arber, University of Toronto, Canada

Reviewed by:

Greg Murray, The University of Sydney, Australia
Diana K. Sarko, Southern Illinois University Carbondale, United States

Copyright © 2019 Al Sayegh, Borgwardt, Svensson, Kumar, Grigoriadis and Christidis. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Samaa Al Sayegh, samaa.al.sayegh@ki.se

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.