ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Pediatr., 28 March 2025

Sec. Neonatology

Volume 13 - 2025 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fped.2025.1523952

This article is part of the Research TopicMaternal and Infant Nutrition: Impact on Breast Milk, Infant Gut Microbiota and Health DevelopmentView all 8 articles

Sex-differences in Mothers' own milk and neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm infants

  • 1School of Nursing, Yale University, Orange, CT, United States
  • 2School of Nursing, Columbia University, New York, NY, United States
  • 3Department of Statistics, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, United States
  • 4School of Nursing, University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT, United States
  • 5College of Nursing, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL, United States
  • 6Division of Neonatology, Connecticut Children’s Medical Center, Hartford, CT, United States
  • 7Department of Pediatrics, University of Connecticut School of Medicine, Farmington, CT, United States
  • 8Department of Pediatrics, Yale University, New Haven, CT, United States

Objective: To identify sex-specific feeding patterns and associations with growth and neurodevelopment in preterm infants during NICU through 2 years of corrected age (CA).

Methods: A cohort study was conducted with 216 preterm infants (gestational age 28 0/7 to 32 0/7 weeks). Daily feeding regimens, including mother's own milk (MOM), human donor milk, and formula; daily growth; acute and chronic pain/stress were documented during NICU. NICU Network Neurobehavioral Scale (NNNS) (36 to 38 postmenstrual age), and Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley) Edition III (1 and 2 years of CA) were measured.

Results: Between week 9 to 16 after birth, only females showed a positive association between growth z-score and proportion of MOM intake before week 8 (p < 0.05). Sex-differentiated associations between MOM and stress were observed (p < 0.05). MOM proportion was positively correlated with language or cognitive scores at 2 years of CA in females (p = 0.01), this correlation not evident in males.

Conclusions: We discovered a sex-specific “window of opportunity” for feeding, growth and risk predictors for neurodevelopment up to 2 years of CA. These insights may inform development of tailored feeding regimens, potentially mitigating growth and development differences observed between males and females.

Introduction

Even with advances in neonatal intensive care, preterm infants [<37 weeks of gestational age (GA)] still face tremendous challenges to surviving and thriving. Notably, males born preterm are consistently observed to have a higher risk of perinatal complications, with worse mortality and morbidity (14), and neurodevelopmental impairments (57) than females. The etiology of the “sex bias” in preterm growth and health remains unclear and likely multifactorial (4). Sex variation in nutritional need is increasingly recognized as playing a significant role in the growth and health outcomes of preterm infants (8). Emerging studies show sex-specific responses to nutritional intake (813), suggesting potential sex-based nutrition requirements in early life for optimal development.

Mother's own milk (MOM) is ideal for preterm infants' growth, immunity, and neurodevelopment (14, 15). The amount and composition of MOM has been found dynamically adjusted to support the infants' nutritional requirements along with their developmental phase (16). Emerging research shows the interplay between the MOM characteristics and infant sex for tailoring sex-specific needs of the offspring (16). While mothers of term male infants are more likely to produce milk with higher levels of energy, free amino acids, total lipids, phospholipids, and gangliosides, which are significantly associated with infants' neurobehavioral development (1620). There is limited evidence of sex-specific response to feeding in preterm infants. Moreover, it is challenging to conclude the impact of sex-specific MOM on the growth and development of preterm infants. This challenge arises from the intricate difficulties faced by mothers in terms of breastfeeding and providing MOM for their infants (15, 21).

Our study aims to examine the associations between sex-specific responses to feeding, especially MOM intake, and the growth and neurodevelopment of preterm infants from NICU up to 2 years of corrected age (CA). We hypothesize that male and female preterm infants have distinct feeding needs at the early stage of their lives, which contribute to their growth and neurodevelopmental outcomes. Therefore, we used linear mixed models to evaluate the significance of factors including feeding and clinical data, in predicting the growth and neurodevelopmental outcomes in both male and female infants. Further, we applied SHapley Additive exPlanations (SHAP) to determine the importance of risk factors on predicting neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Methods

Study design and participants

We conducted a cohort study to investigate the associations of feeding regimens with growth and neurodevelopment outcomes in male and female infants from NICU up to 2 years of CA. The study was approved by the IRBs at the study institutes and the medical center in Northeastern United States. A total of 216 preterm infants were recruited between 2017 and 2022 at two affiliated NICU sites. Inclusion criteria were preterm infants: (1) born at 28 0/7 to 32 0/7 weeks of GA, and (2) received consent from parents who were ≥18 years old. Exclusion criteria were infants who have: (1) known congenital or chromosomal abnormalities; (2) severe periventricular/intraventricular hemorrhage (≥ grade III); (3) undergone surgery; and/or (4) illicit substances exposure history during pregnancy.

Demographic and health characteristics

Infants' demographic and health information were collected. The Score for Neonatal Acute Physiology with Perinatal Extension-II (SNAPEII) (22) and treatment with antibiotics before or after the first 3 days after birth were also measured.

Feeding regimens

Infants' daily feeding information was collected by research nurses, including the frequency of infant fed by MOM, fortified human donor milk (HDM), and/or fortified formula during their NICU study. Based on the NICU feeding protocol, all infants with a birth weight <1,800 grams and/or birth GA < 32 weeks were eligible to receive HDM when MOM was unavailable. The transition from HDM to formula occurred at 34 weeks of GA. The total number of frequency of feedings (up to 8 times per day) and quantity of MOM, HDM, and formula given to infants during each shift and daily were collected. The daily proportion of MOM was then calculated given its frequency over the total number of daily feedings. Given that a significant number of preterm infants received a combination of feeds, we classified feeding types using a 70% threshold for both frequency and amount for a specific period (23, 24). For example, if an infant was predominantly fed with MOM for over 70% of the total feeding during week 1, the infant was categorized as MOM for that week. Similarly, if an infant received a mixture of formula and MOM, with the combination accounting for more than 70% of the feeds during week 2, the coding would be MOM + formula for week 2.

Early life painful/stressful experiences during the NICU stay

Infants' acute and chronic painful/stressful events experienced during NICU based on the revised NICU Infant Stressor Scale (NISS) were documented in the REDCap by research nurses (25, 26). Acute and chronic painful/stressful procedures were categorized into five domains, ranging from 1 (not painful/stressful) to 5 (signifying extremely painful/stressful). The average daily pain/stress for the first 28 days after birth was computed by adding the daily frequencies (for acute) and hours (for chronic) of pain/stress events for all four weeks, then dividing by 28 days.

Growth measurements

From birth to 16 weeks of postnatal age, infants' daily or weekly growth parameters including weight, length, and head circumferences were documented by research nurses. Infants' weight was assessed using an electronic scale with a capacity of 20 kilograms and an accuracy within 10 grams. The scale was calibrated every 2–3 months. Body length was measured using the Shorr Infant/Child Height Measuring Board, subject to monthly inspections for accuracy, with measurements recorded to the nearest 0.1 cm. Head circumference was gauged with a disposable tape accurate to the nearest 0.1 cm. Measures were taken 3 times, assuring agreement within 30 grams and 1 cm. If the established limit was surpassed, a fourth measurement was obtained, and the three closest measurements were utilized.

Neurobehavior responses and neurodevelopment measurements

NICU Neonatal Neurobehavioral Scale (NNNS) was assessed when infants reached their 36–38 weeks post-menstrual age (PMA) (27). NNNS includes 115 items which are categorized into 13 summary scores for evaluating habituation, attention, arousal, self-regulation, handling, quality of movement, excitability, lethargy, non-optimal reflexes, asymmetric reflexes, hypertonicity, hypotonicity, and stress/abstinence. Our study focused on stress/abstinence (NSTRESS), quality of movement (NQMOVE), and arousal (NAROUSAL). Lower NSTRESS and NAROUSAL subscale scores, and higher NQMOVE subscale scores indicate better neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development (Bayley) Edition III (28) was used to assess children's cognitive, language, and motor skills at 1 and 2 years of CA. A higher score within each domain or a total score across all domains indicates better neurodevelopmental outcomes for infants. This is the most widely used, valid, and reliable assessment tool for developmental assessment of preterm infants (29, 30). Certified research nurses administered the NNNS assessment and formally accredited neonatologists conducted the Bayley assessment. Evaluators were blinded to infant feeding exposure.

Statistical analysis

The analysis focused on assessing the effects of various exposure variables including MOM proportion, total feeding volume, sex, and the interaction of MOM proportion and sex, on infant growth and development. We evaluated infant growth outcomes (weight z-score), NNNS, and Bayley scores at 1 and 2 years of CA, as the primary outcomes in our study. To examine the association between exposure variables and weight z-score, we employed linear mixed models (LMM) (31). To analyze NNNS and Bayley scores, we employed linear models to examine their direct association with exposure, as well as adjusted for confounders including race, ethnicity, birth GA, weight and length, acute and chronic pain experience, SNAPPEII, NISS, and delivery mode. This adjustment is critical for isolating the effects of key exposure variables on outcomes. Furthermore, to identify the most influential factors affecting NNNS and Bayley scores and understand their degree of influence, we employed XGBoost (32) for feature importance analysis. This was complemented by the SHapley Additive exPlanations (SHAP) (33) to determine the importance of the risk factors in predicting neurodevelopmental outcomes. Additionally, we utilized growth curve modeling (34) to construct infant growth trajectory scores, labeled as 's' representing the growth slope. All analyzes were performed using R version 4.2.0 and p < 0.05 was considered statistically significant.

Results

Preterm infant and maternal demographic and clinical characteristics

Our study recruited a total of 216 infants with a majority of males (57.9%), White (67.6%), and non-Hispanics (70.5%). The infants were born at a median GA of 28.6 weeks (IQR, 26.6–30.1), with a median birth weight of 1052.5 gram (IQR, 815.0–1322.5), a median SNAPPEII score of 21.0 (IQR, 10.0–34.0), and treated with antibiotics during first 3 days after birth (94%). In addition, male infants exhibited higher birth weight, body length, and head circumference (p < 0.05), compared to female infants (see Table 1), while there was no significant difference in birth GA between two groups. The median age of mothers of preterm infants was 31.0 years (IQR, 27.0–35.0), and 54.2% of them were married (see Supplementary Table S1).

Table 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Infant demographic characteristics (N = 216).

Feeding intakes during NICU hospitalization

We observed a consistent, although not statistically significant, increase in total feeding amount and variations in the proportion of MOM, HDM, and formula, during postnatal weeks 1 to 16 for both male and female infants (Figure 1, Supplementary Figure S1). Initially, there was an increase in the proportion of MOM during the first 3 weeks of the NICU stay. However, the proportion of MOM and HDM feeding decreased over time, in contrast, the proportion of formula increased. Specifically, from the 2nd postnatal week to the 8th, the proportion of MOM decreased from 81.6% to 62.1% in males, and from 83.1% to 58.5% in females, there was no significant difference in the proportions of feeds between sexes (Table 2).

Figure 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. The weekly proportion of feeding regimen in the NICU. The weekly proportion of feeding regimens, including mother's own milk, Donor's milk, and Formula, is compared between males and females throughout their NICU stay from week 1 to 16.

Table 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Proportion of feeding types for male and female preterm infants at postnatal week 2 and week 8.

Sex-specific feeding and growth

Although both males and females showed similar patterns in growth, females had a slightly higher average growth z-score compared to males between week 9 to 16 postnatal age, (Figure 2), despite males having a significantly higher birth weight (Table 1) and receiving similar feeding proportions between week 2 to 8 (Figure 1). The proportion of MOM intake and birth weight showed a positive correlation with the growth z-score, while SNAPPEII was negatively correlated with the growth z-score in both males and females (all p < 0.05). From weeks 1 to 8, total feeding amount was found to be positively correlated with the growth z-score in female infants (p < 0.05), while no significant relationship was observed in males (Supplementary Table S2). Furthermore, the growth of females from 9 to 16 weeks was positively associated with a higher proportion of MOM intake during the first 8 weeks (p < 0.05), whereas the growth of males was associated with the total feeding amount at that time (p < 0.05) (Supplementary Table S2).

Figure 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Weekly mean growth trajectory by sex.

Proportion of MOM intake and neurobehavioral responses in the NICU

After adjusting for confounders such as birth weight, length, GA, early life painful and stressful experience, and delivery types in the regression models, we observed the association of sex-dependent patterns in how the feeding intake during the first 8 postnatal weeks with the neurobehavioral responses (NSTRESS and NAROUSAL) at NICU. The association between MOM intake and NSTRESS differed by sex, with a quadratic relationship between MOM intake and NSTRESS in females compared to males (1st order coefficient b = 0.02, p = 0.9; 2nd order coefficient b = −0.5, p < 0.05) (Supplementary Figure S2). Females consuming more than 1,000 cc of MOM per week had significantly lower NAROUSAL compared to males (coefficient b = −0.5, p < 0.05) (Supplementary Figure S3).

Proportion of MOM intake and neurodevelopment at 1- and 2-year follow-up

Unless specified otherwise in our findings, the MOM proportion refers to the period during the first 8 postnatal weeks. At the 1-year follow-up, no significant correlations were observed between MOM and Bayley scores. At 2 years of age, these associations differed by sex. In females, the proportion of MOM intake was positively correlated with cognitive scores (coefficient b = 15.8, p = 0.03) and language (coefficient b = 18.77, p = 0.01), whereas such correlation was not observed in males (Supplementary Table S3).

SHAP predicting sex-differentiated risk factors of early neurobehavior (NICU) and later neurodevelopment (1 and 2 years of age)

Overall, birth GA, birth weight, SNAPPEII, total feeding amount, total amount of MOM intake, proportion of MOM, and growth during 1 to 8 postnatal weeks emerged as significant predictive factors influencing early-life neurobehavioral outcomes and later neurodevelopment in both males and females (Figure 3). However, the relative importance of each specific risk factor varied between sexes. For males, the top 5 risk predictors, in order of importance, were birth weight, total MOM, SNAPPEII, growth slope, and birth gestational age. In females, the top 5 risk predictors, in order of importance, were growth slope, birth weight, birth GA, total feeding amount, and total MOM.

Figure 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. SHAP predicting risk factors for neurobehavioral and neurodevelopmental outcomes in infants. (a–c) Risk factors predicting NQMOVE, NAROUSAL, and NSTRESS. (d,e) Risk factors predicting Language and Motor skills at 8 to 12 months of corrected age (CA). (f–h) Risk factors predicting Cognitive, Language, and Motor skills at 18 to 24 months of CA.

Discussion

Sex differences become apparent early in development and persist into later stages in preterm infants (35), however, the underlying mechanisms remain poorly understood. Feeding regimen has emerged as a potential factor that may influence the sex-differentiated growth in infants (36). Therefore, our study investigated sex-specific response, such as early life growth (1 to 16 postnatal weeks), to different feeding regimens. This timeframe aligns with the “window of opportunity” identified in other studies, playing a critical role in shaping infants' health outcomes (37). We also consider week 1 to 8 as “early time” and week 9 to 16 as “later time” to look at even closer to the sex-varied growth during this window of opportunity. We observed the association of both total feeding amount and proportion of MOM with the growth of males and females is sensitive to their postnatal age. MOM proportion during postnatal weeks 1–8 is associated with the growth of both males and females. This association persists in females beyond 8 postnatal weeks, but not in males. In addition, early total feeding is also associated with females' growth but not for males. This could be explained by the different responses of females and males to the nutritional composition of feeding (38). Personalizing essential nutrient intake may help reduce gender differences. Furthermore, sex-differentiated breastmilk components, such as oligosaccharides (HMOs) (39), mineral (40), cortisol (41), and protein profiles (42) amplify this distinction. This disparity was referred to as “male disadvantage” (43). We suspected that females may benefit more from early MOM, with this benefit continuing as they age. This promotes essential nutrient intake and accelerated growth. This consideration arises while recognizing the growth of males might be “delayed” due to reduced MOM intake in their later life, or perhaps they require different nutrients not provided by MOM. Additionally, other components of breast milk, such as 8- hydroxy-2′-deoxyquanosine (8-OHdG), histone deacetylase sirtuins (SIRT3) and serotonin (5-HT) have been associated with inflammation and oxidative stress induced by premature and/or cesarian delivery (44). However, the sex specific effects of these breast milk components on infant growth remain unknown (44). Further research is needed to explore the association between MOM components and infant growth and neurodevelopment.

The earliest indication of sex-differentiated post-birth growth is reflected in males having significantly higher birth weights than females (45). Our study aligns with this pattern; however, we also observed a decrease in body weight over time in male infants, which was consistent with the findings of other study (46). In our study, body weights in both males and females exhibited a significant post-birth increase, although the growth was relatively slower, as suggested by the trajectories of their weight z-scores, possibly due to their non-term equivalent age (47). It is noteworthy that post 9 weeks after birth, females' growth (z-score) was faster than males. Interestingly, females had a higher intake of mother's breastmilk before week 9, and a higher intake of formula post week 9 in comparison to males, potentially contributing to their accelerated catch-up in body weight post week 9 compared to males. Our finding aligns with other studies indicating that formula intake is associated with fast increased weight gain (48). Although six-month exclusive breastfeeding is the recommended gold standard for infants' nutrition (49), our study observed that preterm infants often encountered medical issues and/or lactation difficulties, leading to disrupted breast milk expression. Therefore, supplementing breastfeeding with alternatives like HDM and formula is recommended to ensure their well-being when MOM is limited (50).

Although preterm infants typically undergo catch-up growth after birth, this rapid weight gain could bridge the growth gaps between preterm and term infants (51); however, it does not necessarily eliminate the risk of neurodevelopment disorders (5254). Our study observed that at 36 to 38 weeks of PMA, male infants demonstrated a higher level of fussing and crying during the assessment. In contrast, females showed a more mature neurobehavioral development if they received a higher amount of MOM before 8 postnatal weeks. This may be explained by the fact that earlier and higher MOM exposure boosts infants' brain structure (55) and connectivity (56). Further, whole brain volume increases in direct proportion to macronutrient and protein intake (57), however, a gender-specific scaling law exists, such that the cerebral cortex in males grows at a relatively slower rate compared to overall brain volume (58). Additionally, compared to females, resting neurophysiological network communication is more significantly altered in very preterm males, which further contribute to their disrupted cognitive outcomes later in childhood (at 8 years old) (59). Our study identified that the positive association between earlier and higher proportion of MOM exposure and better language and cognitive persist until the age of 2 in females. These findings were supported by other studies indicating the positive impact of earlier and longer breastfeeding on language ability performance and cognitive development (18, 6062, 63).

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first time using SHAP, one of the Machine Learning methods, to determine the importance of total feeding amount, the quantity of MOM, and the proportion of MOM within 8 weeks postnatal in predicting neurodevelopmental outcomes in both males and females from the NICU until their 2 years of age. Other studies found birth complications such as intubation, durations of ventilatory support, length of hospital stay, and whether the infant was receiving breastmilk can also be used as predictors to predict cognitive abilities at 2 years of age (64). Consistent with other studies that have highlighted factors such as birth weight, GA, SNAPPEII, etc. (65), our analysis also identified these as predictors of cognitive and language development. Males in our study had relatively older birth GA and higher birth weight, but experienced delayed growth compared with females, these findings underscore the importance of recognizing that males with older birth GA, yet experiencing delayed weight gain, may be at risk for delayed neurobehavioral outcomes. Notably, these sex-differentiated neurodevelopmental outcomes should be interpreted with caution, as comparable sex differences have also been reported in term infants (66). Our future study will include both preterm and term infants to investigate the impact of preterm birth risk on sex-related neurodevelopmental outcomes.

Various practical barriers and challenges impact breast milk utilization and infant care, including adverse maternal social determinants of health such as lack of family support, availability of caregiver, and knowledge of breastfeeding (67), single mother (68), younger maternal age and lower education level (69), and the inability of extremely preterm infants to breastfeed (69). To improve infant health outcomes, multiple strategies should be considered. First, breast milk intake should be recognized as a key parameter in NICU protocols for monitoring and assessing preterm infant outcomes. Additionally, promoting breast milk intake alongside early intervention programs, such as developmental therapies for preterm infants, may further enhance neurological outcomes (70, 71).

Strength and limitations

Large sample size with equally distributed sex and race subsamples, and subjects follow up from NICU until 1 and 2 years of CA providing a comprehensive understanding of infants' health outcomes. In addition, the SHAP analysis validated the results obtained from linear mixed models. This approach provided further insights into how each predictor influences the developmental outcomes in infants. However, the study has limitations. The composition such as neurotrophic factors, lipids, and proteins, or calories of MOM, HDM, formula, and fortification were not evaluated, which could offer insights into the biomechanisms underlying the clinical manifestations and sex-specific micronutrients and macronutrients. The duration of breast milk exposure is another factor we will consider in future studies to better understand its impact on infants' growth and neurodevelopment. Additionally, the study did not incorporate maternal social determinants of health, which could potentially broaden our understanding of the maternal factors influencing sex-specific feeding regimens in preterm infants.

Conclusion and relevance

Sex-specific MOM intake, but not HDM and formula, was associated with infants' growth and neurodevelopmental outcomes. In addition, sex-specific “window of opportunity” for feeding, growth, and development in early life and sex-specific risk predictors for neurodevelopment from the NICU to 2 years of CA can inform the development of tailored, sex-specific feeding plans. Our study further emphasizes that early initiation of breastfeeding and exclusive breastfeeding beyond 8 weeks after birth are critical for infants' growth and neurodevelopment. To meet their nutritional requirements for growth, infants—especially males —should receive appropriate supplements. Individualized assessment and feeding regimens should be provided to preterm infants during their NICU stay.

Data availability statement

The data generated and/or analyzed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Ethics statement

The studies involving humans were approved by Connecticut Children's Medical Center IRB Office (CCMC IRB#: 16-001). The studies were conducted in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent for participation in this study was provided by the participants' parents. Patient consent was required for our study. Eligible infants were identified by the research study personnel upon birth and/or admission to the NICU, and parental consent was obtained for their participation in our study.

Author contributions

TZ: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. AL: Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. XC: Data curation, Formal analysis, Software, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. WX: Validation, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. TQ: Visualization, Writing – review & editing. JC: Visualization, Writing – review & editing, Validation. AM: Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing. M-HC: Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Software, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. ST: Resources, Visualization, Writing – review & editing. XC: Conceptualization, Data curation, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.

Funding

The author(s) declare that financial support was received for the research and/or publication of this article. This study was supported by a grant from NIH/NCATS (TL1 TR001864), NIH/NINR (Grant numbers: F31NR019940, PI: Tingting Zhao), and ENRS Council for the Advancement of Nursing Science Dissertation Award (PI: Tingting Zhao). The study was also supported by a grant from NIH/NINR (grant number: R01NR016928, PI: Xiaomei Cong).

Conflict of interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Generative AI statement

The author(s) declare that no Generative AI was used in the creation of this manuscript.

Publisher's note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Supplementary material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fped.2025.1523952/full#supplementary-material

References

1. Shim SY, Cho SJ, Kong KA, Park EA. Gestational age-specific sex difference in mortality and morbidities of preterm infants: a nationwide study. Sci Rep. (2017) 7(1):6161. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-06490-8

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Zisk JL, Genen LH, Kirkby S, Webb D, Greenspan J, Dysart K. Do premature female infants really do better than their male counterparts? Am J Perinatol. (2011) 28(3):241–6. doi: 10.1055/s-0030-1268239

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Stevenson DK, Verter J, Fanaroff A, Oh W, Ehrenkranz R, Shankaran S, et al. Sex differences in outcomes of very low birthweight infants: the newborn male disadvantage. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2000) 83(3):F182–5. doi: 10.1136/fn.83.3.f182

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Garfinkle J, Yoon EW, Alvaro R, Nwaesei C, Claveau M, Lee SK, et al. Trends in sex-specific differences in outcomes in extreme preterms: progress or natural barriers? Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. (2020) 105(2):158–63. doi: 10.1136/archdischild-2018-316399

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

5. Kent AL, Wright IM, Abdel-Latif ME, New South Wales and Australian Capital Territory Neonatal Intensive Care Units Audit Group. Mortality and adverse neurologic outcomes are greater in preterm male infants. Pediatrics. (2012) 129(1):124–31. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-1578

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Hintz SR, Kendrick DE, Vohr BR, Kenneth Poole W, Higgins RD, Nichd Neonatal Research Network. Gender differences in neurodevelopmental outcomes among extremely preterm, extremely-low-birthweight infants. Acta Paediatr. (2006) 95(10):1239–48. doi: 10.1080/08035250600599727

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Stalnacke J, Lundequist A, Bohm B, Forssberg H, Smedler AC. A longitudinal model of executive function development from birth through adolescence in children born very or extremely preterm. Child Neuropsychol. (2019) 25(3):318–35. doi: 10.1080/09297049.2018.1477928

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Alur P. Sex differences in nutrition, growth, and metabolism in preterm infants. Front Pediatrics. (2019) 7:22. doi: 10.3389/fped.2019.00022

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Christmann V, Roeleveld N, Visser R, Janssen AJ, Reuser JJ, van Goudoever JB, et al. The early postnatal nutritional intake of preterm infants affected neurodevelopmental outcomes differently in boys and girls at 24 months. Acta Paediatr. (2017) 106(2):242–9. doi: 10.1111/apa.13669

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Alur P, Kalikkot Thekkeveedu R, Meeks M, Hart KC, Desai J, Johnson M, et al. Calorie intake is associated with weight gain during transition phase of nutrition in female extremely low birth weight infants. Biol Sex Differ. (2020) 11(1):16. doi: 10.1186/s13293-020-00295-7

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Tottman AC, Bloomfield FH, Cormack BE, Harding JE, Taylor J, Alsweiler JM. Sex-specific relationships between early nutrition and neurodevelopment in preterm infants. Pediatr Res. (2020) 87(5):872–8. doi: 10.1038/s41390-019-0695-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

12. van den Akker CH, te Braake FW, Weisglas-Kuperus N, van Goudoever JB. Observational outcome results following a randomized controlled trial of early amino acid administration in preterm infants. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr. (2014) 59(6):714–9. doi: 10.1097/MPG.0000000000000549

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Miliku K, Richelle J, Becker AB, Simons E, Moraes TJ, Stuart TE, et al. Sex-specific associations of human milk long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids and infant allergic conditions. Pediatr Allergy Immunol. (2021) 32(6):1173–82. doi: 10.1111/pai.13500

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Ballard O, Morrow AL. Human milk composition: nutrients and bioactive factors. Pediatr Clin North Am. (2013) 60(1):49–74. doi: 10.1016/j.pcl.2012.10.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Stam J, Sauer PJ, Boehm G. Can we define an infant’s need from the composition of human milk? Am J Clin Nutr. (2013) 98(2):521S–8. doi: 10.3945/ajcn.112.044370

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

16. Thakkar SK, Giuffrida F, Cristina CH, De Castro CA, Mukherjee R, Tran LA, et al. Dynamics of human milk nutrient composition of women from Singapore with a special focus on lipids. Am J Hum Biol. (2013) 25(6):770–9. doi: 10.1002/ajhb.22446

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

17. Baldeon ME, Zertuche F, Flores N, Fornasini M. Free amino acid content in human milk is associated with infant gender and weight gain during the first four months of lactation. Nutrients. (2019) 11(9):2239. doi: 10.3390/nu11092239

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Bzikowska-Jura A, Sobieraj P, Szostak-Wegierek D, Wesolowska A. Impact of infant and maternal factors on energy and macronutrient composition of human milk. Nutrients. (2020) 12(9):2591. doi: 10.3390/nu12092591

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Belfort MB, Knight E, Chandarana S, Ikem E, Gould JF, Collins CT, et al. Associations of maternal milk feeding with neurodevelopmental outcomes at 7 years of age in former preterm infants. JAMA Netw Open. (2022) 5(7):e2221608. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2022.21608

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

20. McCrory C, Murray A. The effect of breastfeeding on neuro-development in infancy. Matern Child Health J. (2013) 17(9):1680–8. doi: 10.1007/s10995-012-1182-9

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

21. Fusch C. Avoiding postnatal growth retardation by individualized fortification of breast milk: implications for somatic and neurodevelopmental outcomes. Breastfeed Med. (2019) 14(S1):S15–7. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2019.0031

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

22. Richardson DK, Corcoran JD, Escobar GJ, Lee SK. SNAP-II and SNAPPE-II: simplified newborn illness severity and mortality risk scores. J Pediatr. (2001) 138(1):92–100. doi: 10.1067/mpd.2001.109608

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Zhao T, Griffith T, Zhang Y, Li H, Hussain N, Lester B, et al. Early-life factors associated with neurobehavioral outcomes in preterm infants during NICU hospitalization. Pediatr Res. (2022) 92(6):1695–704. doi: 10.1038/s41390-022-02021-y

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Cong X, Judge M, Xu W, Diallo A, Janton S, Brownell EA, et al. Influence of feeding type on gut microbiome development in hospitalized preterm infants. Nurs Res. (2017) 66(2):123–33. doi: 10.1097/NNR.0000000000000208

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Newnham CA, Inder TE, Milgrom J. Measuring preterm cumulative stressors within the NICU: the neonatal infant stressor scale. Early Hum Dev. (2009) 85(9):549–55. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2009.05.002

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Cong X, Wu J, Vittner D, Xu W, Hussain N, Galvin S, et al. The impact of cumulative pain/stress on neurobehavioral development of preterm infants in the NICU. Early Hum Dev. (2017) 108:9–16. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2017.03.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

27. Lester BM, Tronick EZ, Brazelton TB. The neonatal intensive care unit network neurobehavioral scale procedures. Pediatrics. (2004) 113(3 Pt 2):641–67. doi: 10.1542/peds.113.S2.641

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Bayley N. Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development—third Edition. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Assessment, Inc. (2006). doi: 10.1037/t14978-000

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

29. George JM, Boyd RN, Colditz PB, Rose SE, Pannek K, Fripp J, et al. PPREMO: a prospective cohort study of preterm infant brain structure and function to predict neurodevelopmental outcome. BMC Pediatr. (2015) 15:123. doi: 10.1186/s12887-015-0439-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

30. Velikos K, Soubasi V, Michalettou I, Sarafidis K, Nakas C, Papadopoulou V, et al. Bayley-III scales at 12 months of corrected age in preterm infants: patterns of developmental performance and correlations to environmental and biological influences. Res Dev Disabil. (2015) 45–46:110–9. doi: 10.1016/j.ridd.2015.07.014

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Gałecki A, Burzykowski T. Linear Mixed-effects model. New York: Springer (2013).

Google Scholar

32. Chen T, Guestrin C. XGBoost: a scalable tree boosting system. Proceedings of the 22nd ACM SIGKDD International Conference on Knowledge Discovery and Data Mining. New York, NY, USA: ACM (2016). p. 785–94. doi: 10.1145/2939672.2939785

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Lundberg SM, Lee S-I. A unified approach to interpreting model predictions. Adv Neural Inf Process Syst. (2017) 30. doi: 10.48550/arXiv.1705.07874

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Rosseel Y. Lavaan: an R package for structural equation modeling. J Stat Softw. (2012) 48:1–36. doi: 10.18637/jss.v048.i02

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Inkster AM, Fernández-Boyano I, Robinson WP. Sex differences are here to stay: relevance to prenatal care. J Clin Med. (2021) 10(13):3000. doi: 10.3390/jcm10133000

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Klingenberg C, Muraas FK, Isaksen CE, Nilsen T, Torgersen M, Melum-Hansen C. Growth and neurodevelopment in very preterm infants receiving a high enteral volume-feeding regimen—a population-based cohort study. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med. (2019) 32(10):1664–72. doi: 10.1080/14767058.2017.1414796

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Zhonggui X, Ping Z, Jian K, Feimin S, Zeyuan X. The growth rates and influencing factors of preterm and full-term infants: a birth cohort study. Medicine (Baltimore). (2022) 101(34):e30262. doi: 10.1097/MD.0000000000030262

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Tottman AC, Oliver CJ, Alsweiler JM, Cormack BE. Do preterm girls need different nutrition to preterm boys? Sex-specific nutrition for the preterm infant. Pediatr Res. (2021) 89(2):313–7. doi: 10.1038/s41390-020-01252-1

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Borewicz K, Gu F, Saccenti E, Hechler C, Beijers R, de Weerth C, et al. The association between breastmilk oligosaccharides and fecal microbiota in healthy breastfed infants at two, six, and twelve weeks of age. Sci Rep. (2020) 10(1):4270. doi: 10.1038/s41598-02061024-z

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Naima K, Benhafsa FM, Khaled MB, Ghomari FN, Fizir M, Haoud K. Effect of infant’s gender on the mineral breast milk composition of Algerian breastfeeding women during lactation periods. J Food Compos Anal. (2023) 123:105577. doi: 10.1016/j.jfca.2023.105577

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Zielinska-Pukos MA, Bryś J, Kucharz N, Chrobak A, Wesolowska A, Grabowicz-Chądrzyńska I, et al. Factors influencing cortisol concentrations in breastmilk and its associations with breastmilk composition and infant development in the first six months of lactation. Int J Environ Res Public Health. (2022) 19(22):14809. doi: 10.3390/ijerph192214809

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Bernardes-Loch RM, Ribeiro AC, Ramírez-López CJ, Loch Gomes RA, Barros E, Filomeno Fontes EA, et al. Human milk proteins differentiate over the sex of newborns and across stages of lactation. Clin Nutr ESPEN. (2024) 62:144–56. doi: 10.1016/j.clnesp.2024.05.013

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

43. White-Traut R, Pham T, Rankin K, Norr K, Shapiro N, Yoder J. Exploring factors related to oral feeding progression in premature infants. Adv Neonatal Care. (2013) 13(4):288–94. doi: 10.1097/ANC.0b013e31829d8c5a

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Nyárády K, Turai R, Funke S, Györgyi E, Makai A, Prémusz V, et al. Effects of perinatal factors on sirtuin 3, 8-hydroxy-2′- deoxyguanosine, brain-derived neurotrophic factor and serotonin in cord blood and early breast milk: an observational study. Int Breastfeed J. (2020) 15(1):57. doi: 10.1186/s13006-020-00301-z

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Peacock JL, Marston L, Marlow N, Calvert SA, Greenough A. Neonatal and infant outcome in boys and girls born very prematurely. Pediatr Res. (2012) 71(3):305–10. doi: 10.1038/pr.2011.50

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Van Vliet G, Liu S, Kramer MS. Decreasing sex difference in birth weight. Epidemiology. (2009) 20(4):622. doi: 10.1097/EDE.0b013e3181a82806

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Chou FS, Yeh HW. Sex differences in postnatal weight gain trajectories of extremely preterm newborns. J Perinatol. (2021) 41(8):1835–44. doi: 10.1038/s41372-021-01099-2

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Quigley M, Embleton ND, McGuire W. Formula versus donor breast milk for feeding preterm or low birth weight infants. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. (2018) 6(6):CD002971. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD002971.pub4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Zielińska MA, Sobczak A, Hamułka J. Breastfeeding knowledge and exclusive breastfeeding of infants in first six months of life. Rocz Panstw Zakl Hig. (2017) 68(1):51–9.

Google Scholar

50. Whitburn S. Breastfeeding: expressing and supplementing. Aust J Gen Pract. (2022) 51(3):112–5. doi: 10.31128/AJGP-06-21-6021

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Martin A, Connelly A, Bland RM, Reilly JJ. Health impact of catch-up growth in low-birth weight infants: systematic review, evidence appraisal, and meta-analysis. Matern Child Nutr. (2017) 13(1):10.1111/mcn.12297. doi: 10.1111/mcn.12297

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Kindt A, Kraus Y, Rasp D, Foerster KM, Ahmidi N, Flemmer AW, et al. Improved macro- and micronutrient supply for favorable growth and metabolomic profile with standardized parenteral nutrition solutions for very preterm infants. Nutrients. (2022) 14(19):3912. doi: 10.3390/nu14193912

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Griffin IJ, Cooke RJ. Development of whole body adiposity in preterm infants. Early Hum Dev. (2012) 88(1):S19–24. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.12.023

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Ou-Yang MC, Sun Y, Liebowitz M, Chen CC, Fang ML, Dai W, et al. Accelerated weight gain, prematurity, and the risk of childhood obesity: a meta-analysis and systematic review. PLoS One. (2020) 15(5):e0232238. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0232238

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Sullivan G, Vaher K, Blesa M, Galdi P, Stoye DQ, Quigley AJ, et al. Breast milk exposure is associated with cortical maturation in preterm infants. Ann Neurol. (2023) 93(3):591–603. doi: 10.1002/ana.26559

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Blesa M, Sullivan G, Anblagan D, Telford EJ, Quigley AJ, Sparrow SA, et al. Early breast milk exposure modifies brain connectivity in preterm infants. Neuroimage. (2019) 184:431–9. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.045

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Cormack BE, Harding JE, Miller SP, Bloomfield FH. The influence of early nutrition on brain growth and neurodevelopment in extremely preterm babies: a narrative review. Nutrients. (2019) 11(9):2029. doi: 10.3390/nu11092029

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Koolschijn PC, Crone EA. Sex differences and structural brain maturation from childhood to early adulthood. Dev Cogn Neurosci. (2013) 5:106–18. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2013.02.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

59. Kozhemiako N, Nunes AS, Vakorin VA, Chau CMY, Moiseev A, Ribary U, et al. Sex differences in brain connectivity and male vulnerability in very preterm children. Hum Brain Mapp. (2020) 41(2):388–400. doi: 10.1002/hbm.24809

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

60. Belfort MB, Rifas-Shiman SL, Kleinman KP, Guthrie LB, Bellinger DC, Taveras EM, et al. Infant feeding and childhood cognition at ages 3 and 7 years: effects of breastfeeding duration and exclusivity. JAMA Pediatr. (2013) 167:836–44. doi: 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2013.455

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Kaya Özçora GD. The relationship between breast milk intake and speech in children with cerebral palsy. Turk J Med Sci. (2021) 51(4):1809–13. doi: 10.3906/sag-2011-43

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Rodrigues C, Zeitlin J, Zemlin M, Wilson E, Pedersen P, Barros H, et al. Never-breastfed children face a higher risk of suboptimal cognition at 2 years of corrected age: a multinational cohort of very preterm children. Matern Child Nutr. (2022) 18(3):e13347. doi: 10.1111/mcn.13347

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Mangili G, Garzoli E, Sadou Y. Feeding dysfunctions and failure to thrive in neonates with congenital heart diseases. Pediatr Med Chir. (2018) 40(1):10.4081/pmc.2018.196. doi: 10.4081/pmc.2018.196

Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Kim KM, Choi JW. Associations between breastfeeding and cognitive function in children from early childhood to school age: a prospective birth cohort study. Int Breastfeed J. (2020) 15(1):83. doi: 10.1186/s13006-020-00326-4

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Ahn SH, Kim SA. Assessment of preterm infants using the bayley-III scales in Korea. Ann Rehabil Med. (2017) 41(5):843–50. doi: 10.5535/arm.2017.41.5.843

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Lai DC, Tseng YC, Guo HR. Characteristics of young children with developmental delays and their trends over 14 years in Taiwan: a population-based nationwide study. BMJ Open. (2018) 8(5):e020994. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2017-020994

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Kopp SJ, Kelly EA, DeFranco EA. Influence of social determinants of health on breastfeeding intent in the United States. Birth. (2023) 50(4):858–67. doi: 10.1111/birt.12740

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Carpay NC, Kakaroukas A, Embleton DN, van Elburg RM. Barriers and facilitators to breastfeeding in moderate and late preterm infants: a systematic review. Breastfeed Med. (2021) 16(5):370–84. doi: 10.1089/bfm.2020.0379

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Zukova S, Krumina V, Buceniece J. Breastfeeding preterm born infant: chance and challenge. Int J Pediatr Adolesc Med. (2021) 8(2):94–7. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpam.2020.02.003

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

70. Merhar SL, Miller SP. Maternal milk in the NICU: an everyday intervention to improve brain development. Pediatr Res. (2024) 96(1):27–9. doi: 10.1038/s41390-024-03125-3

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Silveira RC, Valentini NC, O’Shea TM, Mendes EW, Froes G, Cauduro L, et al. Parent-guided developmental intervention for infants with very low birth weight: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Network Open. (2024) 7(7):e2421896. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2024.21896

PubMed Abstract | Crossref Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: preterm, neurodevelopment, feeding, sex, breastmilk

Citation: Zhao T, Li A, Chang X, Xu W, Quinn T, Chen J, Matson AP, Chen M-H, Taylor SN and Cong X (2025) Sex-differences in Mothers' own milk and neurodevelopmental outcomes in preterm infants. Front. Pediatr. 13:1523952. doi: 10.3389/fped.2025.1523952

Received: 6 November 2024; Accepted: 11 March 2025;
Published: 28 March 2025.

Edited by:

Xinran Liu, Peking University People's Hospital, China

Reviewed by:

Augusto Biasini, Italian Association of Donated Milk Banks (AIBLUD), Italy
Marcel Henrique Marcondes Sari, Federal University of Paraná, Brazil

Copyright: © 2025 Zhao, Li, Chang, Xu, Quinn, Chen, Matson, Chen, Taylor and Cong. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xiaomei Cong, xiaomei.cong@yale.edu

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.